Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: Psychiatr Genet. 2009 Aug;19(4):186–194. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e32832cec89

Racial and ethnic differences in willingness to participate in psychiatric genetic research

Eleanor J Murphy 1, Priya Wickramaratne 1, Myrna M Weissman 1
PMCID: PMC2742945  NIHMSID: NIHMS121763  PMID: 19593860

Abstract

Objective

The National Institute of Mental Health’s effort to rectify the underrepresentation of American Blacks in the genetic studies of psychiatric disorders has met with mixed success. This study was designed to understand some of the barriers to recruitment.

Methods

Men and women, who were of Black, White or Hispanic race/ethnicity, aged 18–79 years (Nλ=λ353), were recruited from clinical and community settings in New York City Participants responded to a survey that was designed to measure willingness to participate and attitudes toward genetic research. Principal components analyses generated eight factors including perceived benefits, concerns about, and drawbacks of genetic research, and beliefs about genetic or environmental contributions to psychopathology. Analysis of variance assessed within-ethnic group differences on factor scores, as they related to willingness to participate in genetic research.

Results

Ethnic groups did not differ significantly in stated willingness to participate in genetic research; more than 70% in each group were willing to participate. Among Blacks and Hispanics, mistrust and wariness, and stigma were significantly increased in those unwilling to participate; for Whites, perceived benefit to society and perceived importance for knowledge/education were associated with willingness to participate. For Blacks and Hispanics, youth (aged 18–29 years) and college education reduced, but did not eliminate the association between wariness and mistrust and willingness to participate.

Conclusion

Findings suggest that recruitment efforts aimed at increasing the representation of Blacks should be aware of the barriers among those who are less educated, and involve interactive community collaborations, to fully address the mistrust in this population.

Keywords: attitudes, Blacks, depression, genomic, recruitment, mental illness

Introduction

Despite the increase in genetic research of psychiatric disorders in the last decade, (Rutter, 2000; Sullivan et al., 2000; Levinson, 2003), and major efforts by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) to ensure adequate representation of non-Hispanic American Blacks (NIMH, 2007), the success in increasing representation has been mixed. To reduce health disparities, the NIMH hasrequired inclusion of Blacks in these studies and has made efforts to increase recruitment. However, American Blacks remain underrepresented in family (Weissman et al., 1984; Hettema et al., 2001; Sullivan et al., 2000), twin/adoption (Lyons et al., 1998; Kendler and Prescott, 1999; Kendler, 1993, Kendler, 2001) and genetic studies (Badner and Gershon, 2002; Lohmueller et al., 2003; Levinson et al., 2007) of schizophrenia, mood and anxiety disorders. Non-Hispanic Blacks roughly comprise 12% of the US population over the last three decades (US Census Bureau, 1980, 1990, 2006), but typically account for less than 5% of the sample in these studies. Consequently, little is known about the hereditary component of depression or other psychiatric disorders among Blacks. The reasons for their underrepresentation in biomedical research, including genetic research is of scientific interest. (Buchwald et al., 2006; Sterling et al. 2006).

The underrepresentation of Blacks in genetic studies of major depression is particularly important because differences in rates of disorders suggest possible genetic and/or social differences in causes. Compared with Whites, Blacks suffer higher rates of many serious physical illnesses such as hypertension and diabetes, (Williams, 1999), but paradoxically have lower rates of major depression (Breslau et al., 2006; Williams & Earl, 2007). Several community-based epidemiological studies in the United States over the last 20 years show similar patterns of racial/ethnic differences, with Blacks reporting lower rates of major depression than Whites (Somervile et al., 1989; Kessler et al., 1994; Jackson et al., 2004; Hasin et al., 2005). Whether the difference is an artifact of sampling or reporting, is unclear. Assuming that the difference is nonartifactual, the mechanisms responsible for this persistent finding, genetic, environmental, or a combination of both, are poorly understood. Although preliminary research has suggested possible genetic differences between Blacks and Whites with regard to depression risk and the response to pharmacological treatments of depression (Lotrich et al., 1997; Gelertner et al., 1998; Gelertner et al., 1999; Lotrich et al., 2003; McMahon et al., 2006), the findings are contradictory and difficult to interpret because these studies are few and the sample sizes are relatively small.

Efforts to include representative numbers of Blacks in biomedical research have been met with some difficulty, with successful recruitment usually entailing careful planning, extensive community engagement, time, and human resources (Shavers et al., 2002; Clay et al., 2005). The usual explanations for this difficulty typically involve mistrust, which is thought to be more prevalent among Blacks compared with Whites, because of the well-known historical legacy of the Tuskegee Syphilis Study, among others (Schulz et al., 2003; Bates and Harris, 2004).

With regard to genetic research, much of Blacks’ mistrust is thought to stem not only from fear of harm while participating in the study, but also from misgivings about the ultimate uses of the information garnered from the studies (Schulz et al., 2003), because genetics have been misapplied in explaining racial differences on a number of indices including intelligence test scores (Hernstein and Murray, 1994), and pain perception (Hama, 2000). Like Blacks, Hispanics have also been shown to share similar concerns about misuses of genetic information (Schulz et al., 2003).

Another element related to mistrust, and also relevant to genetics in psychiatry, is the stigma associated with having a mental illness, or carrying a genetic risk for one’s family. Compared with Whites, Blacks have been shown to have more stigmatizing attitudes toward psychiatric disorders, perceiving individuals who suffer from these disorders as being violent or dangerous (Whaley, 1997; Anglin et al., 2006; Rao et al., 2007), and endorsing social distance from them or their relatives (Whaley, 1997; Rao et al., 2007). Some research has suggested that Blacks are less likely than Whites to use genetic explanations as causes of psychiatric disorders, because they associate the term ‘genetics’ with dysfunctional families and upbringing, a stigmatizing phenomenon stereotypically associated with Black communities (Schnittker et al., 2000). Although studies have linked beliefs about causes of mental illness to Blacks’ willingness to seek professional treatment for psychiatric disorders (Schnittker et al., 2000), few other studies have also linked their beliefs about genetic causes of nonpsychiatric illness to their willingness to participate in genetic research, it is currently unknown whether this association is true when it comes to participating in genetic research for psychiatric illness.

Research has shown that age and educational levels, regardless of race/ethnicity background, determine participation, with older or less educated individuals less willing to participate in genetic research, (Wang et al., 2001; Audrain et al., 2002; Moorman et al., 2004; Bogner et al., 2004). Still, other studies have shown that the shared experiences of historical and institutional racism among Blacks contribute to stigma and mistrust independent of education or socioeconomic status (Furr, 2002; Laskey et al., 2003; McQuillan et al., 2003).

In summary, commonly occurring beliefs that may be associated with genetic research participation among Blacks include mistrust, stigma, perceived benefits or harm, and perceived causes of psychiatric illness. Although these themes have been identified, few, if any, studies have directly linked them to willingness to participate in psychiatric genetic research. Using an educationally and ethnically diverse sample of Blacks, Hispanics, and Whites, the purpose of this study is to first determine differences among the groups in willingness to participate in genetic studies of psychiatric disorders, and how these differences relate to attitudes, beliefs, age, and education.

Materials and methods

Sample

The study used a convenience sample, in which participants of any racial or ethnic background, aged 18 years or older (all based on self-report), and who were English-speaking, were included. To obtain a range of education levels, we recruited volunteers from local hospitals, community settings including local businesses, and college campuses in the New York City area. The study was advertised through word-of-mouth referrals, and through flyers posted on bulletin boards in college and community service lobbies, and in clinical waiting room areas. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the New York State Psychiatric Institute, and by the Institutional Review Boards from two local hospitals, which granted permission to recruit individuals receiving treatment at their outpatient mental health clinics.

Procedure

Individuals who indicated an interest in participating were asked to read and sign written consent that explained the purpose of the study, procedures involved, risks and benefits, compensation, limits of confidentiality and participant rights. Participants were then provided with a confidential self-report questionnaire, containing a cover note with a brief introduction and basic description of genetic research on psychiatric disorders and some of the procedures associated with that kind of genetic research. Staff members were always present and available to respondents who needed further explanation of the consent form and the study procedures, or for a few who needed assistance in completing the questionnaire. Respondents were compensated $10 in cash for participating.

Research instrument

The questionnaire consisted of demographics (Table 1), 38 items consisting of statements reflecting attitudes, beliefs, and concerns about genetic research, two items assessing willingness to participate in genetic research, and four items assessing mental illness (clinical) and research participation history (available upon request). The 38 attitudinal statements were developed for this study and were based on themes described in the literature on attitudes toward research, and themes obtained from a series of focus groups, consisting of mostly Black participants (Murphy and Thompson, in press). Generally, most respondents in the focus groups felt that genetic research would be helpful for the prevention and/or cure of certain diseases, and beneficial for educating society in general. The primary negative themes that emerged from the groups were fear of harmful or unpleasant procedures (e.g. having blood drawn, having to ingest substances, etc.), fear of loss of confidentiality, stigma associated with mental illness and family involvement, and lack of knowledge or education about genetics and genetic research. An example of one of the attitudinal statements in the questionnaire is ‘genetic research is important for educating patients and health professionals’. Items were scored on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 1 to 4 (1λ=λstrongly disagree, 2λ=λsomewhat disagree, 3λ=λsomewhat agree, 4λ=λstrongly agree). Higher scores on a given item indicated greater agreement with that item.

Table 1.

Demographic, clinical, and research experience characteristics by ethnic group

Characteristic Blacks, n (%) Hispanics, n (%) Whites, n (%) χ2
λSex 3.60
νMale 67 (39) 45 (44) 39 (51)
νFemale 107 (62) 58 (56) 37 (49)
Age (years) 25.53
ν18–29 86 (52) 69 (70) 61 (84)
ν30–49 53 (32) 24 (24) 6 (8)
ν50 and above 25 (15) 6 (6) 6 (8)
Marital status 16.20***
νSingle 122 (70) 83 (81) 64 (85)
νMarried 22 (13) 15 (15) 7 (9)
νDivorced/separated/widowed/ 30 (17) 4 (4) 4 (5)
Education level completed 7.34
νLess than high school 19 (11%) 8 (8) 4 (5)
νHigh school/GED 59 (34) 28 (28) 17 (23)
νCollege or higher 95 (55) 66 (65) 54 (72)
Annual income (household) 19.27***
νLess than $20λ000 65 (39) 50 (51) 32 (44)
ν$20λ000–$59λ000 80 (48) 28 (28) 19 (26)
ν$60λ000 or higher 21 (13) 21 (21) 21 (29)
Has suffered mental illness 3.22
νYes 39% 49% 43%
νNo 61% 51% 57%
Has sought or received treatmenta 5.72
νYes 69% 72% 84%
νNo 31% 28% 15%
Knows friend/family with mental illness 7.40*
νYes 51% 37% 38%
νNo 49% 63% 62%
Has volunteered for health research 7.92**
νYes 21% 65% 29%
νNo 79% 35% 71%
a

These percentages are based on the numbers reported for history of mental illness – n (Blacks)λ=λ66; n (Whites)λ=λ33; n (Hispanics)λ=λ50.

The χ2 test two-tailed significance:

*

P<0.10.

**

P<0.05.

***

P<0.01.

P<0.0001.

The two items specifically assessing willingness to participate were also scored on the same 4-point Likert scale as the attitudinal items. One was positively worded (‘I would be willing to participate in genetic research’) and the other was negatively worded (‘I would not be willing to participate in genetic research’). We selected the negatively worded item as our outcome variable of interest because it correlated more strongly with the other items, and yielded a higher response variance (0.91 vs. 0.86), than its positively worded counterpart, which enabled superior differentiation among respondents. We reverse-scored this item for further analyses.

