Skip to main content
Age and Ageing logoLink to Age and Ageing
letter
. 2009 Jan 15;38(2):237–241. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afn278

C-reactive protein and memory function suggest antagonistic pleiotropy in very old nondemented subjects

Jeremy M Silverman 1,2,1,2,*, Michal Schnaider Beeri 1, James Schmeidler 1, Clive Rosendorff 2,3,2,3, Gary Angelo 1, Rizalina S Mavris 2, Hillel T Grossman 1,2,1,2, Gregory A Elder 1,2,1,2, Jose Carrion-Baralt 1, Rebecca West 1
PMCID: PMC2724888  PMID: 19147740

SIR—A possible role of inflammation in the development of dementia [1] has led to investigations examining whether C-reactive protein (CRP), a systemic marker of inflammation, is associated with worse cognitive function and decline in old age. Elevated CRP has been associated with worse global and specific cognitive functioning [2–7], although other studies have found no relationship between CRP and cognition [8–10]. Most studies have examined samples averaging <75 years [3–6, 9, 10]. We hypothesised that elevated CRP would be related to worse cognitive function in very old cognitively healthy subjects.

Methods

Subjects

Cognitively healthy subjects with no other preconditions beyond their age being 75 years or more were recruited from the Bronx Veterans Affairs Medical Center (BxVAMC) and senior centres in New York metropolitan area for a project investigating cardiovascular risk factors and cognition. The Mount Sinai School of Medicine and the BxVAMC both provided IRB approval and all subjects gave written informed consent.

Cognitive assessment

We evaluated overall cognitive functioning by direct assessments and informant interviews using the Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR) [11] and included only subjects with CDR = 0 (not demented). In addition, subjects with a history of a neurological, medical or psychiatric disorder potentially affecting cognition (e.g. stroke, schizophrenia)—by self-report, informant report or medical charts—were excluded.

The subjects received a neuropsychological test battery, described previously [12]: the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE), tests of memory (sum of immediate recall trials 1, 2 and 3), delayed recall, recognition and savings (100*delayed recall/immediate recall trial 3), executive functions (Trail making A, Trail making B), intelligence (Shipley) and language (word fluency).

Blood samples

The subjects’ blood samples were sent to the BxVAMC haematology laboratory and wide-range CRP, possessing near-perfect correlation with high-sensitivity CRP [13], was measured from plasma using the ADVIA 1650 Chemistry System with a CRP latex reagent. Also, apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotyping and other values relevant to cardiovascular risk factors (e.g. cholesterol, haemoglobin A1C) were measured.

Statistical procedures

Subjects were first categorised into conventionally identified ‘low’ (≤1.0 mg/l), ‘normal’ (>1.0 mg/l and ≤3.0 mg/l) and ‘high’ (>3.0 mg/l) CRP groups [14]. Logarithmic transformation of the non-normally distributed CRP was applied for all other statistical analyses. The MMSE was used to examine global cognitive function. To summarise the other neuropsychological tests, we conducted a factor analysis using varimax rotation. Stepwise regression assessed the linear and quadratic associations of CRP with each factor and MMSE, controlling for age, sex and education, for the entire sample, and divided by sex, or by the presence of an APOE-e4 allele. Two-sided tests were employed.

Results

Of the 189 nondemented subjects with CRP measurements, 13 subjects did not have neuropsychological test data allowing for the factor analysis, and were excluded. This left 176 subjects (112 men, 64 women) with full neuropsychological test data [mean age: 85 ± 6 years; mean years of education: 15 (±3)]. The preponderance of males reflects the many subjects recruited through the BxVAMC. There were 123 subjects with no APOE-e4 allele (3/3: n = 92; 2/3: n = 31), 50 subjects with an e4 allele (3/4: n = 35; 2/4: n = 10; 4/4: n = 5) and three missing DNA. The factor analysis produced a model with two factors, one primarily including memory functions (high weights from delayed recall, savings and recognition) and one primarily including executive/language functions (high weights from Trail making B, word fluency, Trail making A, immediate recall and Shipley).

Untransformed mean CRP level was 2.5 mg/l ± 5.2 mg/l (ranging from 0.00 to 41.50) with 97 having ‘low’, 40 ‘normal’ and 39 ‘high’ CRP levels. Memory scores were significantly different [F(2, 170) = 4.34, P = 0.01] across groups, where the low-CRP group had worse memory scores than the other two groups. The groups did not differ in executive/language score or MMSE. As indicated in Table 1, we found that higher CRP values were significantly associated with lower (worse) executive/language function scores and also with higher (better) memory scores. The CRP relationship with MMSE was not significant. No quadratic associations were significant.

Table 1.

Demographics and partial correlations for total sample and APOE-e4 non-carriers and carriers

Variable Total samplea Non-APOE-e4 carriers APOE-e4 carriers
Demographics
n 176 123 50
Mean age (SD) 85 (6) 85 (6) 85 (6)
Male/female 112/64 79/44 31/19
Mean years of education  (SD) 15 (3) 15 (3) 14 (4)
Partial correlations (lineareffect) with log(CRP)b
Memory 0.26*** 0.26** −0.06
Executive/language −0.18* −0.15 −0.19
MMSE −0.11 −0.06 −0.21

aIncludes three subjects not genotyped for APOE.

bControlling for age, sex and years of education in multiple regression analysis.

*P < 0.05, **P < 0.005, ***P < 0.001

There were no differences in CRP, memory, executive/language or MMSE scores in subjects divided by the presence of an APOE-e4 allele. CRP was significantly positively related to memory in those subjects with no APOE-e4 allele, but not in those with an e4 allele. We divided the sample by sex and found a similar pattern of associations in both sexes (memory: males, r = 0.21, P < 0.05; females, r = 0.26, P < 0.05; executive/language: males, r = −0.20, P < 0.05; females, r = −0.26, P < 0.05; MMSE: males, r = −0.14, n.s.; females, r = −0.17, n.s.). We also divided the sample by whether subjects were ascertained from the BxVAMC (n = 48, all males) or from the community at large (n = 128, 64 males and 64 females), and observed similar significant patterns in each group. Finally, the results were not affected by the inclusion of cholesterol level (total, LDL or HDL), haemoglobin A1C or smoking history as covariates.

Discussion

Contrary to our prediction, elevated CRP was positively associated with good memory. This relationship was specific to subjects without an APOE-e4 allele. The unexpected direction of this relationship might be due to the age characteristics of our sample. Evidence implicating elevated CRP as a correlate of impaired cognition comes primarily from samples of younger old [2–6, 15], and the few studies that included the very old have not generally found such a relationship [3, 8]. As studies begin to focus on risk factors for cognitive impairment, decline, dementia and AD in the oldest old, the effects of risk factors previously identified for relatively younger cohorts appear to vary considerably. For example, the increased risk attributable to a positive family history for AD or carrying the APOE-e4 allele is substantially lower for an 85 year old than for a 65 year old [16, 17]. Interestingly, other factors associated with cardiovascular risk and disease have shown similar apparently age-related discrepant effects. The metabolic syndrome is associated with cardiovascular disease and diabetes and has been found to be a risk factor for cognitive impairment and dementia in younger old [18]. However, the metabolic syndrome was associated with a decelerated cognitive decline in a very old sample in a recent longitudinal study [19]. Also, we recently observed an unexpected relationship between both total and LDL cholesterol with good memory function [20]. Similarly, evidence for other factors (e.g. low blood pressure, weight loss) associated with reduced risk for cardiovascular disease and sometimes also for dementia may, in the very old, be associated with an increased risk of dementia [21, 22].

We found no association between CRP and MMSE but, consistent with our initial prediction, elevated CRP was associated with lower levels of executive/language functioning. Previous studies of younger samples have also observed that elevated CRP is related to worse performance in cognitive functions including tests of executive function [3], and also of memory [2, 6] and global cognitive functioning [3, 4]. In our study, relationships were observed between CRP with both memory and executive/language factors, calculated through factor analysis as independent of each other, but those relationships ran in opposite directions. Thus, our study suggests that the relationship between CRP and cognitive function is not a simple one but varies depending on the specific type of cognitive function examined. In this context, we note that impairments in these two areas of cognitive function may have different implications for subsequent overall cognitive health; reduced executive/language functions may be a normal characteristic of aging, while age-associated memory impairments may indicate a degenerative process [23]. If so, then in a very old population, elevated CRP may be associated with more normal age-related impairments in executive/language function, whereas it also is associated with individuals who, by virtue of their better memory function, are less liable to dementia.

A limitation of any cross sectional study is the bias introduced by studying only those eligible subjects who survive long enough to be ascertained. It is possible that, like CRP [24], a relatively weak memory even in cognitively normal individuals may increase mortality [25]. An interaction between them would be an additional possible selection bias. A more fundamental limitation of a cross sectional study is that it does not distinguish between differences in stable memory level and differences in change in memory over time. We plan to follow our sample longitudinally to see whether the association of high CRP with good memory is observed for changes in memory over time. Our sample was clearly biased in favour of male subjects, due primarily to recruiting from the BxVAMC, and this reduces the representativeness of the sample. We note, however, that the patterns observed remain both when we examine each sex separately and when we examined BxVAMC and other community subjects separately.

While the association of elevated CRP with worse executive/language functioning is consistent with findings in younger samples, the association of elevated CRP with good memory runs in the opposite direction. An age-related variable effect of CRP on memory function may reflect antagonistic pleiotropy, which refers to gene and other effects that may be favourable at one point in life and unfavourable at another [26]. This has been associated with genes that improve early fitness but lead to diminished functioning later, but the opposite age effect has also been described [27]. An individual who survives to a late age may develop processes that adapt to the challenges that these risk factors pose at an earlier age. Such an adaptation might fall under the maxim of ‘what doesn't kill you makes you stronger’. Alternatively, it is possible that rare individuals possess inherited protection from factors which to most other people pose a threat throughout life. At earlier ages, such protected individuals would be greatly outnumbered by those who face increased risk of disease in the presence of these factors. At these ages, elevated levels in a given risk factor would be associated with less successful cognitive ageing. At very late ages, however, mortality will have disproportionately diminished the ranks of the less protected individuals and survivors from this frailer group will tend to have a lower risk factor profile. The initially small protected group would tend to disproportionately survive to late ages and include those with ‘high risk’ factor profiles. Collectively examining these very old people would lead to associations between those high levels of ‘risk factors’ and successful cognitive ageing. Thus, a dynamic reapportionment of those with liability versus resistance may occur at the extremes of old age and this may help explain a variable effect of risk factors with age.

Key points

  • Reports that increased CRP is associated with cognitive functioning deficits in the elderly have not looked at very old populations.

  • We examined, cross-sectionally, CRP and memory and executive function in a very old, nondemented population.

  • We found that higher CRP was modestly associated with weaker executive functioning, as predicted, but unexpectedly it was also associated with a better memory function.

  • The latter may be an example of antagonistic pleiotropy, i.e. effects that may be unfavourable at one point in life and favourable at another.

  • Alternatively, the high-functioning survivors at these very old ages may disproportionately include genetically protected individuals who at earlier ages were rare in their age cohort.

Funding

The work was supported by Project 4 in P01-AG02219; K01 AG023515; P50-AG05138; United States Department of Veterans Affairs; Berkman Charitable Trust.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

  • 1.McGeer PL, Rogers J, McGeer EG. Inflammation, anti-inflammatory agents and Alzheimer disease: the last 12 years. J Alzheimers Dis. 2006;9(3 Suppl):271–6. doi: 10.3233/jad-2006-9s330. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Komulainen P, Lakka TA, Kivipelto M, et al. Serum high sensitivity C-reactive protein and cognitive function in elderly women. Age Ageing. 2007;36:443–8. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afm051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schram MT, Euser SM, de Craen AJ, et al. Systemic markers of inflammation and cognitive decline in old age. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2007;55:708–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2007.01159.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Yaffe K, Lindquist K, Penninx BW, et al. Inflammatory markers and cognition in well-functioning African-American and white elders. Neurology. 2003;61:76–80. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000073620.42047.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Sweat V, Starr V, Bruehl H, et al. C-reactive protein is linked to lower cognitive performance in overweight and obese women. Inflammation. 2008;31:198–207. doi: 10.1007/s10753-008-9065-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Teunissen CE, van Boxtel MP, Bosma H, et al. Inflammation markers in relation to cognition in a healthy aging population. J Neuroimmunol. 2003;134:142–50. doi: 10.1016/s0165-5728(02)00398-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tilvis RS, Kahonen-Vare MH, Jolkkonen J, Valvanne J, Pitkala KH, Strandberg TE. Predictors of cognitive decline and mortality of aged people over a 10-year period. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2004;59:268–74. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.3.m268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Van Den Biggelaar AH, Gussekloo J, de Craen AJ, et al. Inflammation and interleukin-1 signaling network contribute to depressive symptoms but not cognitive decline in old age. Exp Gerontol. 2007;42:693–701. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2007.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Dik MG, Jonker C, Hack CE, Smit JH, Comijs HC, Eikelenboom P. Serum inflammatory proteins and cognitive decline in older persons. Neurology. 2005;64:1371–7. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000158281.08946.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Weuve J, Ridker PM, Cook NR, Buring JE, Grodstein F. High-sensitivity C-reactive protein and cognitive function in older women. Epidemiology. 2006;17:183–9. doi: 10.1097/01.ede.0000198183.60572.c9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hughes CP, Berg L, Danzier WL, et al. A new clinical rating scale for staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–72. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Beeri MS, Schmeidler J, Sano M, et al. Age, gender, and education norms on the CERAD neuropsychological battery in the oldest old. Neurology. 2006;67:1006–10. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000237548.15734.cd. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rogowski O, Vered Y, Shapira I, Hirsh M, Zakut V, Berliner S. Introducing the wide range C-reactive protein (wr-CRP) into clinical use for the detection of microinflammation. Clin Chim Acta. 2005;358:151–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cccn.2005.02.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Sabatine MS, Morrow DA, Jablonski KA, et al. Prognostic significance of the Centers for Disease Control/American Heart Association high-sensitivity C-reactive protein cut points for cardiovascular and other outcomes in patients with stable coronary artery disease. Circulation. 2007;115:1528–36. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.649939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Alley DE, Crimmins EM, Karlamangla A, Hu P, Seeman TE. Inflammation and rate of cognitive change in high-functioning older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63:50–5. doi: 10.1093/gerona/63.1.50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Farrer LA, Cupples LA, van Duijn CM, et al. Apolipoprotein E genotype in patients with Alzheimer's disease: implications for the risk of dementia among relatives. Ann Neurol. 1995;38:797–808. doi: 10.1002/ana.410380515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Silverman JM, Ciresi G, Smith CJ, Marin DB, Schnaider-Beeri M. Variability of familial risk of Alzheimer disease across the late life span. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:565–73. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Yaffe K. Metabolic syndrome and cognitive decline. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2007;4:123–6. doi: 10.2174/156720507780362191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Van Den BE, Biessels GJ, de Craen AJ, Gussekloo J, Westendorp RG. The metabolic syndrome is associated with decelerated cognitive decline in the oldest old. Neurology. 2007;69:979–85. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271381.30143.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.West R, Beeri MS, Schmeidler J, et al. Better memory functioning associated with higher total and LDL cholesterol levels in very elderly subjects without the APOE4 allele. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16:781–5. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181812790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Verghese J, Lipton RB, Hall CB, Kuslansky G, Katz MJ. Low blood pressure and the risk of dementia in very old individuals. Neurology. 2003;61:1667–72. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000098934.18300.be. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Johnson DK, Wilkins CH, Morris JC. Accelerated weight loss may precede diagnosis in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1312–7. doi: 10.1001/archneur.63.9.1312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Goldman WP, Morris JC. Evidence that age-associated memory impairment is not a normal variant of aging. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2001;15:72–9. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200104000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Strandberg TE, Tilvis RS. C-reactive protein, cardiovascular risk factors, and mortality in a prospective study in the elderly. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2000;20:1057–60. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.20.4.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Schupf N, Tang MX, Albert SM, et al. Decline in cognitive and functional skills increases mortality risk in nondemented elderly. Neurology. 2005;65:1218–26. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180970.07386.cb. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Williams GC. Pleiotropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution. 1957;11:398–411. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Toupance B, Godelle B, Gouyon PH, Schachter F. A model for antagonistic pleiotropic gene action for mortality and advanced age. Am J Hum Genet. 1998;62:1525–34. doi: 10.1086/301865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Age and Ageing are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES