Abstract
Objectives. We examined the long-term health consequences of relationship violence in adulthood.
Methods. Using data from the Welfare, Children, and Families project (1999 and 2001), a probability sample of 2402 low-income women with children living in disadvantaged neighborhoods in Boston, Massachusetts; Chicago, Illinois; and San Antonio, Texas, we predicted changes in the frequency of intoxication, psychological distress, and self-rated health over 2 years with baseline measures of relationship violence and a host of relevant background variables.
Results. Our analyses showed that psychological aggression predicted increases in psychological distress, whereas minor physical assault and sexual coercion predicted increases in the frequency of intoxication. There was no evidence to suggest that relationship violence in adulthood predicted changes in self-rated health.
Conclusions. Experiences with relationship violence beyond the formative and developmental years of childhood and adolescence can have far-reaching effects on the health status of disadvantaged urban women.
Over the past 2 decades, numerous studies have examined the long-term health consequences of relationship violence during childhood. This body of research suggests that physical and sexual abuse in early life can be devastating to health in adulthood, contributing to poor mental1–6 and physical health3–5,7 and to higher rates of substance abuse.5,6,8,9 These patterns are remarkably consistent across studies and notably persistent through the life course. In a recent study of more than 21 000 older adults, Draper et al.3 reported that physical and sexual abuse before 15 years of age is associated with poor mental and physical health well into late life.
Although previous research has made significant contributions to our understanding of the lasting effects of abuse in early life, few studies have considered the long-term health consequences of relationship violence in adulthood. Our review of the literature revealed 5 longitudinal studies of relationship violence and health in adulthood. Not surprisingly, research suggests that women who experience relationship violence in adulthood are vulnerable to poor health trajectories, including increases in depressive symptoms,10–12 functional impairment,10,12 and alcohol consumption.13,14
Relationship violence is an important issue in all segments of society; however, studies consistently show that women of low socioeconomic status exhibit higher rates of intimate partner victimization than do their more affluent counterparts.15–17 For example, Tolman and Raphael17 reported that between 34% and 65% of women receiving welfare report having experienced some form of relationship violence in their lifetime, and between 8% and 33% experience some form of relationship violence each year, levels that surpass those for women overall.18 Research also shows that residence in disadvantaged neighborhoods19,20 and the presence of children in the household21,22 may elevate the odds of relationship violence. Given their high violence-risk profile, attention must be directed to the patterns and health consequences of intimate partner victimization in the lives of disadvantaged urban women with children.23–25
Building on previous research, we used data collected from a large probability sample of low-income women with children living in low-income neighborhoods in Boston, Massachusetts, Chicago, Illinois, and San Antonio, Texas, to predict changes in the frequency of intoxication, psychological distress, and self-rated health over 2 years with measures of relationship violence in early life and adulthood and a host of relevant background variables. In accordance with previous research, we expected that intimate partner victimization in adulthood would predict increases in psychological distress and the frequency of intoxication and decreases in self-rated health over the study period.
METHODS
The data for this investigation came from the Welfare, Children, and Families (WCF) project,26 a household-based, stratified, random sample of 2402 low-income women living in low-income neighborhoods in Boston, Chicago, and San Antonio. The data were collected in 1999 with a follow-up in 2001. The baseline response rate was 75%, and 89% of the original sample was interviewed again. Census blocks (or neighborhoods) in which at least 20% of the residents were below the federal poverty line according to the 1990 census were sampled first. Within these neighborhoods, households under 200% of the poverty line were sampled, and households below 100% of the poverty line were oversampled. Because one of the goals of the WCF project was to assess the impact of welfare policy and work on children, households were screened for the presence of children. Households with at least 1 infant or child (aged 0–4 years) or young adolescent (aged 10–14 years) were sampled. The children's caregivers, all of whom were women, were interviewed face-to-face.
Measures
The first outcome under study was frequency of intoxication. This outcome was intended to measure the most fundamentally problematic aspect of alcohol consumption, that is, getting drunk. Respondents were asked, “In the past 12 months, how often have you gotten drunk?” Response categories for this item were coded as (0) never, (1) once or twice, or (2) several times or often. This measure of intoxication has shown construct validity in previous research. Using the 1985 National Survey on Drug Abuse, Robbins27 used a similar measure of intoxication and found strong positive associations with psychological distress and social and behavioral problems.
The second outcome under study was psychological distress. This outcome was measured with the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI-18),28 which contains subscales for depression, anxiety, and somatization. Psychological distress was measured as the mean response to 18 items. For example, respondents were asked to indicate how much in the past 7 days they were distressed or bothered by “feeling no interest in things,” “feeling tense or keyed up,” and experiencing “nausea or upset stomach.” Response categories for all psychological distress items were coded as (1) not at all, (2) a little bit, (3) moderately, (4) quite a bit, or (5) extremely.
The final outcome under study was self-rated health. This outcome is widely used to measure general physical health status. Respondents were asked, “In general how is your health?” Response categories were coded as (1) poor, (2) fair, (3) good, (4) very good, or (5) excellent. Self-rated health is strongly correlated with more objective measures of physical health, including physician diagnoses, various measures of morbidity, and all-cause mortality.29
Measures of relationship violence in adulthood were drawn from the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2).30 The CTS2 has established reliability and validity.30,31 Drawing on the work of Straus et al.,30 we assessed 4 major types of relationship violence, including psychological aggression, minor physical assault, severe physical assault, and sexual coercion. Psychological aggression captured nonphysical or verbal acts of violence. Respondents were asked to indicate how often in the past 12 months a romantic partner had threatened to (1) hit them, (2) use a weapon on them, and (3) hurt their child or take [him/her] away. Minor physical assault assessed nonsevere or “common couple” acts of physical violence. Respondents were asked to indicate how often in the past 12 months a romantic partner had (1) thrown something at them and (2) pushed, grabbed, or shoved them. Severe physical assault measured some of the more brutal and vicious acts of physical violence. Respondents were asked to indicate how often in the past 12 months a romantic partner had (1) slapped, kicked, bit, or punched them, (2) beaten, and (3) choked or burned them. Finally, sexual coercion was intended to capture forced sexual acts of violence. Respondents were asked to indicate how often in the past 12 months a romantic partner had forced them into any sexual activity against their will. Because of limited incidence rates, measures of adult violence were coded into dummy variables. Respondents were given a value of (1) if they reported a particular act of violence in the past 12 months and (0) if they did not. For example, the dummy variable for psychological aggression was coded (1) for one or more acts of psychological aggression in the past year and (0) for no acts of psychological aggression in the past year.
Relationship violence during childhood and adolescence was measured with 2 items. These items captured physical assault and sexual coercion before age 18. Respondents were asked to indicate whether they were ever hit, beaten up, burned, or assaulted with a weapon or whether their life had been threatened by an adult in their family or household before the age of 18 years. Respondents were also asked to indicate whether anyone—a stranger, friend, acquaintance, date, or relative—had ever tried or succeeded in doing something sexual to them or made them do something sexual against their wishes. Once again, because of limited incidence rates, these measures were coded into dummy variables. For each of the 2 measures, respondents were given a value of (1) if they reported being victims of physical assault or sexual coercion before the age of 18 years, respectively, and (0) if they did not.
In accordance with previous research on relationship violence and mental health among low-income women,23,25 our multivariate analyses included controls for numerous background factors, including age (in years), race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic White, Mexican, and other Hispanic compared with Black), education (in years), employment status (1 = worked for pay in past week), current welfare status (1 = currently receiving welfare), financial hardship (13-item index assessing the inability to provide for food, clothing, housing, and other essential material needs), marital status (1 = married and living with spouse), cohabiting status (1 = cohabiting, not married), and number of children (1–6 or more). Because city of residence was correlated with several of these measures (e.g., race and employment status), subsequent analyses also controlled for city (Boston and San Antonio compared with Chicago).
Statistical Procedures
In our multivariate analysis, we modeled changes in health indicators over 2 years (1999 and 2001). In lieu of standard lagged endogenous dependent variable models, we used change score models to assess 2-year health trajectories. A recent comparison of 2-wave panel designs concluded that change score models are generally preferable to lagged endogenous dependent variable models.32 We first computed change scores by subtracting baseline health scores from follow-up health scores. Change scores are continuous variables that range from some negative number to some positive number. Negative numbers indicate a decrease in a given health indicator (e.g., fewer symptoms of psychological distress in 2001 than in 1999). Positive numbers suggest an increase in a given health indicator (e.g., greater frequency of intoxication in 2001 than in 1999). Many respondents exhibited a change score of zero, which indicated no change across waves (e.g., same self-rated health score in 1999 and in 2001). Because change scores are continuous variables, we used ordinary least squares regression to model changes in the frequency of intoxication, psychological distress, and self-rated health with predictor variables measured at baseline. We presented both unstandardized and standardized ordinary least squares coefficients for corresponding independent variables. In this case, unstandardized coefficients were estimated to describe the difference in the expected change in health status for every 1-unit change in an independent variable. We used SPSS version 16 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) for all analyses.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistics for selected background factors at baseline are provided in Table 1. Most respondents lived in Boston (38%), followed by Chicago and San Antonio (31% each). The sample included Blacks (43%), Mexicans (24%), other Hispanics (24%), and non-Hispanic Whites (9%). The average respondent was 33 years of age, with approximately 10 years of formal education. Less than half of the respondents were employed or currently receiving welfare benefits. Few respondents were cohabiting or married and living with a spouse. The average respondent was responsible for nearly 3 children and exhibited a low level of financial hardship.
TABLE 1.
Mean (SD) or % | Range | Reliabilitya | |
Age, y | 33.27 (9.77) | 19–74 | … |
Non-Hispanic White | 9 | … | … |
Black | 43 | … | … |
Mexican | 24 | … | … |
Other Hispanic | 24 | … | … |
Education, y | 10.51 (2.35) | 0–14 | … |
Employed | 41 | … | … |
Receiving welfare | 37 | … | … |
Financial hardship | 0.01 (0.58) | −1 to 3 | 0.83 |
Cohabiting, not married | 6 | … | … |
Married, spouse in house | 14 | … | … |
Married, no spouse in house | 12 | … | … |
Single | 68 | … | … |
No. of children | 2.61 (1.34) | 1–6 | … |
Living in Boston | 38 | … | … |
Living in Chicago | 31 | … | … |
Living in San Antonio | 31 | … | … |
Estimated by Cronbach α.
Descriptive statistics for all violence measures at baseline are provided in Table 2. Before we consider the results of the present study, it is important to note that approximately 22% of all women report physical assault by an intimate partner in their lifetime, and roughly 1.3% of all women are physically assaulted by an intimate partner each year.18 The results from Table 2 indicate that most respondents reported no relationship violence in their lifetime. Although reports of physical assault (20%) and sexual coercion (24%) before the age of 18 years were similar to national lifetime prevalence rates, 12-month incidence rates for psychological aggression (18%), minor physical assault (22%), severe physical assault (12%), and sexual coercion (7%) were noticeably higher than expectations derived from national estimates.
TABLE 2.
% or Mean (SD) | Range | Reliabilitya | |
Relationship violence | |||
Psychological aggression, past y | 18 | … | … |
Minor physical assault, past y | 22 | … | … |
Severe physical assault, past y | 12 | … | … |
Sexual coercion, past y | 7 | … | … |
Physical assault before age 18 y | 20 | … | … |
Sexual coercion before age 18 y | 24 | … | … |
Health indicators | |||
Frequency of intoxicationb | |||
1999 | 0.37 (0.60) | 0–2 | … |
2001 | 0.35 (0.61) | 0–2 | … |
Frequency changec | −0.02 (0.61) | −2 to 2 | … |
Psychological distressd | |||
1999 | 1.44 (0.54) | 1–5 | 0.92 |
2001 | 1.42 (0.54) | 1–5 | 0.93 |
Distress changec | −0.01 (0.48) | −3 to 3 | … |
Self-rated health,e | |||
1999 | 3.22 (1.17) | 1–5 | … |
2001 | 3.20 (1.13) | 1–5 | … |
Health changec | −0.02 (1.18) | −4 to 4 | … |
Estimated by Cronbach α.
Coded as: 0 = never, 1 = once or twice, or 2 = several times or often.
2001 minus 1999 data.
Coded as: 1 = not at all, 2 = a little bit, 3 = moderately, 4 = quite a bit, or 5 = extremely.
Coded as: 1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, 4 = very good, or 5 = excellent.
Also provided in Table 2 are descriptive statistics for selected health indicators, including baseline and follow-up measurements and change score specifications. Average health scores were generally consistent across waves. The average respondent exhibited low levels of intoxication and psychological distress. Respondents also tended to report “good” health. With respect to the change scores, we observed an average decline in frequency of intoxication, psychological distress, and self-rated health. These patterns suggest that although risky drinking practices and mental health improved between waves, self-rated health deteriorated.
The results of our multivariate change analysis are shown in Table 3. The first column of Table 3 shows that only minor physical assault and sexual coercion were associated with the change in frequency of intoxication. As expected, experiences with minor physical assault and sexual coercion in the past year predicted increases in the frequency of intoxication from baseline to follow-up, net of controls for baseline frequency of intoxication (outcome pretest), violence before the age of 18 years, and all background factors. Interestingly, the intoxication trajectories of respondents who reported abuse before the age of 18 years were similar to those respondents who reported no experiences with relationship violence in early life. A comparison of standardized parameter estimates also suggested that minor physical assault in the past year was among the strongest predictors of the change in frequency of intoxication. For example, the standardized parameter estimate for minor physical assault (B = 0.10) was larger in magnitude than those for age (B = 0.08) and race/ethnicity (B = 0.05–0.07).
TABLE 3.
Change in Frequency of Intoxicationa |
Change in Psychological Distressa |
Change in Self-Rated Healtha |
||||
b (SE) | B | b (SE) | B | b (SE) | B | |
Relationship violence | ||||||
Psychological aggression, past y | −0.06 (0.04) | −0.03 | 0.10 (0.03) | 0.08** | 0.16 (0.08) | 0.05 |
Minor physical assault, past y | 0.14 (0.04) | 0.10*** | 0.01 (0.03) | 0.01 | −0.13 (0.07) | −0.04 |
Severe physical assault, past y | −0.04 (0.05) | −0.02 | −0.06 (0.04) | −0.04 | 0.02 (0.09) | 0.01 |
Sexual coercion, past y | 0.14 (0.06) | 0.05* | −0.08 (0.05) | −0.04 | −0.10 (0.11) | −0.02 |
Physical assault before age 18 y | 0.04 (0.03) | 0.03 | 0.07 (0.03) | 0.06* | −0.07 (0.06) | −0.02 |
Sexual coercion before age 18 y | −0.01 (0.03) | −0.01 | 0.08 (0.03) | 0.08** | −0.20 (0.06) | −0.07** |
Outcome pretest | −0.55 (0.02) | −0.54*** | −0.41 (0.02) | −0.45*** | −0.59 (0.02) | −0.59*** |
Background Factors | ||||||
Age | −0.01 (0.00) | −0.08*** | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.01 | −0.01 (0.00) | −0.13*** |
Non-Hispanic White | 0.13 (0.04) | 0.06** | 0.07 (0.04) | 0.04 | −0.04 (0.08) | −0.01 |
Mexican | −0.10 (0.03) | −0.07** | 0.01 (0.03) | 0.01 | 0.10 (0.06) | 0.04 |
Other Hispanic | −0.07 (0.03) | −0.05* | 0.03 (0.03) | 0.03 | −0.03 (0.06) | −0.01 |
Education | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.00 | 0.00 (0.00) | 0.01 | 0.00 (0.01) | 0.01 |
Employed | 0.02 (0.03) | 0.02 | −0.05 (0.02) | −0.05* | 0.06 (0.05) | 0.02 |
Receiving welfare | −0.03 (0.03) | −0.02 | 0.03 (0.02) | 0.03 | −0.12 (0.05) | −0.05* |
Financial hardship | 0.00 (0.02) | 0.00 | 0.00 (0.02) | 0.00 | −0.13 (0.04) | −0.07** |
Cohabiting, not married | −0.09 (0.05) | −0.04 | 0.02 (0.04) | 0.01 | −0.11 (0.09) | −0.02 |
Married, spouse in house | −0.05 (0.03) | −0.03 | −0.05 (0.03) | −0.04 | 0.02 (0.06) | 0.01 |
No. of children | 0.02 (0.01) | 0.04 | 0.01 (0.01) | 0.02 | 0.00 (0.02) | 0.00 |
Living in Boston | −0.02 (0.03) | −0.01 | 0.01 (0.03) | 0.01 | 0.16 (0.06) | 0.07* |
Living in San Antonio | 0.05 (0.03) | 0.03 | 0.02 (0.03) | 0.02 | 0.01 (0.06) | 0.00 |
Model statistics | ||||||
Model F | 36.33*** | 21.74*** | 45.25*** | |||
R-squared | 0.27 | 0.19 | 0.32 |
Note: Shown are unstandardized ordinary least squares parameter estimates (b) and standardized estimates (B). The sample size was n = 1954.
2001 minus 1999 data.
*P < .05, **P < .01, ***P < .001.
The second column of Table 3 shows the multivariate results for psychological distress. These results indicated that only psychological aggression was associated with a change in psychological distress between waves. As one would expect, experiences with psychological aggression in the past year were associated with an increase in psychological distress from baseline to follow-up, net of controls. The results also indicate that individuals who experienced physical assault and sexual coercion before the age of 18 years were vulnerable to increases in psychological distress over the study period, even with controls for baseline psychological distress, more recent experiences with relationship violence, and a host of background factors. An examination of standardized parameter estimates suggested that psychological aggression in the past year was among the strongest predictors of the change in psychological distress. For example, the standardized parameter estimate for psychological aggression (B = 0.08) was equal to or larger in magnitude than those for physical assault before the age of 18 years (B = 0.06), sexual coercion before the age of 18 years (B = 0.08), and employment (B = 0.05).
The third column of Table 3 shows the multivariate results for self-rated health. Surprisingly, these results suggested that the self-rated health trajectories of respondents who reported intimate partner victimization in the past year were similar to those of respondents who reported no recent experiences with relationship violence. The results also show that individuals who experienced sexual coercion (not physical assault) before the age of 18 years were vulnerable to declines in self-rated health over the study period.
Although we were not primarily interested in the effects of background factors, we would like to briefly acknowledge several significant patterns. Our data showed favorable intoxication trajectories for older respondents and for Mexicans and other Hispanics (compared with Blacks). We also observed that the frequency of intoxication increased from baseline to follow-up for non-Hispanic Whites compared with Blacks. With respect to psychological distress, we found favorable mental health trajectories for respondents who were employed at baseline. Finally, our results indicated poorer self-rated health trajectories for respondents who were older, receiving welfare, and experiencing greater financial hardship at baseline.
DISCUSSION
Although numerous studies have examined the enduring health consequences of child abuse, few studies have documented the long-term effects of relationship violence in adulthood. We used data collected from a large probability sample of low-income urban women with children to predict changes in the frequency of intoxication, psychological distress, and self-rated health over 2 years with baseline measures of relationship violence and a host of relevant background characteristics.
Our results showed that minor physical assault and sexual coercion in the past year predicted increases in the frequency of intoxication over 2 years. Surprisingly, psychological aggression and severe physical assault in the past year were not significant predictors. In a recent cross-sectional analysis of WCF data, Hill et al.33 found that minor and severe physical assault in the past year are associated with greater frequency of intoxication. Our results for minor physical assault corroborate these findings. Because sexual coercion in the past year failed to predict the frequency of intoxication in the cross-sectional study, the results of the present investigation suggest that the effects of sexual coercion in adulthood are more long-term. By the same rationale, it appears that the effects of severe physical assault are more immediate. Nevertheless, our results are generally inconsistent with other longitudinal studies of severe physical assault.13,14 Although Zlotnick et al.12 failed to observe any long-term effect of physical violence, their mixture of minor and severe physical assault obscures the bearing of these results. To the best of our knowledge, we are among the first to consider the long-term effects of psychological aggression in the past year. Because physical assault and sexual coercion before the age of 18 years failed to predict changes in the frequency of intoxication, our results emphasize the unique impact of relationship violence in adulthood.
We also found that psychological aggression predicted increases in psychological distress. Although this pattern is generally consistent with prior longitudinal research,11 our results for minor and severe physical assault are inconsistent with other longitudinal studies.10,12 In a recent cross-sectional analysis of the WCF data, Hill et al. showed that psychological aggression, minor physical assault, and sexual coercion in the past year are associated with higher levels of psychological distress.23 The results of the present study highlight the consistent impact of psychological aggression and underscore the more immediate effects of minor physical assault and sexual coercion. Our results also suggest that psychological forms of relationship violence may be more detrimental to the mental health of women than physical forms of violence. This interpretation is generally supported by qualitative evidence. In one memorable interview excerpt, a woman expressed the following:
I started to go a little crazy. I just couldn't think of everything. All the time he was around I felt like I walking on a tightrope. Just one little slip and I knew I might get it. When he did hit me, I was relieved. At least I didn't have to be so tense anymore.34(p131)
To the best of our knowledge, we are among the first to consider the long-term mental health consequences of severe physical assault and sexual coercion in adulthood. In contrast with our results for the frequency of intoxication, we observed that individuals who experienced physical assault and sexual coercion before the age of 18 years were vulnerable to increases in psychological distress over the study period. Taken together, these results speak to the unique mental health effects of abuse in early life as compared with relationship violence in adulthood.
Our analysis of the change in self-rated health revealed no significant patterns for violence in adulthood. Because we were unable to find any longitudinal studies of overall self-rated health, it is unclear how our results (or lack thereof) compare with other examinations. If self-rated health actually measures physical health, one might expect the adverse effects of relationship violence in adulthood to surface over an extended period of time. The traumatic effects of severe physical assault on self-rated health can be instantaneous; however, the effects of psychological aggression and minor physical assault, for example, would conceivably take much longer, presumably through mechanisms like psychological distress, chronic stress, and risky health behaviors. Our result showing that individuals who experienced sexual coercion (not physical assault) before the age of 18 years were vulnerable to declines in self-rated health over the study period is generally consistent with this idea.
The present study makes several advances over previous research on relationship violence and health. Because our analysis included measures of physical assault and sexual coercion before the age of 18 years and past-year assessments of psychological aggression, minor physical assault, severe physical assault, and sexual coercion, it is one of the more comprehensive studies of relationship violence and health on record. Because we used a prospective design and controlled for baseline health status, we are confident about the causal order of our associations.
Our study also had several limitations. Because the data were restricted to Boston, Chicago, and San Antonio, it is unclear whether our results are generalizable beyond these cities. Data limitations also prevented us from considering the effects of important aspects of intimate terrorism, including dominance, control, and isolation. The measure of intoxication used in this study is a valid assessment of risky drinking practices, but it was based on a single item, which reduces reliability. Self-rated health is also difficult to interpret because it captures so much beyond physical health, including perceptions of body mass, health behaviors, and mental health. It would be informative to compare the results for self-rated health with patterns for specific health conditions, such as high blood pressure, heart disease, and a range of sexually transmitted diseases. We also suspect that the especially modest incidence rates for severe physical assault and sexual coercion in the past year may have limited our ability to detect statistically significant differences. Finally, the current study was limited by the self-report nature of the data. Specifically, we acknowledge the potential for bias resulting from social desirability. To protect individual values, self-concepts, and identities, it is possible that some individuals might falsely respond to questions by underestimating experiences with relationship violence and ill health. Because we were unable to control for social desirability in our models, our results may have exaggerated or even misrepresented the strength of the association between relationship violence and health status.
Our results show that experiences with relationship violence beyond the formative and developmental years of childhood and adolescence have far-reaching effects on the drinking behaviors and mental health of women. Our analysis also indicates that relationship violence in adulthood is among the strongest predictors of these outcomes. On the basis of these results, we recommend that future research continue to examine the enduring health consequences of relationship violence among low-income urban women. Indeed, much remains to be investigated. How far-reaching are the consequences of relationship violence in adulthood? If relationship violence in adulthood is associated with poor health trajectories, why is it? Future research must consider the mechanisms through which relationship violence in adulthood might lead to changes in health status. If we are going to develop effective strategies for recognizing and treating women who have experienced relationship violence, we need to know as much as possible about how these experiences undermine health.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Journal of Marriage and Family for valuable comments on a previous draft of the article.
Human Participant Protection
This study was based on publicly available data and was exempt from full review by the Human Subjects Research Office at the University of Miami.
References
- 1.Banyard VL. Childhood maltreatment and the mental health of low-income women. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1999;69:161–171 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Banyard VL, Williams LM. Characteristics of child sexual abuse as correlates of women's adjustment: a prospective study. J Marriage Fam 1996;58:853–865 [Google Scholar]
- 3.Draper B, Pfaff J, Pirkis J, et al. Long-term effects of childhood abuse on the quality of life and health of older people: results from the Depression and Early Prevention of Suicide in General Practice Project. J Am Geriatr Soc 2008;56:262–271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Springer KW, Sheridan J, Kuo D, Carnes M. Long-term physical and mental health consequences of childhood physical abuse: results from a large population-based sample of men and women. Child Abuse Negl 2007;31:517–530 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Thompson MP, Kingree JB, Desai S. Gender differences in long-term health consequences of physical abuse of children: data from a nationally representative survey. Am J Public Health 2004;94:599–604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kaukinen C, DeMaris A. Age at first sexual assault and current substance use and depression. J Interpers Violence 2005;20:1244–1270 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bensley L, Van Eenwyk J, Wynkoop Simmons K. Childhood family violence history and women's risk for intimate partner violence and poor health. Am J Prev Med 2003;25:38–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Epstein JN, Saunders BE, Kilpatrick DG, Resnick HS. PTSD as a mediator between childhood rape and alcohol use in adult women. Child Abuse Negl 1998;22:223–234 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wilsnack SC, Vogeltanz ND, Klassen AD, Harris TR. Childhood sexual abuse and women's substance abuse: national survey findings. J Stud Alcohol 1997;58:264–271 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Choi H, Marks NF. Marital conflict, depressive symptoms, and functional impairment. J Marriage Fam 2008;70:377–390 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Katz J, Arias I. Psychological abuse and depressive symptoms in dating women: do different types of abuse have differential effects? J Fam Violence 1999;14:281–295 [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zlotnick C, Johnson D, Kohn R. Intimate partner violence and long-term psychosocial functioning in a national sample of American women. J Interpers Violence 2006;21:262–275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kilpatrick DG, Acierno R, Resnick HS, Saunders BE, Best CL. A 2-year longitudinal analysis of the relationships between violence assault and substance use in women. J Consult Clin Psychol 1997;65:834–847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Martino SC, Collins RL, Ellickson PL. Cross–lagged relationships between substance use and intimate partner violence among a sample of young adult women. J Stud Alcohol 2005;66:139–148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Acierno R, Resnick H. Health impact of interpersonal violence 1: prevalence rates, case identification, and risk factor for sexual assault, physical assault, and domestic violence in men and women. Behav Med 1997;23:53–65 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Cunradi CB, Caetano R, Schafer J. Socioeconomic predictors of intimate partner violence among White, Black, and Hispanic couples in the United States. J Fam Violence 2002;17:377–389 [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tolman RM, Raphael J. A review of research on welfare and domestic violence. J Soc Issues 2000;56:655–682 [Google Scholar]
- 18.Tjaden P, Thoennes N. Full Report of the Prevalence, Incidence, and Consequences of Violence Against Women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey. Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice; 2000. NCJ183781 [Google Scholar]
- 19.Benson ML, Fox GL, DeMaris A, Van Wyk J. Neighborhood disadvantage, individual economic distress and violence against women in intimate relationships. J Quant Criminol 2003;19:207–235 [Google Scholar]
- 20.DeMaris A, Benson ML, Fox GL, Hill T, Van Wyk J. Distal and proximal factors in domestic violence: a test of an integrated model. J Marriage Fam 2003;65:652–657 [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bradley C. Veteran status and marital aggression: does military service make a difference? J Fam Violence 2007;22:197–209 [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fox GL, Benson ML, DeMaris AA, Van Wyk J. Economic distress and intimate violence: testing family stress and resources theory. J Marriage Fam 2002;64:793–807 [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hill TD, Mossakowski K, Angel RJ. Relationship violence and psychological distress among low-income urban women. J Urban Health 2007;84:537–551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lown EA, Schmidt LA, Wiley J. Interpersonal violence among women seeking welfare: unraveling lives. Am J Public Health 2006;96:1409–1415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tolman RM, Rosen D. Domestic violence in the lives of women receiving welfare. Violence Against Women 2001;7:141–158 [Google Scholar]
- 26.Johns Hopkins University Welfare, Children, and Families: A Three City Study. Available at: http://www.jhu.edu/∼welfare Accessed August 5, 2007
- 27.Robbins C. Sex differences in psychosocial consequences of alcohol and drug abuse. J Health Soc Behav 1989;30:117–130 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Derogatis L. Brief Symptom Inventory 18, Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual. Minneapolis, MN: National Computer System; 2000 [Google Scholar]
- 29.Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self–rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. J Health Soc Behav 1997;38:21–37 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Straus M, Hamby S, Boney-McCoy S, Sugarman D. The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): development and preliminary psychometric data. J Fam Issues 1996;17:283–316 [Google Scholar]
- 31.Straus M, Douglas E. A short form of the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales, and typologies for severity and mutuality. Violence Vict 2004;19:507–520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Johnson D. Two-wave panel analysis: comparing statistical methods for studying the effects of transitions. J Marriage Fam 2005;67:1061–1075 [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hill T, Nielsen A, Angel R. Relationship violence and frequency of intoxication among low–income urban women. Subst Use Misuse 2009;44(5):684–701 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Gelles R, Straus M. Intimate Violence: Causes and Consequences of Abuse in the American Family. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster; 1988 [Google Scholar]