Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2009 Sep;24(9):1010–1019. doi: 10.1002/gps.2213

The moderating role of personality factors in the relationship between depression and neuropsychological functioning among older adults

Brian J Ayotte 1,*, Guy G Potter 2, Heather T Williams 2, David C Steffens 2, Hayden B Bosworth 1,2,3,4
PMCID: PMC2730210  NIHMSID: NIHMS137267  PMID: 19226526

SUMMARY

Objective

Depression is often associated with decreased cognitive performance among older adults. The current study focused on the association of neuropsychological functioning and personality traits in depressed and non-depressed older adults.

Methods

Data from 75 depressed and 103 non-depressed adults over the age of 60 were analyzed. All participants underwent standardized clinical assessment for depression prior to participation and completed the NEO-PI-R and a series of neuropsychological assessments.

Results

A series of multiple linear regressions were conducted to examine the relationships between personality and neuropsychological performance among depressed and non-depressed older adults. Results indicated that higher Openness to Experience was related to better performance on Parts A and B of the Trail Making Test among depressed older adults, and to better Digit Span Backward performance among all participants. Higher levels of neuroticism were related to poorer performance on Digit Span Backward, but only among depressed older adults. Depressed participants performed more poorly on the Symbol Digit Modalities Test and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.

Conclusions

Personality characteristics, particularly Openness to Experience, modified the relationship between depression and neuropsychological functioning among older adults. Results indicate that interventions aimed at increasing one’s Openness to Experience could potentially attenuate some of the neuropsychological impairments that are associated with depression.

Keywords: depression, personality, neurocognitive function

INTRODUCTION

Depression in older adults is a common, but not normal, part of aging (National Institute of Mental Health; NIMH, 2007). In fact, approximately 15–25% of older adults report depressive symptoms that could be considered clinically significant (Jeste et al., 1999). The high prevalence rate of depression among the elderly is related to a number of factors, including medical comorbidities and sociodemographic factors (NIMH, 2008). Depression is related to a number of non-optimal outcomes among older adults, including poor physical functioning (Penninx et al., 1999), decreased well-being (Ormel et al., 1998), increased consumption of medical care (Koopmans and Lamers, 2001), and cognitive impairment (Arve et al., 1999) The co-occurrence of depression and cognitive impairment produces a higher risk of adverse outcomes for physical health, functional status, and mortality relative to either condition alone (Mehta et al., 2003), which makes cognition an important focus of diagnosis and treatment in geriatric depression.

Although most older adults with depression do not experience severe cognitive impairment, depressed individuals consistently demonstrate worse neuropsychological test performances than non-depressed individuals. Experimental studies suggest that depressed individuals are most prominently affected in the domains of executive function (Boone et al., 1995; Beats et al., 1996), memory (Beats et al., 1996; Austin et al., 1999), and processing speed (Nebes et al., 2000), which is consistent with meta-analysis of the broader literature (Veiel, 1997; Zakzanis et al., 1998). Executive functions have received particular attention in geriatric depression because performance in this domain is associated with lower rates of treatment remission and higher rates of depression recurrence (Kalayam and Alexopoulos, 1999; Dunkin et al., 2000; Potter et al., 2004). As a construct, ‘executive functions’ refers to a number of cognitive processes that are largely mediated by the prefrontal cortex, including attentional control, working memory, performance monitoring, behavioral initiation, and behavioral inhibition (Lezak et al., 2004). Deficits in executive functions can contribute to a more generalized lack of cognitive flexibility associated with decreased problem-solving ability and perseverative thinking patterns.

Trait personality characteristics are also related to cognitive performance. Much of the literature exploring this relationship, however, is based on data from younger and middle-aged adults, and much of this has focused on neuroticism and extraversion (Booth et al., 2006). A meta-analysis examining the relationships between personality traits and cognitive abilities found that neuroticism was negatively related to fluid and crystallized abilities (Ackerman and Heggestad, 1997). Fluid abilities reflect underlying capacity for problem solving and novel reasoning abilities, including executive function-type processes, whereas crystallized abilities generally reflect the application of previously acquired knowledge (Cavanaugh and Blanchard-Fields, 2006). This same analysis suggested that extraversion was associated with increased cognitive performance in fluid and crystallized abilities.

Fewer studies have investigated the association between cognition and other personality traits included in the ‘Big Five’ model of personality among older adults (i.e. Five-Factor Model: Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness; McCrae and John, 1992). A meta-analysis by Ackerman and Heggastad (1997) suggested that Openness to Experience was positively related to fluid and crystallized abilities, with correlations exceeding 0.30. In addition, Ashton et al. (2000) found that Openness, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness were positively related to performance on a number of cognitive tests assessing crystallized and fluid abilities. Only Openness, however, remained a significant correlate of multiple cognitive tests when entered into regression models. Using data from the Seattle Longitudinal Study, Schaie et al. (2004) found that Openness was positively related to inductive reasoning, spatial orientation, perceptual speed, verbal memory, and verbal comprehension. Additionally, Agreeableness was negatively related to performance on inductive reasoning and spatial orientation tasks, Extraversion was positively related to inductive reasoning, perceptual speed, numeric facility, and verbal memory, and Conscientiousness was positively related to numeric facility. These studies, however, did not assess depression, and the data were from primarily high socioeconomic status community samples.

Although there is substantial evidence supporting associations among depression and personality traits, few studies have examined these factors simultaneously, and fewer still have investigated how personality might moderate the relationship between depression and cognitive performance in older adults. Booth et al. (2006) investigated the relative contributions of depression and personality to cognitive performance in a sample of community-dwelling older adults. Results indicated that personality traits accounted for between 2–7% of the variance in a number of cognitive tests after controlling for age and education, with Openness being the most consistent correlate across tests. Depression, on the other hand, was not significantly related to cognitive performance after controlling for age, education, and personality characteristics. It should be noted that depression was conceptualized as a state characteristic in this study and was not defined in a clinical sample. An additional limitation in this study was that the interactions between personality and depression were not analyzed, leaving open the possibility that personality characteristics moderate the relationship between depression and cognition.

In a study examining personality as a moderator between depression and cognitive performance, van den Heuvel et al. (1996) found that among women, Neuroticism moderated the relationship between scores on the Mini-Mental State Exam (Folstein et al., 1975) and depressive symptomatology, as well as the relationship between information processing speed (van den Heuvel et al., 1996) and depression. Among men, Neuroticism moderated the relationship between memory and depressive symptomatology. Unfortunately, other factors of the Five-Factor Model of personality were not included in this study. In addition, depression was operationalized as scores on the Centers for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (Radloff, 1977) and did not include a clinical diagnosis (van den Heuvel et al., 1996).

The present study sought to extend this previous research in three important ways. First, participants were older adults. Identifying factors associated with neuropsychological functioning is particularly important in this population given the tendency for decreased cognitive performance during late life. Second, participants included both non-depressed individuals and a clinically-defined group of older adults with Major Depressive Disorder. Including a clinically depressed group of older adults is important due to the relatively high rates of depression among older adults and the co-occurrence of cognitive deficits with late life depression (Butters et al., 2004). Finally, all of the ‘Big Five’ personality traits were included in the current analyses, along with the interaction terms between these traits and depression status. Including these interactions allows for a more detailed investigation of how multiple personality dimensions might moderate the relationship between depression and cognitive ability. Understanding these interactive relationships among older adults is in order to better identify those who are most likely to experience deficits in executive function as part of their depression syndrome. The current study focused on the association of executive functions in depression with personality traits. We specifically hypothesized that: (a) lower Openness to Experience would be associated with poorer executive functioning due to the cognitive inflexibility associated with low levels of this trail, and (b) that this relationship would be particularly strong in older adults with depression based on the additional cognitive inflexibility associated with both aging and depression (Raskin, 1986).

METHODS

Participants

Data were collected from a total of 178 individuals who were at least 60 years old. Of these, 75 met the DSM-IV criteria for major depression and were currently participants from the National Institute of Mental Health Clinical Research Center (MHCRC) for the Study of Depression in Later Life, located at Duke University. On average, these individuals had a score of 15.61 (SD = 9.72) on the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Montgomery and Asberg, 1979). The remaining 103 participants were a non-depressed comparison group recruited from a volunteer registry maintained by the Center for the Study of Aging and Human Development at Duke University Medical Center. All participants underwent a standardized clinical assessment for depression during a clinical evaluation prior to participation in the study, as discussed below. Enrolled participants completed a battery of neuropsychological measures, and the current study represents a subset of individuals who also a self-report inventory of personality characteristics.

The MHCRC operates in a guideline-based treatment milieu, using an algorithm established by the Duke Affective Disorders Program rather than a standardized treatment protocol (Steffens et al., 2002). Inpatients and outpatients of the Duke University Psychiatric Service presenting with clinically significant depressive symptoms or a previous diagnosis of mood disorder were screened with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies-Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). Eligibility was limited to patients with CES-D scores ≥16 or a diagnosis of major depression, single (incident/new case) or recurrent (prevalent cases), and was restricted to patients aged 60 years or older who could speak and write English. Please see Bosworth et al. (2002) for more details regarding the study exclusion criteria.

Procedure

At baseline, individuals who provided written informed consent were enrolled. A trained interviewer administered the Duke Depression Evaluation Schedule (DDES). The DDES, a composite diagnostic interview instrument based on patient self-reports, includes sections of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS; Robins et al., 1981) depression assessment as well as items related to cognitive status, physical health, stress, and social support. DSM-IV major depression diagnoses were established by MHCRC geriatric psychiatrists via clinical interviews and were confirmed by responses on the DIS. Patients also received a battery of standardized clinical assessments including the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale (MADRS; Montgomery and Asberg, 1979). Testing was administered according to standardized procedures under the supervision of a clinical neuropsychologist (GGP). Neuropsychological tests were also administered at baseline. In order to decrease participant burden and increase the likelihood of completion, participants completed the personality measure at a later date when the patient was not acutely depressed.

Independent measures

Demographics

Data were collected on a number of demographic variables, including age, sex, race, and years of education.

Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R)

Participants completed the 240-item NEO PI-R (Costa and McCrae, 1992). This scale measures five dimensions of personality: Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. Participants are asked to respond to statements on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree.’ Summed scores for each domain are then converted to T-scores with norm means of 50 and norm standard deviations of 10. This measure has been validated and used extensively with older adult samples (e.g. Costa and McCrae, 1992; Duberstein et al., 1999).

Dependent measures

Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWA)

The COWA (Benton et al., 1994) is a test of verbal fluency that asks participants to say as many words starting with a specified letter that they can in 60 sec. This procedure is performed with three different letters (C, F, and L). Scores indicate the total number of words the participant produced, excluding repeated words and non-eligible words as indicated in the standardized instructions.

Trail Making Test Parts A and B

The Trail Making Test (Reitan, 1992) consists of two parts. Part A requires participants to draw lines between consecutively numbered circles. This component of the test involves visual scanning, number recognition, numeric sequencing, and motor abilities. Part B of the Trail Making Test requires participants to connect a series of circles alternatively containing letters and numbers; thus it involves the additional demand of switching of cognitive sets. Scores reflect the time in seconds that the participant took to complete the task.

Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT)

The SDMT (Smith, 1982) is a test of attention skills and information-processing speed that requires participants to identify nine symbols that correspond to the numbers 1–9. The participants are asked to transcribe the numbers in place of the corresponding symbols as rapidly as possible. Scores reflect the number of correct responses.

Digit Span Backward (Wechsler, 1987)

In order to assess working memory, participants were asked to repeat in reverse order an increasing number of digits that were read aloud by the examiner.

Analyses

The analyses involved two steps. The first step included calculating the descriptive statistics, examining group differences among study variables, and the bivariate correlations among study variables. Differences between the group with depression and the non-depressed group were investigated with t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables. The second step of the analysis included a series of multiple regression analyses––one for each neuropsychological measure––that included age, education, sex, depression status (depressed vs non-depressed), the five personality factors, and the five interactions between depression status and the personality factors. Prior to analyses, all variables involved in the interactions were centered around the grand mean. Due to the number of tests, we set our significance level at p<0.01.

RESULTS

Preliminary analyses

The groups differed on a number of demographic variables (see Table 1). Specifically, t-tests indicated that non-depressed individuals were significantly older and had significantly more years of education than the individuals with depression. With regard to personality traits, non-depressed participants had lower Neuroticism scores, higher Extraversion scores, higher Openness scores, and higher Conscientiousness scores compared to the participants with depression. The groups did not differ in terms of Agreeableness scores. On average, the two groups differed on the neuropsychological measures, with non-depressed participants outperforming the depressed participants on all of the tests.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics

Variable Non-depressed (n = 103) Depressed (n = 75) Test of significance



N % N % X2(df = 1)
Demographics
  Female 72 69.9   49 65.3   0.42
  White 90 87.4   71 94.7   2.67
  Married 65 63.1   38 57.9   2.76

M (Range) SD M (Range) SD     t(df = 176)

  Age 70.36 (58–73) 5.82 67.83 (60–−82) 6.19 2.79*
  Years of education 15.28 (8–17) 1.97 14.07 (7–17) 2.63 3.53*
Personality
  Neuroticism 41.79 (20.46–71.00) 8.97 55.58 (29.68–91.89) 12.09 −.74*
  Extraversion 50.02 (30.27–72.12) 9.57 44.34 (25.14–68.86) 10.21 3.80*
  Openness 51.97 (32.56–71.51) 9.24 46.92 (22.87–76.16) 11.03 3.32*
  Agreeableness 55.23 (30.21–81.60) 9.12 54.28 (37.15–77.43) 9.26 0.68
  Conscientiousness 51.76 (19.27–79.94) 10.05 43.37 (16.90–68.71) 11.33 5.21*
Neuropsychological performance
  Trail making A 35.98 (15–78) 11.83 51.24 (18–220) 44.80 −3.31*
  Trail making B 87.01 (31–258) 40.78 129.90 (42–301) 77.12 −4.72*
  SDMT 43.96 (18–72) 9.53 37.71 (12–57) 11.21 3.98*
  Backwards digit span 7.71 (3–13) 2.53 6.76 (3–12) 2.26 2.35
  COWA 41.19 (23–76) 11.34 35.17 (12–63) 11.00 3.54*

Scores on the personality measures are reflected as T-scores; COWA=Controlled Oral Word Association Test; SDMT=Symbol Digit Modalities Test.

*

p<.01.

Table 2 presents the bivariate correlations among the demographic, personality, and neuropsychological variables. The pattern of relationships between personality dimensions and scores on neurocognitive measures differed for depressed and non-depressed participants. Among those with depression, increased Openness was related to better performance on all of the measures, while the other personality traits were not related to performance on these tests. Among non-depressed participants, increased Agreeableness was related to better performance on Part A of the Trail Making Test.

Table 2.

Intercorrelations between predictor variables and outcome variables

Predictor variables Neuropsychological tests

Trail making A Trail making B SDMT Digit span backward COWA
Non-depressed
  1. Age 0.28* 0.39** −0.42* −0.03 −0.11
  2. Sex −0.05 0.05 0.08 0.10 0.18
  3. Education −0.20 −0.18 0.33* 0.06 0.26*
  4. Race 0.27* 0.37* −0.48* −0.32* −0.27*
  5. Neuroticism −0.09 0.09 −0.06 −0.05 −0.06
  6. Extraversion 0.03 −0.02 −0.07 −0.14 −0.03
  7. Openness −0.05 −0.08 0.15 0.08 0.13
  8. Agreeableness −0.26* −0.05 −0.02 −0.19 −0.10
Depressed
  1. Age 0.25* 0.30* −0.38* −0.06 0.01
  2.Sex 0.17 0.10 −0.05 0.08 −0.04
  3. Education −0.44* −0.50* 0.50* 0.38* 0.52*
  4. Race 0.18 0.06 −0.13 −0.14 −0.15
  5. Neuroticism −0.04 0.18 −0.08 0.13 0.06
  6. Extraversion −0.10 −0.12 0.12 0.08 0.12
  7. Openness −0.34* −0.51* 0.45* 0.46* 0.47*
  8. Agreeableness −0.04 0.02 0.01 −0.01 −0.11

COWA = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test.

*

p<0.01.

Multiple regressions

To more fully investigate the relationships between personality and neuropsychological performance among depressed and non-depressed older adults, a series of multiple linear regressions were performed (Table 3). Independent variables in the models included age, sex, years of education, race (White vs African American), depression status (diagnosed vs non-diagnosed), Openness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism. In addition, the five interactions of depression status and personality dimensions were tested, with only the significant interactions remaining in the final model.

Table 3.

Multiple linear regressions examining personality, depression, and neurocognitive functioning

Variable TrailA TrailB SDMT Digit COWA





B SE β B SE β B SE β B SE β B SE β
Age 0.89 0.30 0.17* 2.90 0.64 0.28* −0.62 0.11 −0.36* −0.01 0.03 −0.01 −0.06 0.14 −0.04
Sex 3.15 5.00 0.05 9.70 8.50 0.07 0.60 1.42 0.03 −0.26 0.45 0.01 1.97 1.78 0.08
Education −3.79 1.04 −0.28* −6.07 1.95 −0.22* 1.45 0.32 0.32* 0.13 0.10 0.12 1.30 0.40 0.26*
Race 6.60 7.49 0.06 23.77 13.34 0.11 −7.17 2.25 −0.20* −2.17 0.69 −0.25* −4.90 2.83 −0.13
Depression Status (D) 14.70 5.19 0.23* 33.04 9.32 0.26* −4.78 1.57 −0.22* −0.86 0.46 −0.17 −4.18 1.16 −0.20*
Neuroticism (N) −0.10 0.27 −0.04 1.03 0.47 0.27 −0.17 0.08 −0.16 −0.02 0.03 −0.11 −0.08 0.10 −0.09
Extraversion (E) −0.20 0.26 −0.06 0.56 0.46 −0.10 0.08 0.08 −0.10 −0.03 0.02 −0.12 −0.02 0.10 −0.02
Openness (O) −0.19 0.24 −0.06 −1.00 0.29 −0.19* 0.11 0.07 0.10 0.07 0.02 0.20* 0.17 0.09 0.16
Agreeableness (A) −0.36 0.23 −0.10 0.05 0.42 0.01 −0.04 0.07 −0.04 −0.03 0.02 −0.11 −0.12 0.09 −0.10
Conscientiousness (C) 0.31 0.23 0.11 0.38 0.41 0.07 −0.03 0.07 −0.03 0.01 0.02 0.07 −0.13 0.09 −0.13
D × N −0.10 0.03 −0.22*
D × O −1.04 0.33 −0.18* −2.72 0.70 −0.26*
R2 0.29 0.45 0.45 0.22 0.17
Model Fit F (11, 166) = 6.06,
p < 0.001
F (11, 158) = 11.66,
p < 0.001
F (10, 165) = 13.27,
p < 0.001
F (12, 135) = 3.12,
p < 0.001
F (10, 167) = 3.43,
p < 0.001

COWA = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test.

Only significant interactions were included in the final models.

*

p<0.01.

With regard to part A of the Trail Making Test, both lower age and more years of education were related to better performance. In addition, depression was related to worse performance; however, this relationship was qualified by an interaction between depression between and Openness. Follow-up analyses of the simple slopes indicated that increased Openness was related to better performance on Part A of the Trail Making Test among participants with depression, but that this relationship was not significant among non-depressed participants.

As with Part A, both lower age and more years of education were related to better performance on Part B of the Trail Making Test. In addition, depression status and Openness were related to scores on this test, and these relationships were qualified by an interaction between depression status and Openness. As with Part A, follow-up tests indicated that increased Openness was related to better performance among depressed participants but not among non-depressed participants. To test whether the relationships between predictor variables and performance on Part B of the Trail Making Test could be explained by abilities common to Part A of the Trail Making Test (e.g. motor performance), we conducted follow-up tests where performance on Part A was entered into the model as a covariate. Although performance on Part A was significantly related to performance on Part B, the pattern of results concerning the other predictor variables was identical to the reported model. Thus, it can be assumed that the similar relationships among personality, depression, and performance on Parts A and B of the Trail Making Test were not merely due to shared variance between the two tests.

There were a number of significant variables associated with the SDMT. Specifically, increased age, being African American, and being diagnosed with depression were significantly related to poorer performance. More years of education, on the other hand, was related to better SDMT performance. None of the personality dimensions were significantly related to performance on this particular measure.

Performance on Digit Span Backward was worse among African Americans relative to Whites. In addition, increased Openness was related to better performance regardless of depression status. There were two significant interactions between depression status and personality on this measure. First, Neuroticism modified the relationship between depression and performance on the Digit Span Backward test in that increased Neuroticism was related to poorer performance among depressed participants but not among non-depressed participants. Second, Agreeableness modified the relationship between performance on the Digit Span Backward test and depression. Follow-up tests indicated that increased Agreeableness was related to poorer performance, but only among non-depressed participants.

Finally, only education and depression status were related to scores on the COWA, with more years of education and non-depressed status associated with higher scores on this test.

DISCUSSION

In terms of mean differences, older adults with depression performed significantly worse than non-depressed older adults on all of the neuropsychological assessments. Depressed individuals were also significantly lower in Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness, and Extraversion, but were higher in Neuroticism. The neuropsychological results are consistent with the broad body of literature that supports an association between depression and neurocognitive deficits (e.g. Lichtenberg et al., 1995; Porter et al., 2003; Butters et al., 2004), and executive functions in particular (Boone et al., 1995, Beats et al., 1996; Butters et al., 2004). Our neuropsychological findings are consistent with neurobiological research indicating that mood and executive functions are influenced by common brain regions (Phillips et al., 2003; Ochsner and Gross, 2005), which is exacerbated by cognitive and neurobiological changes in aging (Parkin and Lawrence, 1994; Gunning-Dixon and Raz, 2000; Resnick et al., 2007). In addition, there is increased brain pathology among depressed older patients compared to non-depressed older adults (e.g. Morris and Rapoport, 1990; Kramer-Ginsberg et al., 1999), which might additionally contribute to executive function deficits among some depressed patients. The current results suggest, however, that characteristic personality traits among depressed individuals may also contribute to neuropsychological functioning.

The multiple regression analyses revealed a number of relationships among personality, depression, and neuropsychological functioning. As hypothesized, Openness was related to performance on a number of neuropsychological assessments. Across all individuals, increased Openness was related to better performance on the Digit Span Backward test, even after considering all of the other variables in the model. Elements associated with Openness (e.g. intellectual curiosity) might promote engaging in stimulating activities such as education pursuits and life-long learning throughout the life span. In turn, some research suggests that engaging in cognitively stimulating activities could result in improvement or at least maintenance of cognitive abilities such as working memory (Wilson et al., 2002). Higher levels of Openness were also associated with better performance on both parts of the Trail Making Test, but only among depressed individuals. Raskin (1986) suggested that the interaction of age and depression results in decreased cognitive flexibility, which is captured by the Openness scale on the NEO-PI-R, along with related characteristics reflecting imagination and intellectual curiosity. Thus, increased cognitive flexibility and related characteristics associated with trait Openness could potentially attenuate the negative effect of depression on executive function tasks that involve an element of flexibility. There would also be an expectation that Openness would be associated with performance on COWA and the SDMT, but it was not. We do not have an empirical explanation for this lack of association. On the whole, studies of how personality characteristics may promote cognitive reserve in depression represent a promising direction for future research, but additional research is needed to characterize the mechanisms by which specific personality traits are related to specific neurocognitive measures.

Higher Neuroticism was related to poorer performance on the Digit Span Backwards test among depressed individuals but not among non-depressed individuals. This is consistent with previous research finding that trait Neuroticism moderated the relationship between cognitive performance and depression (van den Heuvel et al., 1996). This finding is particularly important because it suggests older adults who have depression and score highly on Neuroticism might be at increased risk for cognitive deficit, at least in terms of working memory ability.

Personality factors and depression status were not significantly associated with scores on the SDMT. Finally, only depression status was related to scores on the COWA, with depressed individuals performing worse than non-depressed individuals.

Clinical implications

The current results highlight the importance of assessing both neuropsychological performance and personality in the context of late-life depression. Deficits in executive functions predict attenuated treatment response and higher rates of depression recurrence (Kalayam et al., 1999; Dunkin et al., 2000; Potter et al., 2004), and low Openness to Experience may exacerbate these effects. Interventions aimed at increasing Openness to Experience could potentially produce effective strategies to compensate for the cognitive inflexibility associated with depression, as well as increase the chances that problem-solving therapies are effective in the treatment of depression (Alexopoulos et al., 2003).

Limitations

Although this study contributes to literature examining the relationships among depression, personality, and cognitive performance, the results should be interpreted within the context of the following limitations. First, the data are correlational, thereby precluding any causal interpretations of the data (see Campbell and Stanley, 1966). Further longitudinal data are needed to in order to establish the temporal pattern of the indicated relationships. Second, the sample consisted primarily of White participants, thus reducing the generalizability of the results. Future research should investigate these relationships in more diverse samples given the differing patterns of depression and cognitive change in older adult minorities (Albert et al., 1995; Brown et al., 1996). Third, the current study follows a naturalistic treatment protocol and did not control for differences in use of anti-depressant medication among the depressed group; however, our findings of group differences in executive functions are consistent with numerous other studies, including medication-free studies (Boone, 1995) and clinical community-based studies with multiple psychotropic medications used among the sample (Beats et al., 1996; Airaksinen et al., 2004; Bierman et al., 2005)

CONCLUSION AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS

This study examined the associations of depression and the ‘Big Five’ personality traits with neuropsychological performance among older adults. Consistent with most research, participants with depression scored significantly worse on the neuropsychological measures compared to non-depressed individuals. However, personality traits moderated many of these relationships, with a particularly strong relationship between executive functions and Openness to Experience. Further understanding of the complex relationships among neuropsychological performance, personality, and mood might be enhanced with correlations to neuroimaging as well as genetics.

KEY POINTS

  • Personality traits moderated the relationship between depression and neurocognitive functioning.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grants P50 (MH60451), R01 (MH54846), and K24 (MH70027). BJA is supported by a post-doctoral fellowship from the VA Office of Academic Affairs. HBB is supported by an American Heart Association Established Investigator award. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Department of Veterans Affairs.

Footnotes

CONFLICT OF INTEREST

None.

REFERENCES

  1. Ackerman PL, Heggestad ED. Intelligence, personality, and interests: evidence for overlapping traits. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:219–245. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Airaksinen E, Larsson M, Lundgerg I, Forsell Y. Cognitive functions in depressive disorders: evidence from a population-based study. Psychol Med. 2004;34:83–91. doi: 10.1017/s0033291703008559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Albert MS, Jones K, Savage CR, et al. Predictors of cognitive change in older persons: MacAurthur studies of successful aging. Psychol Aging. 1995;10:578–589. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.4.578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alexopoulos GS, Raue P, Arean P. Problem-solving therapy versus supportive therapy in geriatric major depression with executive dysfunction. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;11:46–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arve S, Tilvis RS, Lehtonen A, et al. Coexistence of lowered mood and cognitive impairment of elderly people in five birth cohorts. Aging. 1999;11:90–95. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ashton MC, Lee K, Vernon PA. Fluid intelligence, crystallized intelligence, and the openness/intellect factor. J Res Personality. 2000;34:198–207. [Google Scholar]
  7. Austin MP, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, et al. Cognitive function in depression: a distinct pattern of frontal impairment in melancholia? Psychol Med. 1999;29:73–85. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beats BC, Sahakian BJ, Levy R. Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol Med. 1996;26:591–603. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700035662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Benton AL, Hamsher KS, Sivan AB. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, IA: AJA Associates; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bierman EJM, Comijs HC, Jonker C, Beekman ATF. Effects of anxiety versus depression on cognition in later life. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2005;13:686–693. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajgp.13.8.686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Boone K, Lesser B, Miller B, et al. Cognitive functioning in a geriatric depressed population: Relationship of presence and severity of depression to neuropsychological scores. Neuropsychol. 1995;9:390–398. [Google Scholar]
  12. Booth J, Schinka J, Brown L, et al. Five-factor personality dimensions, mood states, and cognitive performance in older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28:676–683. doi: 10.1080/13803390590954209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bosworth HB, McQuoid DR, George LK, Steffens DC. Time-to-remission from geriatric depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;10:551–559. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Brown C, Schulberg HC, Madonia MJ. Clinical presentations of major depression by African Americans and whites in primary medical care practice. J Affect Disord. 1996;41:181–191. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(96)00085-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Butters MA, Whyte EM, Nebes RD, et al. The nature and determinants of neuropsychological functioning in late-life depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:587–595. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cavanaugh JC, Blanchard-Fields F. Adult Development and Aging. 5th edn. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing/Thomson Learning; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  17. Campbell DT, Stanley JC. Experimental and Quasi-Experimental Designs for Research. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally and Co; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  18. Costa PT, McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five Factor Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  19. Duberstein PR, Conwell Y, Seidlitz L, et al. Age and suicidal ideation in older depressed inpatients. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1999;7:289–296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dunkin JJ, Leuchter AF, Cook IA, et al. Executive dysfunction predicts nonresponse to fluoxetine in major depression. J Affective Disord. 2000;60:13–23. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(99)00157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Folstein M, Folstein S, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-Mental State’: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatric Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gunning-Dixon FM, Raz N. The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychol. 2000;14:224–232. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jeste DV, Alexopoulos GS, Bartels SJ, et al. Consensus statement on the upcoming crisis in geriatric mental health: Research agenda for the next 2 decades. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:848–853. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.848. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kalayam B, Alexopoulos GS. Prefrontal dysfunction and treatment response in geriatric depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:713–718. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.8.713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Koopmans GT, Lamers LM. Comparing patients with depressive complaints and patients with chronic medical conditions on their functioning and medical consumption. J Ment Health Policy and Econ. 2001;4:91–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kramer-Ginsberg E, Greenwald BS, Krishnan RR, et al. Neuropsychological functioning and MRI signal hyperintensities in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156:438–444. doi: 10.1176/ajp.156.3.438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  28. Lichtenberg PA, Ross T, Millis SR, Manning CA. The relationship between depression and cognition in older adults: A cross-validation study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995;50:25–32. doi: 10.1093/geronb/50b.1.p25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications. J Personality. 1992;60:175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Montgomery SA, Asberg M. A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry. 1979;134:382–389. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.4.382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Mehta KM, Yaffe K, Langa KM, et al. Additive effects of cognitive function and depressive symptoms on mortality in elderly community-living adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2003;58:M461–M467. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.5.m461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Morris P, Rapoport SI. Neuroimaging and affective disorders in late life: A review. Can J Psychiatry. 1990;35:347–354. doi: 10.1177/070674379003500415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. National Institute of Mental Health. Older adults: Depression and suicide facts. 2007 Retrieved April 2, 2008, from National Institutes of Mental Health, http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/older-adults-and-mental-health/index.shtml.
  34. National Institute of Mental Health. Older adults and mental health. 2008 Retrieved April 2, 2008, from National Institutes of Mental Health, http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/older-adults-and-mental-health/index.shtml.
  35. Nebes RD, Butters MA, Mulsant BH, et al. Decreased working memory and processing speed mediate cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Psychol Med. 2000;30:679–691. doi: 10.1017/s0033291799001968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cog Science. 2005;9:242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Ormel J, Kempmen GI, Deeg DJ, et al. Functioning, well-being, and health perception in late middle-aged and older people: comparing the effects of depressive symptoms and chronic medical conditions. J Am Geriatc Soc. 1998;46:39–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1998.tb01011.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Parkin AJ, Lawrence A. A dissociation in the relation between memory tasks and frontal lobe tests in the normal elderly. Neuropsychologia. 1994;32:1523–1532. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90124-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Penninx BW, Leveille S, Ferrucci L, et al. Exploring the effect of depression on physical disability: Longitudinal evidence from the established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:1346–1352. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.9.1346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Bioll Psychiatry. 2003;54:504–514. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00168-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Potter GG, Kittinger JD, Wagner HR, et al. Prefrontal neuropsychological predictors of treatment remission in late-life depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2266–2271. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Porter RJ, Gallagher P, Thompson JM, Young AH. Neuro-cognitive impairment in drug-free patients with major depressive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2003;182:214–220. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.3.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Radloff LS. The CES-D Scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  44. Raskin A. Partialing out the effects of depression and age on cognitive functions: Experimental data and methodological issues. In: Poon LW, editor. Handbook for Clinical Memory Assessment of Older Adults. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1986. pp. 244–256. [Google Scholar]
  45. Reitan RM. Trail-Making Test: Manual for Administration and Scoring. South Tucson, AZ: Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  46. Resnick SM, Lamar M, Driscoll I. Vulnerability of the orbitofrontal cortex to age-associated structural and functional brain changes. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:562–575. doi: 10.1196/annals.1401.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KS. National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen psychiatry. 1981;38:381–389. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780290015001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schaie KW, Willis SL, Caskie GIL. The Seattle Longitudinal Study: Relationship between personality and cognition. Aging Neuropsychol Cog. 2004;11:304–324. doi: 10.1080/13825580490511134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith A. Symbol Digits Modalities Test (SDMT) Manual (Revised) Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  50. Steffens DC, McQuoid DR, Krishnan KR. The Duke Somatic Treatment Algorithm for Geriatric Depression (STAGED) approach. Psychopharmacol Bull. 2002;36:58–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. van den Heuvel N, Smits CHM, Deeg DGH, Beekman ATF. Personality: A moderator of the relation between cognitive functioning and depression in adults aged 55–85? Affective Disord. 1996;41:229–240. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(96)00088-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Veiel HOF. A preliminary profile of neuropsychological deficits associated with major depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1997;19:587–603. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Barnes LL, et al. Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer Disease. JAMA. 2002;287:742–748. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.6.742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  55. Zakzanis KK, Leach L, Kaplan E. On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1998;3:111–119. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES