Abstract
In the primate retina the small bistratified, “blue-yellow” color-opponent ganglion cell receives parallel ON-depolarizing and OFF-hyperpolarizing inputs from short (S)-wavelength sensitive and combined long (L)- and middle (M)-wavelength sensitive cone photoreceptors, respectively. However, the synaptic pathways that create S versus LM cone-opponent receptive field structure remain controversial. Here, we show in the macaque monkey retina in vitro that at photopic light levels, when an identified rod input is excluded, the small bistratified cell displays a spatially coextensive receptive field in which the S-ON-input is in spatial, temporal, and chromatic balance with the LM-OFF-input. ON pathway block with l-AP-4, the mGluR6 receptor agonist, abolished the S-ON response but spared the LM-OFF response. The isolated LM component showed a center-surround receptive field structure consistent with an input from OFF-center, ON-surround “diffuse” cone bipolar cells. Increasing retinal buffering capacity with HEPES attenuated the LM-ON surround component, consistent with a non-GABAergic outer retina feedback mechanism for the bipolar surround. The GABAa/c receptor antagonist picrotoxin and the glycine receptor antagonist strychnine did not affect chromatic balance or the basic coextensive receptive field structure, suggesting that the LM-OFF field is not generated by an inner retinal inhibitory pathway. We conclude that the opponent S-ON and LM-OFF responses originate from the excitatory receptive field centers of S-ON and LM-OFF cone bipolar cells, and that the LM-OFF- and ON-surrounds of these parallel bipolar inputs largely cancel, explaining the small, spatially coextensive but spectrally antagonistic receptive field structure of the blue-ON ganglion cell.
Introduction
An early step in the neural coding for human color vision occurs when signals arising from long (L)-, middle (M)-, and short (S)-wavelength sensitive cone photoreceptors interact antagonistically to create spectral opponency in retinal ganglion cells. Two classes of spectral opponent neurons have long been recognized: an L versus M cone or “red-green” pathway and an S versus combined L + M cone (LM) or blue-yellow pathway (Solomon and Lennie, 2007). However, the synaptic mechanisms that create cone opponency remain controversial. Wiesel and Hubel originally observed a receptive field in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) that lacked center-surround organization but instead showed nearly spatially matched ON and OFF fields that differed in spectral tuning (Wiesel and Hubel, 1966) (see Fig. 1A). This “coextensive” receptive field structure suggested a novel circuitry that sacrificed spatial opponency to transmit an unambiguous chromatic signal (De Monasterio and Gouras, 1975; de Monasterio, 1978b; Derrington et al., 1984; Reid and Shapley, 1992; Reid and Shapley, 2002; Solomon et al., 2005). Such a circuitry was suggested when the LGN-projecting, blue-yellow pathway or blue-ON cell was shown to originate from a bistratified ganglion cell that extended dendrites in both the inner-ON and outer-OFF subdivisions of the inner plexiform layer (Rodieck, 1991; Dacey and Lee, 1994) (see Fig. 1B). Thus, coextensive, cone-opponent fields were hypothesized to arise by parallel excitatory S-ON and LM-OFF cone bipolar inputs to the small bistratified dendritic tree (Dacey, 1993, 1996; Dacey and Lee, 1994) (see Fig. 1C). Identification of synaptic inputs to the bistratified tree confirmed presumed S-ON bipolar and LM-OFF bipolar contacts with the inner and outer dendritic tiers, respectively (Calkins et al., 1998; Ghosh and Grünert, 1999). However, l-AP-4, the mGluR6 receptor agonist and selective ON pathway blocker, unexpectedly appeared to attenuate both the S-ON and LM-OFF responses (Dacey, 2000; Davenport, 2007; Field et al., 2007). These observations, together with recent anatomical (Schein et al., 2004) and physiological (Packer et al., 2007) evidence for LM surrounds in S cone photoreceptors and reports of center-surround receptive field structure in blue-ON cells (Field et al., 2007), suggested that S versus LM opponency arises not primarily postsynaptically by convergent ON and OFF bipolar inputs but presynaptically by horizontal cell to S cone feedback. In such a center-surround model, the small bistratified cells might show both chromatic and spatially tuned achromatic response components (Gouras and Zrenner, 1979; Derrington and Lennie, 1984; Lennie and Movshon, 2005).
Recording from blue-ON cells in the macaque retina in vitro, we now show new evidence that the S-ON and LM-OFF fields are spatially well matched and that the LM-OFF response is spared after block of both the ON-pathway and/or GABAergic and glycinergic transmission, suggesting that neither ON-bipolar nor inhibitory pathways transmit the LM-OFF signal. ON pathway block unmasked an OFF-center/ON-surround structure for the LM field, consistent with input to the small bistratified cell from an OFF cone bipolar. We hypothesize that this LM-ON surround normally sums with an LM-OFF surround of the S-ON cone bipolar. The spatially coextensive blue-yellow receptive field can then be explained by summation of parallel excitatory inputs from S-ON center/LM-OFF surround “blue-cone” bipolar cells with LM-OFF center/LM-ON surround diffuse cone bipolar cells.
Materials and Methods
Choroid-attached in vitro preparation
Eyes from macaque monkeys (either M. nemestrina, M. fascicularis, or M. mulatta) were acquired through the tissue distribution program at the National Primate Research Center at the University of Washington. Eyes were removed at the time of kill under deep barbiturate anesthesia. After enucleation the anterior pole of the eye was removed, the vitreous drained, and the remaining eyecup placed in oxygenated Ames medium. The retina, choroid, and pigment epithelium were then dissected away from the sclera with care not to cause retinal detachment from the supporting layers. Radial cuts were made in the retina to create a flatmount that was adhered, ganglion cell layer up, to the glass bottom of a steel superfusion chamber that had been coated with poly-l-lysine (Sigma P1399, 10 mg in 10 ml of H2O). The chamber was mounted on a steel heating element and four perfusion inlet tubes, a single outlet tube, an agar bridge, and a thermistor were inserted into the bath. The entire chamber assembly was mounted on the fixed stage of an upright microscope equipped with water-immersion optics (Nikon Eclipse E600FN). Visual stimuli were delivered to the photoreceptors from the vitreal or ganglion cell side of the retina as in situ, via the microscope objective lens as described further below. Superfusion rate was maintained at 18–20 ml/min by a peristaltic pump that recirculated a 3 L reservoir of Ames medium (pH 7.37; constant oxygenation with 95% O2/5% CO2); temperature was thermostatically maintained (Warner Instruments TC-344B) at 35–36°C. Receptor agonists and antagonists (as described further below) were maintained in Ames medium in separate oxygenated reservoirs and could be introduced to the bath quickly by switching a solenoid valve at a common input to the chamber. In a limited number of experiments, retinal buffering was increased by adding 20 mm HEPES to the Ames medium, pH. 7.4; this increased the osmolarity of the solution from ∼280 to 300 mOsm; in control experiments we substituted 20 mm sucrose for HEPES and found no effects of the small increase in osmolarity on the morphology or physiology of ganglion cells (Davenport et al., 2008).
Electrophysiology
Intracellular recordings were made from single ganglion cells using high impedance glass micropipettes (∼400 MΩ; filled with 1 m K acetate; 1% neurobiotin and 0.5% pyranine). To target ganglion cells for intracellular penetration, the in vitro retina was stained with the dye acridine orange (several drops added to the superfusate in the chamber) which, as a fluorescent, basophilic dye stained the ganglion cell Nissl substance and under epifluorescent illumination permitted direct observation of ganglion cell bodies in vitro using a 40× water-immersion objective. Small bistratified cells were targeted by their distinct cell body size and shape relative to the more common midget and parasol ganglion cells. The identity of recorded cells was confirmed from their dendritic morphology after iontophoresis of pyranine (Fig. 1). Extracellular recordings were made with low impedance patch pipettes using the “loose-patch” method. The retinal surface was observed with a 60× water-immersion objective under infrared illumination on a video display using differential contrast optics. Patch pipettes were used to make a small hole in the inner limiting membrane at the retinal surface so as to expose a targeted ganglion cell body for intimate contact with the patch electrode. Small bistratified ganglion cells could be reliably identified based on fine details of soma morphology and size. Extracellular spikes were recorded in current or voltage clamp (0 mV or 0 pA holding potential). Data acquisition and stimulus presentation were coordinated by custom software running on an Apple Macintosh computer. Intracellular and extracellular recordings were amplified (Axon Instruments Axoprobe 2B; Axopatch 200B) and digitized (Instrutech ITC1600) at a sampling rate of 10 kHz. Response amplitude was measured as the first or second Fourier component of the averaged membrane potential or spike rate at the temporal frequency of stimulus modulation.
We include data from blue-ON cells that showed maintained activity in the range of 20 spikes/s (mean ± SD = 22 ± 13; n = 19). To quantitatively measure the maintained activity responses we recorded the activity of 19 cells to a mean background (S:L:M = 1:1:1) for ∼5 s. Otherwise maintained activity was judged qualitatively by ear when obtaining and throughout a recording. We found a good correlation between maintained discharge and the overall sensitivity of a cell to our subsequent stimulus sets. In some cells in which maintained activity was low or absent we found reduced sensitivity to low contrast stimuli. Such reduced sensitivity tended to correlate with retinal detachment from the choroid-retinal pigment epithelium (RPE) and we did not further collect or include data from these cells.
Stimulus generation
Projection and control of stimulus primaries.
Spatial stimuli at lower temporal frequencies were generated by a stimulus generator (VSG5, Cambridge Research Systems) input to a digital light projector (DLP) (VistaGRAPHX 2500, Christie Digital). Full-field stimuli at higher temporal frequencies were generated by a light emitting diode (LED) based stimulator (Pokorny et al., 2004). Stimuli were focused on the retina through a 4× objective for intracellular recording and a 10× objective for loose patch recordings.
The LM and S cone inputs to ganglion cells were measured by modulating the light output of the stimulator primaries. The irradiances of the primary spectra were measured with a PR705 spectroradiometer (Photo Research). The peak wavelengths and integrated photon fluxes of the red, cyan, and blue LED primary spectra (Fig. 2A) were 660, 514, and 457 nm and 7.0 × 107, 1.6 × 107, and 2.5 × 107 photons/s/μm2, respectively. The peak wavelengths and integrated photon fluxes of the red, green, and blue primaries of the DLP stimulator (Fig. 2B) were 636, 550, and 465 nm and 2.7 × 106, 6.9 × 105, and 1.8 × 105 photons/s/μm2, respectively.
To quantitatively predict the effectiveness of the light delivered by each primary to the cone aperture, we calculated the products of each primary irradiance spectrum and each cone spectral sensitivity function. The sensitivity functions (Baylor et al., 1987) were corrected for the spectrally broadening effects of self-screening by assuming an outer segment length of 5 μm and a pigment density of 0.016/μm. Each product was then summed across wavelength. The resulting nine numbers have units of effective photons/s/μm2 and represent irradiance corrected by cone spectral sensitivity. Effective photons/s/μm2 was then converted to photoisomerizations/s/cone by multiplying by the area of the cone aperture. In previous studies involving transverse illumination of the cone outer segment where funneling of the inner segments plays no role, the conversion factor commonly used is 0.37 μm2 (Baylor et al., 1979). The efficiency of photoisomerization (0.67) (Dartnall, 1972) is included in this value. In our retinal preparation (see above), as in vivo, light is incident on the vitreal surface of the retina. Photoreceptor quantal catch thus depends on the axial illumination of the cone, as well as the funneling of the cone inner segment. Funneling by the inner segment has been estimated to increase the effective area of the peripheral inner segment aperture by at least a factor of 3 (Packer et al., 1996). Thus, for our whole-mount in vitro preparation, we consider the use of 0.37 μm2 as a very conservative estimate of cone aperture to make the conversion to photoisomerizations/s/cone.
Often, the intensity of stimuli used in human visual psychophysics or in physiological experiments in the intact primate eye are expressed in units of retinal illuminance, or Trolands (Td). To aid comparison with our data, we calculated that for a peripheral cone with an inner segment aperture of 9 μm, 1 Troland (Td) was equivalent to ∼30 photoisomerizations/s/cone. For a rod with an inner segment aperture of 2.5 μm, 1 scotopic troland was equivalent to ∼4 photoisomerizations/s/rod.
Construction of cone-type selective stimuli.
Cone-isolating stimuli (Fig. 2E,F) were obtained by the method of silent substitution (Estévez and Spekreijse, 1974, 1982) and were based on the cone absorption spectra and the stimulator primary spectra detailed above. The intensities of three primary lights were modulated and their relative irradiances adjusted (Fig. 2C,D) so that quantal catch in a subset of the cone types was modulated in isolation. To modulate the L and M cones equally but not the S cones (Fig. 2C, right; D, right), the red and green primaries were modulated with means and amplitudes chosen to create equivalent modulation depths (cone contrast) in both cone types. A small counter modulation of the blue primary was used to offset the effects of the red and green primaries on the S cone. The primary means and amplitudes used to selectively stimulate S cone inputs followed the opposite pattern (Fig. 2C, left; D, left). Modulation of the short wavelength primary effectively modulated S cones, whereas counter modulations of the long and middle wavelength primaries offset the effect of the blue primary on the L and M cones. The primary LED and DLP stimuli shown in Figure 2, E and F, were calculated to give cone contrasts (amplitude/mean) of 48% and 16%, respectively, around a matched mean quantal catch for all three cone types. (LM)−S and LMS cone-isolating stimuli were generated using the same principles. All stimuli were presented as a 2000 μm diameter spot whose intensity was sinusoidally modulated at a temporal frequency of 2 Hz. For the intracellular recordings, cone-isolating stimuli were generated as outlined above but a much higher cone contrast (∼80%) was achieved by modulating around a mean quantal catch that differed for each cone type (L:M:S = ∼9:7:1) (see Figs. 2G, 7A,B).
Estimating cone synaptic weights.
To determine the relative strengths of the S and LM cone inputs to a blue-ON cell's receptive field, we systematically varied the sign and depth of S and LM cone modulation while holding mean quantal catch equal for all three cone types (see Fig. 4A). Underlying this stimulus design is the assumption that, at low to moderate contrasts under conditions of equivalent adaptation states across cone types, inputs from different cone types contribute linearly to the response of the cell. If this assumption is true, it should be possible to fit the response with a simple linear model relating calculated cone contrast to response amplitude. This model has been described in detail previously and used to measure the relative weights of synaptic input to the H1 horizontal cells and the midget and parasol ganglion cells (Dacey et al., 2000a; Diller et al., 2004).
The amplitude of the sinusoidal modulation of each primary was chosen to create a set of 13 stimuli whose S and LM contrasts are varied systematically (see Fig. 4A). Initially, S and LM cone contrast are equal and of the same sign. S cone contrast is then reduced to zero in a stepwise manner while LM cone contrast is increased to a peak (LM cone isolation). S cone contrast then is reversed in sign (equivalent to a 180 degree phase shift) and is increased until LM and S contrast are equal and in antiphase (LM−S). LM contrast is then reduced to zero while S cone contrast peaks (S cone isolation). Finally, the two cone contrasts are returned to the same sign and in the last stimulus are again of equal contrast. For a cell that received equally weighted LM OFF and S ON inputs (see Fig. 4B), a response null would occur when LM and S cones are stimulated equally and in the same phase (see Fig. 4B, first and last points) and a peak response when LM and S cone contrasts are equal and opposite in phase (see Fig. 4B, point 7). If the S input dominated, the peak response would occur at point >7 or vice versa. The relative strengths were calculated by fitting the responses with a weighted sum of S and LM cone inputs to determine the peak: response amplitude = WSCS + W(LM)C(LM), where WS and W(LM) are the weights of the cone inputs, and CS and C(LM) are the contrasts of the stimulus.
Finally, this model predicts that the phase of an opponent response would be constant, whereas the phases of LM and S nonopponent responses would show a 180 degree phase shift at the stimulus whose contrast ratio produced a response minimum.
Spatial stimuli and receptive field model.
The S and LM components of the blue-ON receptive field were mapped using cone-isolating drifting-sinusoidal gratings of increasing spatial frequency, spots of increasing diameter, or annuli of increasing inner diameter. To center the stimulus on the cell's receptive field, the cell body was first placed in the middle of the stimulus field. Horizontal and vertical fine diameter slits (10 or 25 μm width) were then systemically moved to further locate the most sensitive point in the receptive field. The location of the peak response was defined to be the receptive field center; stimuli were positioned relative to this point. In cases where intracellular dye filling was coupled with receptive field mapping, this point typically corresponded to the approximate center of a circle encompassing both the inner and outer dendritic tiers of the bistratified tree. Stimuli were modulated at a temporal frequency of 2 or 4 Hz. Contrast (LMAX − LMIN)/(LMAX + LMIN) of gratings, spots and annuli was set to 28% or 36% and was modulated around a mean level that equated the quantal catch for all three cone types. The first Fourier (F1) components of the harmonic responses were averaged over 8 or 12 stimulus repetitions and fit with a Gaussian or a Difference of Gaussians (DOG) receptive field model that incorporates both the amplitude and phase of the neural response (Enroth-Cugell et al., 1983), as described in detail previously (Dacey et al., 2000b). In brief, the receptive field center had a radial profile C(r), as follows:
where Wc is the gain of the center and Rc is the radius of the center (where sensitivity has fallen to 1/e of its peak). The amplitude of responses to stimulus modulation was computed as the two-dimensional integral of the product of the receptive field and stimulus profiles. Control responses to S and LM gratings were fit with only a center, responses to LM gratings in the presence of l-AP-4 were fit with a center and a surround S(r), with gain Ws and radius Rs. The overall amplitude and phase of the responses were computed by combining the center, and if appropriate, the surround responses with the following equation:
where Ac and As are the amplitudes of center and surround responses and θc and θs are the phases of their responses at the temporal frequency of the stimulus modulation. Initial fitting parameters were manually set, and numerical search was used to minimize the mean square difference between the measured and predicted responses.
Contrast response functions.
The contrast sensitivity and gain values for the blue-ON cell's S and LM inputs and for parasol cells' LM inputs (parasol cells did not respond to the S cone-isolating stimuli) were measured and calculated using the same cone-isolating stimuli described above with gratings of optimal spatial and temporal frequency. The gratings were presented at contrasts ranging from 2 to 28% or from 2 to 38% depending on the background light level. The blue-ON cell's S and LM contrast response functions were fit with a line by least-squares regression of which the slope indicated the contrast gain. The parasol cells contrast response functions were fit with a Naka–Rushton saturation function: Y = ax/(b + x), where a is the maximal response in impulses per second; x the Michelson contrast; and b the semisaturation constant (the contrast at which response amplitude is half the maximal response). Such curves are characterized by their percentage contrast gain, a/b.
Temporal modulation.
The temporal sensitivities and latencies of the S and LM components of the receptive field were characterized using the LED stimulator. Full-field cone-isolating stimuli were sinusoidally modulated at temporal frequencies ranging from 0.61 to 78 Hz. The contrast was set to 24% or 48%. The mean light level equated the quantal catch for all three cone types. Responses were averaged for 4 s at each temporal frequency. The F1 phase was found to be an approximately linear function of the temporal frequency and its slope (cycles/Hz) was used as a measure of the cell's visual latency (Frishman et al., 1987).
Pharmacology.
The circuitry underlying the receptive field of the blue-ON ganglion cell was investigated with pharmacological agents. The role of inner retinal inhibition was investigated with the glycine receptor antagonist strychnine (1 μm, S8753, Sigma) and the GABAA/C receptor antagonist picrotoxin (200 μm, P1675–1G, Sigma). The role of outer retinal inhibition was tested by adding the pH buffer HEPES (20 mm, H7523, Sigma) to Ames with the pH adjusted to 7.4. The metabotropic glutamate receptor agonist, l-AP-4 (100 μm, A1910, Sigma) was used to block the ON pathway at the photoreceptor-bipolar synapse. All pharmacological agents were dissolved in Ames medium and applied to the entire retina by continuous superfusion. We waited ∼4 min for full drug effect and typically recorded for ∼30 min before wash out (Crook et al., 2008).
Results
Targeting small bistratified blue-ON cells
Small bistratified, blue-ON cells were targeted in vitro by their relatively large cell bodies comparable to that of the parasol ganglion cells (Dacey, 1993; Dacey et al., 1994) observed either by epiillumination after vital staining with acridine orange or by infrared illumination of the unstained retina. Blue-ON cells were easily distinguished from parasol cells by sustained OFF responses to 500 ms light steps using the red or green LED primary and sustained ON responses to the blue LED primary. Parasol cells and other nonopponent cell types (Crook et al., 2008) showed transient responses that varied in amplitude but did not change in sign in response to the LED primary lights. A large bistratified blue-ON cell (Dacey and Packer, 2003) would have had responses similar to a small bistratified blue-ON, but these cells show smaller cell body size and have significantly larger receptive fields than those mapped for the small bistratified blue-ON cells. Altogether, we include data collected from 89 blue-ON cell recordings. For a subset of these cells we mapped S and LM receptive fields, compared S and LM sensitivity, and ran a series of pharmacology experiments. The effect of the inhibitory receptor antagonists, picrotoxin and strychnine, was tested on 17 cells, l-AP-4 was tested on 22 cells either alone or in combination with the inhibitory antagonists, and for 3 cells that had been tested with picrotoxin, strychnine, and l-AP4, HEPES was also applied. Rod inputs were characterized in an additional 16 cells. Finally, data from cells recorded intracellularly (n = 6) were also included.
Identification of a rod-mediated ON input to blue-ON cells
One of the main goals of this study was to measure the relative weights and spatial dimensions of S-ON and LM-OFF input to the small bistratified blue-ON cell. However, the stimuli used to isolate the S versus LM cone inputs described in Materials and Methods can also provide strong stimulation of rods. Conflicting reports describe a distinctive excitatory rod input (Virsu et al., 1987) or an apparent lack of rod input for cells of the blue-ON pathway (Lee et al., 1997). At the start of an experiment, the macaque in vitro preparation was maintained in a dark adapted state and when first recording from a blue-ON cell, we often observed strong ON responses to both S and LM cone-isolating stimuli in identified small bistratified cells at retinal illuminances (<10 photoisomerizations/cone/s) below the threshold for cone-mediated responses in our preparation (Dacey et al., 2005). Since additivity of rod and cone signals at mesopic levels in ganglion cells has been well documented (Gouras and Link, 1966; Gouras, 1967; Enroth-Cugell et al., 1977; Lee et al., 1997), it thus became critical to provide a preliminary characterization of this apparent rod input and, crucially, to determine the illuminance at which rod signals in our in vitro preparation could potentially interact with and effect our measurement of cone signal properties. The response of a blue-ON cell to a 1500 ms blue or green primary LED light step shows a dramatic change in temporal response, as a transition is made from cone-dominated to a rod-dominated response over a 4 log unit illuminance range (Fig. 3A) that spans the mesopic level (∼0–3 log10 photopic trolands). At medium to high photopic levels (∼103–105 photoisomerizations/cone/s), a sustained ON response to the blue primary and an OFF response to the green primary are observed and these responses are presumably driven primarily by an S-ON response to the blue and an LM-OFF response to the green primary (Fig. 3A, top traces). Latency from blue stimulus onset (or green stimulus offset) to first spike is ∼40–60 ms (Fig. 3B, top traces). As retinal illuminance is reduced, however, from ∼104 to 10 photoisomerizations/cone/s, the OFF response to the green primary is replaced by an ON response (n = 16). Thus, at ∼15–150 photoisomerizations/cone/s, both the blue and green stimuli elicit similar, sustained ON responses (Fig. 3A, bottom traces). At this light level, the rod quantal catch dominates and the latency from stimulus onset to spike threshold increases to ∼150–200 ms (Fig. 3B, bottom traces). At intermediate illuminances (Fig. 3A, middle traces; 102–103 photoisomerizations/cone/s), the rod-ON and LM-OFF responses are not easily distinguished and there appears to be some mixing of the two signals. The large latency difference between the rod- and cone-dominated responses was also manifest in the temporal frequency response to sinusoidal modulation at high versus low retinal illuminances (Fig. 3C). At low illuminances, the rod mediated response peaked at 2 Hz, rolling off sharply between 4 and 7 Hz compared with the cone mediated response peak at ∼10 Hz, rolling off between 40 and 50 Hz. Response latency derived from linear fits to response phase was ∼5 times slower (177 ± 36 ms, mean ± SD; n = 10) than cone latencies (40 ± 3 ms, mean ± SD; n = 20). Finally, at the low retinal illuminances eliciting a sustained ON response to both the blue and green LED primaries, bath application of l-AP4, the mGluR6 receptor agonist, completely eliminated the ON light response (n = 2), consistent with an origin of this signal from the depolarizing rod bipolar cell (Fig. 3D). These results provide evidence for a strong rod-mediated ON input to the small bistratified blue-ON cells. Below cone threshold, the light response is converted from an S-ON, LM-OFF response to a rod-ON response. From cone threshold to rod saturation (between 102 and 103 photoisomerizations/cone/s), there is potential for interaction of the rod-ON input with the S and LM cone inputs. Given the large difference between the rod and cone signal dynamics, the nature of this interaction would be dependent on the stimulus temporal frequency as well as the illuminance level. At 104 photoisomerizations/cone/s and above we assume rods are in saturation; we believe this is a safe assumption as psychophysical experiments show that saturation of primate scotopic vision begins when a background light reaches ∼100 scotopic trolands (Hayhoe et al., 1976); this is ∼400 photoisomerizations/cone/s, and is consistent with recordings from individual primate rods (Baylor et al., 1984). We have used stimuli well above this level for all subsequent measurements of cone responses in the small bistratified blue-ON cells.
Weighting of S-ON versus LM-OFF input to the blue-ON cells
The balance and linearity of S ON versus LM OFF inputs to the blue-ON cell was assessed with two different protocols. As a first rough test, cone-isolating pulses were modulated at 16% contrast in the following combinations; S, L+M (LM), L+M+S (LMS) and LM−S, the negative sign indicating a 16% contrast decrement. For a cell with balanced inputs the S ON and LM OFF responses should be equal and for a linear cell the LM−S response should equal the sum of individual S ON and LM OFF responses, whereas an LMS stimulus would elicit no net modulation. For a typical response, shown in Figure 2H, the calculated fundamental Fourier (F1) response amplitude to modulation of the S cone = 20 spikes/s, LM cones = 21 spikes/s, (LM−S)-cones = 41 spikes/s, and no significant response to in-phase modulation of all three cones (LMS). This result was consistent across cells and showed that at low contrasts blue-ON cells receive relatively equally weighted (S:LM = 1.16 ± 0.24, mean ± SD, median = 1.13; n = 67) (Table 1) and linear (LM−S:S+LM = 0.86 ± 0.14, mean ± SD, median = 0.87; n = 67) (Table 1) S and LM inputs.
Table 1.
S:LM pulse responses (spikes/s) | (LM−S):(S) + (LM) pulse responses (spikes/s) | S:LM cone input weights | S:LM radius at 1 SD (μm) | S:LM integrated volume | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Control | 1.16 ± 0.24 (n = 67) | 0.86 ± 0.14 (n = 67) | 1.09 ± 0.21 (n = 51) | 0.92 ± 0.13 (n = 55) | 1.15 ± 0.31 (n = 55) |
Picrotoxin | 1.11 ± 0.25 (n = 7) | 1.05 ± 0.13 (n = 7) | 1.00 ± 0.30 (n = 10) | 1.00 ± 0.19 (n = 3) | 1.19 ± 0.02 (n = 3) |
Strychnine | 1.07 ± 0.19 (n = 9) | 1.02 ± 0.17 (n = 9) | 1.10 ± 0.20 (n = 6) | 0.93 ± 0.31 (n = 3) | 1.05 ± 0.18 (n = 3) |
Picrotoxin and strychnine | 1.08 ± 0.25 (n = 7) | 1.03 ± 0.22 (n = 7) | 1.07 ± 0.16 (n = 5) | 0.96 ± 0.24 (n = 3) | 1.12 ± 0.27 (n = 3) |
Summary of blue-ON cell chromatic response properties for four experimental conditions including controls and the effects of GABAa/c and glycine receptor antagonists (rows). From left to right, the first column summarizes the balance of S:LM amplitudes in response to 16% contrast, 2 Hz pulses; second column, response linearity of S and LM inputs calculated as (LM−S) responses versus the sum of individual S and LM responses to 16% contrast, 2 Hz pulses; third column, relative balance of S:LM cone input weights from sinusoidal fits to responses described in Figure 3; fourth column, S:LM radii (in micrometers) estimated from Gaussian fits to the spatial modulation transfer functions; and fifth column, estimated S:LM integrated volumes from the Gaussian fits to the spatial modulation transfer functions.
A second stimulus protocol was designed to quantify the relative S versus LM cone input weights more systematically. Complete datasets were achieved in a subset of 51 cells in the total sample of 89 in which cone weights were estimated. The stimulus varied S and LM cone contrast and phase stepwise from equal cone contrasts in phase to equal contrasts in anti-phase (Fig. 4A). The data were fit by a simple linear model of cone summation (Diller et al., 2004) and is described in detail in the Materials and Methods (see also Fig. 4, legend). As expected, blue-ON cells peak response typically occurred when the S and LM cones were modulated at equal depth (equal cone contrasts) and in anti-phase and the response minimum was reached when the cones were modulated at equal depth and in phase (Fig. 4B). Assuming linear summation, the fit quantifies the S:LM weight. Thus, for the cell illustrated in Figure 4B the calculated S:LM cone input weight equaled 0.97. S and LM normalized and averaged response amplitudes and phases were averaged to give an input weight ratio (S:LM) of 1.09 ± 0.21 (mean ± SD; median = 1.56; n = 51) (Fig. 4C, Table 1).
S and LM cone contrast and temporal sensitivity
The relative balance of S and LM inputs was also tested at a range of lower and higher contrasts and temporal frequencies. Contrast sensitivity was compared using S and LM cone-isolating stimuli between 2 and 38%, which were the maximal cone contrasts that we could achieve on a background of equal mean quantal catch for all three cone types. Both LM and S responses increased linearly with contrast (Fig. 5A). The slope of a straight line fit to the data was used to quantify contrast gain (Fig. 5B). The average contrast gain for the S was 1.26 ± 0.49 (mean ± SD; n = 8) and for the LM input 1.25 ± 0.25 (mean ± SD; n = 8). These overlap with contrast gain values calculated from extracellular recordings of L versus M and S versus LM opponent cells in the intact animal (Yeh et al., 1995; Solomon et al., 2005; Tailby et al., 2008) and were somewhat lower than that for the well studied magnocellular-projecting ganglion cells in response to an achromatic stimulus (Crook et al., 2008). To illustrate this distinct difference, we recorded from neighboring magnocellular projecting parasol cells using the same LM stimulus (parasol cells did not respond to S cone-isolating stimuli) at an optimal spatial and temporal frequency (10.5 Hz). Parasol cells show extreme sensitivity to low contrasts with response amplitudes more than doubling to two-fold increases in contrast (Fig. 5A, squares). The parasol cell contrast response functions were well fit with Naka–Rushton saturation functions (see Materials and Methods) and contrast gain values were calculated from the initial linear portion of the curve. On average the contrast gain was 4.85 ± 0.37 (mean ± SD; n = 3).
We compared LM and S temporal sensitivities with cone-isolating stimuli (2 mm diameter field) sinusoidally modulated at 25–48% contrast at temporal frequencies ranging from 0.61–78 Hz. S-ON and LM-OFF inputs were both sensitive to temporal frequencies up to 60 Hz with similar peak responses (S cone = 11 ± 7 Hz and LM cone = 7 ± 2 Hz, mean ± SD; n = 20) (Fig. 5C). The inset in Figure 5C plots the corresponding phase data for one cell. The phases of the S and LM inputs were initially ∼180 degrees offset and converged with increasing temporal frequency. For each cell, the linear portion of the phase data was fit with a straight line to calculate the latency. For the cell shown in the inset to Figure 5C, the S input latency was 42 ms and the LM 48 ms. Across cells, the response to S cone stimuli showed slightly shorter latencies than that for LM stimuli (S mean latency ± SD = 40 ± 3 ms; LM mean latency ± SD = 46 ± 3 ms; n = 20) (Fig. 5D). Our data are approximately consistent with those reported from measurements using comparable stimuli made in the intact eye in vivo (Yeh et al., 1995). In the study by Yeh et al. (1995), latencies of ∼35 ms were found for both the S and LM cone driven responses at comparable light levels to those used here. The reason for this discrepancy is unclear, although it could reflect effects of the in vitro maintenance on the temporal sensitivity of the retina. We have observed anecdotally a profound effect of retinal temperature on the response dynamics; while we attempt to keep our in vitro retinas as close to physiological temperature as possible (typically ∼35–36°C), it is only possible to monitor temperature and a location in the superfusion chamber relatively distant from the recording site, so precise temperatures and temperature fluctuations are not known. In another recent study recording from blue-ON cells in the in vitro retina, much longer latencies of ∼50 and 70 ms for the S and LM cone inputs, respectively, were reported (Field et al., 2007). However, retinal illuminance was lower in this study and latencies were calculated from time to peak of the spike-triggered average derived from a white noise stimulus, so it is possible that these methodological differences also contributed to the additional deviation from that found in vivo. Overall, our data are consistent with previous work from recordings in the intact animal suggesting similar temporal and contrast sensitivities for S ON and LM OFF inputs to the blue-ON ganglion cell.
Coextensive S and LM cone receptive fields
To test whether blue-ON cells had center-surround or coextensive receptive field structure, the S-ON and LM-OFF receptive fields of 55 blue-ON cells were mapped with either drifting sinusoidally modulated gratings that varied in spatial frequency, or sinusoidally modulated spots and annuli as a function of increasing diameter. Regardless of stimulus, both receptive fields showed similar or identical spatial tuning (Fig. 6). For drifting grating stimuli, both the S and LM spatial frequency responses were low pass (Fig. 6A,D) with comparable peak responses at the lowest spatial frequencies tested and similar high spatial frequency cutoffs. To a spot centered on the cell's receptive field (Fig. 6B), LM and S response magnitudes peaked when spot diameter approximated the expected diameter of the dendritic tree of a small bistratified cell (Dacey, 1993). The response plateaued at this diameter (usually between 75 and 200 μm) (Fig. 6B), suggesting little or no surround antagonism. If a surround was present, increasing spot size to encroach on the surround should attenuate the response. To an annulus centered on the cell's receptive field (Fig. 6C), the maximum response for both the LM and S inputs occurred when the inner diameter was smallest (9 μm). As the annulus center increased, both ON and OFF inputs rolled off with no sign of a reversal in the response phase (Fig. 6C, inset) as would be indicative of a surround. Low-pass S and LM spatial frequency responses were thus well fit with a single Gaussian. Examples in Figure 6, A and D, show the Gaussian fits (solid lines) and the inset in each plot shows the model two-dimensional (2D) receptive field profile and calculated Gaussian radii (μm). S and LM cone driven receptive field sizes were nearly identical (S to LM diameter ratio from the Gaussian fits = 0.92 ± 0.13, mean ± SD; median = 0.94; n = 55) (Table 1) and the slope of the fit to LM versus S Gaussian radii in Figure 6E was equal to 0.97 (R2 = 0.90). The peak response amplitudes (∼30 spikes/s) were also typically matched for the S and LM responses (S:LM response ratio = 1.19 ± 0.38, mean ± SD; median = 1.19; n = 55). Thus, the S and LM fields are well matched in both size and sensitivity.
To summarize, the S-ON and LM-OFF inputs show similar contrast and temporal sensitivities; the two fields are also nearly equal in area and sensitivity, consistent with a spatially coextensive pattern (Fig. 1A). To explore the retinal circuitry that generates the S-ON and LM-OFF responses (Fig. 1C), we used pharmacological manipulation to assess the role of excitatory ON pathway and inner retinal inhibitory pathway input to the small bistratified cell.
Opponent responses are not generated by inner retinal inhibitory pathways
A large percentage of inputs to the small bistratified cell are from amacrine cells (Calkins et al., 1998; Ghosh and Grünert, 1999). We therefore assessed whether inhibitory amacrine input had a major role in generating the balance of S versus LM inputs or shaping their coextensive receptive fields. We recorded responses from blue-ON cells in the presence of picrotoxin, a general GABAa/c receptor antagonist, and strychnine, a glycine receptor antagonist, both in combination and alone, using the same stimuli used to measure S versus LM input strength (Fig. 4) and receptive field spatial structure (Fig. 6). Qualitatively, the intracellular and spike histogram responses to cone-isolating stimuli presented in Figure 7A–C clearly illustrate that the blue-ON cell's S and LM opponency was not greatly enhanced or attenuated in the presence of picrotoxin (Fig. 7A), strychnine (Fig. 7B), or picrotoxin and strychnine together (Fig. 7C). Furthermore, the S and LM response amplitudes and LM−S versus LMS ratios calculated from the histograms were indistinguishable from the controls for all three conditions (Table 1). The sensitivity and balance of the S and LM cone input weights to incremental changes in contrast, measured with the protocol described in Figure 4, was also maintained. For picrotoxin and strychnine alone (Fig. 7D,E, open circles), and picrotoxin and strychnine together (Fig. 7F, open circles), the responses, fits, and relative S versus LM cone input weights (Table 1) overlapped the controls (Fig. 7D–F, solid circles). Blocking the inhibitory pathways also had no effect on the relative sensitivity or spatial extent of the S and LM receptive fields. Using cone-isolating drifting gratings, the S and LM receptive fields were measured in the presence of picrotoxin (Fig. 7G), strychnine (Fig. 7H), and picrotoxin and strychnine together (Fig. 7I). For each condition, the S and LM spatial frequency responses remained low pass, coextensive, and well fit with a Gaussian receptive field model (solid lines). These results are summarized in Table 1, showing the S:LM radii and integrated volumes ratios. For all three conditions, there was no change compared with the controls. Overall, blue-ON cells remained opponent (Fig. 7A–C) with equally weighted S and LM inputs (Fig. 7D–F) and with intact, approximately coextensive, low-pass LM and S receptive fields (Fig. 7G–I). However, for the intracellular recordings to high contrast S versus LM cone modulation (Fig. 7A–C, upper traces), we noted subtle changes to S and LM amplitudes and kinetics after blocking GABAergic inhibition. In the presence of picrotoxin both the S and LM responses increased in amplitude, quantified as the amplitude Fourier component of the voltage response at the stimulus frequency (Fig. 7A) (picrotoxin/control S = 1.63 ± 0.14 and LM = 1.44 ± 0.22, mean ± SD; n = 3). The LM responses also hyperpolarized more slowly at the onset of the light stimulus (Fig. 7A). The width of the LM response, measured at 1/3 the maximum response amplitude, increased from 264 ± 10 ms to 316 ± 13 ms (mean ± SD; n = 3), while the width of the S response was unchanged, from 246 ± 4 ms to 245 ± 8 ms (mean ± SD; n = 3). In the presence of strychnine, the light responses were not significantly changed in amplitude (Fig. 7B) (strychnine/control S = 0.92 ± 0.12 and LM = 1.04 ± 0.07, mean ± SD; n = 3) or width (S control = 241 ± 6 ms and S strychnine = 243 ± 8 ms; LM control = 257 ± 14 ms and LM strychnine = 252 ± 19 ms, mean ± SD; n = 3). We conclude that inner retinal inhibitory pathways may play a role in shaping the amplitude and dynamics of the S and LM response, but that these pathways are not essential for generating the LM-OFF component of the blue-ON cell response.
ON pathway block isolates an LM OFF field with center-surround organization
To directly test the hypothesis that input from the ON pathway generates the small bistratified cell's S-ON response and that the LM-OFF response arises from a parallel OFF bipolar pathway synaptic input (Calkins et al., 1998; Ghosh and Grünert, 1999), we recorded responses to cone-isolating stimuli in the presence of the mGluR6 receptor agonist l-AP-4 to block ON pathway transmission (Slaughter and Miller, 1981). The l-AP-4 was also applied in combination with strychnine and picrotoxin to isolate excitatory OFF pathway input to the small bistratified cell. The addition of l-AP-4 abolished the S-ON response while sparing the LM-OFF response. Robust LM-OFF responses of similar amplitude were recorded to LM, LM−S and LMS cone modulation (Fig. 8A). With S-ON input blocked the LM−S and LMS stimuli now elicited responses equivalent to pure LM stimuli. In 22 out of 22 blue-ON cells, we recorded only LM-OFF responses in the presence of l-AP-4 or l-AP-4, picrotoxin, and strychnine.
After elimination of the S-ON response by l-AP-4, we mapped the spatial receptive field of the isolated LM-OFF input for 13 cells and found responses at low spatial frequencies were strongly attenuated, indicating the presence of surround antagonism not observed when the S-ON component was present in the control situation. The presence of a surround in the isolated LM response was confirmed using spot and annular stimuli. Thus, a 300 μm diameter spot elicited the expected LM-OFF response, but a 300 μm inner diameter annulus elicited an LM-ON response (Fig. 8B). The surround was clearly observed by centering a spot on the cell's receptive field and increasing its inner diameter. Initially, like the control, responses increased and peaked as the size of the spot covered the center, but with increasing spot size the response amplitude decreased as the spot grew and stimulated the antagonistic surround (Fig. 8C). Similarly, the control LM OFF response to an annulus of increasing inner diameter peaks and then declines (Fig. 8D, open circles) with no change in response phase (Fig. 8D, inset, open circles). In contrast, in the presence of l-AP-4, picrotoxin, and strychnine the LM-OFF response initially peaks but then declines until the annulus inner diameter reaches ∼150 μm and isolates an LM-ON surround response as indicated by an ∼1800 shift in the response phase. (Fig. 8D, inset, solid circles). The LM-ON response then peaks and declines as the inner diameter increases further (Fig. 8D, solid circles). Finally, in the presence of either l-AP-4 alone (Fig. 8E, left column) or l-AP-4 and the GABAa/c and glycine receptor antagonists (Fig. 8E, right column), low spatial frequency drifting gratings stimulated the broad antagonistic surround changing the spatial tuning from low pass to bandpass. l-AP-4 thus shifted the average peak response from a low spatial frequency (control: 0.047 ± 0 cpd; mean ± SD; n = 13) to a much higher spatial frequency (0.461 ± 0.217 cpd; mean ± SD; n = 13). The resulting spatial frequency responses were best fit with a difference-of-Gaussian receptive field model rather than the single Gaussian model used for the controls (Fig. 8E). From the fits, the surround was estimated to be on average ∼4 times larger than the center. By contrast, the center radius was unchanged under all conditions (control center radius/center radius after l-AP-4 alone = 1.35 ± 0.16, mean ± SD, n = 7; after l-AP-4, picrotoxin, and strychnine = 1.34 ± 0.42, mean ± SD, n = 6). The average sensitivity, measured as the amplitude of the peak response, was unchanged but more variable with the blockers present (control: 37 ± 9 spikes/s vs drug: 34 ± 17 spikes/s; mean ± SD; n = 13). Thus, responses in the presence of either l-AP-4 alone or l-AP-4, picrotoxin, and strychnine reveal that blue-ON cells receive S-ON input via the ON pathway and are consistent with LM input from OFF cone bipolar cells that have center-surround organization. A hypothesis for the appearance of the surround in the LM OFF pathway when the ON pathway is blocked will be considered in Discussion.
LM-ON surround may originate from a non-GABAergic feedback pathway
The surround response was robust in the presence of either l-AP4 alone or l-AP-4 with strychnine and picrotoxin (Fig. 8E). The lack of effect of inner retinal inhibitory block on the center-surround structure of the isolated LM-OFF response suggested that a non-GABAergic and nonglycinergic pathway generated the surround. In previous studies, we showed that the cone-driven surrounds of parasol ganglion cells were also maintained after block of inner retinal inhibition (McMahon et al., 2004) but could be significantly attenuated by increasing the buffering capacity of the retina (Davenport et al., 2008), consistent with the interpretation that the surround was mediated by a novel non-GABAergic outer retinal feedback mechanism (Kamermans and Spekreijse, 1999; Hirasawa and Kaneko, 2003; Kamermans and Fahrenfort, 2004). We therefore used a similar protocol here, by adding HEPES buffer (20 mm) to the Ames medium to measure its effect on the center-surround organization for three of the cells in which the LM-OFF response had been isolated by l-AP-4, picrotoxin, and strychnine. The addition of HEPES attenuated the LM-ON response leaving only the center LM-OFF response intact (Fig. 9A,B), and with washout of HEPES the LM-ON responses were partially recovered (Fig. 9B). With HEPES buffering, the peak response to drifting gratings also now occurred at the lowest spatial frequency again (0.047 cpd), like the original LM field, and the low-pass spatial frequency responses were fit with a Gaussian model (Fig. 9C). The results for the cell in Figure 9C are summarized in the 2D Gaussian receptive field profiles (Fig. 9D) showing the LM receptive field before drug application (control, far left), the center and surround LM receptive field in the presence of l-AP-4, strychnine and picrotoxin (middle), and after the addition of HEPES (attenuating the LM-ON surround; right). Similar measurements for two other cells were also made and showed the same pattern (Fig. 9D, legend). Thus, much like that shown previously for the LM surround of parasol ganglion cells (Davenport et al., 2008) the “unmasked” ON-surround of the LM-OFF pathway input to the blue-ON cells may be mediated by non-GABAergic outer retinal feedback (Kamermans and Spekreijse, 1999; Kamermans and Fahrenfort, 2004; Cadetti and Thoreson, 2006).
Discussion
Identification of Rod-ON input to small bistratified blue-ON cell
After dark adaptation, rod input to the small bistratified cell is clearly present (Fig. 3). The sign of the rod signal follows that of the S-ON input: when rod input prevails, the blue-ON yellow-OFF cell becomes a rod-ON cell. These results confirm an early observation of strong excitatory rod input to blue-ON cell relay cells recorded in the parvocellular LGN (Virsu et al., 1987), although a more recent study failed to observe a rod signal in blue-ON ganglion cells (Lee et al., 1997). In the primary rod pathway the depolarizing AII amacrine cell receives input from the rod bipolar cell and transmits the rod signal to ON cone bipolar cells via a sign-conserving gap junction and to OFF cone bipolar cells by a sign-inverting glycinergic synapse (Famiglietti and Kolb, 1975; Müller et al., 1988; Strettoi et al., 1992). The rod-ON response of the blue-ON ganglion cell could thus be mediated by an AII amacrine gap junction with the blue cone bipolar axon terminal; a preliminary report suggests such a contact is present (Marshak et al., 2009). In contrast, our data suggest a lack of the glycinergic synapse from the AII cell outer dendrites to the LM-OFF bipolar cell population(s) that are presynaptic to the small bistratified outer dendrites (Ghosh et al., 1997; Calkins et al., 1998). In this regard, there is growing evidence that certain mammalian bipolar cell types may not participate in the primary rod pathway (Völgyi et al., 2004; Petrides and Trexler, 2008). The degree to which rod signals might be restricted to distinct ON and OFF cone bipolar pathways remains to be investigated in the primate. The rod-ON input to the blue-ON cell is consistent with a large body of human psychophysical data suggesting that rods interact synergistically with S-cones at mesopic levels to introduce a blue-bias to color appearance (for review, Buck, 2004). However, the current results do not exclude a rod-OFF input that is masked by a larger rod-ON signal or that arises at lower scotopic levels. Experiments that pharmacologically isolate potential ON and OFF components of the rod signal near threshold are needed to carefully address this question.
Blue-ON cells show spatially coextensive receptive field structure
Recording from the small bistratified cells in the choroid-attached in vitro retina, we found that the S-ON and LM-OFF responses were spatially coextensive, or nearly so (Fig. 6), that the contrast gain and temporal response of the opponent fields were comparable (Fig. 5), and that at low to moderate cone contrasts LM and S cone inputs appear to sum linearly (Fig. 4). These results are broadly consistent with previous descriptions of the blue-ON cell at both the LGN (Wiesel and Hubel, 1966; Dreher et al., 1976; Derrington et al., 1984; Tailby et al., 2008) and retinal levels (De Monasterio and Gouras, 1975; de Monasterio, 1978a; Smith et al., 1992; Yeh et al., 1995; Lee et al., 2000; Solomon et al., 2005; Tailby et al., 2008). The spatially comparable S-ON and LM-OFF fields of the blue-ON cell contrast sharply with the classic center-surround receptive field structure of the other two common LGN-projecting ganglion cells of the primate—the midget and parasol cells (Derrington and Lennie, 1984; Croner and Kaplan, 1995; White et al., 2001), where surrounds are about fifty times the area of but 100 times less sensitive (per unit area) than the center and the integrated sensitivity (peak sensitivity × collecting area) is only about half that of the center. Consequently, the center response dominates even for stimuli that encompass the much larger surround (Croner and Kaplan, 1995). For the blue-ON cells, the opposing fields were comparable both in area and peak sensitivity (Fig. 6, Table 1), so that stimuli approximating the size of the dendritic tree can drive both S-ON and LM-OFF responses equally. There is a tendency for the LM-OFF field to be on average slightly larger than the S-ON field (S:LM = 0.92 ± 0.15; n = 55) and a similar result was reported using the intact, anesthetized animal (Solomon et al., 2005). Another recent study using multielectrode recordings from the isolated, in vitro retina reported the LM field to be on average 48% larger (∼2 times the area) than the S field and concluded that the blue-ON cells showed a classic center-surround organization (Field et al., 2007). Field et al. (2007) did not report the relative sensitivities of the S versus the LM fields or construct a center-surround spatial receptive field model, so it is difficult to assess this data further in relation to our own. It is possible that eccentricity dependent changes in blue-ON receptive field structure could reconcile differences across studies. In the present and previous results coextensive S versus LM fields in either the LGN or retina of blue-ON cells were recorded within the central 20 degrees (< 4 mm eccentricity). In contrast, the data in Field et al. (2007) were collected in the far retinal periphery (30–75 degrees eccentricity). Chromatic sensitivity in human vision declines significantly in the far periphery (Mullen, 1991; Mullen and Kingdom, 1996; Sakurai and Mullen, 2006). Whether blue-ON cells show reduced chromatic and/or increased achromatic sensitivity in the retinal periphery remains to be determined.
OFF cone bipolar origin for the LM field
Sparing of the LM OFF response after l-AP-4 application is consistent with OFF bipolar synaptic input to the distinct outer dendritic arbor of the small bistratified cell (Fig. 1) (Calkins et al., 1998; Ghosh and Grünert, 1999). Our results appear inconsistent with a reported total loss of the blue-ON cell light response and maintained discharge in the presence of l-AP-4 (Field et al., 2007). We would argue caution in interpreting these results. The action of l-AP-4 on mGluR6 mimics tonic glutamate release and hyperpolarizes the ON bipolar cell (Awatramani and Slaughter, 2001) and subsequently the ON ganglion cell (Zaghloul et al., 2003; Davenport, 2007; Margolis and Detwiler, 2007). Thus, if l-AP-4 hyperpolarizes the blue-ON cell the LM OFF input may not drive the membrane potential to spike threshold. Our recordings were made in the RPE-choroid-attached retina and permitted stimulation at high photopic levels. Under these conditions maintained discharge and the LM-OFF response were consistently spared after bath application of l-AP-4.
Failure of picrotoxin and/or strychnine to significantly modify the S ON or LM OFF response amplitude was also consistent with an excitatory cone bipolar origin for the LM OFF field, and we suggest that amacrine circuitry is not critical for generating S versus LM cone opponency. The majority of synaptic inputs to the small bistratified cell are from amacrine cells, however (Ghosh and Grünert, 1999), so the significance of these inputs remains open. There is strong evidence that inner retinal inhibition to some extent works in a push-pull manner to reinforce excitation, and that the OFF pathway, at least for certain ganglion cell types, receives more inhibition from the ON pathway than vice versa (Pang et al., 2003; Zaghloul et al., 2003; Murphy and Rieke, 2006; Margolis and Detwiler, 2007; Molnar and Werblin, 2007; Manookin et al., 2008). It seems unlikely that the S-ON bipolar would drive such “cross-pathway” amacrine inhibition to the LM-OFF bipolar as this would work against the basic cone opponent response in which S increments reinforce LM decrements. It is possible, however, that disinhibition that arises in LM ON cone bipolar cells reinforces the LM OFF response–although at the high photopic levels and moderate contrasts used here we did not observe a change in the LM OFF response relative to the S ON response after inhibitory block (Fig. 7). Whole-cell recordings of blue-ON cells under voltage clamp designed to measure the phase and amplitude of excitatory and inhibitory conductances generated by S and LM selective stimuli are required to directly address this question.
A striking but consistent effect of l-AP-4 was the dramatic change in the LM-OFF response to bandpass spatial frequency tuning that could be fit with a center-surround receptive field model (Fig. 8). This center-surround structure was maintained during block of inhibition but was attenuated by increasing retinal buffering capacity with HEPES (Fig. 9). Surround reduction by HEPES buffering has been described in nonmammalian retina and it has been suggested that HEPES acts on a proton-dependent, non-GABAergic horizontal to cone feedback pathway (Hirasawa and Kaneko, 2003; Vessey et al., 2005; Cadetti and Thoreson, 2006). The surrounds of parasol ganglion cells are also maintained after block of retinal inhibition (McMahon et al., 2004) but attenuated by HEPES (Davenport et al., 2008). We suggest therefore that l-AP-4 unmasks the basic center-surround structure of the LM-OFF bipolar input to the blue-ON cell, consistent with the presence of surrounds in cone bipolar cells (Dacey et al., 2000b). What underlies the unmasking of this basic center-surround structure by l-AP-4? We hypothesize that the S-ON bipolar cell also possesses an LM OFF surround and that under conditions where S-ON and LM-OFF fields are functioning, there is no or little net surround antagonism (Fig. 10). The LM-OFF surround of the S-ON bipolar would arise by feedback from H2 horizontal cells, whose processes contact S, L, and M cones. The LM-ON surround of the LM-OFF bipolar would arise by feedback from the H1 horizontal cell that contacts L and M cones nonselectively (Dacey et al., 1996; Goodchild et al., 1996). The light response of the blue-cone bipolar cell (Kouyama and Marshak, 1992) is unknown. However, the S cone already possesses a large opponent surround driven by L and M cones (Packer et al., 2007) that is likely transmitted to the S-ON bipolar and in turn to the small bistratified ganglion cell.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Paul Kayser International Award from the Retina Research Foundation (D.M.D.), the Human Frontier Science Program Grant RGP 00067/2004-C (P.B.D.), the National Primate Research Center at the University of Washington Grant RR00166, and National Institutes of Health Grants EY06678 (D.M.D.), EY01730 (Vision Research Core), EY02048 (P.B.D.), and GM07108 (C.M.D.). We thank Julian Vrieslander for programming assistance through the Vision Core and the development of the Vision Core Toolbox (http://vrc.biostr.washington.edu/index.html). We also thank John Troy and Barry Lee for advice and discussion and Toni Haun for technical assistance.
References
- Awatramani GB, Slaughter MM. Intensity-dependent, rapid activation of presynaptic metabotropic glutamate receptors at a central synapse. J Neurosci. 2001;21:741–749. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-00741.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Lamb TD, Yau KW. Responses of retinal rods to single photons. J Physiol. 1979;288:613–634. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. The photocurrent, noise and spectral sensitivity of rods of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol. 1984;357:575–607. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. Spectral sensitivity of cones of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J Physiol. 1987;390:145–160. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buck SL. Rod-cone interactions in human vision. In: Chalupa LM, Werner JS, editors. The visual neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT; 2004. pp. 863–878. [Google Scholar]
- Cadetti L, Thoreson WB. Feedback effects of horizontal cell membrane potential on cone calcium currents studied with simultaneous recordings. J Neurophysiol. 2006;95:1992–1995. doi: 10.1152/jn.01042.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calkins DJ, Tsukamoto Y, Sterling P. Microcircuitry and mosaic of a blue-yellow ganglion cell in the primate retina. J Neurosci. 1998;18:3373–3385. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03373.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croner LJ, Kaplan E. Receptive fields of P and M ganglion cells across the primate retina. Vision Res. 1995;35:7–24. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)e0066-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crook JD, Peterson BB, Packer OS, Robinson FR, Troy JB, Dacey DM. Y-cell receptive field and collicular projection of parasol ganglion cells in macaque monkey retina. J Neurosci. 2008;28:11277–11291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2982-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM. Morphology of a small-field bistratified ganglion cell type in the macaque and human retina. Vis Neurosci. 1993;10:1081–1098. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800010191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM. Circuitry for color coding in the primate retina. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;93:582–588. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM. Parallel pathways for spectral coding in primate retina. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:743–775. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Lee BB. The blue-ON opponent pathway in primate retina originates from a distinct bistratified ganglion cell type. Nature. 1994;367:731–735. doi: 10.1038/367731a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Packer OS. Colour coding in the primate retina: diverse cell types and cone-specific circuitry. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:421–427. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00103-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Lee BB, Stafford DK, Pokorny J, Smith VC. Horizontal cells of the primate retina: cone specificity without spectral opponency. Science. 1996;271:656–659. doi: 10.1126/science.271.5249.656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Diller LC, Verweij J, Williams DR. Physiology of L- and M-cone inputs to H1 horizontal cells in the primate retina. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2000a;17:589–596. doi: 10.1364/josaa.17.000589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Packer OS, Diller L, Brainard D, Peterson B, Lee B. Center surround receptive field structure of cone bipolar cells in primate retina. Vision Res. 2000b;40:1801–1811. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(00)00039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dacey DM, Liao HW, Peterson BB, Robinson FR, Smith VC, Pokorny J, Yau KW, Gamlin PD. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature. 2005;433:749–754. doi: 10.1038/nature03387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dartnall HJA. Photosensitivity. In: Dartnall HJA, editor. Photochemistry of vision. New York: Springer; 1972. pp. 122–145. [Google Scholar]
- Davenport CM, Detwiler PB, Dacey DM. Effects of pH buffering on horizontal and ganglion cell light responses in primate retina: evidence for the proton hypothesis of surround formation. J Neurosci. 2008;28:456–464. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2735-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davenport CM. University of Washington; 2007. Neural circuitry of retinal receptive fields in primate. PhD thesis. [Google Scholar]
- de Monasterio FM. Properties of ganglion cells with atypical receptive-field organization in retina of macaques. J Neurophysiol. 1978a;41:1435–1449. doi: 10.1152/jn.1978.41.6.1435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Monasterio FM. Center and surround mechanisms of opponent-color X and Y ganglion cells of retina of macaques. J Neurophysiol. 1978b;41:1418–1434. doi: 10.1152/jn.1978.41.6.1418. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Monasterio FM, Gouras P. Functional properties of ganglion cells of the rhesus monkey retina. J Physiol. 1975;251:167–195. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1975.sp011086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derrington AM, Lennie P. Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivities of neurones in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol. 1984;357:219–240. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015498. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Derrington AM, Krauskopf J, Lennie P. Chromatic mechanisms in lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque. J Physiol. 1984;357:241–265. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diller L, Packer OS, Verweij J, McMahon MJ, Williams DR, Dacey DM. L and M cone contributions to the midget and parasol ganglion cell receptive fields of macaque monkey retina. J Neurosci. 2004;24:1079–1088. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3828-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dreher B, Fukada Y, Rodieck RW. Identification, classification and anatomical segregation of cells with X-like and Y-like properties in the lateral geniculate nucleus of old-world primates. J Physiol. 1976;258:433–452. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enroth-Cugell C, Hertz BG, Lennie P. Convergence of rod and cone signals in the cat's retina. J Physiol. 1977;269:297–318. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1977.sp011903. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Enroth-Cugell C, Robson JG, Schweitzer-Tong DE, Watson AB. Spatio-temporal interactions in cat retinal ganglion cells showing linear spatial summation. J Physiol. 1983;341:279–307. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014806. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estévez O, Spekreijse H. A spectral compensation method for determining the flicker characteristics of the human colour mechanisms. Vision Res. 1974;14:823–830. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90147-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estévez O, Spekreijse H. The “silent substitution” method in visual research. Vision Res. 1982;22:681–691. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90104-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Famiglietti EV, Jr, Kolb H. A bistratified amacrine cell and synaptic circuitry in the inner plexiform layer of the retina. Brain Res. 1975;84:293–300. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(75)90983-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field GD, Sher A, Gauthier JL, Greschner M, Shlens J, Litke AM, Chichilnisky EJ. Spatial properties and functional organization of small bistratified ganglion cells in primate retina. J Neurosci. 2007;27:13261–13272. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3437-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frishman LJ, Freeman AW, Troy JB, Schweitzer-Tong DE, Enroth-Cugell C. Spatiotemporal frequency responses of cat retinal ganglion cells. J Gen Physiol. 1987;89:599–628. doi: 10.1085/jgp.89.4.599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh KK, Grünert U. Synaptic input to small bistratified (blue-ON) ganglion cells in the retina of a new world monkey, the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. J Comp Neurol. 1999;413:417–428. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19991025)413:3<417::aid-cne5>3.0.co;2-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghosh KK, Martin PR, Grünert U. Morphological analysis of the blue cone pathway in the retina of a new world monkey, the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. J Comp Neurol. 1997;379:211–225. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodchild AK, Chan TL, Grünert U. Horizontal cell connections with short-wavelength-sensitive cones in macaque monkey retina. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13:833–845. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800009093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouras P. The effects of light adaptation on rod and cone receptive field organization of monkey ganglion cells. J Physiol. 1967;192:747–760. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouras P, Link K. Rod and cone interaction in dark-adapted monkey ganglion cells. J Physiol. 1966;184:499–510. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1966.sp007928. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gouras P, Zrenner E. Enhancement of luminance flicker by color-opponent mechanisms. Science. 1979;205:587–589. doi: 10.1126/science.109925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayhoe MM, MacLeod DIA, Bruch TA. Rod-cone independence in dark adaptation. Vision Res. 1976;16:591–600. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(76)90005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirasawa H, Kaneko A. pH changes in the invaginating synaptic cleft mediate feedback from horizontal cells to cone photoreceptors by modulating Ca2+ channels. J Gen Physiol. 2003;122:657–671. doi: 10.1085/jgp.200308863. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamermans M, Fahrenfort I. Ephaptic interactions within a chemical synapse: hemichannel-mediated ephaptic inhibition in the retina. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:531–541. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamermans M, Spekreijse H. The feedback pathway from horizontal cells to cones - a mini review with a look ahead. Vision Res. 1999;39:2449–2468. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00043-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouyama N, Marshak DW. Bipolar cells specific for blue cones in the macaque retina. J Neurosci. 1992;12:1233–1252. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-04-01233.1992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BB, Smith VC, Pokorny J, Kremers J. Rod inputs to macaque ganglion cells. Vision Res. 1997;37:2813–2828. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(97)00108-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BB, Silveira LC, Yamada ES, Hunt DM, Kremers J, Martin PR, Troy JB, da Silva-Filho M. Visual responses of ganglion cells of a New-World primate, the capuchin monkey, Cebus apella. J Physiol. 2000;528:573–590. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.00573.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lennie P, Movshon JA. Coding of color and form in the geniculostriate visual pathway (invited review) J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2005;22:2013–2033. doi: 10.1364/josaa.22.002013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manookin MB, Beaudoin DL, Ernst ZR, Flagel LJ, Demb JB. Disinhibition combines with excitation to extend the operating range of the OFF visual pathway in daylight. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4136–4150. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4274-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margolis DJ, Detwiler PB. Different mechanisms generate maintained activity in ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells. J Neurosci. 2007;27:5994–6005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0130-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshak DW, Rangal C, O'Brien J. Gap junctions between AII amacrine cells and blue cone bipolar cells in macaque retina. Paper presented at The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2009 Annual Meeting; May; Fort Lauderdale, FL. 2009. [Google Scholar]
- McMahon MJ, Packer OS, Dacey DM. The classical receptive field surround of primate parasol ganglion cells is mediated primarily by a non-GABAergic pathway. J Neurosci. 2004;24:3736–3745. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5252-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molnar A, Werblin F. Inhibitory feedback shapes bipolar cell responses in the rabbit retina. J Neurophysiol. 2007;98:3423–3435. doi: 10.1152/jn.00838.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen KT. Colour vision as a post-receptoral specialization of the central visual field. Vision Res. 1991;31:119–130. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90079-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen KT, Kingdom FAA. Losses in peripheral colour sensitivity predicted from “hit and miss” post-receptoral cone connections. Vision Res. 1996;36:1995–2000. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)00261-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Müller F, Wässle H, Voigt T. Pharmacological modulation of the rod pathway in the cat retina. J Neurophysiol. 1988;59:1657–1672. doi: 10.1152/jn.1988.59.6.1657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy GJ, Rieke F. Network variability limits stimulus-evoked spike timing precision in retinal ganglion cells. Neuron. 2006;52:511–524. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packer O, Verweij J, Schnapf J, Dacey D. Primate S cones have blue-yellow opponent receptive fields. Paper presented at The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology 2007 Annual Meeting; May; Fort Lauderdale, FL. 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Packer OS, Williams DR, Bensinger DG. Photopigment transmittance imaging of the primate photoreceptor mosaic. J Neurosci. 1996;16:2251–2260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-07-02251.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pang JJ, Gao F, Wu SM. Light-evoked excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs to ON and OFF alpha ganglion cells in the mouse retina. J Neurosci. 2003;23:6063–6073. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-14-06063.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrides A, Trexler EB. Differential output of the high-sensitivity rod photoreceptor: AII amacrine pathway. J Comp Neurol. 2008;507:1653–1662. doi: 10.1002/cne.21617. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pokorny J, Smithson H, Quinlan J. Photostimulator allowing independent control of rods and the three cone types. Vis Neurosci. 2004;21:263–267. doi: 10.1017/s0952523804213207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid RC, Shapley RM. Spatial structure of cone inputs to receptive fields in primate lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature. 1992;356:716–718. doi: 10.1038/356716a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reid RC, Shapley RM. Space and time maps of cone photoreceptor signals in macaque lateral geniculate nucleus. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6158–6175. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-06158.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodieck RW. Which cells code for color? In: Valberg A, Lee BB, editors. From pigments to perception. New York: Plenum; 1991. pp. 83–93. [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai M, Mullen KT. Cone weights for the two cone-opponent systems in peripheral vision and asymmetries of cone contrast sensitivity. Vision Res. 2006;46:4346–4354. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.08.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schein S, Sterling P, Ngo IT, Huang TM, Herr S. Evidence that each S cone in macaque fovea drives one narrow-field and several wide-field blue-yellow ganglion cells. J Neurosci. 2004;24:8366–8378. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1063-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schein SJ. Anatomy of macaque fovea and spatial densities of neurons in foveal representation. J Comp Neurol. 1988;269:479–505. doi: 10.1002/cne.902690403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Slaughter MM, Miller RF. 2-Amino-4-phosphonobutyric acid: a new pharmacological tool for retina research. Science. 1981;211:182–185. doi: 10.1126/science.6255566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith VC, Lee BB, Pokorny J, Martin PR, Valberg A. Responses of macaque ganglion cells to the relative phase of heterochromatically modulated lights. J Physiol. 1992;458:191–221. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp019413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon SG, Lennie P. The machinery of colour vision. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:276–286. doi: 10.1038/nrn2094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon SG, Lee BB, White AJ, Rüttiger L, Martin PR. Chromatic organization of ganglion cell receptive fields in the peripheral retina. J Neurosci. 2005;25:4527–4539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3921-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strettoi E, Raviola E, Dacheux RF. Synaptic connections of the narrow-field, bistratified rod amacrine cell (AII) in the rabbit retina. J Comp Neurol. 1992;325:152–168. doi: 10.1002/cne.903250203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tailby C, Solomon SG, Lennie P. Functional asymmetries in visual pathways carrying S-cone signals in macaque. J Neurosci. 2008;28:4078–4087. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5338-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vessey JP, Stratis AK, Daniels BA, Da Silva N, Jonz MG, Lalonde MR, Baldridge WH, Barnes S. Proton-mediated feedback inhibition of presynaptic calcium channels at the cone photoreceptor synapse. J Neurosci. 2005;25:4108–4117. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5253-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Virsu V, Lee BB, Creutzfeldt OD. Mesopic spectral responses and the purkinje shift of macaque lateral geniculate nucleus cells. Vision Res. 1987;27:191–200. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(87)90181-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völgyi B, Deans MR, Paul DL, Bloomfield SA. Convergence and segregation of the multiple rod pathways in mammalian retina. J Neurosci. 2004;24:11182–11192. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3096-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White AJ, Solomon SG, Martin PR. Spatial properties of koniocellular cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. J Physiol. 2001;533:519–535. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0519a.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesel TN, Hubel DH. Spatial and chromatic interactions in the lateral geniculate body of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1966;29:1115–1156. doi: 10.1152/jn.1966.29.6.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yeh T, Lee BB, Kremers J. The temporal response of ganglion cells of the macaque retina to cone-specific modulation. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 1995;12:456–464. doi: 10.1364/josaa.12.000456. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zaghloul KA, Boahen K, Demb JB. Different circuits for ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells cause different contrast sensitivities. J Neurosci. 2003;23:2645, 2654. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-07-02645.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]