Skip to main content
Journal of Pediatric Psychology logoLink to Journal of Pediatric Psychology
. 2008 May 12;33(10):1108–1116. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn046

Chronic Maternal Depression and Children's Injury Risk

David C Schwebel 1,, Carl M Brezausek 2
PMCID: PMC2734122  PMID: 18474518

Abstract

Objective A substantial number of mothers of young children suffer from depression. One understudied consequence of maternal depression is how it affects toddlers’ injury risk. This study examined links between chronic maternal depression and child injury. Methods A national sample of 1,364 American children was studied. Results Chronic levels of severe maternal depression placed children at increased risk of concurrent injury from birth to age 3. The relation between chronic, severe maternal depression and child injury risk held even after controlling for variance from family SES, child sex, child temperament and externalizing behavior, and parenting. Chronic maternal depression during infancy and toddlerhood did not influence children's subsequent risk for injury, between age 3 and first grade. Less severe symptoms of chronic maternal depression were unrelated to concurrent or future child injury. Conclusions Chronic, severe levels of maternal depression are linked to concurrent child injury risk during infancy and toddlerhood.

Keywords: chronic depression, injury, mothers, parents, safety, toddlers


Epidemiological data suggest roughly 15% of women are depressed at any one time (Elgar, McGrath, Waschbusch, Stewart, & Curtis, 2004), and some research suggests that mothers of young children may have rates of depression even higher than the general population of women. In one study of over 800 disadvantaged British mothers of young children, about a third of mothers reported high levels of depressive symptoms (Mulvaney & Kendrick, 2005).

One consequence of maternal depression is its effect on children. There is considerable evidence that maternal depression is linked to children's behavior, with children of depressed mothers demonstrating higher levels of behavior problems and psychopathology; academic problems; social problems and negative peer relationships; and internalizing pathology such as depression, low self-esteem, and poor self-regulation (see Elgar et al., 2004; Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare, & Neuman, 2000; for reviews). The mechanism behind these links is likely bidirectional and via multifaceted mediated pathways, including genetic/biological lines, psychosocial influences, and presence of tangible resources and social capital.

One area that is less well understood is the influence of maternal depression on children's health. In particular, potential links between maternal depression and children's risk of unintentional injury are poorly understood. Injury is the leading cause of pediatric mortality in the US (National Center for Injury Prevention and Control [NCIPC], 2008), and obtaining an accurate picture of behavioral risk factors—including parental traits—that might increase or decrease risk of injury is critical to development of empirically supported prevention programs.

Two previous studies have directly examined links between maternal depression and child injury risk. The first, conducted by O’Connor and colleagues (O’Connor, Davies, Dunn, Golding, & the ALSPAC Study Team, 2000), used a prospective design with over 10,000 English families. The researchers discovered that mother's self-reported symptoms indicative of potential clinical depression, assessed when children were 21 months of age, were related to risk of two or more significant injuries over the subsequent 3 months. The second, by Phelan and colleagues (Phelan, Khoury, Atherton, & Kahn, 2007), studied about 1,100 children and their parents in the US over 2 years. Severe levels of chronic depression, as measured at the start and end of the 2-year period, were related to child injury risk during the second year of the study. This finding was particularly strong among the boys in the sample. Phelan and colleagues (2007) tested child externalizing behavior as a possible mediating variable between maternal depression and child injury risk, but results did not support externalizing behavior as a mediator.

Other research supports the possibility that maternal depression is associated with increased child injury risk. These findings include correlational links between maternal neuroticism and child injury (Davidson, Hughes, & Richards, 1987; Morrongiello, Corbett, McCourt, & Johnston, 2006), between maternal use of antidepressants or tranquilizers and toddler poisoning incidents (Beautrais, Fergusson, & Shannon, 1981), and between maternal anxiety and child injury (Bradbury, Janicke, Riley, & Finney, 1999). Three other reports have investigated how maternal depression might influence mother's behaviors to protect children from injury. Two publications based on data from the National Maternal and Infant Health Survey (NMIHS) indicate mothers who are depressed are less likely to use injury prevention strategies such as installing car seats, covering electrical outlets in the home, and storing syrup of ipecac (Leiferman, 2002; McLennan & Kotelchuck, 2000). Data from a British sample of over 1,700 families contradict this finding, however. Mulvaney and Kendrick (2006) found similar rates of child safety practice (safe storage of medicine, use of smoke alarms, use of fireguards, and safe storage of sharp objects) among depressed and nondepressed mothers.

