Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2000 Jun 17;320(7250):1635–1640. doi: 10.1136/bmj.320.7250.1635

US women's attitudes to false positive mammography results and detection of ductal carcinoma in situ: cross sectional survey

Lisa M Schwartz a, Steven Woloshin a, Harold C Sox b, Baruch Fischhoff c, H Gilbert Welch a
PMCID: PMC27408  PMID: 10856064

Abstract

Objective

To determine women's attitudes to and knowledge of both false positive mammography results and the detection of ductal carcinoma in situ after screening mammography.

Design

Cross sectional survey.

Setting

United States.

Participants

479 women aged 18-97 years who did not report a history of breast cancer.

Main outcome measures

Attitudes to and knowledge of false positive results and the detection of ductal carcinoma in situ after screening mammography.

Results

Women were aware that false positive results do occur. Their median estimate of the false positive rate for 10 years of annual screening was 20% (25th percentile estimate, 10%; 75th percentile estimate, 45%). The women were highly tolerant of false positives: 63% thought that 500 or more false positives per life saved was reasonable and 37% would tolerate 10 000 or more. Women who had had a false positive result (n=76) expressed the same high tolerance: 39% would tolerate 10 000 or more false positives. 62% of women did not want to take false positive results into account when deciding about screening. Only 8% of women thought that mammography could harm a woman without breast cancer, and 94% doubted the possibility of non-progressive breast cancers. Few had heard about ductal carcinoma in situ, a cancer that may not progress, but when informed, 60% of women wanted to take into account the possibility of it being detected when deciding about screening.

Conclusions

Women are aware of false positives and seem to view them as an acceptable consequence of screening mammography. In contrast, most women are unaware that screening can detect cancers that may never progress but feel that such information would be relevant. Education should perhaps focus less on false positives and more on the less familiar outcome of detection of ductal carcinoma in situ.

Introduction

Screening mammography is vigorously promoted in the United States. With the exception of the US government's Preventive Health Services Task Force, professional organisations recommend that women begin annual or biannual screening at 40 years of age.13 Mammography is promulgated by hospitals, insurance plans, and breast care centres. Efforts for quality improvement commonly focus on increasing the screening rates for breast cancer, and health plans highlight these rates on cards used for reporting quality of health care. Although there has been much discussion about the potential benefit of mammography there has been far less about the potential harms.

The harm that has received the most attention is false positive results. Mammograms that give false positive results are common. A 60 year old woman screened annually for 10 years has a 50% chance of having at least one false positive leading to follow up testing and a 20% chance of a false positive leading to biopsy.4 Consequently, many people are concerned about the physical, psychological, and economic costs of false positives.410 Several experts in screening have concluded that women would benefit from education about false positive results if they are to make informed decisions about whether to undergo, or continue with, screening.2,1113

Little attention has been paid to the increasingly frequent detection of ductal carcinoma in situ, a subtle but potential harm of screening.14 Although the clinical course of ductal carcinoma in situ is poorly understood, most lesions do not progress.1417 Consequently, an increasing number of women with lesions that would never have become clinically apparent are worried about cancer, and most of them will undergo invasive treatment of unknown benefit (for example, mastectomy, lumpectomy with radiation).14

It is not known if or how women who are offered screening are being counselled about false positives and ductal carcinoma in situ. To determine what women know we conducted a national survey of women in the United States, a population with high exposure to mammography—more than 85% of US women aged 40 years or more have had at least one screening mammography.18 We wanted to find out if women are aware of false positives and if they have a sense of the chance of having one; if false positives are tolerated because women have an unrealistic sense of the benefit of mammography; and if women are aware of ductal carcinoma in situ and, if not, whether they want to know about it.

Participants and methods

Design

We randomly selected women from details compiled from telephone directories and administrative records (for example, applications for a driver's licence, electoral registries, house purchases) by National Decision Systems (Atlanta, GA). We restricted our sample to the 80% of US women in households with telephones. We used stratified random sampling to oversample women of screening age. Specifically, we selected women by age (18-39 years old, 200 women; 40-49, 250; 50-69, 250; 70 or older, 100), estimated income (income more or less than twice the 1992 poverty threshold for a family of four people19), and area of residence.

From August to October 1997 we posted a questionnaire (with $2 (£1.25) as an incentive) to 800 women. We chose to conduct a postal survey to utilise visual analogue scales. We posted reminder letters to non-respondents after two weeks, sent a second copy of the questionnaire after four weeks, and attempted to telephone those who had still not responded after six weeks.

Of the 800 people selected, 33 were ineligible (21 had died, 12 were male), leaving 767 possible respondents. Of these women, 55 had incorrect addresses, two did not speak English, and 207 did not return the questionnaire. Overall, 503 of the 767 women (65.6%) returned completed questionnaires. We report on the 479 respondents with no history of breast cancer.

Survey

We developed a 13 page questionnaire as part of a larger project on women's decision making about mammography. A pilot was tested on women veterans served by the Veterans Administration Medical Center at White River Junction, Vermont.

Visual analogue scales

We asked women to estimate the sensitivity and false positive rate of mammography with a previously validated visual analogue scale.20 To familiarise respondents with the scale, we included two practice questions about events having extreme probabilities. Overall, 94% of respondents used the correct end of the scale for each event—close to 1 for the chance of stopping at a red light, and close to 0 for the chance of being hit by a meteorite.

We also asked women to compare the benefits of mammography with those of other preventive activities that would extend the life of a 60 year old woman (fig 1). For each prevention strategy, we asked respondents to mark anywhere on a line scaled from “much less benefit” to “much greater benefit” compared with 10 years of annual mammography, the mid-point being “same benefit.” We measured the distance of each woman's mark from “much less benefit” and calculated the median value.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

 Women's ranking of benefits of health promoting activities and 10 year programme of mammography for extending life in a 60 year old woman. Arrows show median response for each strategy

Analysis

Because we used stratified random sampling, we calculated sample weights to account for probability of selection and to compensate for small differences in response rates across sample strata. We then adjusted the sample distribution to conform to known marginal distributions of the US population based on data from the 1990 US census19,21 by creating “balance weights.”22 Because the crude results and the weighted results were almost identical, for simplicity we present the crude data. Based on our sample size, we estimate the margin of error of the results to be 4-6% in either direction.23 All analyses were done with STATA software (College Station, TX).

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the women. Respondents were from all 50 states and the District of Columbia. Most women reported having had at least one mammogram: 35% of women less than 40 years of age, 87% of women in their 40s, 93% of women aged 50-69, and 87% of women aged 70 or more.3 Similarly high proportions of women planned to have a mammogram in the next two years.

Table 1.

Characteristics of sample compared with women 18 years and older from the 1990 US census. Values are numbers (percentages) unless stated otherwise

Characteristic Patients (n=479) 1990 US census (%)
Age (years):
  18-39 120 (25) 46
 40-49 153 (32) 16
 50-69 158 (33) 24
 ⩾70  48 (10) 14
Ethnicity:
  White 431 (90) 78
 African-American 19 (4) 11
 Hispanic 10 (2) 7
 Other 19 (4) 4
Household income ($):
  <10 000 24 (5) 13
 10 000-24 999  86 (18) 24
 25 000-49 999 158 (33) 33
 50 000-99 999 163 (34) 22
 ⩾100 000  48 (10) 8
Highest level of education:
  < High school graduate 29 (6) 25
 High school degree 268 (56) 53
 College degree 139 (29) 17
 Postgraduate degree 43 (9) 5
Region:
  North east  67 (14) 21
 Midwest 115 (24) 24
 South 153 (32) 35
 West 144 (30) 20

95% confidence intervals ranged from 4% to 6% in either direction for all percentages. 

Perception of harm

Overall, 441 (92.0%) women believed that mammography could not harm a woman without breast cancer (table 2). Thirty of the 40 women who thought harm was possible responded to our request for an explanation. The most common responses were exposure to radiation (16 women), stress or anxiety (four), and false positives (three). None mentioned the effects of treating non-progressive cancer.

Table 2.

Women's perceptions of benefits and harms of screening

No (%) of patients
Perceptions of harms
 Do you agree that “if a woman getting mammograms turns out not to have breast cancer, she may have been harmed by the mammograms”? 38 (8)
False positives
 “Imagine a typical, healthy 60 year old woman. Assume that you know nothing else about her. Suppose that this 60 year old woman has yearly mammograms for the next 10 years and she does not have breast cancer. What is the chance that she will have a ‘false alarm’ where one of her mammograms will look like she has cancer even though she doesn't?” Median 20/100, 25th percentile=10%, 75th percentile=48%
“Is information about false alarms something you want to factor into your decision about getting a mammogram?”* 182 (38)
Non-progressive cancer
 Do you agree that “some types of breast cancer grow so slowly that even without treatment they would not affect a woman's health”? 34 (7)
Presentation of ductal carcinoma in situ (DCIS) information: “Have you heard about DCIS before this survey?”* 29 (6)
“Is information about DCIS something you want to factor into your decision about getting a mammogram?”* 287 (60)
Perceptions of benefits
 Do you agree that “if a woman getting mammograms turns out to have breast cancer, she may have benefited from the mammograms”? 450 (94)
Sensitivity of mammography
 “Imagine a typical, healthy 60 year old woman. Assume that you know nothing else about her. Now imagine this 60 year old woman has breast cancer but no obvious symptoms. What is the chance a mammogram will find the cancer?” Median 73/100, 25th percentile=50%, 75th percentile=86%
Magnitude of benefit
 “All things being equal, if this 60 year old woman got yearly mammograms for the next 10 years, she would have . . .”

 A higher or unchanged chance of dying of breast cancer 34 (7)
A lower chance of dying of breast cancer:
 By one fifth to one tenth  62 (13)
 By one third 120 (25)
 By a half 263 (55)
 Reduced to zero  0 (0)

95% confidence intervals ranged from 4% to 6% in either direction for all percentages. Questions preceded by ‘do you agree’ used a five point Likert scale (strongly agree to strongly disagree); the proportion agreeing is those who answered “strongly agree” or “agree.” 

*

Percentage of women answering “yes.” 

Women were given a brief explanation of ductal carcinoma in situ as a lesion that does not always progress to invasive cancer. 

False positive results

Overall, 99% of women believed that false positive results occur during a 10 year programme of annual mammography beginning at age 60 years. The women's median estimated chance of a false positive during such a programme was 20%. This estimate is in line with a recent report citing a 47% 10 year probability of a false positive mammogram leading to any follow up testing for a 60 year old woman and a 19% probability of a false positive mammogram leading to a biopsy.4

To understand the importance of false positives, we asked respondents whether they wanted to take into account such results when deciding about mammography; only 38% did. When asked how many false positives would be acceptable for each life saved, women showed a high tolerance: 63% would tolerate 500 or more false positives and 37% would tolerate 10 000 or more (fig 2). The best estimate of the actual number of false positive mammograms for each life saved is somewhere between 30 and 200, assuming 2-6 lives saved for every 1000 women screened for 12 years24,25 and a 10 year false positive rate between 20% and 40%.4 Thus, the actual number of false positives is far below the number most women deemed acceptable.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

No of false positive results women were prepared to tolerate for each life saved

Women who had had a false positive mammogram expressed a similarly high tolerance for false positive results. Seventy six women (16%) reported having had a false positive mammogram—that is, a breast biopsy but no diagnosis of breast cancer. In this subgroup, 93% believed that mammography could not harm a woman who turned out not to have breast cancer, 35% wanted to take false positives into acount when deciding on screening, 71% would tolerate 500 or more false positives per life saved, and 39% would tolerate 10 000 or more (fig 2).

Perception of benefit

To explore whether this high tolerance reflected an unrealistic sense of the benefit of mammography, we examined perceptions of benefit. As expected, most (94%) of the women believed that women whose breast cancer was diagnosed by screening mammography benefited from having been screened (table 2). Although most believed that mammography reduced the chance of dying of breast cancer, none thought it reduced the risk to zero. The most common expectation was that mammography would reduce the chance of dying of breast cancer by half and the second most common expectation was that it would reduce the chance by one third24,25 (we considered this to be the correct answer; however, a recent study suggests that a one third reduction in risk may be an overestimate26). Women were aware that mammograms did not find all cancers. Their median estimated sensitivity for a single mammogram (for a 60 year old woman) of 73% underestimated the reported sensitivity of 94% (95% confidence interval 83% to 99%).27

Most women (82%) recognised that a 10 year programme of mammography was more beneficial than mammography performed only once (fig 1). Women rated health promoting strategies like not smoking, exercising regularly, and eating a low fat diet as much more beneficial than mammography. Surprisingly, women believed breast self examinations to be more beneficial than the 10 year programme of annual mammography.

Non-progressive cancer

Few women knew about the possibility of non-progressive breast cancer (table 2). Only 7% agreed that some breast cancers grow so slowly that even without treatment they would not affect a woman's health. We gave the following brief explanation of ductal carcinoma in situ: “We would like to ask your opinion about ductal carcinoma in situ or DCIS, a breast abnormality which can only be picked up by mammograms. Cancer specialists are confused about DCIS because sometimes it becomes invasive and sometimes it doesn't. If DCIS does not become invasive, it will not affect how long a person will live even without treatment. Doctors don't know which DCIS will become invasive. Nowadays, almost everyone with DCIS gets treated. Many people receive surgery, chemotherapy, or radiation who would never have gotten sick. For these people, treatment provides no physical benefit.”

Only 6% of women reported having heard of ductal carcinoma in situ by name or corresponding to the description provided, and 60% wanted to take ductal carcinoma in situ into account when deciding about mammography, with interest highest among younger women (71% of women aged 18-39).

To assess how information about non-progressive cancer might influence decision making, we asked women to decide about treatment for ductal carcinoma in situ under different assumptions of its probability of becoming invasive (fig 3). In the hypothetical scenario that ductal carcinoma in situ became increasingly likely to become invasive, more women chose treatment as the chance of invasion increased. At a 1% chance of ductal carcinoma in situ becoming invasive, 42% of women chose treatment whereas at a 33% chance of invasion, 78% chose treatment. The limited published estimates of the chance of ductal carcinoma in situ becoming invasive varies and are as high as 33%.14,17

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Women's threshold for treatment of ductal carcinoma in situ according to chance of becoming invasive

Discussion

The women in our study were aware of false positive results from screening mammography but seemed to view them as an acceptable consequence of screening. Although studies attest to the short term physical and psychological impact of false positive results,59 our respondents were highly accepting of them: most would not take them into account when deciding about screening, and almost 40% would tolerate 10 000 or more false positives requiring biopsy for each life saved.

One explanation for this high tolerance is that women have an overly optimistic sense of the benefit of mammography. We found no evidence to support this explanation. No respondent thought screening mammography eliminated the chance of dying of breast cancer. Women were aware that mammography misses some cancers (actually underestimating the reported sensitivity). Most women also recognised that health promoting behaviours such as not smoking, exercising regularly, and eating a low fat diet were more beneficial than mammography in prolonging life, which is true for the average 60 year old woman.28

Alternatively, it might be posited that women did not fully understand the consequences of a false positive result (for example, anxiety, pain, inconvenience, or extremely rare harms such as severe infection or death related to anaesthesia). We found, however, that women who had had false mammograms expressed the same high tolerance as women who had not. Thus, women seemed to think that false positives are worth the reassurance of being told they do not have cancer. Similarly, Gram et al found that almost half of women with false positive mammograms viewed the experience as having an overall positive impact on their lives,8 and most women continue to undergo mammography.29

Women's perceptions about a potential diagnosis of ductal carcinoma in situ differed noticeably from their perceptions about false positive mammograms. In contrast to false positives, most women were unaware of non-progressive forms of breast cancer and even doubted their existence. Once informed about non-progressive cancer, the women seemed concerned. Most wanted to take into account the possibility of ductal carcinoma in situ when deciding about screening. Younger women, in whom 90% of the cancers found by screening mammography are ductal carcinoma in situ,15 were the most interested in such information. In addition, women's reported willingness to treat ductal carcinoma in situ increased as we hypothetically increased the chance of ductal carcinoma in situ progressing to invasive breast cancer, suggesting that such information might influence decisions.

Limitations

One limitation of our study is the representativeness of the sample: we did not include women in households without a telephone and those who requested that their name be removed from the database. This left about 80% of US women eligible for sampling. Secondly, although our sample represents women across a broad range of age, education, and income, it differed from the general population: the women were wealthier and better educated, and almost all were white. Women from ethnic minorities and women with the lowest socioeconomic indicators were underrepresented. Subsequent studies are needed to assess whether such women have different perceptions.

One concern is the possibility of systematic bias in our sample because respondents differed from non-respondents. Our response rate of 66% lessens but does not eliminate this concern. Respondents and non-respondents did not differ by age (the one variable available for comparison). Our main findings were, however, extreme enough (for example, only 7% of women were aware of non-progressive breast cancer) to suggest that even if respondents and non-respondents differed noticeably, our overall conclusions should remain robust. Although the survey was long and complex, response rates for items were high. Response rates were lowest for the question on the treatment threshold for ductal carcinoma in situ (82%) and averaged 98% for all other questions.

Although experts on screening have focused much attention on the anxiety experienced by women with false positive mammograms,13 we believe clinicians counselling women about mammography should spend less time reviewing what most women know and accept—that is, that false positives are part of screening. In contrast, more time should probably be spent educating women about the less familiar outcome of the ambiguity associated with the detection of ductal carcinoma in situ.

What is already known on this topic

False positive results and diagnoses of non-progressive cancer are recognised problems of screening mammography

Little is known about how women feel about these problems

What this study adds

Almost all of the 479 women (99%) knew that false positive mammograms occur

Women do not seem to think that false positive mammograms are an important harm of screening—even women who have had a false positive result

Women's tolerance of false positives is not explained by overly optimistic beliefs about the benefit of mammography

Few of the women (6%) were aware of the possibility of non-progressive cancer

Supplementary Material

[extra: Prepublication history]

Acknowledgments

We thank Robert Pritchard for help with the study design, R Peter Mogielnicki for helpful comments, and Jennifer Winder and Suzy Shukwit for assistance with data collection.

Footnotes

Funding: SW and LMS are supported by the veterans affairs career development awards in health services research and development. This work was also funded by a new investigator award from the Department of Defense breast cancer research program (DAMD17-96-MM-6712). The views expressed do not necessarily represent those of the Department of Veterans Affairs or the US government.

Competing interests: None declared.

This article is part of the BMJ's randomised controlled trial of open peer review. Documentation relating to the editorial decision making process is available on the BMJ's website

References

  • 1.American Cancer Society. Summary of current guidelines for the cancer-related checkup: recommendations. New York: ACS; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.National Cancer Advisory Board. National Cancer Advisory Board mammography recommendations for women ages 40-49. Bethesda, MD: National Cancer Advisory Board; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.US Preventive Services Task Force. Guide to clinical preventive services. 2nd ed. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Elmore J, Barton M, Moceri V, Polk S, Arena P, Fletcher S. Ten-year risk of false positive screening mammograms and clinical breast examinations. N Engl J Med. 1998;338:1089–1096. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199804163381601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lerman C, Trock B, Rimer B, Boyce A, Jepson C, Engstrom P. Psychological and behavioral implications of abnormal mammograms. Ann Intern Med. 1991;114:657–661. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-114-8-657. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sutton S, Saidi G, Bickler G, Hunter J. Does routine screening for breast cancer raise anxiety? Results from a three wave prospective study in England. J Epidemiol Community Health. 1995;49:413–418. doi: 10.1136/jech.49.4.413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Lerman L, Trock B, Rimer B, Jepson C, Brody D, Boyce A. Psychological side effects of breast cancer screening. Health Psychol. 1991;10:259–267. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.10.4.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Gram I, Lund E, Slenker S. Quality of life following a false positive mammogram. Br J Cancer. 1990;62:1018–1022. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1990.430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ellman R, Angeli N, Christians A, Moss S, Chamberlain J, Maguire P. Psychiatric morbidity associated with screening for breast cancer. Br J Cancer. 1989;60:781–784. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1989.359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lidbrink E, Elfving J, Frisell J, Jonsson E. Neglected aspects of false positive findings of mammography in breast cancer screening: analysis of false positive cases from the Stockholm trial. BMJ. 1996;312:273–276. doi: 10.1136/bmj.312.7026.273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ransohoff D, Harris R. Lessons from the mammography screening controversy: can we improve the debate? Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:1029–1034. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-199712010-00016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Fletcher S. False-positive screening mammograms: good news, but more to do. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131:60–62. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-1-199907060-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.National Institutes of Health consensus statement. Breast cancer screening for women ages 40-49. Bethesda, MD: National Cancer Institute; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ernster VL, Barclay J, Kerlikowske K, Grady D, Henderson C. Incidence of and treatment for ductal carcinoma in situ of the breast. JAMA. 1996;275:913–918. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ernster VL, Barclay J. Increases in ductal carcinoma in situ (DCIS) of the breast in relation to mammography: a dilemma. Monogr Natl Cancer Inst. 1997;22:151–156. doi: 10.1093/jncimono/1997.22.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Welch H, Black W. Using autopsy series to estimate the disease “reservoir” for ductal carcinoma in situ of the breast: how much more breast cancer can we find? Ann Intern Med. 1997;127:1023–1028. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-127-11-199712010-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Page D, Dupont W, Rogers L, Jensen R, Schuyler P. Continued local recurrence of carcinoma 15-25 years after a diagnosis of low grade duc-tal carcinoma in situ of the breast treated only by biopsy. Cancer. 1995;76:1197–2000. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(19951001)76:7<1197::aid-cncr2820760715>3.0.co;2-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Blackman DK, Bennett EM, Miller DS. Trends in self-reported use of mammograms (1989-1997) and Papanicolaou tests (1991-1997)—behavioral risk factor surveillance system. MMWR CDC Surveill Summ. 1999;48:1–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.US Bureau of the Census. Statistical abstract of the United States. 117th ed. Washington, DC: US Bureau of the Census; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Woloshin S, Schwartz LM, Byram S, Fishhoff B, Welch H. A new scale for assessing perceptions of chance: a validation study. Med Decis Making 2000. (In press.) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 21.Ruggles S, Sobek M. Minneapolis: Historical Census Projects, University of Minnesota; 1997. Integrated public use microdata series: version 2.0. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Blendon R, Scheck A, Donelan K, Hill C, Smith M, Beatrice D, et al. How white and African Americans view their health and social problems. JAMA. 1995;273:341–345. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Fowler F., Jr . Survey research methods. 2nd ed. Newbury Park: Sage; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kerlikowske K, Grady D, Rubin SM, Sandrock C, Ernster V. Efficacy of screening mammography. A meta-analysis. JAMA. 1995;273:149–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Nystrom L, Rutqvist L, Wall S, Lindgren A, Lindqvist M, Ryden S, et al. Breast cancer screening with mammography: overview of Swedish randomized trials. Lancet. 1993;341:973–978. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)91067-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gotzsche P, Olsen O. Is screening for breast cancer with mammography justifiable? Lancet. 2000;355:129–134. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)06065-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kerlikowskie K, Grady D, Barclay J, Sickles E, Ernster V. Effect of age, breast density and family history on the sensitivity of first screening mammography. JAMA. 1996;276:33–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.McGinnis J, Foege W. Actual causes of death in the United States. JAMA. 1993;270:2207–2212. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Burman M, Taplin S, Herta D, Elmore J. Effect of false-positive mammograms on interval breast cancer screening in a health maintenance organization. Ann Intern Med. 1999;131:1–6. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-131-1-199907060-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

[extra: Prepublication history]
bmj_320_7250_1635__1.html (41.5KB, html)

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES