Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychologia. 2009 Mar 28;47(10):2044–2055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.015

Preserved frontal memorial processing for pictures in patients with mild cognitive impairment

Brandon A Ally 1,2, Joshua D McKeever 1,2, Jill D Waring 1,2,3, Andrew E Budson 1,2
PMCID: PMC2724267  NIHMSID: NIHMS106233  PMID: 19467355

Abstract

Amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) has been conceptualized as a transitional stage between healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease (AD). Therefore, understanding which aspects of memory are impaired and which remain relatively intact in these patients can be useful in determining who will ultimately go on to develop AD, and subsequently designing interventions to help patients live more engaged and independent lives. The dual-process model posits that recognition memory decisions can rely on either familiarity or recollection. Whereas research is fairly consistent in showing impaired recollection in patients with aMCI, the results have been mixed regarding familiarity. A noted difference between these studies investigating familiarity has been stimulus type. The goal of the current investigation was to use high-density event-related potentials (ERPs) to help elucidate the neural correlates of recognition decisions in patients with aMCI for words and pictures. We also hoped to help answer the question of whether patients can rely on familiarity to support successful recognition. Patients and controls participated in separate recognition memory tests of words and pictures while ERPs were recorded during retrieval. Results showed that ERP components typically associated with familiarity and retrieval monitoring were similar between groups for pictures. However, these components were diminished in the patient group for words. Based on recent work, the authors discuss the possibility that implicit conceptual priming could have contributed to the enhanced ERP correlate of familiarity. Further, the authors address the possibility that enhanced retrieval monitoring may be needed to modulate increased familiarity engendered by pictures.

Keywords: familiarity, recognition memory, picture superiority effect, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, event-related potentials

1. Introduction

Researchers and clinicians conceptualize mild cognitive impairment (MCI) as a transitional state between healthy aging and dementia (Petersen, 2004). Petersen and colleagues have divided MCI into distinct subtypes based on whether these patients demonstrate memory impairment (amnestic subtype) or impairment in some other cognitive domain (nonamnestic subtype). The amnestic subtype (aMCI) has received a great deal of attention, as it has been associated with a estimated tenfold increase in yearly conversion rate to Alzheimer's disease (AD) compared to age-matched controls with no cognitive impairment (Petersen, 2004). Characterization of the exact nature of the memory impairment associated with the amnestic subtype has only recently taken shape. Using experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience to characterize aspects of memory early in the disease course can be useful in not only helping to determine who will go on to ultimately develop AD, but also in designing interventions to help patients live more engaged and independent lives by relying on aspects of memory that remain relatively intact.

In understanding how memorial decisions are made, researchers have proposed a dual-process model of recognition memory (Jacoby and Dallas, 1981; Yonelinas, 1994). Although there is much debate as to whether these two processes are completely independent, the dual-process model posits that recognition decisions can rely on the processes of recollection or familiarity (Yonelinas, 1994). Recollection refers to the retrieval of specific context-bound information about an item or event, whereas familiarity is defined as a more general, acontextual sense that an item or event has been previously encountered. These two constructs are often commonly experienced in daily life. For example, the unexpected sight of a particular man on a crowded city street may elicit an immediate feeling of knowing him without being able to produce any specific details about who he is or how he is known. After some deliberation, details may come to mind regarding the man's identity – perhaps the waiter at a restaurant you had visited one week earlier. Familiarity describes the initial feeling of knowing the man without being able to place him, while recollection captures the subsequent remembering of the specific details of his identity.

Recollection has been well studied in both healthy and diseased memory. Research suggests that the hippocampus (Cansino, Maquet, Dolan, and Rugg, 2002; Dobbins, Rice, Wagner, and Schacter, 2003; Eldridge, Knowlton, Furmanski, Bookheimer, and Engel, 2000; Yonelinas, Otten, Shaw, and Rugg, 2005), prefrontal regions (Burgess and Shallice, 1996; Dobbins, Foley, Schacter, and Wagner, 2002; Simons, Owen, Fletcher, and Burgess, 2005), and parietal regions (Ally, Simons, McKeever, Peers, and Budson, 2008; Skinner and Fernandes, 2007; Wagner, Shannon, Kahn, and Buckner, 2005) are activated during recollection. Event-related potential (ERP) studies of recognition memory associate recollection with a medial parietal old/new effect where studied items elicit more positive scalp activity than unstudied items (see Rugg and Curran 2007; Friedman and Johnson, 2000 for reviews). This parietal effect increases with study–test repetitions (Johnson, Hashtroudi, and Lindsay, 1993), when items are rated as consciously recollected (Smith, 1993; Smith and Guster, 1993), and when source information is retrieved along with the studied item (Trott, Friedman, Ritter, Fabiani, and Snodgrass, 1999; Wilding, Doyle, and Rugg, 1995; Wilding and Rugg, 1996). However, recent work with parietal lesion patients has shown that parietal cortex is not critical to successful memory retrieval (Ally et al., 2008b; Simons, Peers, Hwang, Ally, Fletcher, and Budson, 2008), and in fact, an ERP study has shown that parietal regions are activated to a similar degree for falsely remembered items as they are for correctly remembered items (Goldmann, Sullivan, Droller, Rugg, Curran, Holcomb, et al., 2003). Although the exact role of parietal cortex in episodic retrieval remains unclear at present, recent hypotheses have suggested perhaps the parietal old/new effect indexes the amount of information retrieved (Fjell, Walhovd, and Reinvang, 2005; Vilberg, Moosavi, and Rugg, 2006), subjective aspects of recollection (Ally et al. 2008a; Davidson, Anaki, Ciaramelli, Cohn, Kim, Murphy, et al., 2008), or attention to memorial representations (Cabeza, Ciaramelli, Olson, and Moscovitch, 2008; Ciaramelli, Grady, and Moscovitch, 2008).

A number of behavioral, neuropsychological, and ERP studies have suggested that recollection becomes degraded during the normal aging process (Daselaar, Fleck, Dobbins, Madden, and Cabeza, 2006; Fjell et al., 2005; Joyce, Paller, McIsaac, and Kutas, 1998; Howard, Bessette-Symons, Zhang, and Hoyer, 2006; Jacoby, 1999; Jennings and Jacoby, 1993, 1997; Morcom and Rugg, 2004; Nielsen-Bohlman and Knight, 1995; Rugg, Mark, Gilchrist, and Roberts, 1997; Rybash and Hoyer, 1996; Senkfor and Van Petten, 1998; Swick and Knight, 1997; Spencer and Raz, 1995; Titov and Knight, 1997; Yonelinas, 2001). It has been hypothesized that a decline in the attentional resources allocated at encoding and/or retrieval, perhaps due to frontal lobe changes associated with normal aging, may be responsible for a decrease in recollection in this group (Anderson, Craik, and Naveh-Benjamin, 1998; Park, Smith, Dudley, and Lafronza, 1989; Salthouse, 1994; Whiting and Smith, 1997). In addition to the changes that accompany normal aging, patients with aMCI and AD undergo pathological changes in brain regions critical to recollection. By the time memory loss is clinically evident, warranting a diagnosis of aMCI, significant AD neurofibrillary pathology is seen in limbic regions, including transentorhinal regions, perirhinal cortex, amygdala, nucleus basalis (Arriagada, Growdon, Hedley-Whyte, and Hyman, 1992; Braak and Braak, 1991; Mesulam, 2000; Van Hoesen, Hyman, and Damasio, 1991), and most prominently in hippocampus and entorhinal cortex (Gomez-Isla, Price, McKeel, Morris, Growdon, and Hyman, 1996). These regions continue to be affected as AD progresses (Mesulam,1999), with pathology spreading further to neocortical areas such as temporal, parietal, occipital association, and frontal cortex in clinical AD (Braak and Braak, 1991; Delacourte, David, Sergeant, Buee, Wattez, Vermersch, et al.,1999; Grady, Haxby, Horwitz, Sundaram, Berg, Schapiro, et al., 1988; Ibanez, Pietrini, Alexander, Furey, Teichberg, Rajapakse, et al., 1998; McKee, Au, Cabral, Kowall, Seshadri, Kubilus, et al., 2006).

Consistent with these areas of pathological involvement, behavioral studies suggest that recollection is impaired in patients with aMCI (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009a; Westerberg, Paller, Weintraub, Mesulam, Holdstock, Mayes, et al., 2006; Wolk, Signoff, and DeKosky, 2008) and patients with mild AD (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009a; Balota, Burgess, Cortese, and Adams, 2002; Budson, Daffner, Desikan, and Schacter, 2000; Gallo, Sullivan, Daffner, Schacter, and Budson, 2004; Koivisto, Portin, Seinela, and Rinne, 1998; Knight, 1998; Smith and Knight, 2002). It has been proposed that in the face of impaired recollection, patients with mild AD become over reliant on familiarity (Budson et al., 2000; Budson, Desikan, Daffner, and Schacter, 2001; Budson, Sullivan, Mayer, Daffner, Black, and Schacter, 2002; Gallo et al., 2004; Pierce, Sullivan, Schacter, and Budson, 2005; Smith and Knight, 2002). Though the evidence is fairly clear that familiarity is aberrant in patients with AD (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009a; Budson et al., 2000, Budson, Sullivan, Meyer, Daffner, Black, and Schacter, 2002; Gallo et al., 2004; Pierce et al., 2005; Westerberg et al., 2006), it remains unclear as to whether this process is impaired or disrupted in patients with aMCI.

ERP research has typically associated familiarity with an early midfrontal old/new effect, or FN400, where familiar unstudied items elicit more positive scalp activity between 300 and 500 ms than unfamiliar unstudied items (Ally and Budson, 2007; Curran, 2000; Goldmann et al., 2003; Rugg and Curran, 2007; Rugg, Mark, Walla, Schloerscheidt, Birch, and Allan, 1998). Woodruff, Hayama, and Rugg (2006) showed that the magnitude of the early frontal old/new effect varied directly with familiarity strength based on confidence ratings. Studies of healthy older adults show that familiarity remains intact (Ally, Waring, McKeever, Milberg, and Budson, 2008; Howard, et al., 2006; Yonelinas, 2001), whereas the evidence for intact familiarity in patients with aMCI remains mixed. Westerberg and colleagues (2006) administered two separate picture recognition memory tests to groups of healthy older adults, patients with aMCI, and patients with mild AD. The first test required subjects to make standard old/new recognition memory decisions, while the second test required subjects to make forced-choice recognition decisions in which the target was grouped with highly related foils. Based on earlier evidence (Bastin and Van der Linden, 2003; Gardiner, Java, and Richardson-Klavehn, 1998), it has been suggested that standard recognition memory tests rely more on the process of recollection, whereas forced-choice tests rely more on familiarity. Results of Westerberg et al. (2006) showed that patients with aMCI and mild AD performed significantly worse on the standard old/new test compared to the healthy older adults, but performance on the forced-choice test was indistinguishable for the aMCI group and the healthy older adults. The authors concluded that while recollection is impaired in patients with aMCI, familiarity remains intact, and that it could be successfully used even in difficult tasks requiring subjects to identify a target from highly similar foils.

In contrast, two recent studies using words found impaired estimates of familiarity in patients with aMCI. In a multi-experiment study, Wolk, Signoff, and DeKosky (2008) used process-dissociation and task-dissociation techniques to show that patients with aMCI demonstrated both impaired recollection and familiarity. Similarly, Ally, Gold, and Budson (2009a) examined recollection and familiarity in patients with aMCI and mild AD using confidence-based receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves and a depth of processing manipulation. Patients with aMCI showed decreased estimates of recollection and familiarity in both shallow and deep encoding conditions compared to the healthy older adults. However, estimates of familiarity for the aMCI group were not as impaired as the mild AD group. It should be noted that in both the Wolk et al. and Ally et al. studies, when the two processes were directly compared, familiarity was estimated to be at least as impaired as recollection in patients with aMCI. Because previous research has shown that recollection tends to be impaired in healthy older adults, but familiarity remains relatively spared, Wolk, Signoff, and DeKosky (2008) proposed that impairment in familiarity may reflect early tangle pathology in the perirhinal and entorhinal regions, thereby being a possible marker for early pathological changes indicative of future AD.

Ally, Gold, and Budson (2009a) discussed stimulus type as a possible reason for the conflicting results regarding familiarity in patients with aMCI. Westerberg and colleagues used pictures, whereas Wolk et al. and Ally et al. used words. It is entirely plausible that pictures and words differentially affect familiarity in patients with aMCI. This hypothesis has some support from Ally et al., (2008b), which showed that pictures differentially affected recollection in healthy older adults. The goal of the current investigation is to use ERPs to help elucidate the neural correlates of recognition for pictures versus words in patients with aMCI, and to provide neurophysiologic evidence as to whether familiarity might remain intact in these patients. Based on the findings of Westerberg et al. (2006), we predicted that for pictures, early frontal ERP activity would be similar for patients with aMCI and healthy older adults, suggesting intact familiarity. In contrast, based on Wolk et al. (2008) and Ally et al. (2009a) we predicted that for words, early frontal ERP activity would be degraded in patients with aMCI compared to healthy older adults, suggesting impaired familiarity. In addition to examining the early frontal effect, we also examined the parietal and late frontal effects. As stated earlier, the parietal effect is thought to reflect recollection, and typically occurs between 500 and 800 ms post-stimulus. We predicted that the parietal effect would be significantly diminished in patients with aMCI for both pictures and words, suggesting impaired recollection. The late frontal effect, which is thought to reflect executive retrieval monitoring, typically occurs at right anterior frontal regions between 1000 ms and 1800 ms post-stimulus (Allan, Wilding, and Rugg, 1998; Ranganath, Heller, and Wilding, 2007; Wilding and Rugg, 1996). Executive abilities remain intact in a number of patients with aMCI, suggesting that that there are qualitative differences between aMCI and AD (Bisiacchi et al., 2008; Voss and Bullock, 2004). In fact, Bisiachhi et al., (2008) proposed that executive dysfunction may be the separating feature signifying a pathological change in the brain leading to dementia. Therefore, we predicted that the late frontal effect would remain intact, or even be enhanced as a compensatory mechanism, in patients with aMCI for pictures and words, suggesting intact executive retrieval monitoring.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design Overview

Each subject completed two separate study-test sessions consisting of words and pictures. Each session consisted of 50 stimuli at study and 100 stimuli at test (50 old, 50 new). Order of study-test condition was counterbalanced across subjects, with a 10-minute break between sessions. High-density ERPs were recorded at test.

2.2. Subjects

Eighteen healthy older adults (9 female) with a mean education of 15.2 years, and an age range of 69 to 83 (mean 74.6) participated in the experiment. Eighteen patients with a diagnosis of probable aMCI (Petersen, 2004) (8 female) with a mean education of 17.3 years, and an age range of 62 to 86 (mean 71.8) participated in the experiment. Patients with aMCI (either single or multiple domain) in the current study reported a subjective memory compliant, showed abnormal memory performance for their age as evidenced by performing greater than 1.5 standard deviations below the healthy adult group on either the recall or the recognition portion of the word list memory test of the CERAD, and did not display functional impairment according to caregiver report. Subjects were excluded if they had a history of cerebrovascular disease, another neurodegenerative disorder, another brain disorder or injury, or if they were currently being treating for a psychiatric disorder such as major depression. Healthy older adults were also excluded if they had a first-degree relative with a history of a memory disorder or Alzheimer's disease. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants and from their caregivers where appropriate. Participants were paid $25/hour for their participation.

All subjects were right-handed, English was their native language, and subjects were required to have corrected 20/30 or better color vision. The human subjects committees of the Bedford VA Hospital and Boston University School of Medicine approved the study. All subjects completed a brief neuropsychological battery to evaluate their cognitive functioning, administered in a separate 45-minute session. Subjects were first administered the Mini Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, and McHugh, 1975), for which a score of 27 or better was required for the healthy older adults. Subjects were then administered the CERAD word list memory test (Morris, Heyman, Mohs, Hughes, van Belle, Fillenbaum, et al., 1989), Trail Making Test Part B (Adjutant General's Office, 1944), Verbal Fluency (Monsch, Bondi, Butters, Salmon, Katzman, and Thal, 1992), and the 15-item Boston Naming Test (Mack, Freed, William, and Henderson, 1992). Demographic and neuropsychological test data can be seen in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographic and standard neuropsychological test data by group.

OC aMCI
Age 74.6 (4.1) 71.8 (9.2)
Years education 15.2 (2.7) 17.3 (4.2)
MMSE 29.4 (0.8) 28.1 (1.3)
CERAD
Immediate 22.3 (3.1) 14.1 (4.6)
Delayed 7.6 (1.4) 2.5 (1.8)
Recognition 9.9 (0.4) 7.8 (1.7)
Trails-B 83.5 (22.2) 117.7 (66.3)
FAS 48.6 (7.4) 40.7 (11.4)
CAT 50.4 (11.6) 35.4 (13.7)
BNT-15
No cue 14.5 (1.0) 13.6 (1.5)
Semantic cue 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.5)
Phonetic cue 0.4 (0.9) 0.9 (1.3)

Notes: OC = healthy older adults; MMSE = Mini Mental State Examination; CERAD = CERAD Word List Memory Test; Trails-B = Trail Making Test Part B; FAS and CAT = Verbal Fluency; BNT-15 = 15-item Boston Naming Test.

2.3. Experimental Material and Methods

The color pictures used in the current study were the same stimuli set used by Ally and Budson (2007). The original pool of experimental stimuli consisted of 480 high-resolution clip-art style color pictures of nameable objects (nouns) and 480 words corresponding to the names of the objects. From the total pool, 200 pictures were randomly selected and were piloted in a naming study involving 5 subjects. Pictures with poor naming reliability scores were replaced as needed. The final 200 items were counterbalanced across study-test lists for word length, concreteness, and imagibility ratings. In addition, condition order (pictures, words) and study status (old, new) were counterbalanced across subjects. Color pictures were presented in central vision on a white background, with an average height of 13 cm and an average width of 15 cm, and a visual angle subtended of 7°. Words were presented in central vision in black uppercase letters 4 cm in height, also on a white background, with a visual angle subtended of 5.6°. All stimuli were presented on a 21-inch flat screen computer monitor positioned 48 inches from the subject.

Each trial began with a 1000 ms fixation character (“+”) prior to the presentation of the stimuli. Study stimuli were then presented for 2000 ms followed by the question, “Do you like this item?” Subjects were then prompted to button press to signify their like/dislike judgment, and to remember the items for a subsequent memory test. Test stimuli were presented for 1500 ms, followed by the question, “Is this item old or new?” Subjects were then prompted to verbally signify their old/new judgment, and the experimenter entered the response on a button box. Subjects were asked to hold their responses until the question appeared immediately after stimuli presentation to minimize response-related ERP artifact during the commonly used 1800 ms epoch. We acknowledge that asking participants to keep their response “in mind” (or alternatively, inhibiting their natural inclination to respond before the prompt) may affect the electrophysiological data; particularly the late frontal effect. However, because subjects would be engaging in this activity in all trials, this activity should be removed when subtracting correct rejections from hits.

2.4. ERP Procedure

Subjects were seated in a hardback chair and fitted with an Active Two electrode cap (Behavioral Brain Sciences Center, Birmingham, UK). A full array of 128 Ag-AgCl BioSemi (Amsterdam, Netherlands) “active” electrodes were connected to the cap in a pre-configured montage which places each electrode in equidistant concentric circles from 10-20 position Cz (Web Appendix Figure A). Active electrodes are amplified through the electrode at the source and do not require abrading of the skin or measuring skin-electrode impedance levels. In addition to the 128 scalp electrodes, two mini-biopotential electrodes were placed on each mastoid process. Finally, vertical and horizontal EOG activity was recorded from bipolar electrodes placed below the left eye and on the outer canthus of the left and right eye. EEG and EOG activity was amplified with a bandwidth of 0.03-35 Hz (3 dB points) and digitized at a sampling rate of 256 Hz. Recordings were referenced to a vertex reference point, but were later re-referenced to a common average reference to minimize the effects of reference-site activity and accurately estimate the scalp topography of the measured electrical fields (Ally and Budson, 2007; Curran, DeBuse, Woroch, and Hirshman, 2006; Dien, 1998; Norman, Tepe, Nyhus, and Curran, 2008).

The sampling epoch for each test trial lasted for a total of 2000 ms, which included a 200 ms pre-stimulus baseline period. This pre-stimulus period was used to baseline correct averaged ERP epochs lasting 1800 ms. ERPs were averaged and corrected using the EMSE Software Suite (Source Signal Imaging, San Diego, CA). Trials were corrected for excessive EOG activity using the EMSE Ocular Artifact Correction Tool. The tool first allows the investigator to manually distinguish artifact data from artifact-free data. Then, using a covariance technique that simultaneously models artifact and artifact-free EEG, a logarithmic ratio of artifact data to clean data is produced by EMSE. Finally, ocular artifact is subtracted from the recording where it is detected by the correction tool. Trials were discarded from the analyses if they contained baseline drift or movement greater than 90 μV. Individual bad channels were corrected with the EMSE spatial interpolation filter.

3. Results

3.1 Behavioral Performance

Recognition accuracy was calculated using the discrimination index Pr (%Hits - %False Alarms; Snodgrass and Corwin, 1988) to compare the performance of the healthy older adults and the patients with aMCI. A repeated measures ANOVA with the factors of Group (older adults, aMCI) and Condition (word, picture) was performed. The results of the ANOVA revealed an effect of Group [F(1, 30) = 13.18, p = .001], such that overall accuracy was better for the healthy older adults compared to the patients with aMCI, and an effect of Condition [F(1, 30) = 38.69, p < .001], indicating that pictures were better remembered than words for both groups. There was also an interaction of Group and Condition [F(1, 30) = 5.66, p = .024]. Follow-up analyses of this interaction shows that although both healthy older adults [t(15) = 3.68, p = .002] and patients with aMCI [t(15) = 4.94, p < .001] demonstrated greater memory for pictures than for words, this picture superiority effect was larger in the patients with aMCI [t(15) = 2.34, p = .028]. Further analyses of the aMCI data revealed more hits and fewer false alarms for the picture compared to the word condition [t(15) = 2.52, p = .025, t(15) = 2.94, p = .011]. Table 2 shows the behavioral data for both groups.

Table 2.

Behavioral performance for healthy older adults and patients with aMCI using the discrimination index Pr (percent hits – percent false alarms).

Percent Hits Percent FA Pr
Older Adults
 Picture .96 .07 .89
 Word .88 .08 .80
aMCI
 Picture .91 .15 .76
 Word .79 .24 .55

In addition to accuracy, response bias was analyzed using the measure C (Snodgrass and Corwin, 1988).1 A repeated measures ANOVA with the factors of Group (older adults, aMCI) and Condition (word, picture) was performed. There were no main effects of Group [F(1, 30) < 1] or Condition [F(1, 30) = 1.41, p = .245), and there was no interaction of Group and Condition [F(2, 30) < 1].

3.2. ERP Results

Two separate sets of analyses were performed on the ERP data. The first set of analyses was guided by previous research, and began with ANOVAs performed on selected time intervals (300-500 ms, 500-800 ms, 1000-1800 ms) previously associated with three components of the old/new ERP effect (early frontal effect, parietal effect, late frontal effect, respectively) as described above (Ally and Budson, 2007; Budson, Droller, Dodson, Schacter, Rugg, Holcomb, et al., 2005; Curran et al., 2006; Curran, 2000, 2004). Mean amplitudes were calculated for these three time intervals, which were then averaged across groups of seven electrodes that formed eight separate regions of interest [Left Anterior Inferior (LAI), Right Anterior Inferior (RAI), Left Anterior Superior (LAS), Right Anterior Superior (RAS), Left Posterior Superior (LPS), Right Posterior Superior (RPS), Left Posterior Inferior (LPI), and Right Posterior Inferior (RPI), see Web Appendix Figure A]. Regions included in the ANOVA depended on the time interval of interest, such that the four anterior regions were used for the early and late frontal effects (LAI, RAI, LAS, RAS) and the two parietal regions were used for the parietal effect (LPS, RPS). The older adult control waveforms and scalp topographies were formed from a mean (range) of 43 (39-50) hits and 46 (31-50) correct rejections for the word condition and 47 (43-50) hits and 47 (42-49) correct rejections for the picture condition. The MCI waveforms and scalp topographies were formed from a mean of 39 (21-48) hits and 38 (23-48) correct rejections for the word condition and 44 (35-50) hits and 42 (34-49) correct rejections for the picture condition. The older adult control group had an average of 3.9 trials rejected due to artifact or drift in the word condition and 2.9 trials rejected in the picture condition. In contrast, the MCI group had an average of 5.5 trials rejected in the word condition and 3.1 trials rejected in the picture condition. There were no statistical differences between groups in the number of trials rejected for words [t(30) = 0.636, p = 0.53] or pictures [t(30) = 0.061, p = 0.95]. Older adult and aMCI grand average hit and correct rejection ERP waveforms for our two study-test condition comparisons can be seen in Figures 1 and 2.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Older adult and aMCI grand average hit and correct rejection ERP waveforms for the word condition. Each waveform represents the composite average of the seven electrodes subsuming each ROI. Components of interest are highlighted in gray: EF = early frontal effect (300-500ms), PE = parietal effect (500-800ms), LFE = late frontal effect (1000-1800ms). ROIs are listed to the left of each waveform: Left Anterior Inferior (LAI), Right Anterior Inferior (RAI), Left Anterior Superior (LAS), Right Anterior Superior (RAS), Left Posterior Superior (LPS), Right Posterior Superior (RPS), Left Posterior Inferior (LPI), and Right Posterior Inferior (RPI).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Older adult and aMCI grand average hit and correct rejection ERP waveforms for the picture condition. Each waveform represents the composite average of the seven electrodes subsuming each ROI. Components of interest are highlighted in gray: EF = early frontal effect (300-500ms), PE = parietal effect (500-800ms), LFE = late frontal effect (1000-1800ms). ROIs are listed to the left of each waveform: Left Anterior Inferior (LAI), Right Anterior Inferior (RAI), Left Anterior Superior (LAS), Right Anterior Superior (RAS), Left Posterior Superior (LPS), Right Posterior Superior (RPS), Left Posterior Inferior (LPI), and Right Posterior Inferior (RPI).

The second set of analyses consisted of nonparametric permutation tests performed on the old/new scalp topographies for the older adult and aMCI groups seen in Figure 3. Typically used in imaging studies to compare voxels between two different conditions, non-parametric permutation tests can be useful in analyzing high density ERP data (Galan, Biscay, Rodriguez, Perez-Abalo, and Rodriguez, 1997; Greenblatt and Pflieger, 2004; Karniski, Blair, and Snider, 1994). Using this type of analysis allows researchers to use the entire data set rather than averaging over hundreds of millisecond and groups of electrodes, thereby avoiding losing valuable temporal and spatial data. The permutation test calculates the statistical probability of differences between groups or conditions in p-value form at every electrode site for every millisecond of recorded data. We chose to present the data in 50 ms time intervals. Note that these topographic maps represent an average of 50 ms going forward from the labeled time, such that 0 ms is the average from 0 to 49 ms, etc. Non-parametric permutation tests were used to examine differences in these topographic maps, and can be seen in Web Appendix Figures B and C. Only significant statistics will be discussed.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Older adult and aMCI old/new scalp topography maps for the (a) word and (b) picture conditions. Topographies are presented in 50 ms averages going forward.

The initial ANOVA focused on the 300 to 500 ms time interval and used the factors of Group (older adults, aMCI), Condition (word, picture), Item Type (hit, correct rejection) and ROI (LAI, RAI, LAS, RAS). These results found a main effect of Group [F(1, 30) = 6.29, p = .018], such that waveforms were more positive overall for older adults than for patients with aMCI. A main effect of Condition [F(1, 30) = 57.72, p < .001] revealed that words were more positive overall than pictures, and an effect of ROI [F(3, 87) = 10.05, p < .001] showed that left frontal regions were more positive than right frontal regions. There were also significant interactions of Item Type and Condition [F(1, 30) = 13.18, p = .001], and Item Type and Group [F(1, 30) = 7.78, p = .009]. Follow-up ANOVAs showed an overall effect of Item Type for the healthy older adults [F(1, 15) = 17.68, p = .001], but not for the patients with aMCI [F(1, 15) = 2.16, p = .162]. However, the aMCI group showed a significant interaction of Item Type and Condition [F(1, 15) = 11.67, p = .004]. The post-hoc ANOVAs for the aMCI group revealed an effect of Item Type for pictures [F(1, 15) = 9.274, p = .010], but not for words [F(1, 15) < 1]. Follow-up between-subjects t-tests for the word condition revealed that the magnitude of the early frontal old/new effect was greater for the older adults than for the aMCI patients at three of the four frontal regions for words [LAI, t(29) = 2.27, p = .031; LAS, t(29) = 2.47, p = .019; RAS, t(29) = 2.16, p = .039] and a trend towards significance at the fourth [RAI, t(29) = 1.95, p = .061]. There were no between-group differences for pictures. These results show that the early frontal effect was present for both words and pictures in the older adults, but only for pictures in the aMCI group.

These between-subjects ANOVA and t-test results were supported by nonparametric permutation test findings. Scalp topographies for the word condition can be seen in Figure 3a. Between 300 and 500 ms, the older adult group showed the classic old/new effect at right superior and inferior right frontal regions. In contrast, the aMCI group demonstrated a negative old/new effect at left frontal regions, with a positive old/new effect is seen at more posterior regions. These between group differences are highlighted by the nonparametric analysis results. As can be seen in Web Appendix Figure B(1), p-value maps showed a greater old/new effect at frontal electrodes for the older adults compared to the patients with aMCI from approximately 250 to 750 ms in the word condition, because only the healthy older adults showed an early frontal old/new effect for words.

Scalp topographies for the picture condition can be seen in Figure 3b. Similar to the word condition, healthy older adults showed the classic old/new effect for pictures at right superior and inferior frontal regions during the 300 to 500 ms time interval. In contrast to the brain activity elicited in the word condition, patients with aMCI showed a bilateral old/new effect at frontal regions during this time interval for the picture condition. These results are supported by the nonparametric analysis. As can be seen in Web Appendix Figure B(2), there were only very small differences at frontal regions from approximately 400 to 450 ms during this early time interval between the two groups for the picture condition, because both the healthy older adults and the aMCI group showed an early frontal effect for pictures.

The within-subjects ANOVA and t-tests findings for the aMCI group are also supported by nonparametric analyses. As can be seen in Web Appendix Figure C(2), the within-subject p-value maps for the aMCI group show that magnitude of the old/new effect is greater at frontal regions beginning around 250 ms and continuing throughout the majority of the recording epoch for the picture condition compared to the word condition; no such frontal differences were observed in the reverse contrast Web Appendix Figure C(1).

The parietal ERP effect was analyzed in the same way using parietal ROIs (LPS, RPS) for the 500 to 800 ms time interval. Results of the ANOVA revealed a main effect of Condition [F(1, 30) = 122.49, p < .001] such that the waveforms for pictures were more positive than the waveforms for words. A marginal effect of Group [F(1, 30) = 3.99, p = .055], revealed that the waveforms trended towards being more positive for the healthy older adults compared to the patients with aMCI. The ANOVA also revealed an interaction of Condition and Item Type [F(1, 30) = 5.02, p = .033], and a marginal interaction of Group, Condition, and Item Type [F(1, 30) = 3.69, p = .064]. Follow-up analyses revealed no effect of Item Type for words [F(1, 15) = 1.19, p = .292] or pictures [F(1, 15) = 1.95, p = .182] for the patients with aMCI. There was also no effect of Item Type for words [F(1, 15) < 1] for the healthy older adults. However, an interaction of Item Type and ROI [F(1, 15) = 4.55, p = .050] revealed a significant old/new effect at region LPS [t(15) = 2.88, p = .011], but not at region RPS [t(15) = 1.20, p = .248] for pictures. These results demonstrate that whereas patients with aMCI did not show a parietal old/new effect for either words or pictures, healthy older adults showed a parietal effect for pictures but not words.

The nonparametric analyses lend some support to these findings (Figure 3a). Neither group showed a robust parietal old/new effect for words. The word scalp topographies for the healthy older adults showed a prominent old/new effect in bilateral occipital regions beginning around 650 ms, and extending into right lateral parietal regions beginning around 750 ms. This effect became more robust at right hemisphere occipital and lateral parietal regions around 800 and remaining until approximately 1150 ms. During this time interval, the aMCI group showed a bilateral occipital old/new effect. The nonparametric analysis revealed no parietal differences between groups for the word condition. However, the analysis revealed greater occipital activity for the aMCI group during this time interval.

The scalp topographies for the older adults in the picture condition showed a left hemisphere dominant parietal old/new effect beginning at around 500 ms and continuing more on the left side until approximately 650 ms (Figure 3b). In contrast, patients with aMCI showed a diffuse centroparietal old/new effect for pictures from approximately 500 ms 850 ms. The nonparametric analysis revealed no significant areas of difference between the two groups for pictures during the 500 to 800 ms time interval. However, within-subject analysis revealed that there were several areas where parietal activity was greater for pictures than words in the healthy older adults during the 500 to 800 ms. For the aMCI group, activity was greater at frontal regions for pictures during the recollection time interval.

Finally, the late frontal ERP effect was analyzed in a similar way using the anterior ROIs (LAI, RAI, LAS, RAS) for the 1000 to 1800 ms time interval. The ANOVA revealed an effect of Condition [F(1, 30) = 7.34, p = .011] such that pictures were more positive than words, and a weak trend towards an effect of Item Type [F(1, 30) = 3.31, p = .079] because hits were somewhat more positive than correct rejections. There was also a marginal interaction of Condition, Item Type, and Group [F(1, 30) = 3.82, p = .060], as well as significant interactions of Item Type, ROI, and Group [F(3, 87) = 3.02, p = .034] and Condition, Item Type, ROI, and Group [F(3, 87) = 3.70, p = .032]. Post-hoc ANOVAs revealed an overall effect of Item Type for the healthy older adults [F(1, 15) = 4.65, p = .048], but not for the aMCI group [F(1, 15) = 1.73, p = .208]. However, the aMCI group showed a significant interaction of Item Type and Condition [F(1, 15) = 6.39, p = .023]. This interaction was due to a trend towards hits being more positive than correct rejections in the picture condition [F(1, 15) = 3.99, p = .062], but not in the word condition [F(1, 15) < 1]. Following-up on the 4-way interaction of Condition, Item Type, ROI, and Group, paired-samples t-tests revealed a significant effect of Item Type for older adults at right frontal regions RAI [t(15) = 4.55, p < .001] and RAS [t(15) = 2.52, p = .024] for the picture condition, and at region RAS [t(15) = 2.42, p = .029] for the word condition. The paired-sample t-tests revealed a significant effect of Item Type for the aMCI group at regions LAI [t(15) = 2.93, p = .011], RAI [t(15) = 3.33, p = .005], and RAS [t(15) = 2.50, p = .025] for the picture condition, and no effect of Item Type at any of the four regions for the word condition. Follow-up between-subjects t-tests revealed that the magnitude of the late frontal old/new effect for pictures was actually greater at the left inferior frontal region LAI [t(29) = 2.47, p = .019] for the aMCI group compared to the healthy older adults. These results demonstrate that healthy older adults showed a late frontal old/new for both words and pictures, whereas patients with aMCI showed a late frontal effect only for pictures.

Turning to the nonparametric analyses, the word scalp topographies for healthy older adults showed the classic right frontal old/new effect beginning at around 1050 ms and continuing throughout the majority of the remainder of the recording epoch (Figure 3a). In comparison, the aMCI group demonstrated what appears to be a centro-parietal old/new effect early in the interval with a right frontal old/new effect appearing later from approximately 1200 ms to 1650 ms. Consistent with our ANOVA findings that the old/new effect differed between groups at the right superior frontal region, Web Appendix Figure B(1), shows small superior frontal differences between groups (OC > MCI) from approximately 1050 ms to 1250 ms in the word condition.

Similar to the word condition, the older adult scalp topographies for the picture condition show the classic right frontal old/new effect from approximately 750 ms throughout the remainder of the recording epoch in the picture condition (Figure 3b). Although beginning slightly later, the patients with aMCI showed a similar bilateral frontal old/new effect to the healthy older adults during this time interval for the picture condition, beginning around 900 ms and ending more on the right at approximately 1600 ms. Web Appendix Figure B(2) shows no differences between groups (OC > MCI) in the picture condition. However, consistent with the ANOVAs and follow-up t-tests, Web Appendix Figure B(2) (MCI > OC) shows that the magnitude of the old/new effect is actually greater for patients with aMCI compared to healthy older adults at left inferior and superior frontal regions in the picture condition. The within-subjects permutation test was also consistent with the ANOVAs and t-tests. Web Appendix Figure C shows that patients with aMCI demonstrated a greater old/new effect at frontal regions late in the recording epoch for the picture > word condition [Web Appendix Figure C(2)] compared to the word > picture condition [Web Appendix Figure C(1)].

4. Discussion

The present study set out to use ERPs to understand the neural basis of the picture superiority effect in patients with aMCI, and to help determine whether these patients can successfully use familiarity to support recognition memory decisions for pictures. Our ERP data were generally consistent with our hypotheses. We predicted that the neural correlate of familiarity would be similar between the patients with aMCI and healthy older adults for pictures, but not for words. The data were consistent with this prediction. The early frontal old/new effect was similar between groups for pictures, but was diminished in patients with aMCI for words compared to healthy older adults. We also predicted that, due to impaired recollection, patients with aMCI would show diminished parietal activity for both pictures and words compared to healthy older adults. This hypothesis was also confirmed. Whereas healthy older adults showed a parietal effect for pictures, patients with aMCI showed no evidence of a parietal old/new effect for either words or pictures. Lastly, we predicted that patients with aMCI would show intact executive retrieval monitoring, as reflected by the late frontal effect. To our surprise, we found that the late frontal effect was similar between groups for pictures only. Late frontal activity was diminished in patients with aMCI for words compared to healthy older adults.

These ERP results help us to better understand how patients with aMCI make veridical memorial decisions for pictures and words. As part of this understanding, we hoped to address the question of whether patients with aMCI can successfully use familiarity to support these decisions. The behavioral evidence has been mixed. Two recent studies used process-estimation techniques to provide evidence that familiarity is impaired in patients with aMCI for words (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009a; Wolk, Signoff, and DeKosky, 2008), while another study used multiple-choice versus single item conditions to show that familiarity remained intact in these patients for pictures (Westerberg et al., 2006). Our ERP results were very consistent with these behavioral studies, showing a relative absence of the early frontal effect in patients with aMCI for words, but no difference in the early frontal effect compared to controls for pictures. The fact that stimulus type may differentially affect the components of recognition memory has been previously discussed (Ally and Budson, 2007; Ally et al., 2008b; Budson, et al., 2005; Hornberger, Morcom, and Rugg, 2004; Schloerscheidt and Rugg, 2004). In fact, Ally et al. (2008b) showed that pictures allowed healthy older adults to rely on successful recollection, where as words did not. Those data showed that the parietal old/new effect was similar between older adults and young controls for pictures, but was diminished in the older adults for words. It is clear that pictures provide a distinct memorial advantage (see Mintzner and Snodgrass, 1999 for review), and increase recollection over words (Ally and Budson, 2007; Dewhurst and Conway, 1994; Rajaram, 1996), but it is less clear as to why pictures might differentially affect familiarity.

A recent debate in the literature regarding the relationship between memorial familiarity and implicit memory may help to answer this question. Voss and Paller (2006, 2007, 2008) have recently challenged the notion that the early frontal ERP effect reflects explicit memorial familiarity. Voss and Paller (2006) showed that the magnitude of the early frontal old/new effect was correlated with the magnitude of behavioral estimates of conceptual priming, and not with ratings of explicit memory processes across-subjects. Based on further evidence using novel visual stimuli, these authors argued that the early frontal effect might be the neural correlate of conceptual priming for visual objects rather than familiarity (Voss and Paller, 2007; 2008). While this evidence is fairly convincing, recent studies involving face (Curran and Holdstock, 2007) and name (Stenberg, Hellman, Johansson, and Rosen, 2009) recognition have shown that the early frontal effect reflects explicit familiarity rather than implicit processes such as conceptual priming. This debate is complicated by the possibility that conceptual priming may overlap with familiarity, or play some role in engendering memorial familiarity (Wllems, Salmon, and Van der Linden, 2008). Indeed, evidence from behavioral and ERP studies suggest that the phenomenological experience of familiarity may result from implicit memory processes such as priming and fluency (Jacoby and Whitehouse, 1989; Whittlesea, 1993; Whittlesea and Williams, 1998; 2001; Wolk, Schacter, Berman, Holcomb, Daffner, and Budson, 2004).

Related to the current study, Hamilton and Geraci (2006) showed that the distinctive information provided by pictures makes them more easily processed, and that conceptual processing of this distinctive information likely drives the picture superiority effect. Evidence has shown that conceptual priming for picture stimuli appears to remain intact even into the moderate stages of AD (Martins and Lloyd-Jones, 2006). This finding has been extended into explicit memory. Wolk, Schacter, Berman, Holcomb, Daffner, and Budson (2005) revealed that patients with mild AD were able to use conceptual fluency based on prior experience to support their recognition judgments. To compliment these behavioral results, Wolk and colleagues showed that the early frontal old/new effect was similar for patients and healthy older controls when conceptual fluency was used to support recognition judgments. Therefore, it is certainly possible that conceptual priming for pictures contributed to the intact early frontal effect for pictures in patients with aMCI in the current investigation, and led to the pronounced picture superiority effect in this group. Future studies could include behavioral measures to examine the contribution of conceptual priming and memorial familiarity more directly.

The results of the parietal old/new effect data were also consistent with our hypotheses. Based on previous behavioral evidence of impaired recollection for both pictures (Westerberg et al., 2006) and words (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009a), we predicted that the parietal effect would be diminished for both in patients with aMCI. The aMCI ERP data revealed essentially no old/new effect at parietal regions for pictures or words. Healthy older adults showed a parietal old/new effect at left parietal regions for pictures, but not for words, suggesting that pictures allow healthy older adults to successfully use recollection in recognition decisions (Ally et al., 2008b). The lack of parietal activation during memory retrieval in patients with aMCI is not surprising given the number of recent findings suggesting early pathology in parietal cortex, and decreased connectivity to this region from the hippocampus (McKee et al., 2006; Zhou, Dougherty, Hubner, Bai, Cannon, and Hutson, 2008). It is also consistent with previous fMRI research showing significantly diminished parietal activation during memory retrieval in patients with aMCI compared to healthy older adults (Ries, Schmitz, Kawahara, Torgerson, Trivedi, Johnson, 2006).

Despite the lack of a parietal effect, the prominent occipital activity for the aMCI group in the word condition extending from approximately 250 ms to 750 ms is worth noting. To follow-up on this activity we performed a between-subjects repeated measures ANOVA using the two occipital regions, LPI and RPI, which yielded an interaction of Item Type, Condition, and Group [F(1, 30) = 5.23, p = .030]. Follow-up analyses revealed that the healthy older adult group showed no occipital old/new effect for either words (p = .305) or pictures (p = .119). Although patients with aMCI showed no occipital old/new effect for pictures (p = .621), there was a robust old/new effect for words (p < .001), supported by the nonparametric analyses shown in Web Appendix Figure C(1). Although exactly what this occipital activity reflects is unclear, it has been reported in previous studies involving subjects with mild AD (Golby, Silverberg, Race, Gabrieli, O'Shea, Knierim, et al., 2006; Koenig, Smith, Troiani, Anderson, Moore, and Grossman, 2008; Trollor, Sachdev, Haindl, Brodaty, Wen, and Walker, 2006). Similar to our current results, Trollor et al. (2006) found increased cerebral blood flow to occipital regions using PET during successful verbal recognition in patients with mild AD. It has been speculated that areas in the temporal-parietal-occipital region might organize together in the face of hippocampal pathology to provide compensatory function in patients with AD (Scarmeas, Habeck, Zarahn, Anderson, Park Hilton, et al., 2004). Given that the occipital activity in the current study was only present in the word condition, this may be a likely hypothesis. In the face of impaired familiarity and recollection, patients with aMCI may attempt to use strategies such as attempting to remember perceptual aspects of the study item or visual mental imagery to compensate for poor memory. Previous imaging research has shown that primary visual areas are activated when subjects engage in mental imagery (Kosslyn, Thompson, Kim, and Alpert, 1995), and these regions are reactivated at retrieval (Johnson and Rugg, 2007). Yet another possibility, given that occipital activity is greater for successfully identified studied items compared to successfully identified unstudied items, is that patients are visually processing the studied items longer or to a greater extent than unstudied items.

In addition to examining the ERP correlates of familiarity and recollection, we also analyzed the putative correlate of retrieval monitoring and verification. Evidence from neuropsychological (Ranganath and Knight, 2003), neuroimaging (Fletcher and Henson, 2001), and ERP (Friedman and Johnson, 2000; Wilding and Herron, 2006) studies suggest that lateral regions of prefrontal cortex are involved in control or monitoring of retrieval processes. ERP researchers have classically suggested that late right frontal old/new activity reflects the ongoing evaluation of a retrieval attempt, particularly when specific details, features, or source information are needed (Allan, Wilding, and Rugg, 1998; Rugg and Wilding, 2000; Wilding and Rugg, 1996). More recent ERP work has suggested that the late right frontal effect may be critical when memory retrieval is weak (Wolk, Sen, Chong, Riis, McGinnis, Holcomb, et al., 2009) or when subsequent retrieval attempts are needed (Ally and Budson, 2007). Because recent work has highlighted the fact that patients with aMCI demonstrate performance similar to healthy older adults on tasks of executive functioning (Bisiacchi et al., 2008) and inhibition (Zhang, Han, Verhaeghen, and Nilsson, 2007), we hypothesized that the late right frontal effect would remain intact in patients with aMCI. In fact, we predicted that this late frontal activity may even be enhanced in these patients due to impaired recollection (Wolk et al., 2009). Our results were consistent with this hypothesis for pictures, but not for words. The late frontal effect for pictures was similar for patients with aMCI and healthy older adults at right frontal regions, and it was enhanced at left frontal regions in patients compared to controls. In contrast, the late frontal effect for words was significantly diminished in patients compared to controls at all frontal regions. Overall, these data are consistent with previous behavioral work showing that patients with mild AD demonstrate enhanced retrieval monitoring for pictures over words (Gallo, Chen, Wiseman, Schacter, and Budson, 2007). Gallo et al., (2007) speculated that the distinctive perceptual information provided by pictures may impel patients to engage in retrieval monitoring.

While this hypothesis certainly has merit, it should also be noted that the late frontal effect was enhanced in the same condition that the early frontal effect was enhanced. Specifically, during the retrieval of pictures, patients with a MCI demonstrated enhanced correlates of familiarity and retrieval monitoring. Ally et al. (2008b) suggested that perhaps in the absence of recollection, the late frontal effect reflects prefrontal regions working to confirm or deny a response based on other processes such as familiarity. After a sense of familiarity is engendered, prefrontal regions may be involved in judging the degree of familiarity, or may continue attempting to recollect information about the stimulus (Montaldi, Spencer, Roberts, and Mayes, 2006). Indeed, the frontal lobes have been implicated in response inhibition (Shimamura, 1995) and distinguishing between identical versus highly similar and familiar items, and thus are important in avoiding false recognition (Budson, Sullivan, Mayer, Daffner, Black, and Schacter, 2002, Schacter, Curran, Galluccio, Milberg, and Bates, 1996; Swick and Knight, 1999).

Overall, the current results have shown that pictures and words differentially affect how memorial decisions are made in patients with aMCI. Pictures have a clear memorial advantage over words, but the debate as to why is far from settled. One hypothesis suggests that pictures are better remember because they are more likely to be represented by both image and verbal codes (dual-coding), increasing the probability that they will be recollected compared to words (Paivio 1986, Paivio 1991). Given our ERP data, it is certainly possible that the dual coding engenders an enhanced sense of familiarly in patients with MCI and recollection in older adults. However, in the current study, words alone did not elicit the neural correlate of familiarity in patients or the neural correlate of recollection in healthy controls, suggesting that there is something specifically related to pictures that affect the neural correlates of recognition memory. In fact, a large literature base suggests that the distinctive information provided by pictures allows them to be easily encoded and retrieved (see Mintzer and Snodgrass, 1999 for review), and allows for easier or enhanced recollection in healthy younger and older adults (Ally and Budson, 2007; Ally et al., 2008b; Budson et al., 2005; Dewhurst and Conway, 1994; Hamilton and Geraci, 2006; Rajaram, 1996). Further, patients with amnestic MCI and mild AD continue to show the picture superiority effect (Ally, Gold, and Budson, 2009b), despite having impaired recollection. In the present study we argue that it is likely this distinctive information that enhances familiarity in patients with aMCI, a group known to have impaired recollection. In support of this hypothesis, evidence suggests that perhaps conceptual priming drives the picture superiority effect (Hamilton and Geraci, 2006) and may play a role in engendering a sense of familiarity for visual stimuli (Voss and Paller, 2008). Indeed, previous work has shown that conceptual priming remains intact in patients with aMCI (LaVoie and Faulkner, 2008) and mild to moderate AD (Martins and Lloyd-Jones, 2006). Although the current data did not directly examine the contribution of conceptual priming to the recognition of pictures, it certainly seems plausible that the interaction between conceptual priming and memorial familiarity contributed to the intact early frontal effect and enhanced recognition of pictures for patients with aMCI.

An unanswered question is whether the differences in the ERP effects between pictures and words simply reflects the differences in performance between the two conditions. In other words, would the pattern of ERP results remain the same had our subjects engaged in two tasks involving words where behavioral performance followed a similar pattern as in the current study? A recent study finding improved performance with a depth of processing manipulation showed that with multiple study-test repetitions of words, the neural correlate of recollection—but not the early frontal correlate of familiarity--was enhanced (De Chastelaine, Friedman, Cycowicz, and Horton, 2009). Related to the current data, our results reveal similar performance levels in the picture condition for patients with MCI (Pr = .76) and the word condition for healthy older adults (Pr = .80) [t(30) = .899, p = .376], but very different scalp topographies. Therefore, we suggest that simply boosting performance is not responsible for the enhanced early frontal effect in patients with MCI. Alternatively, we propose that pictures are a special class of stimuli that enhance recollection in individuals with healthy memory and enhance familiarity in patients with impaired recollection.

In summary, we found that pictures allowed patients with aMCI to utilize frontally-based memorial processes to support successful recognition. Frontal ERP activity was similar between the two groups only for pictures during the critical time intervals associated with memorial familiarity and retrieval monitoring. Recent work has highlighted the need to investigate the interaction between implicit and explicit memorial processes (Voss and Paller 2008). With respect to the current data, we have discussed the possibility that implicit conceptual priming for visual stimuli contributed to the enhanced ERP correlate of explicit memorial familiarity. The possibility exists that the interaction of these two processes allow patients with aMCI to use familiarity to support successful recognition of pictures, but not words. Future studies can further investigate how implicit memory processes, which tend to remain intact even in the moderate stages of Alzheimer's, can contribute to successful explicit memory in these patients. Additionally, future interventions can be aimed at exploiting these intact processes in pictures to allow patients to retain function and remain in the home longer.

Supplementary Material

Web Appendix Figure A. Positions of the 128 electrodes on the Bio-Semi ActiveTwo headcap with the eight regions of interest shown.

Web Appendix Figure B. Between-Subject topographic p-value maps created by the nonparametric permutation test (1) word and (2) picture conditions. Topographies are presented in 50 ms averages going forward.

Web Appendix Figure C. Within-Subject topographic p-value maps for the (1) older adult and (2) aMCI groups created by the nonparametric permutation test. Topographies are presented in 50 ms averages going forward.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute on Aging grants F32 AG027632 (BAA), K23 AG031925 (BAA), R01 AG025815 (AEB), and P30 AG13846 (AEB) and by Pilot Awards from the Boston University Alzheimer's Disease Center (BAA) and the Harvard NeuroDiscovery Center (BAA). This material is also the result of work supported with resources and the use of facilities at the Bedford VA Hospital in Bedford, MA.

Footnotes

1

Response Bias was calculated using the formula C = zFA – d′/2 = 0.5 (zFA + zH) provided by Snodgrass and Corwin, 1988.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Achim AM, Lepage M. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex involvement in memory post-retrieval monitoring revealed in both item and associative recognition tests. Neuroimage. 2005;24:1113–1121. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adjutant General's Office. The trail making test. War Department, U. S. Army; 1944. [Google Scholar]
  3. Allan K, Wilding EL, Rugg MD. Electrophysiological evidence for dissociable processes contributing to recollection. Acta Psychologica. 1998;98:231–252. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(97)00044-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ally BA, Budson AE. The worth of pictures: using high density event-related potentials to understand the memorial power of pictures and the dynamics of recognition memory. Neuroimage. 2007;35:378–395. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ally BA, Gold CA, Budson AE. An evaluation of recollection and familiarity in Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment using receiver operating characteristics. Brain and Cognition. 2009a doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ally BA, Gold CA, Budson AE. The picture superiority effect in patients with Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia. 2009b doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Ally BA, Simons JS, McKeever JD, Peers PV, Budson AE. Parietal contribution to recollection: evidence from aging and patients with parietal lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2008a;46:1800–1812. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Ally BA, Waring JD, McKeever JD, Milberg WP, Budson AE. Aging memory for pictures: using high-density event-related potentials to understand the effect of aging on the picture superiority effect. Neuropsychologia. 2008b;46:679–689. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Anderson ND, Craik FI, Naveh-Benjamin M. The attentional demands of encoding and retrieval in younger and older adults: 1. Evidence from divided attention costs. Psychol Aging. 1998;13:405–423. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.13.3.405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Arriagada PV, Growdon JH, Hedley-Whyte ET, Hyman BT. Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1992;42:631–639. doi: 10.1212/wnl.42.3.631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Balota DA, Burgess GC, Cortese MJ, Adams DR. Memory for the infrequent in young, old, and early stage Alzheimer's disease: Evidence for two processes in episodic recognition performance. Journal of Memory and Language. 2002;46:199–226. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bastin C, Van der Linden M. The contribution of recollection and familiarity to recognition memory: A study of the effects of test format and aging. Neuropsychology. 2003;17:14–24. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Beason-Held LL, Golski S, Kraut MA, Esposito G, Resnick SM. Brain activation during encoding and recognition of verbal and figural information in older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2005;26:237–250. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bisiacchi PS, Borella E, Bergamaschi S, Carretti B, Mondini S. Interplay between memory and executive functions in normal and pathological aging. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2008;30:723–733. doi: 10.1080/13803390701689587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathologica. 1991;82:239–259. doi: 10.1007/BF00308809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Budson AE, Daffner KR, Desikan R, Schacter DL. When false recognition is unopposed by true recognition: Gist-based memory distortion in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:277–287. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Budson AE, Desikan R, Daffner KR, Schacter DL. Perceptual false recognition in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2001;15:230–243. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Budson AE, Dodson CS, Daffner KR, Schacter DL. Metacognition and false recognition in Alzheimer's disease: further exploration of the distinctiveness heuristic. Neuropsychology. 2005a;19:253–258. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Budson AE, Droller DB, Dodson CS, Schacter DL, Rugg MD, Holcomb PJ, et al. Electrophysiological dissociation of picture versus word encoding: The distinctiveness heuristic as a retrieval orientation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005b;17:1181–1193. doi: 10.1162/0898929055002517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Budson AE, Michalska KJ, Rentz DM, Joubert CC, Daffner KR, Schacter DL, et al. Use of a false recognition paradigm in an Alzheimer's disease clinical trial: a pilot study. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease and Other Dementias. 2002a;17:93–100. doi: 10.1177/153331750201700204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Budson AE, Michalska KJ, Sullivan AL, Rentz DM, Daffner KR, Schacter DL. False recognition in Alzheimer's disease: Evidence from categorized pictures. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology. 2003;16:16–27. doi: 10.1097/00146965-200303000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Budson AE, Sitarski J, Daffner KR, Schacter DL. False recognition of pictures versus words in Alzheimer's disease: The distinctiveness heuristic. Neuropsychology. 2002b;16:163–173. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Budson AE, Sullivan AL, Mayer E, Daffner KR, Black PM, Schacter DL. Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimer's disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions. Brain. 2002c;125:2750–2765. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Burgess PW, Shallice T. Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memory. 1996;4:359–411. doi: 10.1080/096582196388906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cabeza R, Anderson ND, Locantore JK, McIntosh AR. Aging gracefully: compensatory brain activity in high-performing older adults. Neuroimage. 2002;17:1394–1402. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2008;9:613–625. doi: 10.1038/nrn2459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cansino S, Maquet P, Dolan RJ, Rugg MD. Brain activity underlying encoding and retrieval of source memory. Cereb Cortex. 2002;12:1048–1056. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.10.1048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ciaramelli E, Grady CL, Moscovitch M. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: a hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1828–1851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.03.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Curran T. Brain potentials of recollection and familiarity. Memory and Cognition. 2000;28:923–938. doi: 10.3758/bf03209340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Curran T. Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1088–1106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Curran T, DeBuse C, Woroch B, Hirshman E. Combined pharmacological and electrophysiological dissociation of familiarity and recollection. J Neurosci. 2006;26:1979–1985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5370-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Curran T, Hancock J. The FN400 indexes familiarity-based recognition of faces. Neuroimage. 2007;36:464–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.12.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Daselaar SM, Fleck MS, Dobbins IG, Madden DJ, Cabeza R. Effects of healthy aging on hippocampal and rhinal memory functions: an event-related fMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1771–1782. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Davidson PSR, Anaki D, Ciaramelli E, Cohn M, Kim ASN, Murphy KJ, et al. Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory? Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1743–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. De Chastelaine M, Friedman D, Cycowicz YM, Horton C. Effects of multiple study-tesy repetition on the neural correlates of recognition memory: ERPs dissociate remembering and knowing. Psychophysiology. 2009;46:86–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00754.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Delacourte A, David JP, Sergeant N, Buee L, Wattez A, Vermersch P, Ghozali F, Fallet-Bianco C, Pasquier F, Lebert F, Petit H, Di Menza C. The biochemical pathway of neurofibrillary degeneration in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1999;52:1158–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.6.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Delbecq-Derouesné J, Beauvois MF, Shallice T. Preserved recall versus impaired recognition. Brain. 1990;113:1045–1074. doi: 10.1093/brain/113.4.1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dewhurst SA, Conway MA. Pictures, Images, and the recollective experience. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;48:125–140. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.5.1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dickerson BC, Salat DH, Greve DN, Chua EF, Rand-Giovannetti E, Rentz DM, et al. Increased hippocampal activation in mild cognitive impairment compared to normal aging and AD. Neurology. 2005;65:404–411. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000171450.97464.49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dien J. Addressing misallocation of variance in principal components analysis of event-related potentials. Brain Topogr. 1998;11:43–55. doi: 10.1023/a:1022218503558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dobbins IG, Foley H, Schacter DL, Wagner AD. Executive control during episodic retrieval: Multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron. 2002;35:989–996. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00858-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dobbins IG, Han S. Cue- versus probe-dependent prefrontal cortex activity during contextual remembering. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1439–52. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dobbins IG, Rice HJ, Wagner AD, Schacter DL. Memory orientation and success: separable neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:318–333. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00164-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Duarte A, Ranganath C, Trujillo C, Knight RT. Intact recollection memory in high-performing older adults: ERP and behavioral evidence. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:33–47. doi: 10.1162/089892906775249988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Duzel E, Yonelinas AP, Mangun GR, Heinze HJ, Tulving E. Event-related brain potential correlates of two states of conscious awareness in memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1997;94:5973–5978. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.11.5973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Eldridge LL, Knowlton BJ, Furmanski CS, Bookheimer SY, Engel SA. Remembering episodes: a selective role for the hippocampus during retrieval. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:1149–1152. doi: 10.1038/80671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fleck MS, Daselaar SM, Dobbins IG, Cabeza R. Role of prefrontal and anterior cingulate regions in decision-making processes shared by memory and nonmemory tasks. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1623–30. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Fletcher PC, Henson RN. Frontal lobes and human memory: insights from functional neuroimaging. Brain. 2001;124:849–81. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.5.849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB, Reinvang I. Age-differences in verbal recognition memory revealed by ERP. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2005;36:176–187. doi: 10.1177/155005940503600308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Friedman D, Johnson R., Jr Event-related potential (ERP) studies of memory encoding and retrieval: a selective review. Microsc Res Tech. 2000;51:6–28. doi: 10.1002/1097-0029(20001001)51:1<6::AID-JEMT2>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Galan L, Biscay R, Rodriguez JL, Perez-Abalo MC, Rodriguez R. Testing topographic differences between event related brain potentials by using non-parametric combinations of permutation tests. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1997;102:240–247. doi: 10.1016/s0013-4694(96)95155-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Gallo DA, Chen JM, Wiseman AL, Schacter DL, Budson AE. Retrieval monitoring and anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:559–568. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.5.559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gallo DA, Sullivan AL, Daffner KR, Schacter DL, Budson AE. Associative recognition in Alzheimer's disease: Evidence for impaired recall-to-reject. Neuropsychology. 2004;18:556–563. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gardiner JM, Java RL, Richardson-Klavehn A. Experiences of remembering, knowing, and guessing. Consciousness and Cognition. 1998;7:1–26. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1997.0321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Golby A, Silverberg G, Race E, Gabrieli S, O'Shea J, Knierim K, Stebbins G, Gabrieli J. Memory encoding in Alzheimer's disease: an fMRI study of explicit and implicit memory. Brain. 2005;128:773–787. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Goldmann RE, Sullivan AL, Droller DB, Rugg MD, Curran T, Holcomb PJ, Schacter DL, Daffner KR, Budson AE. Late frontal brain potentials distinguish true and false recognition. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1717–1720. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200309150-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gomez-Isla T, Price JL, McKeel DW, Morris JC, Growdon JH, Hyman BT. Profound loss of layer II entorhinal cortex neurons occurs in very mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neuroscience. 1996;16:4491–4500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-14-04491.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Grady CL, Haxby JV, Horwitz B, Sundaram M, Berg G, Schapiro M, Friedland RP, Rapoport SI. Longitudinal study of the early neuropsychological and cerebral metabolic changes in dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1988;10:576–596. doi: 10.1080/01688638808402796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Greenblatt RE, Pflieger ME. Randomization-based hypothesis testing from event-related data. Brain Topogr. 2004;16:225–232. doi: 10.1023/b:brat.0000032856.48286.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Hamilton M, Geraci L. The picture superiority effect in conceptual implicit memory: a conceptual distinctiveness hypothesis. Am J Psychol. 2006;119:1–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Henson RN, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 1999a;19:3962–3972. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-03962.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Henson RN, Shallice T, Dolan RJ. Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrieval: a functional MRI test of the monitoring hypothesis. Brain. 1999b;122(Pt 7):1367–1381. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.7.1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hornberger M, Morcom AM, Rugg MD. Neural correlates of retrieval orientation: Effects of study-test similarity. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16:1196–1210. doi: 10.1162/0898929041920450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hornberger M, Rugg MD, Henson RN. fMRI correlates of retrieval orientation. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:1425–1436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Howard MW, Bessette-Symons B, Zhang Y, Hoyer WJ. Aging selectively impairs recollection in recognition memory for pictures: Evidence from modeling and receiver operating characteristic curves. Psychol Aging. 2006;21:96–106. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ibanez V, Pietrini P, Alexander GE, Furey ML, Teichberg D, Rajapakse JC, Rapoport SI, Schapiro MB, Horwitz B. Regional glucose metabolic abnormalities are not the result of atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1998;50:1585–1593. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.6.1585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Jacoby LL. Ironic effects of repetition: Measuring age-related differences in memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1999;25:3–22. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.25.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Jacoby LL, Dallas M. On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1981;110:306–340. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.110.3.306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Jacoby LL, Whitehouse K. An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1989;118:126–135. [Google Scholar]
  71. Jennings JM, Jacoby LL. Automatic versus intentional uses of memory: Aging, attention, and control. Psychol Aging. 1993;8:283–293. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.8.2.283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Jennings JM, Jacoby LL. An opposition procedure for detecting age-related deficits in recollection: Telling effects of repetition. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:352–361. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Johnson JD, Rugg MD. Recollection and the reinstatement of encoding-related cortical activity. Cereb Cortex. 2007;18:868–875. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Lindsay DS. Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin. 1993;114:3–28. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Joyce CA, Paller KA, McIsaac HK, Kutas M. Memory changes with normal aging: Behavioral and electrophysiological measures. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:669–678. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Karniski W, Blair RC, Snider AD. An exact statistical method for comparing topographic maps, with any number of subjects and electrodes. Brain Topogr. 1994;6:203–210. doi: 10.1007/BF01187710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Kahn I, Davachi L, Wagner AD. Functional-neuroanatomic correlates of recollection: Implications for models of recognition memory. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4172–4180. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0624-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Koenig P, Smith EE, Troiani V, Anderson C, Moore P, Grossman M. Medial temporal Lobe Involvement in an Implicit Memory Task: Evidence of Collaborating Implicit and Explicit Memory Systems from fMRI and Alzheimer's Disease. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:2831–2843. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Koivisto M, Portin R, Seinela A, Rinne J. Automatic influences of memory in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 1998;34:209–219. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70748-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Kim IJ, Alpert NM. Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex. Nature. 1995;378:496–498. doi: 10.1038/378496a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Knight RG. Controlled and automatic memory process in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 1998;34:427–435. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70765-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. LaVoie DJ, Faulkner KM. Production and identification repetition priming in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2008;15:523–44. doi: 10.1080/13825580802051497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Li J, Morcom AM, Rugg MD. The effects of age on the neural correlates of successful episodic retrieval: an ERP study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2004;4:279–293. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.3.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Mack WJ, Freed DM, Williams BW, Henderson VW. Boston Naming Test: shortened versions for use in Alzheimer's disease. J Gerontol. 1992;47:154–158. doi: 10.1093/geronj/47.3.p154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Madden DJ, Gottlob LR, Denny LL, Turkington TG, Provenzale JM, Hawk TC, Coleman RE. Aging and recognition memory: changes in regional cerebral blood flow associated with components of reaction time distributions. J Cogn Neurosci. 1999;11:511–520. doi: 10.1162/089892999563571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Mandler G. Recognizing: the judgment of previous occurrence. Psychological Review. 1980;87:252–271. [Google Scholar]
  87. Martins CA, Lloyd-Jones TJ. Preserved conceptual priming in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex. 2006;42:995–1004. doi: 10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70205-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. McKee AC, Au R, Cabral HJ, Kowall NW, Seshadri S, Kubilus CA, Drake J, Wolf PA. Visual association pathology in preclinical Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol. 2006;65:621–630. doi: 10.1097/00005072-200606000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Mesulam MM. Neuroplasticity failure in Alzheimer's disease: Bridging the gap between plaques and tangles. Neuron. 1999;24:521–529. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81109-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Mesulam MM. Principles of behavioral and cognitive neurology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  91. Mintzer MZ, Snodgrass JG. The picture superiority effect: support for the distinctiveness model. Am J Psychol. 1999;112:113–146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, Salmon DP, Katzman R, Thal LJ. Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:1253–1258. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Montaldi D, Spencer TJ, Roberts N, Mayes AR. The neural system that mediates familiarity memory. Hippocampus. 2006;16:504–20. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Morcom AM, Rugg MD. Effects of age on retrieval cue processing as revealed by ERPs. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:1525–1542. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, Hughes JP, van Belle G, Fillenbaum G, Mellits ED, Clark C. The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1989;39:1159–1165. doi: 10.1212/wnl.39.9.1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Nielsen-Bohlman L, Knight RT. Prefrontal alterations during memory processing in aging. Cereb Cortex. 1995;5:541–549. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.6.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Norman KA, Tepe K, Nyhus E, Curran T. Event-related potential correlates of interference effects on recognition memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 2008;15:36–43. doi: 10.3758/pbr.15.1.36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Park DC, Smith AD, Dudley WN, Lafronza VN. Effects of age and a divided attention task presented during encoding and retrieval on memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1989;15:1185–1191. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.15.6.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. 2004;256:183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Pierce BH, Sullivan AL, Schacter DL, Budson AE. Comparing source-based and gist-based false recognition in aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2005;19:411–419. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Quamme JR, Yonelinas AP, Norman KA. Effect of unitization on associative recognition in amnesia. Hippocampus. 2007;17:192–200. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Ranganath C, Heller AS, Wilding EL. Dissociable correlates of two classes of retrieval processing in prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage. 2007;35:1663–1673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Ranganath C, Knight RT. Prefrontal cortex and episodic memory: Integrating findings from neuropsychology and event-related functional neuroimaging. In: Parker A, Wildng E, Bussey T, editors. The cognitive neuroscience of memory encoding and retrieval. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 2003. pp. 83–99. [Google Scholar]
  104. Rajaram S. Perceptual effects on remembering: recollective processes in picture recognition memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1996;22:365–377. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.22.2.365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Rapcsak SZ, Kaszniak AW, Reminger SL, Glisky ML, Glisky EL, Comer JF. Dissociation between verbal and autonomic measures of memory following frontal lobe damage. Neurology. 1998;50:1259–1265. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.5.1259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Rapcsak SZ, Nielsen L, Littrell LD, Glisky EL, Kaszniak AW, Laguna JF. Face memory impairments in patients with frontal lobe damage. Neurology. 2001;57:1168–1175. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.7.1168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Ries ML, Schmitz TW, Kawahara TN, Torgerson BM, Trivedi MA, Johnson SC. Task-dependent posterior cingulate activation in mild cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. 2006;29:485–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Rugg MD, Curran T. Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:251–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.04.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Rugg MD, Mark RE, Gilchrist J, Roberts RC. Erp repetition effects in indirect and direct tasks: Effects of age and interitem lag. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:572–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb01744.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Rugg MD, Mark RE, Walla P, Schloerscheidt AM, Birch CS, Allan K. Dissociation of the neural correlates of implicit and explicit memory. Nature. 1998;392:595–598. doi: 10.1038/33396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Rugg MD, Wilding EL. Retrieval processing and episodic memory. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:108–115. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01445-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Rybash JM, Hoyer WJ. Process dissociation procedure reveals age differences in unconscious influences on memory for possible and impossible objects. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition. 1996;3:251–263. [Google Scholar]
  113. Salthouse TA. Aging associations: Influence of speed on adult age differences in associative learning. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:1486–1503. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.6.1486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Scarmeas N, Habeck CG, Zarahn E, Anderson KE, Park A, Hilton J, Pelton GH, Tabert MH, Honig LS, Moeller JR, Devanand DP, Stern Y. Covariance PET patterns in early Alzheimer's disease and subjects with cognitive impairment but no dementia: utility in group discrimination and correlations with functional performance. Neuroimage. 2004;23:35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Schacter DL, Curran T, Galluccio L, Milberg W, Bates J. False recognition and the right frontal lobe: A case study. Neuropsychologia. 1996;34:793–808. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00165-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Schloerscheidt AM, Rugg MD. The impact of change in stimulus format on the electrophysiological indices of recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42:451–466. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Senkfor AJ, Van Petten C. Who said what? An event-related potential investigation of source and item memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1998;24:1005–25. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.24.4.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Shimamura AP. Memory and frontal lobe function. In: Gazzaniga M, editor. The cognitive neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1995. pp. 803–13. [Google Scholar]
  119. Simons JS, Owen AM, Fletcher PC, Burgess PW. Anterior prefrontal cortex and the recollection of contextual information. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1774–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Simons JS, Peers PV, Hwang DY, Ally BA, Fletcher PC, Budson AE. Is the parietal lobe necessary for recollection in humans? Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1185–1191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Skinner EI, Fernandes MA. Neural correlates of recollection and familiarity: A review of neuroimaging and patient data. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2163–2179. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Smith ME. Neurophysiological manifestations of recollective experience during recognition memory judgments. J Cog Neurosci. 1993;5:1–13. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Smith ME, Guster K. Decomposition of recognition memory event-related potentials yields target, repetition, and retrieval effects. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1993;86:335–343. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(93)90046-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Smith JA, Knight RG. Memory processing in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia. 2002;46:666–682. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00137-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Spencer WD, Raz N. Differential effects of aging on memory for content and context: A meta-analysis. Psychol Aging. 1995;10:527–539. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.4.527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Sperling RA, Bates JF, Chua EF, Cocchiarella AJ, Rentz DM, Rosen BR, et al. fMRI studies of associative encoding in young and elderly controls and mild Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 2003;74:44–50. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Snodgrass JG, Corwin J. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1988;117:34–50. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.117.1.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Stenberg G, Hellman J, Johansson M, Rosén I. Familiarity or Conceptual Priming: Event-related Potentials in Name Recognition. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009 doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Swick D, Knight RT. Event-related potentials differentiate the effects of aging on word and nonword repetition in explicit and implicit memory tasks. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1997;23(1):123–142. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.23.1.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Swick D, Knight RT. Contributions of prefrontal cortex to recognition memory: electrophysiological and behavioral evidence. Neuropsychology. 1999;13:155–70. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.13.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Titov N, Knight RG. Adult age differences in controlled and automatic memory processing. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:565–573. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.4.565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Trott CT, Friedman D, Ritter W, Fabiani M, Snodgrass JG. Episodic priming and memory for temporal source: event related potentials reveal age-related differences in prefrontal functioning. Psychol Aging. 1999;14:390–413. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.14.3.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Troller JN, Sachdev PS, Haindl W, Brodaty H, Wen W, Walker BM. A high-resolution single photon emission computed tomography study of verbal recognition memory in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21:267–74. doi: 10.1159/000091433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Ukmar M, Makuc E, Onor ML, Garbin G, Trevisiol M, Cova MA. Evaluation of white matter damage in patients with Alzheimer's disease and in patients with mild cognitive impairment by using diffusion tensor imaging. Radiol Med (Torino) 2009 doi: 10.1007/s11547-008-0286-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Van Hoesen GW, Hyman BT, Damasio AR. Entorhinal cortex pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Hippocampus. 1991;1:1–8. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450010102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Dissociation of the neural correlates of recognition memory according to familiarity, recollection, and amount of recollected information. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2216–2225. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Vilberg KL, Rugg MD. Memory retrieval and the parietal cortex: a review of evidence from a dual-process perspective. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1787–1799. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Vilberg KL, Moosavi RF, Rugg MD. The relationship between electrophysiological correlates of recollection and amount of information retrieved. Brain Res. 2006;1122:161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Voss JL, Paller KA. Fluent conceptual processing and explicit memory for faces are electrophysiologically distinct. J Neurosci. 2006;26:926–33. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3931-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Voss JL, Paller KA. Neural correlates of conceptual implicit memory and their contamination of putative neural correlates of explicit memory. Learn Mem. 2007;14:259–67. doi: 10.1101/lm.529807. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Voss JL, Paller KA. Brain substrates of implicit and explicit memory: the importance of concurrently acquired neural signals of both memory types. Neuropsychologia. 2007;46:3021–3029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Voss SE, Bullock RA. Executive function: the core of dementia? Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 2004;18:207–216. doi: 10.1159/000079202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Westerberg CE, Paller KA, Weintraub S, Mesulam MM, Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Reber PJ. When memory does not fail: familiarity-based recognition in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:193–205. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Whiting WLt, Smith AD. Differential age-related processing limitations in recall and recognition tasks. Psychol Aging. 1997;12:216–224. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.12.2.216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Whittlesea BW. Illusions of familiarity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1993;19:1235–1253. [Google Scholar]
  146. Whittlesea BW, Williams LD. Why do strangers feel familiar, but friends don't? A discrepancy-attribution account of feelings of familiarity. Acta Psychol (Amst) 1998;98:141–65. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(97)00040-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Whittlesea BW, Williams LD. The discrepancy-attribution hypothesis: II. Expectation, uncertainty, surprise, and feelings of familiarity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2001;27:14–33. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Wilding EL, Doyle MC, Rugg MD. Recognition memory with and without retrieval of context: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia. 1995;33:743–767. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00017-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Wilding EL, Herron JE. Electrophysiological measures of episodic memory control and memory retrieval. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2006;37:315–21. doi: 10.1177/155005940603700409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Wilding EL, Rugg MD. An event-related potential study of recognition memory with and without retrieval of source. Brain. 1996;119:889–905. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.3.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Willems S, Salmon E, Van der Linden M. Implicit/explicit memory dissociation in Alzheimer's disease: the consequence of inappropriate processing? Neuropsychology. 2008;22:710–717. doi: 10.1037/a0012986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Wolk DA, Schacter DL, Berman AR, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR, Budson AE. An electrophysiological investigation of the relationship between conceptual fluency and familiarity. Neurosci Lett. 2004;369:150–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.07.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Wolk DA, Schacter DL, Berman AR, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR, Budson AE. Patients with mild Alzheimer's disease attribute conceptual fluency to prior experience. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1662–1672. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Wolk DA, Sen NM, Chong H, Riis JL, McGinnis SM, Holcomb PJ, Daffner KR. ERP correlates of item recognition memory: effects of age and performance. Brain Res. 2009;1250:218–31. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Wolk DA, Signoff ED, Dekosky ST. Recollection and familiarity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: a global decline in recognition memory. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:1965–1978. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Woodruff CC, Hayama HR, Rugg MD. Electrophysiological dissociation of the neural correlates of recollection and familiarity. Brain Res. 2006;1100:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Yonelinas AP. Components of episodic memory: The contribution of recollection and familiarity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356:1363–1374. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2001.0939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Yonelinas AP. Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: evidence for a dual-process model. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 1994;20:1341–1354. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.20.6.1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Yonelinas AP, Otten LJ, Shaw KN, Rugg MD. Separating the brain regions involved in recollection and familiarity in recognition memory. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:3002–3008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5295-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Zhang Y, Han B, Verhaeghen P, Nilsson LG. Executive functioning in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: MCI has effects on planning, but not on inhibition. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2007;14:557–70. doi: 10.1080/13825580600788118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Zhou Y, Dougherty JH, Hubner KF, Bai B, Cannon RL, Hutson RK. Abnormal connectivity in the posterior cingulate and hippocampus in early Alzheimer's disease and mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimer's and Dementia. 2008;4:265–70. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2008.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Web Appendix Figure A. Positions of the 128 electrodes on the Bio-Semi ActiveTwo headcap with the eight regions of interest shown.

Web Appendix Figure B. Between-Subject topographic p-value maps created by the nonparametric permutation test (1) word and (2) picture conditions. Topographies are presented in 50 ms averages going forward.

Web Appendix Figure C. Within-Subject topographic p-value maps for the (1) older adult and (2) aMCI groups created by the nonparametric permutation test. Topographies are presented in 50 ms averages going forward.

RESOURCES