Four dichotomously scored items (1λ=λyes; 0λ=λno), assessed whether respondents had suffered from an emotional problem or mental illness such as depression, whether they had ever sought professional treatment for an emotional problem, whether they knew a friend or family member who had suffered from an emotional problem, and whether they had ever participated in a health research study.

Statistical analyses

All analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 14.0 (2005). The three ethnic groups were compared on demographic indicators and clinical characteristics and research participation history, using χ2 goodness-of-fit tests. The item that assessed willingness to participate in genetic research of mental illness was dichotomously recoded (collapsing ‘somewhat’ and ‘strongly’ agree into 1λ=λyes, and collapsing ‘somewhat’ and ‘strongly’ disagree into 2λ=λno). With this recoded item, we used a χ2 test to compare the ethnic groups on willingness to participate, and then within these ethnic groups we examined the demographic and clinical or research factors associated with the willingness to participate.

Of the 38 attitudinal items, we excluded nine items because they were significantly modified because of lack of clarity (based on participant responses), after roughly half of the sample had participated in the study. Therefore, 29 items were used for further statistical analyses. Using an exploratory principal components analysis (PCA) (Jolliffe, 2002) based on varimax rotation with Kaiser Guttman normalization, we obtained factors for which we computed the mean scores for each ethnic group. The initial solution yielded nine factors with eigenvalues of more than 1.00, accounting for 61% of the cumulative variance in the group of questions. We excluded additional five items, which loaded on factors that were uninterpretable, or had factor loadings of less than 30. We reperformed the PCA on the remaining 24 items, which yielded a solution of eight components accounting for 64% of the cumulative variance.

Our decision to retain an item as part of a given factor was based on two criteria: the factor loading for that item had to be ≥ 0.4 (i.e. account for approximately 16% or more of the explained variance in that factor), and the item could not load on another factor within ±0.1. According to Ho, et al. (2001), the minimally acceptable coefficient is 0.33 because the squared proportion accounts for at least 10% of the variance in a given factor. We dropped one item from factor 4 (item 37) because it loaded within 0.1 on factor 5. We retained the remaining 23 items for further analysis.

Once we determined the items comprising each factor, we obtained composite scores for each factor by summing its respective item scores. With the understanding that these summary scores may obscure the variation of individual items in a given factor, we were interested in the general trends for the purposes of this study. We conducted univariate analyses of variance, using the general linear model, to assess within each group, whether average scores on the factors obtained from the exploratory PCA differentiated willingness to participate in genetic research. Using analyses of covariance, we obtained adjusted mean factor scores after controlling for demographic or other relevant factors, where necessary.

Results

Demographic, clinical, and research characteristics

Table 1 shows the details of the Black, Hispanic and White samples (Nλ=λ353). An additional 52 Asians participated, but because of a small and skewed sample with 78% below the age of 23 years, we excluded them from further analyses. Eighty-three individuals (31%) were recruited from outpatient mental health clinics. Compared with the nonpatient sample, these individuals were more likely to be Black [χ2 (4)λ=λ29.95, P<0.0001], female, [χ2 (2)λ=λ7.26, P<0.05], separated/divorced or widowed [χ2 (4)λ=λ76.91], older [χ2 (4)λ=λ194.44], and less educated [χ2 (4)λ=λ68.21]. Blacks (nλ=λ174), Hispanics (nλ=λ103), and Whites (nλ=λ76) did not differ significantly on sex (57% female), education level (61% college or higher), history of emotional problems/mental illness (42%), sought/received mental illness treatment (72%; of those who reported a history of emotional problems), or knowing friend/family member who suffered from mental illness (43%). Blacks were significantly older than Whites or Hispanics; they were also more likely to be divorced, separated, or widowed than Whites or Hispanics. Whites had more participants who had an annual household income of more than $60,000 than Hispanics or Blacks, but Blacks had more participants earning $20λ000 $59λ000 than Whites or Hispanics. More Hispanics than Blacks or Whites had volunteered to participate in health research in the past.

Willingness to participate in genetic research

More than 70% of each ethnic group stated that they would be willing to participate in genetic research. There were no significant differences between groups [χ2 (2)λ=λ1.66, Pλ=λ0.437]. Table 2 shows the within-group comparisons across demographic, clinical, and research characters by willingness to participate in genetic research. For all the three ethnic groups, willingness to participate was not significantly associated with age, marital status, history of mental illness or treatment seeking, knowing friend/family member who had mental illness, and volunteering for health research in the past. Among Whites, significantly more females (89%) than males (69%) stated that they would be willing to participate. The percentage of Blacks willing to participate increased with education, with 79% of college-educated or higher, 70% of high school graduates, and 53% of those less than high school educated being willing to participate.

Table 2.

Willingness to participate in genetic research by demographics and clinical/research characteristics within ethnic groups

Demographic Blacks (nλ=λ174) Hispanics (nλ=λ103) Whites (nλ=λ76)

Agree to participate (%) χ2 Agree to participate (%) χ2 Agree to participate (%) χ2
λSex
νMale 72 80 69
νFemale 73 0.02 75 0.30 89 4.55*
Age (years)
ν18–29 75 78 75
ν30–49 69 75 83
≥ 50 61 2.27 60 0.90 100 2.04
Education
ν≤High school 53 75 75
νHigh school 70 78 77
ν≥ College 79 5.88* 77 0.03 80 0.11
Marital Status
νSingle 74 77 77
νMarried 59 87 86
νWidowed/divorced 73 2.19 50 2.45 100 1.46
Has suffered mental illness
νYes 72 78 79
νNo 71 0.03 77 0.02 79 0.001
Has sought/received mental illness treatmenta
Yes 67 80 79
No 84 2.03 71 0.42 80 0.01
Knows friend/family with mental illness
νYes 71 83 86
νNo 72 0.02 75 1.01 75 1.49
Has volunteered for health research
νYes 74 86 75
νNo 71 0.12 72 2.31 79 0.04
a

Percentages for this item are based on the numbers reported for history of mental illness – n (Blacks)λ=λ66; n (Whites)λ=λ33; n(Hispanics)λ=λ50.

*

P<0.05, χ2 test two-tailed significance.

Attitudinal factors from the survey assessment

Table 3 lists the factors, items, and their loadings, as well as the reliability coefficient of internal consistency, as measured by Cronbach’s α for each factor. On the basis of the questionnaire items associated with the factors, they were named (1) beneficial to individual and society, (2) important to participate for education and knowledge, (3) mistrust and wariness, (4) desire to learn more about genetic research, (5) stigma, (6) participating only for incentives, (7) mental illness is caused by both genes and the environment, and (8) mental illness is caused primarily by genes. Factors 7 and 8 showed poor internal consistency, but we decided to retain them to examine whether they had any impact on stated willingness to participate.

Table 3.

Attitudinal factors and item loadings

Factors and itemsa Factor loadings
λFactor 1: Beneficial to individual and society (αλ=λ0.82)
ν21. I would participate to help future generations 0.48
ν23. Genetic research is beneficial to Society 0.60
ν24. Genetic research will benefit me 0.73
ν25. Genetic research will benefit people of my race 0.86
ν26. Genetic research will benefit people of other races 0.83
Factor 2: Important to participate for education and knowledge (αλ=λ0.83)
ν11. Genetic research is important for education of patients and health professionals 0.86
ν13. It is important for people to know about genetic causes of mental illness 0.86
ν14. It is important to participate in genetic research when given opportunity to do so 0.69
Factor 3: Mistrust and wariness (αλ=λ0.57)
ν27. Do not trust researchers conducting the genetic studies 0.59
ν30. Worry about private information not being kept private 0.59
ν32. Do not like some procedures associated w/genetic research (e.g. blood draws) 0.55
ν34. Participating in genetic research is a hassle 0.69
Factor 4: Desire to learn more about genetic research (αλ=λ0.83)
ν39. I would like to learn more about genetic research 0.82
ν40. Would like to learn more about the genetic causes of mental illness 0.84
Factor 5: Stigma (αλ=λ0.85)
ν28. Ashamed to participate in research that asks questions about my mental health 0.77
ν29. Ashamed to participate in research that asks questions about my family’s mental health 0.76
Factor 6: Participating only for incentives (αλ=λ0.65)
ν22. Would participate only if getting free meds/treatment 0.80
ν35. Would participate only if getting paid (e.g. cash, gift certificates, etc.) 0.75
Factor 7: Mental illness caused by both genes and environment (αλ=λ0.48)
ν7. Mental health problems are caused by the environment 0.76
ν8. Mental health problems are caused by both genes and environment 0.67
ν10. Certain environments will cause depression regardless of genes 0.52
Factor 8: Mental illness caused primarily by genes (αλ=λ0.29)
ν6. Mental health problems are caused mostly by genes 0.60
ν9. If one has genes for depression, they will become depressed, regardless of environment 0.77
a

The six survey items not listed here, but used in the analysis will be furnished upon request from corresponding author.

Attitudes and willingness to participate within ethnic groups

Table 4 shows the within-group association with eight attitudinal factors by the participants’ stated willingness to participate in genetic research. To ensure that we did not distort the structure of the original data through dichotomization of the item that assessed willingness to participate, we performed within-group linear regressions in which we regressed each factor on the original 4-point coded item. With the exception of a nonsignificant trend becoming significant for stigma among Whites, we obtained identical results; we therefore retained the dichotomized item for clarity and interpretability. As individual item scores could range from 1 (lowest) to 4 (highest), scores on a given factor could range from the number of items in that factor (lowest) to 4 times that number (highest). The scores shown in the table represent the average scores on each factor among those who stated that they would be willing to participate in genetic research (yes), and among those who stated that they would not be willing to participate (no). Factors on which the two groups’ scores differed significantly from each other are noted with asterisks. Blacks who were unwilling to participate in genetic research were more likely to endorse mistrust and wariness, stigma, and participating only for incentives. Hispanics showed similar trends for mistrust and wariness, and stigma. Unlike Blacks and Hispanics, the only two factors that were significantly associated with the willingness to participate for Whites were, beneficial to individual and society, and important to participate for education and knowledge.

Table 4.

Differences in attitudes according to willingness to participate in genetic research by ethnic group

Mean scores (standard deviation)a
Blacks Willing to participate Hispanics Willing to participate Whites Willing to participate

Genetic research attitudes Yes (nλ=λ125) No (nλ=λ48) Yes (nλ=λ79) No (nλ=λ23) Yes (nλ=λ60) No (nλ=λ16)
λBeneficial to individual and society 15.12 (4.76) 15.98 (3.63) 16.47 (3.56) 16.26 (3.52) 17.03 (2.58) 14.88 (2.96)b, **
Important to participate for education and knowledge 9.94 (3.20) 10.17 (2.45) 10.59 (2.13) 10.17 (2.03) 11.18 (1.29) 9.63 (1.48)***
Mistrust and Wariness 8.36 (3.58) 10.78 (2.75)*** 8.44 (2.65) 10.09 (2.65)** 8.76 (2.44) 8.33 (2.80)
Desire to learn more about genetic research 6.71 (2.25) 6.70 (1.67) 6.65 (1.49) 6.61 (1.73) 6.93 (1.44) 6.75 (1.66)
Stigma 3.24 (2.35) 4.07 (1.82)* 3.13 (1.51) 3.87 (1.84)* 3.68 (1.64) 4.25 (1.88)
Participate only for incentives 4.09 (2.53) 5.28 (1.96)*** 5.02 (1.76) 5.13 (2.01) 5.55 (1.43) 5.42 (1.64)
Mental illness caused by both genes and environment 8.01 (2.75) 8.46 (2.12) 8.39 (2.11) 8.83 (1.77) 8.77 (1.54) 8.79 (1.77)
Mental illness caused primarily by genes 4.79 (1.93) 5.10 (1.49) 5.34 (1.47) 5.78 (1.24) 5.15 (1.28) 4.75 (1.46)
a

Higher mean scores indicate greater endorsement of a given factor

b

Analysis of variance based on the general linear model, F-ratio two-tailed significance:

*

P≤0.05.

**

P≤0.01.

***

P≤0.001.

Attitudes and willingness to participate in college-educated participants aged 18–29 years

Although we controlled for education and other demographics in these analyses, we were concerned that simply controlling for these variables was not sufficient to overcome the limitations of a skewed age and education distribution. Age and education are important because studies have suggested that younger, more educated persons have more positive views toward genetic research. To address this potential limitation, we selected a subsample consisting of college-educated, younger (aged 18–29 years) participants, in which the three ethnic groups were comparable on the key demographic variables such as age and education.

In the stratified sample (nλ=λ148), the three groups did not differ significantly on demographics or clinical and research characteristics, recruitment venue, or stated willingness to participate in genetic research (data available upon request). We examined within ethnic group associations among sex, marital status, clinical/research characteristics, and willingness to participate, and found that among Whites, significantly more females (89%) agreed to participate than males (59%, χ2(2)λ=λ5.55, P<0.05). Among Hispanics, all of those who had volunteered in the past for health research were willing to participate, compared with 71% of those who did not (χ2 (2)λ=λ4.51, P<0.05).

Table 5 shows identical analyses as in Table 4, but now restricted to college-educated, young participants aged 18–29 years. The results also show that youth and college education substantially reduced, but did not fully eliminate the relationship between negative attitudes and willingness to participate among Blacks and Hispanics. Among Blacks, the association between stigma, participating for incentives only, and willingness to participate disappeared, but the association between mistrust and wariness and willingness to participate remained. For Hispanics the associations between mistrust, stigma and willingness to participate remained, but were generally weaker than that of Hispanics in the full sample.

Table 5.

Differences in attitudes according to willingness to participate in genetic research by ethnic group (among college-educated who are 18–29 years old)

Mean scores (standard deviation)a
Blacks Willing to participate Hispanics Willing to participate Whites Willing to participate

Genetic research attitudes Yes (nλ=λ45) No (nλ=λ12) Yes (nλ=λ36) No (nλ=λ10) Yes (nλ=λ34) No (nλ=λ11)
λBeneficial to individual and society 15.16 (3.64) 15.33 (2.90) 17.17 (2.73) 15.80 (3.90) 17.41 (2.41) 16.40 (2.53)b, **
Important to participate for education and knowledge 10.36 (2.24) 10.67 (1.72) 10.92 (1.77) 9.90 (1.45) 11.38 (1.13) 10.45 (1.19)
Mistrust and wariness 8.96 (2.31) 11.08 (2.47)*** 8.04 (2.84) 10.50 (2.68)* 8.49 (2.37) 8.48 (2.49)
Desire to learn more about genetic research 6.53 (1.56) 6.08 (1.88) 6.83 (1.25) 6.90 (0.99) 6.84 (1.56) 6.73 (1.64)
Stigma 3.71 (1.73) 3.50 (1.68) 3.00 (1.59) 4.30 (1.50)* 3.83 (1.75) 4.58 (1.84)
Participate only for incentives 5.00 (1.89) 5.83 (1.75) 5.22 (1.74) 4.60 (2.22) 5.74 (1.38) 5.46 (1.45)
Mental illness caused by both genes and environment 8.07 (1.90) 8.33 (2.06) 8.67 (1.69) 7.70 (1.59)** 8.66 (1.60) 9.13 (1.68)
Mental illness caused primarily by genes 4.86 (1.14) 4.66 (1.23) 5.61 (1.08) 5.50 (1.51) 5.20 (1.27) 4.67 (1.34)

For Whites, scores shown are adjusted after controlling for sex. For Hispanics, scores shown are adjusted after controlling for research participation history.

a

Higher mean scores indicate greater endorsement of a given factor

b

Analysis of covariance based on general linear model, F-ratio two-tailed significance:

*

P<0.10.

**

P<0.05.

***

P<0.01.

Discussion

This study used an educationally and ethnically diverse sample to examine attitudes associated with the willingness to participate in genetic research of psychiatric disorders. Even after controlling for potential confounders, Blacks, Hispanics, and Whites did not differ significantly on willingness to participate in a study, if fully informed of the procedures, risks, and benefits involved. However, the demographic characteristics and attitudes that are associated with willingness to participate were different for each ethnic group. The results suggest that among Blacks and Hispanics, willingness to participate in genetic research is more strongly influenced by perceived concerns or deterrents, rather than perceived benefits. The trend seems to be in the opposite direction for Whites, with the exception of stigma, which follows a similar but weaker pattern as in Blacks and Hispanics. Finally, we find evidence to support the idea that among Blacks, education and youthful age reduces but does not eliminate the impact of mistrust on willingness to participate in psychiatric genetic research.

Among Blacks and Hispanics, mistrust, wariness, and stigma emerged as the strongest predictors of willingness to participate in genetic research. This pattern remained even after stratifying by age and education and is consistent with earlier research, which has emphasized this robust association found among Blacks at all socioeconomic levels (Furr, 2002; Laskey et al., 2003; McQuillan et al., 2003). The promise of genetics as a potential tool for more effective detection and prevention of serious disorders represents a double-edged sword, an idea that has not been lost on health research professionals and laypersons. The usual research practice of relying on self-reported racial categories, combined with differential patterns of disease etiologies and outcome reflected among these categories, has raised questions in the minds of some scientists, as to the ultimate purpose and consequence of genetic research. Similarly, for Blacks, and to a lesser extent Hispanics, many of these concerns evoke images of a kind of science that is rooted in eugenics, and hail back to historical research projects that used questionable methodologies – the result of which rationalized the division of society by race, and ethnicity, creating further health disparities (Jackson, 1999; Ossario and Duster, 2005; Shields et al., 2005) and engendering a perception of genetic research as being harmful to society (Jackson, 1999; Furr, 2002). With psychiatric disorders, genetic findings still may be perceived as tentative at best (Biesecker and Peay, 2003; Appelbaum, 2004). Therefore, potential research participants may be wary of participating in an exploratory genetic study with questionable benefit and possibly undesirable outcomes. As other studies have shown, engaging black communities in a manner that is transparent, meaningful and collaborative, and including Black recruiters and investigators, could help to alleviate the pervasive mistrust that exists in this population (Shavers et al., 2002; Clay et al., 2003; Fuqua et al., 2005).

Stigma, like mistrust, also seems to be an important determinant of willingness to participate among Hispanics and less educated Blacks. Mental illness, a constellation of emotions, cognitions and behaviors technically labeled as ‘deviant’, is associated with the concept of stigma, which includes negative labeling and characterization from dominant groups in the societal hierarchy, negative emotional reactions from others, social distancing, loss of societal esteem, and discrimination against the sufferer (Link and Phelan, 2001; Link et al., 2004). According to the attribution theory of genetics, (Weiner et al., 1988), the belief that a disorder or trait is etiologically genetic may lead to the destigmatization of that disorder/trait through reduction of personal blame or responsibility, and increased empathy and caring toward the sufferer and kin. The genetic essentialist theory in contrast (Alper and Beckwith, 1993), predicts that believing in genetic causes would indirectly lead to increased stigma through the perceptions of heightened familial risk, gravity and intractability of the disorder/trait, and genetic determinism.

Using the 1996 General Social Survey data, Schnittker et al. (2000) found that Blacks have been shown to be associated with genetics from a stigmatizing perspective (poor family upbringing), and also showed that the relationship between believing in a genetic cause and seeking professional help was stronger among Blacks than Whites. The overall implications of these findings are that, if Blacks endorse genetic causes, they are more likely to consider them from an essentialist perspective, and they are more likely to seek professional treatment. To the degree that they do not invoke genetic explanations, they are less likely to seek professional treatment for psychiatric disorders, believing that the cause is rooted in society. Similarly, studies investigating research participation showed that Blacks were less likely than Whites to consent for DNA donation storage for genetic research, but Blacks who endorsed genetic causes were more likely to consent for DNA storage than those who did not (Wang et al., 2001).

Contrary to our expectations, beliefs about the relative contribution of genes and environment in psychiatric disorders did not seem to play a significant role, if any, in Blacks’ willingness to participate in genetic research. This may have been because of our particular study items, which were not high in internal consistency (see factors 7 and 8), and, therefore, did not yield particularly meaningful information. In contrast, there have been mixed findings with regard to ethnic group differences in beliefs about genetic contributions to mental disorders (Schnittker et al., 2000; Singer et al., 2007) showing that Blacks may be more or less likely than Whites to endorse genetic causes, depending on the specific disorders or traits, and the way in which the questions are asked. This suggests that the Blacks may be using attribution or essentialist notions of genetics, depending on the situation, thereby canceling out specific effects and leading to different outcomes. However, the scope of this study did not allow for the examination of hypothetical influences of particular theories in determining the relative contribution of genes or the environment, and their effects on willingness to participate in genetic research. This is an important area that calls for further research.

The education hypothesis predicts that Blacks with higher education may be more willing to participate in biomedical research or related activities. We found that education level not only influenced the relationship between attitudes and willingness to participate, but was also directly related with willingness to participate, among Blacks. Lower education may also imply poorer communities, which typically are associated with physical distance from research sites, lack of medical insurance required for participation in some studies, working shifts that are at odds with researchers’ schedules, difficulty in taking time off from work to participate, other time pressures, and inability or unwillingness on the part of researchers to adequately engage these communities (Wendler et al., 2006). Researchers’ inability to recruit Blacks compared with Whites may be further exacerbated by the structural realities of differential educational attainment among Blacks and Whites along with racial and ethnic clustering in residential settings. In the general US population, Blacks, on average, have lower education levels than Whites, with 14.3% of Blacks reporting a bachelor’s degree, compared with 26% among Whites (US Census Bureau, Census 2000 Brief, 2003). In less affluent urban areas, populated predominantly by Blacks and other ethnic minorities, residents tend to have lower levels of education, compared with those who live in more ethnically integrated neighborhoods, where their concentrations are lower. If more educated Blacks are the ones who are more inclined to participate in genetic research, then it is probable that recruitment would be more difficult as the more educated residents are more sparsely distributed in a given location.

Although our findings suggest that recruitment efforts might be directed toward Blacks who have had at least a high school education, and preferably some college-level education, caution needs to be exercised in light of some of the study limitations, including relatively small samples, which were conveniently recruited. Moreover, we only examined the stated willingness and not actual enrollment to genetic studies. Although our reports about the respondents’ past participation in health research showed consistent patterns with their reports on willingness to participate in genetic research, there can be discrepancies between people’s responses on a survey and their actual behavior. Finally, the exploratory nature of this study leaves room for further refinement of measures that assess perceived barriers and attitudes toward research of psychiatric disorders.

Along with the need for continued research in this area, our findings have provided additional hints that are consistent with earlier studies. Despite the consensus that education may reduce the effect of stigma and mistrust on willingness to participate in genetic research, less educated or poorer Blacks should not be summarily excluded. However, the additional time, financial, and human resources required to adequately engage such communities, need to be factored into the planning and design of studies in which they are to be recruited. Although this may improve response rates for genetic studies, there remains significant work to be done in terms of addressing the significant reasons for mistrust associated with genetic research. The expected increase in molecular genetic research of psychiatric disorders over the next decade duly calls for continued attention to the perceived barriers and potential concerns expressed by populations that are underrepresented in such research.

Acknowledgments

This study was made possible by funding support from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (NARSAD) – Young Investigator Award and NIMH Diversity Supplemental Grant (Murphy E), and NIMH Grants P01 MH60970 and R01 MH036197-19 (Weissman M).

Dr Weissman M: currently receives research support from the NIMH, NIDA, NARSAD, and the Sakler Foundation and receives royalties from Oxford University Press, Perseus Press, American Psychiatric Association Press, and Multi-healthSystems. None of the royalties relate to this study.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

Murphy Eand Wickramaratne P: none.

References

  1. Alper JS, Beckwith J. Genetic fatalism and social policy: the implication of behavior genetics research. Yale J Biol Med. 1993;66:511–524. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anglin DM, Link BG, Phelan JC. Racial differences in stigmatizing attitudes toward people with mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 2006;57:857–862. doi: 10.1176/ps.2006.57.6.857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Appelbaum PS. Ethical issues in psychiatric genetics. J Psychiatr Pract. 2004;10:343–351. doi: 10.1097/00131746-200411000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Audrain J, Tercyak Goldman P, Bush A. Recruiting adolescents into genetic studies of smoking behavior. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2002;11:249–252. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Badner AJ, Gershon ES. Meta-analysis of whole-genome linkage scans of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. 2002 doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bates BR, Harris TM. The Tuskegee study of untreated syphilis and public perceptions of biomedical research: a focus group study. J Natl Med Assoc. 2004;96:1051–1064. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Biesecker B, Peay L. Ethical issues in psychiatric genetics research: points to consider. Psychopharmacology. 2003;171:27–35. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1502-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bogner HR, Wittink MN, Merz JF, et al. Personal characteristics of older primary care patients who provide a buccal swab for apolipoprotein E testing and banking of genetic material: the Spectrum Study. Community Genet. 2004;7:202–210. doi: 10.1159/000082263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Breslau J, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Kendler KS, Su M, Williams D, Kessler RC. Specifying race-ethnic differences in risk for psychiatric disorder in a USA national sample. Psychol Med. 2006;36:57–68. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Buchwald D, Mendoza-Jenkins V, Croy C, McGough H, Bezdek M, Spicer P. Attitudes of urban American Indians and Alaska natives regarding participation in research. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:648–651. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00449.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Clay C, Ellis MA, Amodeo M, Fassler I, Griffin ML. Recruiting a community sample of African American subjects: the nuts and bolts of a successful effort. Families in Society. 2003;84:396–404. [Google Scholar]
  12. Cowan WM, Kopinsky KL, Hyman SE. The Human genome project and its impact on psychiatry. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2002;25:1–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fuqua SR, Wyatt SB, Andrew ME, Sarpong DF, Henderson FR, Cunningham MF, Taylor HA., Jr Recruiting African-American research participation in the Jackson Heart Study: methods, response rates, and sample description. Ethn Dis. 2005;15 (Suppl 6):18–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Furr LA. Perceptions of genetic research as harmful to society: differences among samples of African-Americans and European-Americans. Genet Test. 2002;6:25–30. doi: 10.1089/109065702760093889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Gelertner J, Kranzler H, Coccaro E. Serontonin transorter protein gene polymorphism and personality measures in African American and European American subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:1332–1338. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.10.1332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Gelertner J, Cubells J, Kidd J, et al. Population studies of polymorphisms of the serontonin transporter protein gene. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88:61–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Halbert CH, Gandi OH, Jr, Collier A, Shaker L. Intentions to participate in genetics research among African American smokers. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2006;15:150–153. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-05-0437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hama A. Racial differences in pain perception: a biological basis. Mankind Quarterly. 2000;41:91–107. [Google Scholar]
  19. Hasin DS, Goodwin RD, Stinson FS, Grant BF. Epidemiology of major depressive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:1097–1106. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hernstein RJ, Murray C. The Bell curve: intelligence and class structure in American life. New York: Free Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS. A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158:1568–1578. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ho R, ForsterLee L, ForsterLee R, Crofts N. Justice versus vengeance: motives underlying punitive judgments. Pers Individual Differences. 2001;33:365–377. [Google Scholar]
  23. Jackson F. African-American responses to the Human Genome Project. Public Understanding Sci. 1999;8:181–191. [Google Scholar]
  24. Jackson J, Torres M, Caldwell C, et al. The national survey of American life: a study of racial, ethnic and cultural influences on mental disorders and mental health. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13:196–207. doi: 10.1002/mpr.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Jolliffe IT. Principal component analysis. 2. New York: Springer; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kendler KS. Twin studies of psychiatric illness: current status and future directions. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50:905–915. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820230075007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kendler KS. Twin studies of psychiatric illness: an update. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:1005–1014. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Kendler KS, Prescott CA. A population-based twin study of lifetime major depression in men and women. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56:39–44. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.1.39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kessler R, McGonagle K, Zhao S, Nelson C, Hughes M, Eshleman S, et al. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51:8–19. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010008002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Laskey SL, Williams J, Pierre-Louis J, O’Riordan M, Matthews A, Robin NH. Attitudes of African-Americans premedical students toward genetic testing and screening. Genet Med. 2003;5:49–52. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200301000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Levinson D. Molecular genetics of schizophrenia: a review of the recent literature. Current Opin Psychiatry. 2003;16:157–170. [Google Scholar]
  32. Levinson DF, Evgrafov OV, Knowles JA, Potash JB, Weissman MM, Scheftner WA, et al. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:259–264. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Link BG, Phelan JC. Conceptualizing stigma. Annual Rev Sociol. 2001;27:363–385. [Google Scholar]
  34. Link BG, Yang L, Phelan JC, Collins P. Measuring mental illness stigma. Schizophr Bull. 2004;30:511–542. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lohmueller KE, Pearce CL, Pike M, Lander ES, Hirschorn JM. Meta-analysis of genetic association studies supports a contribution to common variants to susceptibility to common disease. 2003 doi: 10.1038/ng1071. Http://www.nature.com/Naturegenetics. [DOI] [PubMed]
  36. Lotrich FE, et al. Polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism: basis research and clinical implication. J Neural Transm. 1997;104:1005–1014. doi: 10.1007/BF01273314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lotrich FE, Pollock BG, Ferrell RE. Serotonin transporter promotor polymorphism in African Americans: allele frequencies and implications for treatment. Am J Pharmacogenet. 2003;3:145–147. doi: 10.2165/00129785-200303020-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lyons MJ, Eisen SA, Golberg J, True W, Lin N, Meyer JM, et al. A registry-based twin study of depression in males. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55:468–472. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.5.468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McMahon F, Buevernich S, Charney D, Lipsky R, et al. Variation in the gene encoding the serotonin 2A receptor is associated with outcome of antidepressant treatment. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;M:M–M. doi: 10.1086/503820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. McQuillan G, Porter K, Agelli M, Kington R. Consent for genetic research in a general population: the NHANES experience. Genet Med. 2003;5:35–42. doi: 10.1097/00125817-200301000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Moorman PG, Skinner CS, Evans JP, et al. Racial differences in enrollment in a cancer genetics registry. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2004;13:1349–1354. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Murphy EJ, Thompson A. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes. National Institutes of Health (2001). NIH policy and guidelines on the inclusion of women and minorities as subjects in clinical research; Amended: October , 2001 . An exploration of attitudes among Black Americans towards psychiatric genetic research. in press. Http://grants1.nih.gov/grants/funding/women_min/guidelines_amended_10_2001.htm. [Google Scholar]
  43. National Institutes of Mental Health. Genetic roots of bipolar disorder revealed by first genome-wide study of illness. [25 September 2008];Science Daily. 2007 http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2007/05/07/070508072825.htm.
  44. Ossorio P, Duster T. Race and genetics: controversies in biomedical and forensic sciences. Am Psychol. 2005;60:115–128. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Rao D, Feinglass J, Corrigan P. Racial and ethnic disparities in mental illness stigma. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2007;195:1020–1023. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31815c046e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rutter M. Genetic studies of autism: from the 1970λs into the millennium. Journal of Abnorm Child Psychol. 2000;28:3–14. doi: 10.1023/a:1005113900068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sankar P, Cho M, Condit C, Hunt L, Koenig B, Marshall P, et al. Genetic research and health disparities. J Am Med Assoc. 2004;291:2985–2989. doi: 10.1001/jama.291.24.2985. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Schnittker J, Freese J, Powell B. Nature, nurture, neither, nor: Black-White differences in beliefs about the causes and appropriate treatment of mental illness. Soc Forces. 2000;78:1101–1130. [Google Scholar]
  49. Schulz A, Caldwell C, Foster S. What are they going to do with the information? Latino/Latina and African American perspectives on the human genome project. Health Educ Behav. 2003;30:151–169. doi: 10.1177/1090198102251026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Schwartz MD, Rothenberg K, Joseph L, Benkendorf J, Lerman C. Consent to the use of stored DNA for genetics research: A survey of attitudes in the Jewish population. Am J Med Genet. 2001;98:336–342. doi: 10.1002/1096-8628(20010201)98:4<336::aid-ajmg1100>3.0.co;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Shavers VL, Lynch CF, Burmeister LF. Racial differences in factors that influence the willingness to participate in medical research studies. Ann Epidemiol. 2002;12:248–256. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(01)00265-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Shields A, Fortun M, Hammonds E, King P, Lerman C, Rapp R, Sullivan P. The use of race variables in genetic studies of complex traits and the goal of reducing health disparities: a transdisciplinary perspective. Am Psychol. 2005;60:103. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.77. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Singer E, Antonucci T, Van Hoewyk J. Racial and ethnic variations in knowledge and attitudes about genetic testing. Genet Test. 2004;8:31–43. doi: 10.1089/109065704323016012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Singer E, Antonucci T, Burmeister M, Couper MP, Raghunathan TE, Van Hoewyk J. Beliefs about genes and environment as determinants of behavioral characteristics. Int J Pub Opinion Res. 2007;19:331–353. [Google Scholar]
  55. Somervile P, Leaf P, Weissman M, Bruce M. The prevalence of major depression in Black and White adults in five United States communities. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1989;130:725–735. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) for Windows. Rel. 14.0.1. Chicago: SPSS Inc; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  57. Sterling R, Henderson GE, Corbie-Smith M. Public willingness to participate in and public opinions about genetic variation research: a review of the literature. American Journal of Public Health. 2006;96:1971–1978. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.069286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Sullivan PF, Neal MC, Kendler KS. Genetic Epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:1552–1562. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sullivan PF, Kendler KS, Neale MC. Schizophrenia as a complex trait: evidence From a meta-analysis of twin studies. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 2003;60:1187–1192. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Uhl G, Grow RM. The burden of complex genetics in brain disorders. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:223–229. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wang SS, Fridinger F, Sheedy KM, Khoury MJ. Public attitudes regarding the donation and storage of blood specimens for genetic research. Commun Gen. 2001;4:18–26. doi: 10.1159/000051152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Weiner BR, Perry P, Magnusson J. An attributional analysis of reactions to stigmas. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55:738–748. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.5.738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Merikangas KR, Leckman JF, Prusoff BA, Caruso KA, et al. Onset of major depression in early adulthood. Increased familial loading and specificity. Arc Gen Psychiatry. 1984;41:1136–1143. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790230022003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wendler D, Kington R, Madans J, Van Wye G, Christ-Schmidt H, Pratt L, et al. Are racial and ethnic minorities less willing to participate in health research? PLoS Med. 2006;3:e19. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0030019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Whaley A. Ethnic and racial differences in perceptions of dangerousness of persons with mental illness. Psychiatr Serv. 1997;48:1328–1330. doi: 10.1176/ps.48.10.1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wheelwright J. Native America’s alleles. Discover. 2005;M:65–70. [Google Scholar]
  67. Williams DR. Race, SES and health: the added effects of racism and discrimination. Ann NY Academy Sci. 1999;896:173–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb08114.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Williams DR, Earl TR. Commentary: race and mental health – more questions than answers. Int J Epidemiol. 2007;36:758–760. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES