Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Sep 9.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006 Apr;14(4):307–315. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000196637.95869.d9

Childhood Adversity and Psychosocial Adjustment in Old Age

Robert S Wilson 1, Kristin R Krueger 1, Steven E Arnold 1, Lisa L Barnes 1, Carlos F Mendes de Leon 1, Julia L Bienias 1, David A Bennett 1
PMCID: PMC2740729  NIHMSID: NIHMS133744  PMID: 16582039

Abstract

Objective

The objective of this study was to test the hypothesis that higher level of childhood adversity is associated with lower level of psychosocial adjustment in old age.

Method

Participants are 253 older persons (mean age: 79.8, standard deviation: 2.1; 73.9% women) from the Rush Memory and Aging Project, a clinicopathologic study of common chronic conditions of old age. Childhood adversity was assessed with a 16-item measure based on portions of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire and other inventories. Psychosocial adjustment was assessed with multiple measures of neuroticism (i.e., proneness to negative emotions) and social engagement.

Results

Based in part on a factor analysis, composite measures of total adversity, emotional neglect, parental intimidation, parental violence, family turmoil, and financial need were developed. In a series of linear regression models adjusted for age, sex, own education, and parental education, the composite measure of childhood adversity was associated with all facets of neuroticism and accounted for more than 13% of the variance in the composite neuroticism measure. Emotional neglect and parental intimidation had the strongest associations with neuroticism. Childhood adversity was not related to frequency of participation in social activities, but more adversity was associated with having a smaller network (accounting for 3% of the variance) and feeling more emotionally isolated (accounting for 7% of the variance).

Conclusion

Childhood adversity is associated with less adaptive psychosocial functioning in old age.

Keywords: Childhood Adversity, aging, neuroticism, social engagement


Maltreatment, neglect, and other adverse experiences commonly occur during childhood.13 Among adults, a history of adverse childhood experience has been shown to be associated with diverse psychologic problems, including interpersonal difficulties,4,5 personality disorders,6,7 depression,8,9 alcohol and substance abuse,7,8 and suicidal behavior,10,11 as well as poor physical health.12,13 By contrast, knowledge about the relation of traumatic experiences during childhood to mental health in old age is limited. Most research has focused on parental emotional support, with lower levels of support associated with more physical and emotional problems in older persons.1416 In addition, loss of a parent in childhood has been related to impaired physical health, but not disability, in old age.17 Thus, the extent to which the deleterious effects of childhood adversity persist into old age is not well understood.

In the present study, we examined the relation of childhood adversity to psychosocial adjustment in older persons. Participants are from the Rush Memory and Aging Project, a clinicopathologic study of chronic conditions of old age. We developed from existing questionnaires a self-report measure of adverse childhood experiences, including emotional neglect and parental violence and intimidation. Psychosocial adjustment was assessed with multiple measures of tendencies to experience negative emotional states and to be socially engaged. We hypothesized that level of childhood adversity would be inversely related to level of psychosocial adjustment in old age.

METHODS

Participants

All persons in the study are participants in the Rush Memory and Aging Project, a longitudinal clinicopathologic study of neurobiologic factors linking experiential and genetic risk factors to common chronic health problems in old age.1820 They were primarily recruited from continuous care retirement communities and subsidized housing facilities in and around Chicago. Persons were also recruited from Section 8 and Section 202 housing subsidized by the Department of Housing and Urban Development, retirement homes, and local churches and other social service agencies. The project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Rush University Medical Center.

The recruitment process began with discussions with administrators and staff at potential facilities and in some cases included a meeting with resident councils. These meetings were typically followed by a 60- to 90-minute presentation to interested residents. The presentation covered general information about aging and the need for prospective clinical–pathologic research. Persons attending the presentation were provided with a packet of information about the study and encouraged to discuss it with family members and friends. Attendees were asked to complete a form indicating their level of interest in learning more about the study. Those expressing interest were contacted later by study personnel, and after further discussion, informed consent was obtained and the Anatomical Gift Act was signed.

Only persons without dementia were eligible for analyses. As part of the project, each participant had a uniform structured clinical evaluation that included a medical history, neuropsychologic testing, and a complete neurologic examination. On the basis of this evaluation, an experienced physician diagnosed dementia according to the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association criteria, which require a history of cognitive decline and evidence of impairment in two or more domains of cognitive function.21 Of 297 people who completed the questionnaire on adverse childhood experiences, 44 met criteria for dementia and were excluded. Analyses are based on the remaining 253 people. They had a mean age of 79.8 (standard deviation [SD]: 6.4) and a mean Mini-Mental State Examination score of 28.0 (SD: 2.1); 73.9% were women and 94.9% were white and non-Hispanic. They had completed a mean of 14.6 years of education (SD: 3.0) and reported a mean of 9.8 years of education (SD: 3.8) for their parents.

Assessment of Adverse Childhood Experiences

We constructed a 16-item questionnaire, based mainly on material from the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire22 and other structured questionnaires,9,23 focusing on emotional and physical trauma. We excluded the topic of sexual abuse to enhance the acceptability of the inventory with older participants and simplified item wording to reduce response burden. All questions addressed experiences during the first 18 years of life. Five items were rated on a four-point scale; nine items were rated on a five-point scale. Scores on the remaining two items were based on a count of family problems (i.e., depression, suicide, alcoholism, frightening experience) and family separations (i.e., divorce, separated from mother, hospitalization, prison).

Assessment of Psychosocial Adjustment

We assessed two key components of psychosocial adjustment in old age: the tendency to experience negative emotions and the extent of social engagement.

Negative Emotions

The personality trait of neuroticism is a stable indicator of proneness to experience a range of negative affect. We assessed neuroticism with the 48-item Neuroticism scale from the NEO PI-R.24 In addition to its sound and well-documented psychometric properties, a particular advantage of this scale is that it yields scores on six different components of neuroticism (each based on eight items): anxiety, angry hostility, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, and vulnerability. Persons rated their level of agreement with each item on a five-point scale (0−4), with higher scores indicating a higher level of the trait. Item scores were summed to yield a total neuroticism score (range: 0−192) and scores for each of the six components (range: 0−32).

Social Engagement

We collected three indicators of social engagement: social network size, frequency of social activity, and level of emotional isolation. To quantify social networks, we asked standard questions25 about the number of children, family, and friends each person had and about how frequently they interacted with them. The social network score was the total number of such persons seen at least once a month.

Persons were asked to rate how often they participated in six common social activities (e.g., visiting a relative or friend, attending religious services) on a five-point scale, with five indicating every day or about every day and one indicating once a year or less.26 Item scores were averaged to yield a composite measure of social activity frequency ranging from one to five with higher scores indicating more frequent social activity. Cronbach alpha coefficient alpha was 0.63, indicating a relatively low level of internal consistency.

Emotional isolation was assessed with a five-item scale.27 Persons rated level of agreement with each item (e.g., “I miss having people around”) on a five-point scale. Item scores were averaged an emotional isolation score ranging from one to five, with higher scores indicating more isolation. Cornbach's coefficient alpha was 0.78 indicating a moderate level of internal consistency.

Data Analysis

We used a factor analysis to determine the best number of factors in the childhood adversity scale. We first used principal-components analysis to fit an orthogonal set of factors: five factors had eigenvalues of one or more, representing the most significant determinants of variance. We then conducted a factor analysis with varimax rotation, specifying five factors. Summary measures representing each factor were then computed by adding the raw scores of items with loadings of 0.50 or more on a given factor, resulting in five summary measures. We tested the association of the childhood experience measures with facets of neuroticism and with social engagement measures in a series of linear regression models that controlled for the potentially confounding effects of age, sex, the person's own education, and the mean educational attainment of the participant's mother and father. In these analyses, all variables were simultaneously entered. Because of the sizable number of regression analyses, we set the alpha level at 0.01. Models were validated graphically and analytically. Programming was done in SAS.28

RESULTS

Development of Summary Measures of Childhood Adversity

As shown in Table 1, adverse childhood experiences were not uncommon in this cohort, with 4% (adults exchanged blows, wore dirty clothes) to 40% (adults argued) obtaining scores of two or more on each item, indicative of at least some adversity. To develop summary measures from the scale, we submitted the 16 items to a principal-components analysis to determine the number of factors. This yielded five factors with eigenvalues greater than one which accounted for 66.3% of the variance in the scale. We then performed a factor analysis with varimax rotation on the five factors (Table 1). We labeled the rotated factors as emotional neglect (six items), parental intimidation (four items), parental violence (two items), family turmoil (two items), and financial need (two items). We grouped items into the domains suggested by the factor analysis and added the raw item scores to create summary measures of specific experiences (Table 2). Because the summary measures were moderately intercorrelated (median r=0.25), we also created a composite measure of adverse experience based on all 16 items (Table 2). Less than 5% of the cohort had a score of zero on the composite measure of adverse experience, indicating that most people had experienced at least some adversity in childhood. As shown in Table 2, the composite measure had a roughly symmetric distribution, and its moderate level of internal consistency, as shown by a coefficient alpha of 0.84, supports the idea that the diverse experiences could be adequately summarized in a composite measure. The specific measures with more than two items also had adequate levels of internal consistency (Table 2). Overall, neither the composite nor specific measures of adversity were strongly related to age, and there was some evidence of more adversity in men and those with less education.

TABLE 1.

Psychometric Information on the Modified Childhood Trauma Questionnaire

Distribution of Item Scores* Rotated Factor Loading
Abbreviated item 0 1 >2 1 2 3 4 5
Helped you feel important 43 28 27 0.78 −0.10 0.22 0.10 −0.02
Felt loved 57 25 19 0.83 0.18 0.09 0.15 0.08
Family felt close 43 36 22 0.81 0.14 0.18 0.05 0.12
Family supportive 53 28 19 0.84 0.23 0.10 0.12 0.06
Care/protect you 74 17 9 0.79 0.17 −0.02 0.21 0.10
Take you to doctor 73 11 16 0.63 0.27 −0.17 −0.07 0.27
Said hurtful things 58 23 19 0.23 0.71 0.02 0.23 0.10
Made you afraid 85 9 7 0.19 0.75 0.06 0.14 −0.08
Pushed/grabbed you 77 16 7 0.14 0.75 0.33 −0.03 0.13
Adults argued 19 41 40 0.04 0.52 0.48 0.01 −0.05
Punished you with a belt 63 23 13 0.14 0.18 0.65 0.13 0.27
Adults exchanged blows 91 5 4 0.12 0.11 0.75 0.05 −0.06
Family problems 69 21 9 0.10 0.28 0.11 0.79 −0.03
Family separation 60 30 10 0.27 0.00 0.05 0.80 0.12
Not enough to eat 69 12 19 −0.07 0.03 −0.04 −0.02 0.79
Wore dirty clothes 79 17 4 0.13 0.02 0.15 0.11 0.77
Variance explained
24.6 14.0 9.4 9.2 9.1
*

Percent obtaining each item score with higher scores denoting more adversity.

From a principal-components factor analysis; loadings of 0.50 or greater are in bold.

The communality for each rotated factor divided by the number of items on the entire questionnaire.

TABLE 2.

Summary Measures of Adverse Childhood Experience

Characteristic Emotional Neglect Parental Intimidation Parental Violence Family Turmoil Financial Need Composite Adversity
Number of items 6 4 2 2 2 6
Range 0−18 0−12 0−7 0−6 0−6 0−31
Mean (SD) 3.9 (4.2) 2.5 (2.3) 0.7 (1.0) 1.0 (1.3) 0.9 (1.5) 9.0 (6.8)
Skewness 1.1 1.4 2.2 1.7 1.5 1.1
Coefficient alpha 0.89 0.74 0.84
Correlations*
    Age −0.10 −0.08 −0.07 −0.13 0.01 −0.12
    Education −0.19 0.00 −0.20 −0.04 −0.04 −0.16
Sex differences
    t 0.10 −0.68 −2.05 −2.00 −2.27 −1.51
    df 251 140 90.8 251 251 147
    p
0.920 0.496 0.043 0.047 0.024 0.133
*

p <0.05 for correlations with an absolute value of .13 or more based on t-tests with 251 degrees of freedom.

SD: standard deviation.

To evaluate the temporal stability of the scale, we readministered it to a subset of 50 participants after a mean of 23.8 weeks (SD: 16.9; range: 1.1−66.4). They had a mean age of 81.5 (SD: 7.2), a mean of 14.2 years of education (SD: 2.7), and a mean Mini-Mental State Examination score of 28.4 (SD: 1.9); 80.0% were women and 94.0% were white and non-Hispanic. The test–retest correlation was 0.92 (t[48]=16.3, p <0.001) for the composite measure of childhood adversity, indicating excellent temporal stability. The subscores, which had missing data for one or two persons, had lower test–retest correlations, reflecting the brevity of the scales: r=0.93 for emotional neglect (t[46]=17.1, p <0.001), r=0.79 for parental intimidation (t[47]=8.9, p <0.001), r=0.77 for parental violence (t[46]=8.2, p <0.001), r=0.84 for family turmoil (t[46]=10.6, p <0.001), and r=0.65 for financial need (t[47]=6.0, p <0.001).

Adverse Childhood Experience and Negative Emotions in Late Life

We tested the relation of adverse childhood experience to neuroticism in a series of linear regression models that controlled for the potentially confounding effects of age, sex, education, and education of the parents (Table 3). In the initial analysis, we used the composite measures of neuroticism (mean: 70.8, SD: 19.0, range: 1−132) and adverse childhood experience to make use of all available data. After adjustment for the covariates, the composite measure of adverse experience accounted for an additional 13.4% of the variance in neuroticism (estimated slope: 1.05, standard error [SE]: 0.17, t[245]=6.47, p <0.001). With each one-point increase on the adverse childhood experiences scale, neuroticism score increased by 1.05 points. When the analysis was repeated for each facet of neuroticism, the composite measure of adverse experiences accounted for between 4% (anxiety) and 10% (angry hostility, depression) of the variance in the facets (all p <0.001).

TABLE 3.

Relation of Adverse Childhood Experience to Late-Life Neuroticism*

Outcome Adversity Measure Estimated Slope (SE) t (df) p Value
Neuroticism Emotional neglect 1.35 (0.29) 4.73 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation 3.03 (0.51) 5.91 (245) <0.001
Parental violence 4.44 (1.30) 3.41 (245) <0.001
Financial need 1.13 (0.82) 1.37 (245) 0.172
Family turmoil 2.73 (.095) 2.87 (245) 0.004
Anxiety Emotional neglect 0.15 (0.07) 2.05 (245) 0.041
Parental intimidation 0.65 (0.13) 4.94 (245) <0.001
Parental violence 0.57 (0.33) 1.71 (245) 0.088
Financial need −0.04 (0.21) −0.21 (245) 0.832
Family turmoil 0.79 (0.24) 3.32 (245) 0.001
Angry hostility Emotional neglect 0.29 (0.06) 4.66 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation 0.62 (0.11) 5.60 (245) <0.001
Parental violence 0.89 (0.28) 3.16 (245) 0.002
Financial need 0.03 (0.18) 0.17 (245) 0.861
Family turmoil 0.32 (0.21) 1.55 (245) 0.124
Depression Emotional neglect 0.24 (0.06) 3.77 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation 0.55 (0.12) 4.77 (245) <0.001
Parental violence 0.86 (0.29) 2.98 (245) 0.003
Financial need 0.31 (0.18) 1.69 (245) 0.092
Self-consciousness Emotional neglect 0.26 (0.06) 4.07 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation 0.37 (0.12) 3.19 (245) 0.002
Parental violence 0.77 (0.29) 2.29 (245) 0.008
Financial need 0.40 (0.18) 2.23 (245) 0.026
Family turmoil 0.24 (0.21) 1.15 (245) 0.250
Impulsiveness Emotional neglect 0.19 (0.07) 2.82 (245) 0.005
Parental intimidation 0.54 (0.12) 4.31 (245) <0.001
Parental violence 0.74 (0.31) 2.40 (245) 0.017
Financial need 0.21 (0.19) 1.05 (245) 0.294
Family turmoil 0.38 (0.23) 1.68 (245) 0.095
Vulnerability Emotional neglect 0.22 (0.06) 3.84 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation 0.29 (0.11) 2.78 (245) 0.006
Parental violence 0.60 (0.26) 2.31 (245) 0.022
Financial need 0.24 (0.16) 1.48 (245) 0.141

Family turmoil 0.33 (0.19) 1.79 (245) 0.075
*

Estimated from separate linear regression models that controlled for age, sex, own education, and parental education.

SE: standard error.

To examine the relation of specific forms of childhood adversity to neuroticism, we repeated the initial analysis separately for each domain of adverse experience and each measure of neuroticism (Table 3). Each domain of adversity except financial need was positively related to total neuroticism. Parental intimidation was positively correlated with all six facets of neuroticism; emotional neglect was positively related to five facets and parental violence was related to three. Family turmoil was related to anxiety and depression, and financial need was not associated with any facets of neuroticism.

To evaluate the independent associations of the domains of adversity with neuroticism, we regressed each neuroticism measure on the five adversity measures simultaneously. Emotional neglect and parental intimidation were both related to total neuroticism and angry hostility. Emotional neglect was also related to self-consciousness and vulnerability. Parental intimidation was associated with anxiety, depression, and impulsiveness.

Adverse Childhood Experience and Late-Life Social Engagement

In a similar series of linear regression analyses adjusted for age, sex, own education, and parental education, we examined the relation of adverse childhood experience to three indices of social engagement: frequency of social activity (mean: 2.4, SD: 0.5, range: 1−4), size of social network (mean: 6.2, SD: 5.9, range: 0−49), and level of emotional isolation (mean: 2.2, SD: 0.6, range: 1−4). As shown in Table 4, the composite measure of adverse childhood experience was not associated with frequency of participation in social activities, but more adverse experience was related to having a smaller social network, with approximately 3% shared variance, and feeling more emotionally isolated, with approximately 7% shared variance. Thus, with each one-point increase in the composite measure of adversity, social network size decreased by 0.17 unit and emotional isolation increased by 0.02 unit. These associations remained when each analysis was repeated with a term added for total neuroticism, indicating the association of childhood adversity with late-life social engagement is at least partially independent of neuroticism.

TABLE 4.

Relation of Adverse Childhood Experience to Late-Life Social Engagement*

Outcome Adversity Measure Estimated Slope (SE) t (df) p Value
Social activity Emotional neglect −0.01 (0.01) −1.33 (245) 0.184
Parental intimidation 0.01 (0.02) 0.36 (245) 0.717
Parental violence 0.04 (0.04) 0.99 (245) 0.325
Financial need −0.01 (0.03) −0.53 (245) 0.594
Family turmoil −0.02 (0.03) −0.50 (245) 0.619
Total adversity −0.01 (0.01) −0.78 (245) 0.436
Social network Emotional neglect −0.35 (0.09) −3.90 (245) <0.001
Parental intimidation −0.16 (0.17) −0.92 (245) 0.360
Parental violence −0.49 (0.42) −1.18 (245) 0.239
Financial need 0.41 (0.26) 1.59 (245) 0.114
Family turmoil −0.89 (0.30) −2.99 (245) 0.003
Total adversity −0.17 (0.06) −3.09 (245) 0.002
Emotional isolation Emotional neglect 0.03 (0.01) 3.84 (244) <0.001
Parental intimidation 0.06 (0.02) 3.45 (244) <0.001
Parental violence 0.11 (0.04) 2.61 (244) 0.010
Financial need −0.00 (0.03) −0.01 (244) 0.991
Family turmoil 0.09 (0.03) 2.92 (244) 0.004

Total adversity 0.02 (0.01) 4.53 (244) <0.001
*

Estimated from separate linear regression models that controlled for age, sex, own education, and parental education.

SE: standard error.

In subsequent analyses, emotional neglect and family turmoil but not other experiential measures were related to social network size, whereas all experiential measures except financial need were related to emotional isolation (Table 4). In additional models that included all five measures of childhood adversity, the only effect significant at an alpha level of 0.01 was an inverse association between emotional neglect and social network size.

DISCUSSION

In this group of approximately 250 persons with a mean age of approximately 80, we examined the relation of adverse childhood experiences to psychosocial functioning. Most persons reported at least some adverse experiences, and higher level of such experiences was associated with higher level of a wide range of negative emotions, smaller social networks, and a stronger sense of emotional isolation. The results suggest that traumatic experiences in childhood may adversely affect critical components of psychosocial adjustment in old age.

Prior research has shown that adults often report having had psychologically traumatic experiences as children,13 consistent with the present results. A positive association between childhood trauma and neuroticism has been previously reported in community-dwelling adults29 and substance abuse patients.30 The present analyses suggest that this association persists into old age and that it pertains to all dimensions of neuroticism. Childhood adversity has been associated with interpersonal difficulties in adolescence, suggesting that early life trauma interferes with the development of social skills.4,5 Consistent with this hypothesis, adverse childhood experiences were associated with having fewer close relationships and feeling more emotionally isolated in this cohort of older persons.

In old age, a higher level of neuroticism and lower level of social engagement have been associated with loss of cognitive function3134 and increased risk of disability26,35 and death.3638 The present results suggest that individual differences in these critical dimensions of psychosocial functioning in old age may be in part the result of traumatic experiences decades earlier in life. Prospective studies are needed to investigate the impact of childhood trauma on morbidity and mortality in old age.

This study has several strengths. On the basis of a uniform clinical evaluation and the application of accepted criteria, persons with dementia were identified and excluded from analyses. Because a relatively large group of participants was studied, there was adequate power to detect the associations of interest even after controlling for potentially confounding demographic variables. Results were relatively consistent with multiple measures of childhood trauma and psychosocial adjustment.

The principal limitations are the selected nature of the cohort and the cross-sectional design of the study. Prospective studies of childhood adversity effects in defined samples of older persons are needed. A related limitation is that information on childhood adversity was based on recall of events that occurred 50 or more years earlier, possibly biasing results, although most research does not suggest substantial bias in such recall.39,40 Also, the social activity measure had a relatively low level of internal consistency and three of the childhood adversity subscales had only two items apiece. These psychometric shortcomings may have led us to underestimate the association of childhood adversity with social engagement.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute on Aging grants R01 AG17917 and R01 AG022018 and the Illinois Department of Public Health.

The authors thank the many residents of Illinois who have participated in the Rush Memory and Aging Project; Tracy Colvin, M.P.H., and Tracy Hagman for coordinating the study; Woojeong Bang, M.S., for statistical programming; George Dombrowski, M.S., Greg Klein for data management; and Valerie J. Young for preparing the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Briere J, Elliott DM. Prevalence and psychological sequelae of self-reported childhood physical and sexual abuse in a general population sample of men and women. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:1205–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.MacMillan HL, Fleming JE, Trocme N, et al. Prevalence of child physical and sexual abuse in the community. Results from the Ontario Heath Supplement JAMA. 1997;278:131–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Scher CD, Forde DR, McQuaid JR, et al. Prevalence and demographic correlates of child maltreatment in an adult community sample. Child Abuse Negl. 2004;28:167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Gould MS, et al. Childhood adversities, interpersonal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts during late adolescence and early adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:741–749. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Meyerson LA, Long PJ, Miranda R, Jr, et al. The influence of childhood sexual abuse, physical abuse, family environment, and gender on the psychological adjustment of adolescents. Child Abuse Negl. 2002;26:387–405. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00315-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Johnson JG, Cohen P, Brown J, et al. Childhood maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders during early adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:600–606. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.7.600. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Macmillan HL, Fleming JE, Streiner DL. Childhood abuse and lifetime psychopathology in a community sample. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1878–1883. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Anda RF, Whitfield CL, Felitti VJ, et al. Adverse childhood experiences, alcoholic parents, and later risk of alcoholism and depression. Psychiatr Serv. 2002;53:1001–1009. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.53.8.1001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wainwright NWJ, Surtees PG. Childhood adversity, gender and depression over the life-course. J Affect Disord. 2002;72:33–44. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(01)00420-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dube SR, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, et al. Childhood abuse, household dysfunction, and the risk of attempted suicide throughout the life span: findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences Study. JAMA. 2001;286:3089–3096. doi: 10.1001/jama.286.24.3089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Johnson J, Cohen P, Gould M, et al. Childhood adversities, interpersonal difficulties, and risk for suicide attempts during late adolescence and early adulthood. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;59:741–749. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.8.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dong M, Dube SR, Felitti VJ, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and self-reported liver disease: new insights into the causal pathway. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163:1949–1956. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.16.1949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, et al. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Am J Prev Med. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Andersson L, Stevens N. Associations between early experiences with parents and well-being in old age. J Gerontol. 1993;48:P109–P116. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.3.p109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Russek LG, Schwartz GE. Perceptions of parental caring predicts health status in midlife: a 35-year follow-up of the Harvard Mastery of Stress Study. Psychosom Med. 1997;59:144–149. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199703000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Shaw BA, Krause N, Chatters LM, et al. Emotional support from parents early in life, aging, and health. Psychol Aging. 2004;19:4–12. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Krause N. Early parental loss, recent life events, and changes in health among older adults. J Aging Health. 1998;10:395–421. doi: 10.1177/089826439801000401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Buchman AS, et al. The Rush Memory and Aging Project: study design and baseline characteristics of the study cohort. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;25:163–175. doi: 10.1159/000087446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Bennett DA. Assessment of lifetime participation in cognitively stimulating activities. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2003;25:634–642. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.5.634.14572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Krueger KR, et al. Early and late life cognitive activity and cognitive systems in old age. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2005;11:400–407. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS/ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–1136. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, et al. The impact of adverse childhood experiences on health problems: evidence from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Prev Med. 2003;37:268–277. doi: 10.1016/s0091-7435(03)00123-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Costa PT, McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Psychological Assessment Resources; Odessa, FL: 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Cornoni-Huntley J, Brock DB, Ostfeld A, et al. Established populations for epidemiologic studies of the elderly resources data book. US Department of Health and Human Services; Washington, DC: 1986. NIH Publication No. 86−2443. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mendes de Leon CF, Glass TA, Berkman LF. Social engagement and disability in a community population of older adults: the New Haven EPESE. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157:633–642. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwg028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.de Jong Gierveld J, Kamphuis FH. The development of a Rasch-type loneliness scale. Appl Psychol Meas. 1985;9:289–299. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.SAS/STAT User's Guide (Version 8) SAS Institute Inc; Cary, NC: 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Talley NJ, Boyce PM, Jones M. Is the association between irritable bowel syndrome and abuse explained by neuroticism? A population based study. Gut. 1998;42:47–53. doi: 10.1136/gut.42.1.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Roy A. Childhood trauma and neuroticism as an adult: possible implication for the development of the common psychiatric disorders and suicidal behavior. Psychol Med. 2002;32:1471–1474. doi: 10.1017/s0033291702006566. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wilson RS, Evans DA, Bienias JL, et al. Proneness to psychological distress is associated with risk of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology. 2003;61:1479–1485. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000096167.56734.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Wilson RS, Bennett DA, Mendes de Leon CF, et al. Distress proneness and cognitive decline in a population of older persons. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF, Wilson RS, et al. Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African-Americans and Caucasians. Neurology. 2004;63:2322–2326. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000147473.04043.b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bassuk SS, Glass TA, Berkman LF. Social disengagement and incident cognitive decline in community-dwelling elderly persons. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131:165–173. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-3-199908030-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kempen GI, van Heuvelen MJ, van Sonderen E, et al. The relationship of functional limitations to disability and the moderating effects of psychological attributes in community-dwelling older persons. Soc Sci Med. 1999;48:1161–1172. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(98)00427-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wilson RS, Mendes de Leon CF, Bienias JL, et al. Personality and mortality in old age. J Gerontol Psychol Sci. 2004;59B:P110–P116. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.3.p110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Walter-Ginzburg A, Blumstein T, Chetrit A, et al. Social factors and mortality in the old-old in Israel: CALAS study. J Gerontol Soc Sci. 2002;57B:S308–S318. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.5.s308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Menec VH. The relation between everyday activities and successful aging: a 6-year longitudinal study. J Gerontol Soc Sci. 2003;59B:S74–S82. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.2.s74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, et al. Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry. 1994;151:1132–1136. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.8.1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Brewin CR, Andrews B, Gotlib IH. Psyhcopathology and early experience: a reappraisal of retrospective reports. Psychol Bull. 1993;113:82–98. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.113.1.82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES