Skip to main content
Journal of General Internal Medicine logoLink to Journal of General Internal Medicine
. 2009 Jul 4;24(9):1012–1017. doi: 10.1007/s11606-009-1044-3

Disclosing Medical Errors to Patients: It’s Not What You Say, It’s What They Hear

Albert W Wu 1,2,, I-Chan Huang 3, Samantha Stokes 4, Peter J Pronovost 1,5
PMCID: PMC2726881  PMID: 19578819

ABSTRACT

BACKGROUND

There is consensus that patients should be told if they are injured by medical care. However, there is little information on how they react to different methods of disclosure.

OBJECTIVE

To determine if volunteers’ reactions to videos of physicians disclosing adverse events are related to the physician apologizing and accepting responsibility.

DESIGN

Survey of viewers randomized to watch videos of disclosures of three adverse events (missed mammogram, chemotherapy overdose, delay in surgical therapy) with designed variations in extent of apology (full, non-specific, none) and acceptance of responsibility (full, none).

PARTICIPANTS

Adult volunteer sample from the general community in Baltimore.

MEASUREMENTS

Viewer evaluations of physicians in the videos using standardized scales.

RESULTS

Of 200 volunteers, 50% were <40 years, 25% were female, 80% were African American, and 50% had completed high school. For designed variations, scores were non-significantly higher for full apology/responsibility, and lower for no apology/no responsibility. Perceived apology or responsibility was related to significantly higher ratings (chi-square, 81% vs. 38% trusted; 56% vs. 27% would refer, p < 0.05), but inclination to sue was unchanged (43% vs. 47%). In logistic regression analyses adjusting for age, gender, race and education, perceived apology and perceived responsibility were independently related to higher ratings for all measures. Inclination to sue was reduced non-significantly.

CONCLUSIONS

Patients will probably respond more favorably to physicians who apologize and accept responsibility for medical errors than those who do not apologize or give ambiguous responses. Patient perceptions of what is said may be more important than what is actually said. Desire to sue may not be affected despite a full apology and acceptance of responsibility.

KEY WORDS: medical error, disclosure, apology, video, patient perceptions, vignette

INTRODUCTION

There is consensus among professional organizations, ethicists, physicians and the general public that physicians are obligated to disclose medical errors that cause harm (adverse events) to patients.119

Research suggests that following an adverse event, patients want an apology, an explanation of what happened and someone to take responsibility.4,12,13,2024 Practice appears to fall short of this, with less than a third of patients even told about harmful errors,10,12,25,26 and wide variation in what physicians choose to disclose.2729

Few studies have examined the disclosure process or its consequences for patients. Many patients believe that doctors sometimes withhold information about medical accidents.30 Patients involved in an adverse event expressed concern about the occurrence and timing of disclosure, lack of an apology and inadequate follow-up.31 An Internet survey found that full disclosure may strengthen the patient-physician relationship and may reduce desire to sanction physicians.21 A comparison of different methods of disclosure found that full disclosure resulted in greater trust and satisfaction, and no increased likelihood of seeking legal advice.32 Other studies found that patients were more likely to trust physicians who played an active role in disclosing a serious error,33 whereas inadequate disclosure may increase the likelihood of a malpractice claim.34 Despite these findings, there are unanswered questions about conducting disclosure discussions, including how apology and acknowledgment of responsibility affect patients’ and families’ reactions.27,3435

We showed volunteers videotaped vignettes that depicted physicians disclosing adverse events to patients and surveyed them on their evaluation of the physicians. We hypothesized that disclosures that included full apology and acceptance of responsibility would be associated with improved physician trust and reduced proclivity to sue.

METHODS

Design

This was a survey of volunteers who viewed video vignettes of a physician disclosing an adverse event to a patient or family member. We created videos of three adverse events using actors to portray physicians and patients. Volunteers recruited from Baltimore City were randomized to view vignettes including different combinations of apology and responsibility. Viewers completed a survey on their emotional response, their evaluation of the physician and likelihood of suing.

Vignette Development

Three adverse events were depicted: a year-long delay in noticing a malignant-looking lesion on a mammogram, a chemotherapy overdose ten times the intended amount and a slow response to pages by a pediatric surgeon for a patient who eventually codes and is rushed to emergency surgery. The dialogues were scripted to include elements of an ideal initial disclosure including: (1) notification of an error, (2) description of what happened, (3) a sincere apology, (4) acceptance of responsibility, (5) description of steps to be taken to mitigate harm and correct the situation, and (6) assurance of an investigation to prevent recurrence.12,25,36

We designed five versions of each case by varying two of these elements: the apology and acceptance of responsibility. The apology was varied so that the physician made either a full (personal and specific) apology (A+), a non-specific apology (e.g., I’m sorry that your family member is so ill”)(Ao) or no apology (A−). Responsibility was either accepted (R+) or not accepted (R−) by the physician (Table 1). A possible sixth variation (nonspecific apology, responsibility not accepted) was excluded to simplify the design. Because preliminary analyses showed few differences in responses to variations 2, 3 and 4, we collapsed them into a single category “incomplete apology or responsibility (Ao/Ro).” Variations were further grouped into category I: A+R+, category II: Ao/Ro, and category III: A−R−.

Table 1.

Designed Variations in the Video Vignettes of Disclosure Discussions

Variation 1 (A+R+) Variation 2 (A−R+) Variation 3 (AoR+)
Sincere apology No apology Non-specific apology
Accepted responsibility Accepted responsibility Accepted responsibility
Variation 4 (A+R−) Variation 5 (A−R−)
Sincere apology No apology
Did not accept responsibility Did not accept responsibility

Category I: variation 1: sincere apology/accepted responsibility (A+R+); category II: variations 2, 3 and 4 combined: incomplete apology and/or responsibility Ao/Ro; category III: variation 5: no apology/no responsibility (A−R−)

Different professional actors were used for each of the three cases: four of the actors were female, two were male and all were Caucasian. Johns Hopkins Medical Video produced the videos.

Survey Instrument

We reviewed the literature and consulted with experts to identify domains and draft questions. The survey evaluated seven aspects of the discussion: (1) overall perception of the physician’s handling of the incident, (2) evaluation of the physician, (3) viewer’s emotional response, (4) trust in the physician, (5) desire to have the physician as one's own doctor, (6) inclination to refer a friend/family member to the physician and (7) likelihood of suing the physician. The survey also asked viewers whether or not they perceived an apology or the physician accepting responsibility.

Respondents could rate the physician’s handling of the incident as poor, fair, good, very good or excellent. Evaluation of the physician included: concern for the patient, professionalism, medical competence, sympathy toward the patient and defensive behavior. Emotional responses included feeling: upset, reassured, frustrated and angry. Questions adapted from trust-in-physician scales37 included: cares for the patient as a person, thinks only about what is best for his/her patients, never misleads you about anything and would put the patient’s needs above their own when treating medical problems.

The questions used 5-point Likert-type response options (emotional response: not at all, a little bit, moderately, quite a lot and extremely; evaluation of physician, trust in physician, having physician as own, refer and sue: strongly agree, agree, not sure, disagree or strongly disagree) and were scored as summated rating scales. Cronbach’s alpha38 for the multi-item scales were satisfactory: negative emotional response (0.84), positive perception of physician (0.80) and trust in physician (0.71).

Demographic questions included participant’s age (18–30, 31–40, 41–50, 51–60 and >61), gender, race/ethnicity, number of doctor visits and hospital admissions in the past year, educational attainment (high school, college and graduate school) and whether they had ever experienced a medical error.15

PROCEDURES

Recruitment

Volunteers were recruited using fliers in Baltimore City and e-mail inviting participation in a study about “Difficult Medical Situations.” Between April 1 2004 and July 28 2004 we enrolled 200 participants. Inclusion criteria included age >18 years and a self-reported English reading level at the eighth-grade level or higher. Compensation was $20. The study was approved by the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board.

Interview Procedure

Each volunteer viewed three disclosure dialogues, with variations and order viewed assigned using a random number table. The terms ‘medical error’ and ‘medical mistake’ were not mentioned, and videos were viewed individually to prevent contamination. Prior to each video, volunteers were given information that the patient/family member would have had and encouraged to put themselves in the position of the patient or family member. Volunteers completed a survey after each video. The entire process took 30 to 45 min. Among 600 viewings by the 200 volunteers, 19.5% were of an A+R+ vignette, 19.5% A−R+, 21.5% AoR+, 20.5% A+R− and 18.9% A−R−.

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

Chi-square tests were used for differences in viewer demographic characteristics across the vignettes. We aimed to enroll 186 subjects to have 80% power to detect a difference of 20% in trust score between apologizing vs. not apologizing at a level of p < 0.05.

The 5-point response scale used for each item was dichotomized into positive vs. negative response, based on the distribution of responses. The frequency of positive vs. negative responses was compared by the designed variations for apology and accepting responsibility, and by the viewer’s perception of whether these had occurred.

Chi-square tests were used to examine bivariate relationships of the designed and perceived variations to responses. A p-value of less than 0.05 indicated statistical significance. To adjust for potential confounding effects of viewer characteristics, logistic regression was used to examine the multivariable relationships of variations to responses, adjusted for age, gender, educational background and race. Odds ratios and 95% CI were reported for positive response vs. negative response on each scale for category I vs. category II vs. category III (the reference group). All analyses were performed using STATA 8.0.

RESULTS

Of the 200 participants, 50% were <40 years old, 25% were female, 80% were African American, and 50% had completed high school (Table 2). Demographic variables were balanced across the three cases (p > 0.1), so analyses were based on pooled responses.

Table 2.

Respondent Characteristics (n = 200)

  Frequency (%)
Age (years old)
18–30 22
31–40 27
41–50 34
51–60 15
>60 2
Gender
Male 25
Race
White/Caucasian 14
Black/African American 80
Hispanic/Latino 1
Native American 4
Other 1
Education
High school 50
College 30
Graduate 20

Designed Variations in Apology and Accepting Responsibility

Table 3 shows the association of the designed variation in physician handling of the disclosure with the dependent variables. In A+R+ vignettes, compared to Ao/Ro viewers were more likely to rate physicians as having handled the incident well, have positive perceptions, trust them, and want to refer or have them as their doctor. Physicians in A−R− vignettes received better ratings for handling of the incident than Ao/Ro physicians. Viewers were most likely to want to sue Ao/Ro physicians, followed by A−R− and A+R+. None of these comparisons were statistically significant.

Table 3.

Association of Variation in Error Disclosure (Designed and Viewer Perceived) and Outcomes

  Full apology and responsibility (A+R+) Incomplete apology or responsibility (Ao/Ro) Neither apology nor responsibility (A−R−) P-value for chi-square test
Good handling of incident 57% 45% 51% P = 0.05
56% 44% 32% P < 0.001
Positive perception of physician 48% 42% 47% P = 0.40
58% 40% 21% P < 0.001
Negative emotional response 51% 49% 53% P = 0.83
40% 56% 64% P < 0.001
Trust in physician 71% 59% 64% P = 0.08
81% 52% 38% P < 0.001
Having physician as own doctor 21% 15% 17% P = 0.38
25% 10% 8% P < 0.001
Referral to physician 18% 17% 19% P = 0.85
25% 11% 8% P < 0.001
Sue physician 39% 47% 41% P = 0.19
43% 46% 47% P = 0.65

Unbolded text: Designed variation

Bolded text: Viewer perception

Multivariate results for the designed vignettes and different outcomes were mixed (Table 4). Compared to A−R−, viewers were more likely to rate A+R+ physicians as having handled the incident well (OR = 1.16), less likely to have a negative emotional response, more likely to trust the physician, want them as their doctor and refer. They were slightly less likely to want to sue, but also rated physicians less positively (OR = 0.86). Viewers gave still more negative ratings to Ao/Ro physicians and were more likely to want to sue (OR = 1.38). None of these comparisons were statistically significant.

Table 4.

Multivariate Analysis for the Association of Designed Variation in Error Disclosure and Outcomes: OR (95% Confidence Intervals) b

  Good in handling incident Positive perception of physician Negative emotional response Trust in physician Having physician as own doctor Referral to physician Sue physician
Full apology and responsibility (A+R+)a 1.16 (0.66, 2.01) 0.86 (0.50, 1.50) 0.82 (0.47, 1.42) 1.49 (0.83, 2.64) 1.53 (0.75, 3.14) 1.19 (0.58, 2.41) 0.97 (0.55, 1.69)
Incomplete apology or responsibility (Ao/Ro)a 0.73 (0.47, 1.14) 0.77 (0.49, 1.19) 0.85 (0.55, 1.33) 0.86 (0.55, 1.35) 0.90 (0.49, 1.63) 0.87 (0.49, 1.54) 1.38 (0.88, 2.15)

aReference group: neither apology nor responsibility

bAdjusting for age, gender, education and race

Viewer Perceptions of Apology and Accepting Responsibility

The bolded text in Table 3 shows the association of viewer perceptions of the disclosure and the different outcomes. When viewers perceived A+R+, they gave higher ratings on handling of the incident, reported less negative emotional response, more positive perceptions and more trust, and were more inclined to refer or have the physician as their doctor. Next came Ao/Ro, followed by A−R− (P all < 0.001). Viewers who perceived A−R− were slightly more inclined to sue, followed by perceived Ao/Ro and was least likely to sue perceived A+R+ physicians (P > 0.05).

In multivariate analyses of perceptions and outcomes, compared to A−R−, A+R+ physicians were rated higher for handling the incident, perceived more positively and trusted more (ORs = 3.3, 5.8 and 7.3; p all P < 0.05)(Table 5). Similarly, compared to A−R−, Ao/Ro physicians were rated higher for incident handling, perceived more positively and trusted more (ORs = 1.7, 2.6 and 1.8; all with P < 0.05). A similar pattern was observed for other outcomes, including having physician as own doctor, referring, less negative emotional response and slightly less propensity to sue (not statistically significant).

Table 5.

Multivariate Analysis for the Association of Viewer Perceptions of Variation in Error Disclosure and Outcomes: OR (95% Confidence Intervals)

  Good in handling incident Positive perception of physician Negative emotional response Trust in physician Having physician as own doctor Referral physician to friends Sue physician
Full apology and responsibility (A+R+)a 3.33e (2.05, 5.41) 5.80e (3.42, 9.86) 0.41e (0.26, 0.64) 7.26e (4.44, 77.86) 4.01e (1.88, 8.55) 4.23e (1.99, 8.99) 0.83 (0.53, 1.30)
Incomplete apology or responsibility (Ao/Ro)a 1.71c (1.03, 2.83) 2.6 (1.51, 4.50)1d 0.78 (0.48, 1.26) 1.77c (1.10, 2.85) 1.40 (0.60, 3.56) 1.52 (0.66, 3.50) 0.96 (0.60, 1.53)

aReference group: neither apology nor responsibility

bAdjusting for age, gender, education and race

cP < 0.05, dP < 0.01, eP < 0.001

Relationship of Viewer Characteristics and Outcomes

Multivariate exploration of the relationships of age, gender and education to outcomes yielded mixed results. Older age was significantly associated with negative emotional response [OR = 1.2 (1.02, 1.42)]; having physician as own doctor [OR = 1.4 (1.11, 1.77)] and referral [OR = 1.3 (1.03, 1.63)]. Female gender was associated with negative emotional response [OR = 1.5 (1.02, 2.28)]. College education was associated with positive handling of the incident [OR = 4.0 (1.84, 8.71)] and positive perception of the physician [OR = 3.4 (1.58, 7.40)].

DISCUSSION

In this study, a full apology and acceptance of responsibility were associated with better ratings and greater trust in the physician, but were not associated with decreased propensity to sue. An unexpected finding was that after an adverse event, the patient’s perception of what was said appeared to be more important than what was actually said.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the importance of how patients interpret the physician’s communication about an adverse event. The video vignette methodology we used allowed us to control what was actually said, observing how participants interpreted this and how both of these factors related to their evaluation of the physician.

Our results support those from earlier studies suggesting that full disclosure of an adverse event leads to greater trust and more positive regard by patients and family members.21,32 This was particularly true when the physician acknowledged responsibility for the adverse event. Interestingly, acceptance of responsibility without an accompanying apology yielded no such benefit and may have even resulted in more negative judgments. This is similar to Schwappach’s finding that equivocal statements acknowledging an error had no effect or even increased the probability of negative ratings.21 Our findings complement those of previous studies in that we obtained a community sample rather than health plan members.21,32

A surprising finding was that the perception of what is said was more strongly associated with how physicians were perceived and trusted than what was actually said. This finding has face validity, but there have been few studies of how handling of an incident affects patients’ evaluations.21,32 One interpretation is that just because we think we’ve conveyed a message does not mean that it will be heard and understood. In communicating, the sender encodes meaning in his or her words and the receiver decodes the meaning.39 Ambiguous wording from the sender or preoccupation of the receiver increases the chances of translation error. In addition, multiple factors affect how patients evaluate physicians.40

If it is difficult to modify patient perceptions based on language, what can physicians do? One piece of advice is that once you have decided to disclose an error, you should make sure that the patient knows you really mean it. For a discussion that includes explaining, apologizing and accepting responsibility, this may involve repeating the message and aligning other channels of communication (e.g., posture, demeanor) to be congruent with the expression of regret, contrition and empathy. It is also important that physicians ask questions to help ensure the message is getting through. As in aviation, use of a “read back,” where the receiver is required to verbally repeat the sender’s message before taking any action, may be a useful way for physicians to test patient comprehension. There is a need for further research on the disclosure process.

Physicians and risk managers are particularly concerned that disclosure may increase the chances of being sued41—this is why many physicians never admit their mistakes.42 Studdert has raised a theoretical argument against disclosure43 but empirical evidence on the topic is limited.34,4446 In our study, full disclosure and acceptance of responsibility were related to only a slight reduction in intention to sue. This may have been influenced by the severity of the outcomes, which are important in determining future actions.10 Mazor found that disclosure had a protective effect of 7.1% against seeking legal advice.32 Schwappach noted that in severe adverse outcomes, an honest, empathic and accountable approach decreased patients’ support for strong physician sanctions by 59%.21 Fisseni found that patients were more likely to stay with general practitioners after serious medical errors if the physician played an active role in discovery and disclosure.33

Our results could suggest that disclosure of an unsuspected error could lead to an increase in the number of lawsuits by making more patients aware they have suffered preventable harm. Given that six out of every seven patients who suffer preventable harm are unaware that it might have been prevented, disclosing the error might actually increase the number of suits being filed. Our finding that disclosure does not appear to increase the proclivity to sue among those who were aware that an error had occurred does not change the absolute risk that any given patient will sue. Regardless, the ethical imperative should compel a full disclosure to patients who suffer preventable harm.

Our study had several limitations. Some are inherent in the use of vignettes in research. Vignettes can be valuable tools for studying peoples’ attitudes, perceptions and beliefs.4749 It is also known that response biases can be introduced by a subtle difference in how material is framed.50,51 In our study, the scenarios were lifelike, but were compressed in length, possibly making it difficult to appreciate all the information presented. This is a problem that also occurs in real-life patient-doctor discussions. Another limitation is that some viewers may have judged the physicians who did not take responsibility for the adverse events as not having caused them, and rated them highly based on competence rather than honesty. Although this might also occur in reality, it would be unacceptable to recommend that physicians should dodge their responsibility for the incident. Finally, asking participants to rate vignettes is inherently projective. We don’t actually know how the participants in our study would act if they experienced an adverse event.

We may have been underpowered to detect differences, particularly between levels of apology, and to detect differences among the different vignettes. However, the trends observed were consistent with our hypotheses. There was an over-representation of African American and lower educational attainment participants. This group was fairly representative of East Baltimore and many other major metropolitan centers; the results are also consistent with previous results obtained from a primarily Caucasian sample.32 Our results hinted that viewers with more education perceived the physicians more positively. If these subjects were also more sensitive to our designed variations, they might have responded more in the way we hypothesized. In future studies, it would be important to include enough subjects from different demographic groups to test potential associations.

We omitted a possible sixth version of our cases (AoR−). If this affected viewers’evaluations of the vignettes that they did see (for example, by diminishing the contrast between different versions), this could have introduced measurement error. We believe that it is unlikely to have introduced much bias, since version selection was randomized, and viewers gave their evaluations immediately after each video. Finally, due to constraints of time and money, we did not achieve ethnic diversity among the actors in our vignettes. This could have led some of the participants to rate physicians less positively or to identify less strongly with either the doctors or the patients, which could diminish the reliability of their evaluations.

The results of disclosing adverse events to patients and families, including the finding that 'it's not what you say, it's what they hear,' have implications for education and practice. There is broad consensus that physicians and health-care organizations should disclose adverse events to patients and their families. Physicians need support and assistance to accomplish this difficult task successfully. Training is needed to help those in training and practice carry out the difficult task of disclosing adverse events. It will be important to evaluate the effectiveness of that training, including how it is perceived by patients and their families.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from MCIC Vermont and the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (AHRQ), grant no. U18 H511902-01. The authors have no conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

Presented in part at the 28th Annual Meeting of the Society for General Internal Medicine, New Orleans, LA, May 11–14, 2005

References

  • 1.American College of Physicians Ethics Manual. Philadelphia 1992.
  • 2.American Medical Association Council on Ethical and Judicial Affairs. Code of medical ethics, annotated current opinions. 2004–2005 ed. Chicago: American Medical Association.
  • 3.Lo B. Resolving Ethical Dilemmas: A Guide for Clinicians, 2nd ed. Lippincott: Williams and Wilkins, Philadelphia; 2000.
  • 4.Witman AB, Park DM, Hardin SB. How do patients want physicians to handle mistakes? A survey of internal medicine patients in an academic setting. Arch Intern Med. 1996;156(22):2565–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 5.Wu AW, Cavanaugh TA, McPhee SJ, Lo B, Micco GP. To tell the truth: ethical and practical issues in disclosing medical mistakes to patients. J Gen Intern Med. 1997;12(12):770–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.AHA Management Advisory. Ethical conduct for health care institutions. Chicago: American Hospital Association; 1992.
  • 7.Leape LL. Full disclosure and apology – an idea whose time has come. Physician Exec. 2006;32(2):16–8. [PubMed]
  • 8.American Society for Healthcare Risk Management of the American Hospital Association. Disclosure of unanticipated events: the next step in better communication with patients. Chicago; 2003.
  • 9.Institute of Medicine (UA) Committee on Quality of Health Care in America. Crossing the quality chasm: a new health system for the 21st century. Washington: Naitonal Academy Press; 2001. [PubMed]
  • 10.Blendon RJ, DesRoches CM, Brodie M, Benson JM, Rosen AB, Schneider E, Altman DE, Zapert K, Herrmann MJ, Steffenson AE. Views of practicing physicians and the public on medical errors. N Engl J Med. 2002;347(24):1933–40. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 11.Joint Commission Standard RI. 1.2.2. Revisions to Joint Commission Standards in Support of Patient Safety and Medical/Health Care Error. July 1, 2001. http://www.dcha.org/JCAHORevision.htm Accessed 5 May 2009.
  • 12.Gallagher TH, Waterman AD, Ebers AG, Fraser VJ, Levinson W. Patients' and physicians' attitudes regarding the disclosure of medical errors. JAMA. 2003;289(8):1001–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 13.Mazor KM, Simon SR, Yood RA, Martinson BC, Gunter MJ, Reed GW, Gurwitz JH. Health plan members' views about disclosure of medical errors. Ann Intern Med. 2004;140(6):409–18. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 14.Wusthoff CJ. MSJAMA: medical mistakes and disclosure: the role of the medical student. JAMA. 2001;286(9):1080–1. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 15.Hobgood C, Weiner B, Tamayo-Sarver JH. Medical error identification, disclosure, and reporting: do emergency medicine provider groups differ? Acad Emerg Med. 2006;13(4):443–51. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 16.Finkelstein D, Wu AW, Holtzman NA, Smith MK. When a physician harms a patient by a medical error: ethical, legal, and risk-management considerations. J Clin Ethics. 1997;8(4):330–5. [PubMed]
  • 17.NHS National Patient Safety Agency. Safer practice notice: Being open when patients are harmed. 15 September 2005. http://www.npsa.nhs.uk/nrls/alerts-and-directives/notices/disclosure/ Accessed 5 May 2009.
  • 18.Gallagher TH, Studdert D, Levinson W. Disclosing harmful medical errors to patients. N Engl J Med. 2007;356(26):2713–9. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 19.xGallagher TH, Denham CR, Leape LL, Amori G, Levinson W. National Quality Forum safe practice. Commentary: Disclosing unanticipated outcomes to patients: the art and practice. J Patient Saf. 2007;3:158–65.
  • 20.Vincent CA, Pincus T, Scurr JH. Patients' experience of surgical accidents. Qual Health Care. 1993;2(2):77–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 21.Schwappach DL, Koeck CM. What makes an error unacceptable? A factorial survey on the disclosure of medical errors. Int J Qual Health Care. 2004;16(4):317–26. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 22.Duclos CW, Eichler M, Taylor L, Quintela J, Main DS, Pace W, Staton EW. Patient perspectives of patient-provider communication after adverse events. Int J Qual Health Care. 2005;17(6):479–86. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 23.Bismark M, Dauer E, Paterson R, Studdert D. Accountability sought by patients following adverse events from medical care: the New Zealand experience. CMAJ. 2006;175(8):889–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 24.Cleopas A, Villaveces A, Charvet A, Bovier PA, Kolly V, Perneger TV. Patient assessments of a hypothetical medical error: effects of health outcome, disclosure, and staff responsiveness. Qual Saf Health Care. 2006;15(2):136–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 25.Wu AW, Folkman S, McPhee SJ, Lo B. Do house officers learn from their mistakes? JAMA. 1991;265(16):2089–94. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 26.Schoen C, Osborn R, Huynh PT, Doty M, Zapert K, Peugh J, et al. Taking the pulse of health care systems: Experiences of patients with health problems in six countries. Health Aff (Millwood). 2005; W5: 509–525. Suppl Web Exclusives. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 27.Gallagher TH, Garbutt JM, Waterman AD, Flum DR, Larson EB, Waterman BM, Dunagan WC, Fraser VJ, Levinson W. Choosing your words carefully: how physicians would disclose harmful medical errors to patients. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166(15):1585–93. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 28.Loren DJ, Klein EJ, Garbutt J, Krauss MJ, Fraser V, Dunagan WC, Brownstein DR, Gallagher TH. Medical error disclosure among pediatricians: choosing carefully what we might say to parents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2008;162(10):922–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 29.Kaldjian LC, Jones EW, Wu BJ, Forman-Hoffman VL, Levi BH, Rosenthal GE. Disclosing medical errors to patients: Attitudes and practices of physicians and trainees. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(7):988–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 30.Itoh K, Andersen HB, Madsen MD, Østergaard D, Ikeno M. Patient views of adverse events: comparisons of self-reported healthcare staff attitudes with disclosure of accident information. Appl Ergon. 2006;37(4):513–23. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 31.Iedema R, Sorensen R, Manias E, Tuckett A, Piper D, Mallock N, Williams A, Jorm C. Patients' and family members' experiences of open disclosure following adverse events. Int J Qual Health Care. 2008;20(6):421–32. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 32.Mazor KM, Reed GW, Yood RA, Fischer MA, Baril J, Gurwitz JH. Disclosure of medical errors: what factors influence how patients respond? J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21(7):704–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Fisseni G, Pentzek M, Abholz HH. Responding to serious medical error in general practice—consequences for the GPs involved: analysis of 75 cases from Germany. Fam Pract. 2008;25(1):9–13. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 34.Kachalia A, Shojania KG, Hofer TP, Piotrowski M, Saint S. Does full disclosure of medical errors affect malpractice liability? The jury is still out. Jt Comm J Qual Saf. 2003;29(10):503–11. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 35.Kraman SS, Hamm G. Risk management: extreme honesty may be the best policy. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131(12):963–7. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 36.Vincent C, Young M, Phillips A. Why do people sue doctors? A study of patients and relatives taking legal action. Lancet. 1994;343(8913):1609–13. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 37.Anderson LA, Dedrick RF. Development of the Trust in Physician scale: a measure to assess interpersonal trust in patient-physician relationships. Psychol Rep. 1990;67(3 Pt 2):1091–100. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 38.Nunnaly JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory,. 3rd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994.
  • 39.Dayton E, Henriksen K. Teamwork and communication: communication failure: basic components, contributing factors, and the call for structure. Jt Comm J Qual and Patient Safety. 2007;33(1):34–47. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 40.Quirk M, Mazor K, Haley HL, Philbin M, Fischer M, Sullivan K, Hatem D. How patients perceive a doctor's caring attitude. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;72(3):359–66. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 41.Butcher L. Lawyers say ‘sorry’ may sink you in court. Physician Exec. 2006;32:20–24. [PubMed]
  • 42.Kaldjian LC, Jones EW, Rosenthal GE. Facilitating and impeding factors for physicians' error disclosure: a structured literature review. Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf. 2006;32(4):188–98. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 43.Studdert DM, Mello MM, Gawande AA, Brennan TA, Wang YC. Disclosure of medical injury to patients: an improbable risk management strategy. Health Aff (Millwood). 2007;26(1):215–26. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 44.Hickson GB, Clayton EW, Githens PB, Sloan FA. Factors that prompted families to file medical malpractice claims following perinatal injuries. JAMA. 1992;267(10):1359–63. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 45.Beckman HB, Markakis KM, Suchman AL, Frankel RM. The doctor-patient relationship and malpractice. Lessons from plaintiff depositions. Arch Intern Med. 1994;154(12):1365–70. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 46.Boothman RC. Apologies and a strong defense at the University of Michigan Health System. Physician Exec. 2006;32(2):7–10. [PubMed]
  • 47.Barter C, Renold E. The use of vignettes in qualitative research. Social Research Update. 1999;25:1–4.
  • 48.Hughes R, Huby M. The application of vignettes in social and nursing research. J Adv Nurs. 2002;37:382–86. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 49.Peabody JW, Luck J, Glassman P, Dresselhaus TR, Lee M. Comparison of vignettes, standardized patients, and chart abstraction: a prospective validation study of 3 methods for measuring quality. JAMA. 2000;283(13):1715–22. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 50.Siminoff LA, Fetting JH. Effects of outcome framing on treatment decisions in the real world: impact of framing on adjuvant breast cancer decisions. Med Dec Making. 1989;9:262–71. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 51.Tversky A, Kahneman D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981;211(4481):453–8. [DOI] [PubMed]

Articles from Journal of General Internal Medicine are provided here courtesy of Society of General Internal Medicine

RESOURCES