In summary, previous evidence suggests that there may be links between maternal depression and child injury risk. The present study sought to extend these results by investigating three hypotheses. First, we expected to replicate previous reports by showing chronic levels of severe, clinically relevant, maternal depression are related to young children's risk for injury. Second, we extended previous research by examining moderate levels of subclinical depressive symptomatology. We predicted chronic levels of moderate, subclinical, maternal depression symptoms would also be related to young children's risk for injury. Third, we examined longitudinal influences. We hypothesized that maternal depression during the infancy and toddler years would be related to increased risk of child injury as children begin to make their own decisions about engaging in potentially dangerous situations, from ages 3 through first grade.

Beyond these three primary hypotheses, a secondary objective of this investigation was to conduct initial inquiries into the mechanisms through which chronic maternal depression might influence child injury risk. To do so, we controlled for the variance of possible moderators—child gender, family income, child temperament and externalizing behavior, and parenting characteristics—to determine if the relation between chronic maternal depression and child injury would hold after controlling for those covariates. These covariates were selected because they have been shown to correlate closely to child injury risk (Schwebel & Gaines, 2007) and/or maternal depression (Elgar et al., 2004; Lovejoy et al., 2000) in previous work.

Methods

Data Source

Data came from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care, a longitudinal investigation of the effects of early child care on children's development. Participants were recruited at birth from 31 hospitals located in or near 10 US cities (Little Rock, AR; Irvine, CA; Lawrence, KS; Boston, MA; Morganton, NC; Philadelphia, PA; Pittsburgh, PA; Charlottesville, VA; Seattle, WA; Madison, WI). During selected 24-hr sampling periods, 8,986 women giving birth were visited in the hospital. Of these, 5,416 met the study's eligibility criteria (briefly, English-speaking mothers age 18 or over who planned not to put children up for adoption, not to relocate in the next year, and who lived in safe and accessible neighborhoods for research; for details, see NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2000). A subset of this group was selected in accordance with a conditional-random sampling plan designed to ensure recruited families reflected the economic, educational, and ethnic diversity of the catchment area at each site. When the infants were 1 month old, 1,364 families (58% of those contacted) enrolled in the longitudinal study (see NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1994, 2001, for details of study design and recruitment, including informed consent procedures). The sample included 705 boys (52%) and 659 girls (48%) and was 80% Caucasian, 13% African American, and 7% of other ethnicities. Average education for mothers in the sample was 14.23 years (SD = 2.51).

Measures

Several measures were chosen from the NICHD Early Child Care Study data for the present analysis, as detailed below. Unless otherwise indicated below, the measures were taken when children were 6, 15, 24, and 36 months old, and aggregates were created by averaging those four scores. If constructs were assessed using different scales at different ages, scores were standardized before they were averaged.

Injury

Mothers reported their children's history of injuries requiring professional medical attention on a quarterly basis from birth to first grade (biannually during the kindergarten and first grade years), either through telephone interviews or during scheduled home visits by experimenters. A large body of empirical research indicates that parental reports of child injury history are reliable (Pless & Pless, 1995), particularly when reports are collected at relatively short intervals to reduce recall biases (Cummings, Rivara, Thompson, & Reid, 2005; Harel et al., 1994; Peterson, Harbeck, & Moreno, 1993).

Two measures were computed from the mothers’ reports: number of injuries children incurred from birth through their third birthday, and number of injuries from their third birthday through the end of first grade (approximately age 6).

Maternal depression

Maternal depression was measured using the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977), a self-report scale designed to assess depression in nonclinical populations. The measure was assessed at all four time points; the average intercorrelation across time was r =.52. The CES-D includes a rating of the frequency of 20 depressive symptoms over the past week using a 4-point scale; higher scores represent higher levels of depression. Psychometrics are good (Cronbach's α =.89,.90,.91, and.90 at the four assessment points, respectively; Bilbrey, Batten, Appelbaum, & Wendell, 1994b; Coleman, Batten, Appelbaum, & Wendell, 1995a, 1995b; Little, Appelbaum, Batten, & Wendell, 1993b). Two measures were taken from the CES-D based on standard cutoffs in the literature (McCaffery, Niaura, Swan, & Carmelli, 2003; Santor, Zuroff, Ramsay, Cervantes, & Palacios, 1995): (a) a rating of severe depression with scores on the CES-D above 16 at all four measurement points, and (b) a rating of moderate depression with scores on the CES-D above 8 at all four measurement points. Those mothers with severe depression were excluded from the moderate depression group for analyses. The severe depression score is consistent with chronic levels of clinical depression; the moderate depression score is consistent with chronic levels of subclinical depression.

Family SES

The SES aggregate was created by averaging the family's income-to-needs ratio when the child was 6, 15, 24, and 36 months old (average inter-correlation =.80). The income-to-needs ratio calculated income based on the entire income of all members of the family living in the same household and estimated needs based on the poverty threshold during the year of measurement, the number of people in the household, and the number of children in the household (see Bilbrey, Batten, Appelbaum, & Wendell, 1994a, for details). An income-to-needs ratio that is less than 1 reflects significant poverty; the mean (3.56) and standard deviation (2.97) of this sample reflect a group of families that is generally middle and upper class, but includes some impoverished families.

Child positive affect

Positive affect was assessed via a semi-structured mother–child play interaction adapted from Egeland and Heister (1993). Positive affect (6, 15, and 24 months) and enthusiasm (36 months) were rated on a Likert-style rating scale by two raters (4-point scale used at 6, 15, and 24 months; 7-point scale used at 36 months). Adequate inter-rater reliability was obtained between independent coders at all four ages (Appelbaum, Batten, & Wendell, 1994; Batten & Wendell, 1994; Bland, Batten, Appelbaum, & Wendell, 1995, 1996a). The four scores were standardized and then aggregated; the average inter-correlation was r =.10.

Child negative affect

Negative affect was assessed via the same semi-structured mother–child play interaction as positive affect. Negative affect (6, 15, and 24 months) and negativity (36 months) were rated on a Likert-style rating scale by two raters (4-point scale used at 6, 15, and 24 months; 7-point scale used at 36 months). Adequate inter-rater reliability was obtained between independent coders at all four ages (Appelbaum et al., 1994; Batten & Wendell, 1994; Bland et al., 1995, 1996a). The four scores were standardized and then aggregated; the average inter-correlation was r =.12.

Child externalizing behavior

Externalizing behavior was measured by summing the aggressive and destructive behavior scales of mother-reported Achenbach's Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1992) at 24 and 36 months. The two scores correlated, r =.71. No assessment of externalizing behavior was made at 6 or 15 months. The CBCL is a widely used measure with excellent psychometric properties (test–retest reliability and Cronbach's α for internal reliability of both scales >.70; Achenbach, 1992).

Parenting

The total score of the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME; Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) was used as a measure of quality and style of caregiving. The infant/toddler version (45 items) was used at 6 and 15 months and the early childhood version (55 items) at 36 months; the three measures had an average inter-correlation of r =.55. No assessment was made at 24 months. Items in both versions address a wide range of topics related to the home environment, including language and academic stimulation available in the home, responsiveness, acceptance, enrichment, and modeling by the caregiver, and variety and safety of the physical environment. Items are scored in a binary fashion, and the total number of “yes” responses in the environment is summed to create a score. Psychometric qualities of the measure are good (Cronbach's α =.76 at 6 months,.77 at 15 months and.87 at 36 months; Bland, Batten, Appelbaum, & Wendell, 1996b; Little, Appelbaum, Batten, & Wendell, 1993a, 1994).

Missing Data

There are missing data points in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care data set. In many cases, data are missing due to attrition; in other cases, data are missing due to failure of parents to complete particular measures used in this investigation. Of the 1,364 families who took part in the NICHD Study of Early Child Care, 1,215 had full data available for the analyses conducted. Missing data for the remaining 149 were imputed using Markov Chain Monte Carlo multiple imputation strategies for arbitrary missing data, assuming the data were missing at random (Cameron & Trivedi, 2005).

Analytic Plan

Following consideration of descriptive data, data were analyzed in three steps. First, univariate Poisson regression models were computed to predict concurrent injuries, from birth through 36 months, based on presence or absence of chronic maternal depression during that concurrent time period. One model used severe levels of chronic depression as a predictor variable (with CES-D scores >16, or in the clinical range) and the other used moderate levels of chronic depression as a predictor variable (with CES-D scores >8, or in the subclinical range). Second, potential confounds to the relation between maternal depression and child injury were considered individually as covariates in multivariate Poisson regression models that also included maternal depression. These covariates included demographics (sex, income), child characteristics (positive affect, negative affect, and externalizing behavior), and parenting. Again, separate models were constructed with severe and moderate levels of depression included. Models were also constructed with all covariates entered simultaneously to determine if depression remained a predictor of child injury even after controlling for all other covariates. Third, the same set of univariate and multivariate Poisson regression models were constructed predicting future injuries, from age 3 to when the child was in first grade, based on parent and child characteristics during the toddler years.

Results

Table I presents descriptive data from the sample. As is commonly found in the injury literature, injury events were relatively infrequent. Children experienced a mean of 0.20 injuries (SD = 0.53; range = 0–4) over the 3-year period of birth to age 3 and a mean of 0.56 (SD = 0.80; range = 0–5) injuries over the 3-year period of age 3 to grade 1. Chronic maternal depression was also rare in the sample, with 2.49% of mothers reporting severe, clinical-level depressive symptoms at all four data points and 15.51% of mothers reporting moderate, subclinical depressive symptoms at all four data points.

Table I.

Descriptive Data (N = 1364)

Variable Mean (SD)
Demographic variables
    Sex 51.69% male
    Income: needs ratio 3.56 (2.97)
Mother variables
    Parenting −0.03 (0.87)
    Chronic severe depression 2.49% of sample
    Chronic moderate depression 15.51% of sample
Child variables
    Positive affect −0.01 (0.62)
    Negative affect −0.00 (0.61)
    Externalizing behavior 14.10 (6.69)
    Injuries, birth to age 3 0.20 (0.53)
    Injuries, age 3 to grade 1 0.56 (0.80)

The parenting and affect scores reflect aggregates of standardized values.

We also examined descriptive injury data within the groups of mothers with and without depression. There were 34 mothers with severe levels of chronic depression and 178 with moderate levels of chronic depression. While children developed from birth to age 3, the sample of mothers with severe depression had children with an injury rate (M = 0.56, SD = 0.85, range = 0–3) almost three times that of the sample whose mothers were not chronically depressed (M = 0.19, SD = 0.52, range = 0–4) and that whose mothers had moderate levels of chronic depression (M = 0.20, SD = 0.51, range = 0–3). Injury rates while children developed from age 3 to first grade were more similar across groups: children of mothers without chronic depression had a mean of 0.56 injuries (SD = 0.80, range = 0–4); children of mothers with moderate levels of chronic depression had a mean of 0.58 (SD = 0.78, range = 0–5); and children of mothers with severe levels of chronic depression had a mean of 0.68 (SD = 0.72, range = 0–2).

Table II presents data concerning the effect of chronic maternal depression on child injury. The first line illustrates univariate models; as shown, severe maternal depression was a strong predictor of concurrent child injury risk (t = 4.52, p <.01;.95 CI = 0.61, 1.54). After including various covariates in the model, the effect of severe levels of depression remained statistically significant and strong (lines 2–5, columns 1–4). The effect also remained strong and statistically significant after all covariates were included in the model (t = 4.31, p <.01;.95 CI = 0.58, 1.54). Thus, chronic maternal depression appears to be a robust predictor of child injury risk, even after controlling for potential covarying effects.

Table II.

Descriptive Data and Poisson Models, Maternal Depression Predicting Concurrent Injuries

Chronic severe depression
Chronic moderate depression
B SE t 95% CI B SE t 95% CI
Depressed, univariate model 1.08 0.24 4.52** 0.61, 1.54 0.07 0.19 0.38 −0.30, 0.45
Model controlling for demographicsa 1.04 0.24 4.31** 0.57, 1.50 0.04 0.19 0.20 −0.34, 0.41
Model controlling for child variablesb 1.09 0.24 4.47** 0.61, 1.57 0.06 0.20 0.28 −0.34, 0.46
Model controlling for parenting 1.10 0.24 4.62** 0.64, 1.57 0.13 0.19 0.65 −0.26, 0.52
Model controlling for all covariates 1.06 0.25 4.31** 0.58, 1.54 0.07 0.20 0.36 −0.33, 0.47

aDemographic covariates: child sex and family income.

bChild covariates: positive affect, negative affect and externalizing behavior.

*p <.05; **p <.01.

Corresponding data concerning moderate levels of maternal depression are presented on the latter columns of Table II (children whose mothers had severe levels of maternal depression were omitted from this analysis). As shown, moderate levels of depression did not predict child injury risk in univariate or multivariate models.

Table III replicates Table II, but uses longitudinal modeling to predict injuries children experienced between age 3 and first grade based on child and parent traits from birth to age 3. None of the models emerged as statistically significant.

Table III.

Descriptive Data and Poisson Models, Maternal Depression Predicting Future Injuries

Chronic severe depression
Chronic moderate depression
B SE t 95% CI B SE t 95% CI
Depressed, univariate model 0.19 0.21 0.91 −0.22, 0.61 0.04 0.11 0.36 −0.18, 0.26
Model controlling for demographicsa 0.19 0.21 0.90 −0.23, 0.61 0.03 0.12 0.28 −0.19, 0.26
Model controlling for child variablesb 0.21 0.22 0.98 −0.21, 0.63 0.04 0.12 0.32 −0.19, 0.27
Model controlling for parenting 0.20 0.21 0.94 −0.22, 0.62 0.05 0.12 0.44 −0.18, 0.28
Model controlling for all covariates 0.22 0.22 1.01 −0.21, 0.65 0.05 0.12 0.40 −0.19, 0.29

aDemographic covariates: child sex and family income.

bChild covariates: positive affect, negative affect and externalizing behavior.

Discussion

Replicating previous work (O’Connor et al., 2000; Phelan et al., 2007), results from this study suggest chronic levels of severe maternal depression are related to increased risk of injury among infants and toddlers. This relation remained true even after controlling for variance based on family SES, child sex, child temperament and externalizing behavior, and parenting strategies.

Contrary to our second hypothesis, more moderate, subclinical levels of maternal depression were not related to increased child injury risk. It appears that only chronic and fairly severe maternal depression causes significant increased risk of child injury. And contrary to our third hypothesis, chronic severe maternal depression during the children's infancy and toddler years was not related to increased injury risk as children developed subsequently (from age 3 through grade 1).

As is the case in many novel areas of inquiry, these findings raise as many questions as they answer. In particular, they fail to address what mechanism or mechanisms may be behind a link between maternal depression and child injury risk. The facts that severe but not moderate levels of maternal depression predicted concurrent risk of child injury (ages 0–3) but not future risk of injury (ages 3 to grade 1) are highly consistent with the possibility that parent-related characteristics are particularly important to child safety during the infancy and toddlerhood years, but less so as children develop (Morrongiello, 2005).

Thus, one likely mechanism for the link between maternal depression and young children's injury risk is that chronically depressed mothers do not appropriately safeguard the physical environments children engage in. Results from two studies, both using the same dataset, support this possibility (Leiferman, 2002; McLennan & Kotelchuck, 2000; but see Mulvaney & Kendrick, 2006, for contradicting results). Less severely depressed mothers may have time periods when they are healthy enough to provide basic safeguards in the home environment (e.g., stair gates, outlet covers), and thus it is only chronic and severe maternal depression that increases child injury risk. Similarly, as children mature to the age when they are not supervised 100% of the time, and therefore make some independent decisions about engaging in risky activities, they may be less affected by the influence of parents who have or have not safeguarded the physical environments they engage within.

A second plausible mechanism behind the link between maternal depression and child injury risk has to due to with parental supervision rather than environmental safeguarding. Symptoms of depression include inattention, poor concentration, distractability, and irritability. These symptoms, which are generally more pronounced and serious in mothers with chronic and severe depression, might lead to poor or inconsistent supervision and enforcement of safety-related rules. Again, as children develop greater independence and require less intensive supervision in the late preschool and early school years, inadequate maternal supervision may be less critical for child safety.

Chronic maternal depression could also lead to increased child injury risk through less proximal mechanisms. These mechanisms might include reduced financial or social resources; the passing of genetic or biological traits that influence child behavior, risk-taking, or decision-making when confronted with dangers; and reduced tendency to model safe behavior from peers or other adults. All of these symptoms will be more salient in mothers with more serious and chronic depression. Finally, the issue of causality must not be overlooked. It is possible that repeated child injuries might play a role in development or maintenance of maternal depression rather than vice versa.

Limitations and Implications

This study contributes to an understudied area of public health concern, but its limitations should be considered along with its contributions. The most significant limitations are related to use of archival data. Measures of potential mediating variables of interest—for example, parental supervision strategies and safeguarding practices—were unavailable. Also limiting was the sample size. Although large, it did not permit sufficient power and frequency of injury to consider adequately the effect of possible moderating or mediating effects.

Despite these limitations, the study has both applied and scientific implications. From an applied perspective, the findings highlight the importance of treatment for chronic severe maternal depression. Only a small portion of mothers in the sample met the strict criteria for chronic, severe mental illness (2.49%); this is a set of women with severe and chronic mental illness. There have been multiple cries for improved treatment of chronic and severe mental illness in our society (Arnow & Constantino, 2003; Smith, Schwebel, Dunn, & McIver, 1993). In particular, researchers have called for increased awareness and treatment of postpartum depression, which sometimes leads to more chronic and severe maternal depression (Fawcett, 2005; Logsdon, Wisner, Billings, & Shanahan, 2006). There is ample evidence that chronic depression is a treatable condition; the present findings suggest that proper treatment will improve not only the mothers’ health, but also could address health risks in their children as well.

The results also have multiple implications for future empirical investigation. First, more fine-grained longitudinal issues should be considered. Are there ages when young children are at particular risk for injury if parented by a mother with depression? How does “typical” post-partum depression, which usually wanes as the child approaches the 1-year mark (Wisner, Parry, & Piontek, 2002), affect children's risk for injury, both over the first year and subsequently? Research should also address different stages of development. Does maternal depression influence children's risk for injury during the later years of childhood, or during adolescence? Children make more independent risk-taking decisions at these stages, and suffer more injuries in locations outside the home, but maternal depression might still influence their behavior in as-yet-undiscovered ways.

Future work should also consider the environment children are injured within. As toddlers enter day-care centers and other nonhome environments, does maternal depression influence their risk of injury outside the home, or is the finding reported in this article driven only by increased risk of injury in the home? Another area in need of further inquiry is the role of moderating and mediating factors. Our results suggest several of the more “obvious” covariates were not related to the link between maternal depression and child injury. Phelan and colleagues (2007) reported null results from a test of child externalizing as a mediator. Future work should try to uncover what mechanism or mechanisms might be responsible for the link between maternal depression and child injury.

Finally, more careful consideration of maternal depression should be made. Some individuals with depression have excessive irritability and anger. One might imagine these symptoms would influence child injury risk in a way far different than maternal depression marked by symptoms of fatigue and a vegetative state.

Acknowledgments

This study was conducted by the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network supported by NICHD through a cooperative agreement that calls for scientific collaboration between the grantees and the NICHD staff. We thank the NICHD team for sharing their data. The secondary analyses were supported by funds from the UAB School of Social and Behavioral Sciences and Department of Psychology (D.C.S.) and from the Cecil J. Picard Center for Child Development at the University of Louisiana at Lafayette (C.M.B.).

Conflicts of interest: None declared.

References

  1. Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist / 2-3 and 1992 Profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  2. Appelbaum M, Batten DA, Wendell C. Child behaviors with mother and at child care, six months: Child care data report 23. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  3. Arnow BA, Constantino MJ. Effectiveness of psychotherapy and combination treatment for chronic depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2003;59:893–905. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Batten DA, Wendell C. Quality of maternal care, reliability of the child variables of the structured interaction, 15 month home visit – form 15P: Child care data report addendum 43a. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  5. Beautrais AL, Fergusson DM, Shannon FT. Accidental poisoning in the first three years of life. Australian Paediatric Journal. 1981;17:104–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1981.tb01915.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bilbrey C, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Income pre-birth through fifteen months: Child care data report 53. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1994a. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bilbrey C, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Maternal depression: Fifteen month child care pod data: Child care data report-42. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1994b. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bland S, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Child behaviors with mother at twenty-four months: Child care data report 77. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bland S, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Quality of maternal care and child behaviors with mother: Rating of mother and child in a structured play situation: 36-month home visit – form 38p, family and child outcomes PODS: Child care data report 136. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1996a. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bland S, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. The 36-month early childhood home inventory: Child care data report 100. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1996b. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bradbury K, Janicke DM, Riley AW, Finney JW. Predictors of unintentional injuries to school-age children seen in primary care. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1999;24:423–433. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/24.5.423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home observation for measurement of the environment. Little Rock, AR: University of Arkansas at Little Rock; 1984. [Google Scholar]
  13. Cameron AC, Trivedi PK. Microeconometrics: Methods and applications. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  14. Coleman S, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Maternal depression at 36 months: Child care data report-91. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1995a. [Google Scholar]
  15. Coleman S, Batten DA, Appelbaum M, Wendell C. Parenting stress at 36 months: Child care data report-90. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1995b. [Google Scholar]
  16. Cummings P, Rivara FP, Thompson RS, Reid RJ. Ability of parents to recall the injuries of their young children. Injury Prevention. 2005;11:43–47. doi: 10.1136/ip.2004.006833. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Davidson LL, Hughes SJ, Richards M. Maternal personality and injury in children: Is stability a protective factor? Personality and Individual Differences. 1987;8:961–963. [Google Scholar]
  18. Egeland B. Teaching task rating scales. University of Minnesota: Unpublished manuscript, Institute of Child Development; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  19. Elgar FJ, McGrath PJ, Waschbusch DA, Stewart SH, Curtis LJ. Mutual influences on maternal depression and child adjustment problems. Clinical Psychology Review. 2004;24:441–459. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fawcett J. Editorial: Postpartum depression: A problem that needs our attention. Psychiatric Annals. 2005;35:529. [Google Scholar]
  21. Harel Y, Overpeck MD, Jones DH, Scheidt PC, Bijur PE, Trumble AC, et al. The effects of recall on estimating annual nonfatal injury rates for children and adolescents. American Journal of Public Health. 1994;84:599–605. doi: 10.2105/ajph.84.4.599. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Leiferman J. The effect of maternal depressive symptomatology on maternal behaviors associated with child health. Heath Education and Behavior. 2002;29:596–607. doi: 10.1177/109019802237027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Little S, Appelbaum M, Batten DA, Wendell C. The infant/toddler H.O.M.E., psychometric analysis, six month family pod data: Child care data report 25. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1993a. [Google Scholar]
  24. Little S, Appelbaum M, Batten DA, Wendell C. Maternal depression and personality psychometric analysis, one and six month child care pod data: Child care data report 26. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1993b. [Google Scholar]
  25. Little S, Appelbaum M, Batten DA, Wendell C. The infant/toddler H.O.M.E., psychometric analysis, fifteen month family pod data: Child care data report 37. Research Triangle Park, NC: NICHD Early Child Care Study; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  26. Logsdon MC, Wisner K, Billings DM, Shanahan B. Raising the awareness of primary care providers about postpartum depression. Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 2006;27:59–73. doi: 10.1080/01612840500312860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lovejoy MC, Graczyk PA, O’Hare E, Neuman G. Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2000;20:561–592. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(98)00100-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McCaffery JM, Niaura R, Swan GE, Carmelli D. A study of depressive symptoms and smoking behavior in adult male twins from the NHLBI twin study. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2003;5:77–83. doi: 10.1080/14622200307259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. McLennan JD, Kotelchuck M. Parental prevention practices for young children in the context of maternal depression. Pediatrics. 2000;105:1090–1095. doi: 10.1542/peds.105.5.1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Morrongiello BA. Caregiver supervision and child-injury risk: I. Issues in defining and measuring supervision; II. Findings and directions for future research. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2005;30:536–552. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsi041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Morrongiello BA, Corbett M, McCourt M, Johnston N. Understanding unintentional injury risk in young children II. The contribution of caregiver supervision, child attributes, and parent attributes. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2006;31:540–551. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Mulvaney C, Kendrick D. Depressive symptoms in mothers of pre-school children – effects of deprivation, social supports, stress, and neighbourhood social capital. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2005;40:202–208. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0859-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mulvaney C. Do maternal depressive symptoms, stress, and a lack of social support influence whether mothers living in deprived circumstances adopt safety practices for the prevention of childhood injury? Child: Care, Health & Development. 2006;32:311–319. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2006.00590.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control [NCIPC] WISQARS™ (Web-based injury statistics query and reporting system). 2008. Retrieved March 13, 2008, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncipc/wisqars/
  35. NICHD Early Child Care Network. Child care and child development: The NICHD Study of Early Child Care. In: Friedman SL, Haywood HC, editors. Developmental follow-up: Concepts, domains and methods. New York: Academic; 1994. pp. 377–396. [Google Scholar]
  36. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. The interaction of child care and family risk in relation to child development at 24 and 36 months. Applied Developmental Science. 2000;6:144–156. [Google Scholar]
  37. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Child care and children's peer interaction at 24 and 36 months: The NICHD Study of Early Child Care. Child Development. 2001;72:1478–1500. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. O’Connor TG, Davies L, Dunn J, Golding J The ALSPAC Study Team. Distribution of accidents, injuries, and illnesses by family type. Pediatrics. 2000;106:e68–e73. doi: 10.1542/peds.106.5.e68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Peterson L, Harbeck C, Moreno A. Measures of children's injuries: Self-reported versus maternal-reported events with temporally proximal versus delayed reporting. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 1993;18:133–147. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/18.1.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Phelan K, Khoury J, Atherton H, Kahn RS. Maternal depression, child behavior, and injury. Injury Prevention. 2007;13:403–408. doi: 10.1136/ip.2006.014571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Pless CE, Pless IB. How well they remember: The accuracy of parent reports. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine. 1995;149:553–558. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.1995.02170180083016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
  43. Santor DA, Zuroff DC, Ramsay JO, Cervantes P, Palacios J. Examining scale disciminability in the BDI and CES-D as a function of depressive severity. Psychological Assessment. 1995;7:131–139. [Google Scholar]
  44. Schwebel DC, Gaines J. Pediatric unintentional injury: Behavioral risk factors and implications for prevention. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 2007;28:245–254. doi: 10.1097/01.DBP.0000268561.80204.2a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Smith GB, Schwebel AI, Dunn RL, McIver SD. The role of psychologists in the treatment, management, and prevention of chronic mental illness. American Psychologist. 1993;48:966–971. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.9.966. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wisner KL, Parry BL, Piontek CM. Postpartum depression. New England Journal of Medicine. 2002;347:194–199. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp011542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Pediatric Psychology are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES