Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009 Mar 25;34(8):2011–2018. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.15

The Brattleboro Rat Displays a Natural Deficit in Social Discrimination that is Restored by Clozapine and a Neurotensin Analogue

D Feifel 1,*, S Mexal 2, G Melendez 1, P Y T Liu 1, J Goldenberg 1, P D Shilling 1
PMCID: PMC2744457  NIHMSID: NIHMS86159  PMID: 19322170

Abstract

Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia are a major source of dysfunction for which more effective treatments are needed. The vasopressin-deficient Brattleboro (BRAT) rat has been shown to have several natural schizophrenia-like deficits, including impairments in prepulse inhibition and memory. We investigated BRAT rats and their parental strain, Long Evans (LE) rats, in a social discrimination paradigm, which is an ethologically-relevant animal test of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia based upon the natural preference of animals to investigate conspecifics. We also investigated the effects of the atypical antipsychotic, clozapine and the putative antipsychotic, PD149163, a brain-penetrating neurotensin-1 analogue, on social discrimination in these rats. Adult rats were administered saline or one of three doses of clozapine (0.1, 1.0 or 10 mg/kg) or PD149163 (0.1, 0.3 or 1.0 mg/kg), subcutaneously. Following drug administration, adult rats were exposed to a juvenile rat for a 4-minute learning period. Animals were then housed individually for 30 minutes and then simultaneously exposed to the previously presented juvenile and a new juvenile for 4 minutes. Saline-treated LE rats, but not BRAT rats, exhibited intact social discrimination as evidenced by greater time spent exploring the new juvenile. The highest dose of clozapine and the two highest doses of PD149163 restored social discrimination in BRAT rats. These results provide further support for the utility of the BRAT rat as a genetic animal model relevant to schizophrenia and drug discovery. The potential of neurotensin agonists as putative treatments for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia was also supported.

Keywords: Animal Model, Antipsychotics, Brattleboro Rat, Clozapine, Cognitive Deficit, Social Discrimination, Schizophrenia, Neurotensin

INTRODUCTION

Cognitive deficits of schizophrenia are a core feature of schizophrenia and several other neuropsychiatric disorders. Cognitive deficits of schizophrenia are among the first detectable symptoms and are consistent from the first episode throughout middle age (Hagan and Jones, 2005). Evidence suggests that cognitive deficits have strong adverse effects on the functional status of schizophrenia patients (Green et al, 2000; 2004). The Measure and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) committee identified seven distinct and replicable domains of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia: verbal memory, recognition memory, attention, executive function, social cognition, reasoning, and problem solving (Green, 2006).

Current antipsychotic drugs have, at best, modest effects on cognitive deficits of schizophrenia in the vast majority of schizophrenia subjects (Gold, 2004; Hagan and Jones, 2005). There is strong evidence that atypical antipsychotics generally produce an incomplete amelioration of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia, whereas there is controversy regarding whether typical antipsychotics produce an improvement in cognitive deficits of schizophrenia that is comparable to atypical antipsychotics or inferior to them (Green et al, 2002; Harvey et al, 2005; Keefe et al, 2006; Mishara and Goldberg, 2004; Remillard et al, 2005; Rollnik et al, 2002; Medalia et al, 1988; Stip et al, 2005; Lublin et al, 2005).

Increasing efforts are focused on developing drugs with robust benefits for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. However, several limitations hamper this effort, including a relative dearth of well-established and validated animal models for these deficits. No single preclinical test is likely to model all domains of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Since the pathophysiology of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia, like non-cognitive deficits of schizophrenia features of schizophrenia, is not well understood, developing animal models of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia with etiological validity is challenging. Establishing predictive validity for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia presents another vexing problem since there are no established treatments with complete efficacy.

Pragmatic issues represent still another limitation to the development of new treatments for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. Most preclinical cognitive tests are labor intensive, requiring training animals on a task for several consecutive days and thus hampering the ability to conduct high-throughput drug screening.

Paradigms examining novelty recognition in animals have been proposed as a valuable preclinical model of cognitive deficits that are seen in schizophrenia (Hagan and Jones, 2005). Social recognition and social discrimination are two related examples of preclinical paradigms that assess socially based novelty discrimination and do not require pre-test training of animals (Engelmann et al, 1995).

Investigators have previously focused on pharmacological approaches to induce, in animals, cognitive deficits seen in schizophrenia. For example, Terranova et al demonstrated that acute administration of the psychotomimetics PCP and D-amphetamine disrupted social discrimination in Wistar rats (Terranova et al, 2005). In addition, they found that subchronic administration of PCP during the neonatal period produced spontaneous deficits in social discrimination in adult rats. These deficits were reversed by the atypical antipsychotics clozapine and amisulpride, the putative atypical antipsychotic SSR181507 (Terranova et al, 2005). These findings suggest that drug-induced disruption of social discrimination may be a useful animal model of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia with predictive utility for potential pro-cognitive drugs. However, in regards to developing animal models with optimal utility for drug discovery, pharmacologically-induced social discrimination deficits have the potential to introduce added experimental complexity, data variability, as well as potentially limiting drug discovery to agents, which specifically block the effects of the deficit-inducing drug (Floresco et al, 2005). An animal model based on innate social discrimination deficits could therefore provide a complementary alternative approach to those that required experimental manipulation.

Brattleboro (BRAT) rats are Long Evans (LE)-derived rats with a single-gene mutation that disrupts vasopressin secretion (Birkett and Pickering, 1988). BRAT rats homozygous for the mutation display a number of behaviors that are characteristic of those seen in schizophrenia patients, including abnormalities in memory (Laycock et al, 1983), stress reactivity (Williams et al, 1985), social recognition (Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994), and motivation (Williams et al, 1983) and attention (Williams et al, 1983) (but see, Jentsch et al, 2003). We have shown that BRAT rats exhibit natural schizophrenia-like deficits in prepulse inhibition of startle (PPI), a measure of sensorimotor gating processes. Furthermore, these PPI deficits are reversed by antipsychotics but not non-antipsychotic psychotropics. These findings suggest that the BRAT rat may be a good genetic animal model with predictive validity for antipsychotic drug efficacy. BRAT rats have been shown to exhibit social recognition deficits (Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994). Preliminary exploratory studies from our laboratory revealed that BRAT rats also appeared to exhibit deficits compared to LE rats in social discrimination.

The current study had several goals. First, we sought to confirm that BRAT rats exhibit natural social discrimination deficits. We also sought to begin to investigate the response of social discrimination deficits in BRAT to antipsychotics and putative antipsychotics that have been associated with pro-cognitive effects. In that regard, we tested the effects of the atypical antipsychotic clozapine on social discrimination in BRAT and LE rats, since clozapine has been shown to improve learning and delayed recall in schizophrenia patients (Woodward et al, 2005), two cognitive features required for social discrimination. Furthermore, clozapine reverses the PPI deficits in BRAT rats (Feifel et al, 2004; 2007) and scopolamine-induced social discrimination deficits in rats (Terranova et al, 2005). We also tested PD149163, which is a brain-penetrating agonist of the neurotensin-1 receptor. PD149163 is a putative antipsychotic drug, reversing PPI deficits in BRAT rats (Feifel et al, 2004; Feifel et al, 2007) and demonstrating pro-cognitive effects in novel object recognition, a preclinical test similar to social discrimination (Azmi et al, 2006).

METHODS

Animals

Experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the University of California, San Diego guidelines for animal care and experimentation. All animals were purchased from Harlan Laboratories, San Diego, CA. Ninety-eight adult male BRAT rats and sixty-eight adult male LE rats (170–350 gms at testing) were housed in groups of two or three in clear plastic chambers in a climate controlled room under a 12/12 h light/dark schedule (lights on/off – 0700/1900). Male Sprague Dawley juvenile rats (25 – 30 days old) were used as social stimuli. All rats were allowed free access to food and water for the extent of the study. Behavioral testing was performed no earlier than 7 days after animals' arrival from the breeder. Each adult and each juvenile rat was used only one time.

Behavioral Procedure

The social discrimination procedure was modified from the social recognition test described by Engelmann et al, 1995. The social discrimination apparatus is made of a large plastic tub (30″ × 17″ × 12″) consisting of two small wire cages (6.5″ × 5″ × 4″) on either ends of the large tub in which the juvenile rats were placed. The purpose of the small wire cages was to restrict mobility of juvenile rats while allowing the adult rat visual, olfactory, and tactile access to the juveniles. This eliminated social contact that was initiated by the juveniles toward the adult. It also prevented simultaneous contact of the adult rat with both juveniles as well as juvenile-juvenile interaction, simplifying social interaction scoring. Each test trial included one adult rat, one familiar, previously exposed juvenile (familiar juvenile), and one novel juvenile. The social discrimination paradigm is illustrated in Figure 1. Briefly, adult rats acclimate in the social discrimination box for 20 minutes. Following this acclimation period, the learning trial begins with the adult rat exposed to a juvenile rat in one of the two small wire cages, randomly selected, for 4 minutes. The adult rat is then returned to its home cage for 30 minutes. During the test trial the adult animal is returned to the social discrimination box. Then the familiar juvenile, along with a novel juvenile, are placed in the two wire cages. In half the trials, the familiar juvenile returns to the same wire cage that the animal was initially placed in during the learning trial. In the other half of the trials, the rat is placed in the alternate wire cage. The adult rat is allotted four minutes to explore the juveniles. All sessions in the social discrimination box are videotaped and the tapes are scored by two raters blinded to the drug treatment and the strain of the rats. Each rater recorded the time each adult rat spent exploring the juvenile during learning trial, and the time the animal spent with the familiar and unfamiliar juvenile during the subsequent test trial.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Illustration of the Social Discrimination Paradigm. Adult rats acclimate in the social discrimination box for 20 minutes. Following this acclimation period, the learning trial begins with the adult rat exposed to a juvenile rat for 4 minutes. The adult rat is then returned to its home cage for 30 minutes. During the retrieval trial the adult animal returns to the chamber with the introduction of a new juvenile along with the same juvenile from the learning trial for 4 minutes. The positions of the juveniles are randomly alternated

Drug Treatment

Ten minutes prior to the acclimation period, animals were injected subcutaneously with either saline, clozapine (0.1 mg/kg, 1 mg/kg, 10 mg/kg) or PD149163 (0.1 mg/kg, 0.3 mg/kg or1.0 mg/kg). Social discrimination was then evaluated as described above. These doses selected for clozapine and PD149163 were based on the dose range found to be effective in reversing PPI deficits occurring naturally in BRAT rats and induced by psychotomimetics (Feifel et al, 2004; Feifel et al, 2007; Feifel et al, 1999; Feifel et al, 2003). Clozapine was obtained from Sigma Chemicals, St. Louis, MO and PD149163 was provided courtesy of NIMH Chemical Synthesis and Drug Supply program, and SRI International, Menlo Park, CA. Clozapine was dissolved in half volume HCl and half volume 0.9% saline then titrated with NaOH to PH 7.0 . PD149163 was dissolved in 0.9% saline. Drugs were administered subcutaneously at a concentration of 1 ml/kg.

Statistical Analysis

Prior to analysis, data from animals that did not display any interaction (zero seconds) with a juvenile in either the learning or test periods were not included in the analysis, since animals that did not investigate the juvenile during the learning phase (zero seconds) could not be used to generate data on the test trial that could legitimately be considered a reflection of their retention of previous exposure to the juvenile. Similarly, any rat that displayed zero investigation of both novel and familiar juveniles during testing displayed non-investigatory behavior that cannot be considered a valid test of social memory. Ten LE and nineteen BRAT rats given PD149163 and two LE and ten BRAT rats given clozapine were, therefore, eliminated as was one BRAT rat given saline.

A Discrimination Index was calculated to determine preference of adult rats for exploration of the novel juvenile relative to the familiar juvenile in the test session. This index was calculated for each adult rat as percentage increase above the time each it spent exploring the familiar juvenile using the following formula: [(time exploring novel juvenile - time exploring familiar juvenile) / (time spend exploring familiar juvenile + time spent exploring novel juvenile)] (Grayson et al, 2007; Southam et al, 2008). Data were analyzed using the SPSS Explore function and extreme outliers (> 3 standard deviations from the mean excluded from the analysis). One LE rat in the high dose clozapine group and one LE rat in high dose PD149163 group were considered outliers using this analysis.

To determine whether each strain exhibited social discrimination under baseline (saline) conditions, absolute time spent with familiar juvenile and novel juvenile were compared using separate paired samples t-test for saline-treated LE and BRAT rats.

For LE rats, Discrimination Index, time exploring juvenile in the learning phase, time spent exploring familiar juvenile during test phase, and time spent exploring the novel juvenile during the test phase were each subjected to separate one-way ANOVAs with Drug Treatment as a between subject factor having seven elements (saline, clozapine × 3 doses, PD149163 × 3 doses). Drug effects for BRAT rats were assessed separately using an identical approach. Significant effects were followed up with pair-wise post-hoc comparisons using Fishers LSD. Alpha was set at 0.05.

RESULTS

Baseline Social Discrimination

As shown in Table 1, saline-treated LE rats spent significantly more time with the novel juvenile compared to familiar juvenile (t(23)=−2.80, P=0.01), whereas there was no significant difference between the time spent with novel and familiar juveniles among saline-treated BRAT rats (Table 2). There was no significant difference between LE and BRAT rats in the average time each strain spent exploring the juvenile rats during the learning period.

Table 1.

Social Interaction Time (seconds) Exhibited by Long Evans Rats

TRIAL SALINE CLOZAPINE (mg/kg) PD149163 (mg/kg)
0.1 1.0 10 0.01 0.3 1.0
Learning Exposure 142.13 ± 8.10 113.00 ± 13.85 114.50 ± 9.88 102.71 ± 12.84* 107.00 ± 20.65 104.38 ± 28.21 100.14 ±12.26*
Test Period-
Familiar Juvenile
49.29 ± 5.11 52.13 ± 8.60 67.71 ± 16.85 60.37 ± 16.31 62.88 ± 12.01 61.13 ± 17.46 52.58 ± 11.76

Mean ± Std.

*

Error Mean P< 0.05, significantly less time spent investigating versus the saline-treated rats

#

P<0.05, significantly greater compared to familiar juvenile.

Table 2.

Social Interaction Time (seconds) Exhibited by Brattleboro Rats

TRIAL SALINE CLOZAPINE (mg/kg) PD149163 (mg/kg)
0.1 1.0 10 0.01 0.3 1.0
Learning Exposure 125.71 ± 6.71 91.13 ± 18.09 88.33 ± 14.10 85.50 ± 15.97* 72.36 ± 22.26** 53.27 ± 12.42** 69.60 ± 14.96**
Test Period-
Novel Juvenile
55.10 ± 3.90 43.50 ± 10.91 41.67 ± 8.48 50.63 ± 14.32 62.64 ± 10.57 55.27 ± 13.89 66.87 ± 14.85
Test Period-
Familiar Juvenile
47.38 ± 3.46 44.88 ± 8.87 20.67 ± 4.41** 21.13 ± 5.34** 47.09 ± 7.35 24.09 ± 7.94** 31.13 ± 8.11*

Mean ± Std.

*

Error Mean P< 0.05 significantly less time spent investigating versus the saline-treated rats.

**

Error Mean P<0.01 significantly less time spent investigating versus the saline-treated rats.

Effect of Clozapine and PD149163 on Social Discrimination

As shown in Table 1, analysis of the LE data from the learning exposure period revealed that there was a general tendency for clozapine and PD149163 to reduce the time spent interacting with the juvenile during the learning exposure period although the main effect of drug treatment was not significant. Post hoc pair-wise comparison indicated that the interaction time exhibited by saline-treated LE rats during the learning period was significantly higher than the interaction time exhibited by LE rats treated with the highest dose of clozapine (P<0.05) and PD149163 (P<0.05). Analysis of Discrimination Index data revealed no significant main effect of Drug Treatment in the LE rats (Figure 2). Planned post-hoc comparisons revealed that the 0.3 mg/kg dose of PD149163 significantly increased the Discrimination Index relative to saline in LE rats (P<0.05). There was no significant effect of either drug on the time LE rats spent interacting with the novel juvenile compared to saline. Similarly, there was no significant effect of either drug on the time LE rats spent interacting with the familiar juvenile compared to saline.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The effects of saline (n=20), PD149163 and clozapine (n = 7-8 per dose) on the Discrimination Index in LE rats. Discrimination Index equals (time spent investigating new juvenile - time spent investigating familiar juvenile) / (time spent investigating new juvenile + time spent investigating familiar juvenile). Significantly higher than saline is represented by * (P< 0.05).

In BRAT rats, clozapine and PD149163 produced a more pronounced reduction in exploration of the juvenile during the learning period (Table 2) and this was revealed as a significant main effect of Drug Treatment (F6,97=5.48, P<0.001) in this strain. Post-hoc pair-wise comparisons revealed that BRAT rats treated with saline have significantly higher interaction times with the juvenile during the learning period compared with BRAT rats treated with all three doses of clozapine (P=0.047, P=0.023, P=0.013, respectively) and all three doses of PD149163 tested (P=0.002, P<0.001, and P<0.001, respectively).

Figure 3 illustrates the effects of clozapine and PD149163 on social discrimination in BRAT rats. Analysis of Discrimination Index data revealed there was a significant main effect of drug treatment (F6,97=5.79, P<0.001). Post hoc comparison revealed that the highest dose of clozapine, 10 mg/kg (P<0.001) and the two highest doses of PD149163, 0.3 mg/kg (P<0.001) and 1.0 mg/kg (P=0.02) significantly increased the Discrimination Index compared to saline in BRAT rats.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

The effects of saline (n = 36), PD149163 and clozapine (n = 11-15 per dose) on the Discrimination Index in BRAT rats. Discrimination Index equals (time spent investigating new juvenile - time spent investigating familiar juvenile) / (time spent investigating new juvenile + time spent investigating familiar juvenile). Significantly higher than saline is represented by * (P< 0.05) and ** (P< 0.01).

Both clozapine and PD149163 decreased the time BRAT rats spent interacting with the familiar juvenile during the test period (Table 2), revealed as a main effect of drug treatment (F6,104=4.68, P < 0.001). Post-hoc comparisons showed that the middle and high doses of clozapine (1.0 mg/kg: P<0.001, 10.0 mg/kg: P<0.001) and PD149163 (0.3 mg/kg: P<0.001, 1.0mg/kg: P=0.006) produced a significant reduction in interaction with the familiar juvenile. There was no significant effect of either drug on the time BRAT rats spend interacting with the novel juvenile compared to saline.

As seen in Table 1, neither clozapine nor PD149163 had a significant effect on the time LE rats spent with the novel juvenile during the test period. However in BRAT rats, clozapine and PD149163 tended to produce different effects, neither of which achieved significance. Clozapine tended to decrease, whereas PD149163 tended to increase time spent interacting with the novel juvenile.

DISCUSSION

The findings in this report confirm that BRAT rats exhibit natural social discrimination impairment since saline-treated BRAT rats spent comparable amount of time interacting with the novel and familiar juvenile rats. In contrast, saline-treated LE had intact social discrimination as they spent significantly greater time interacting with the novel juvenile. The social discrimination deficits observed in BRAT rats are consistent with previous studies showing that this strain does not exhibit intact social memory in a social recognition paradigm (Engelmann and Landgraf, 1994). Moreover, the social discrimination deficit exhibited by BRAT rats seemed to be a true deficit in social memory rather than a non-specific reduction in social motivation or general activity, since saline-treated BRAT and LE rats displayed comparable time exploring the juvenile rat during the learning period.

The BRAT rat, to the best of our knowledge, is the only rat strain to display natural social discrimination deficits. Vasopressin receptor and oxytocin receptor null mice have also been shown to exhibit natural deficits in social memory (Bielsky et al, 2004;Wersinger et al, 2004; Winslow and Insel, 2002). Oxytocin and vasopressin are chemically and functionally related neurohormones that have both been implicated in the regulation of social behavior.

Prior to this study, BRAT rats were already known to display several features analogous to schizophrenia, including deficits in memory (Laycock et al, 1983), response to stress (Williams et al, 1985), motivation and attention (Williams et al, 1983), startle habituation and prepulse inhibition (Feifel and Priebe, 2001), as well as elevated levels of striatal D2 binding (Shilling et al, 2006). The current demonstration of natural social discrimination deficits in BRAT rats adds to the schizophrenia-relevant phenotypic profile these animals display.

In this study, the atypical antipsychotic clozapine produced a dose-dependent enhancement of social discrimination in the BRAT rats. Clozapine has been shown to reverse PCP-induced, novel object recognition deficits, an animal test that is very similar to social discrimination (Grayson et al, 2007) and the current results are consistent with results such as this from related preclinical tests. In regards to clozapine's effects on cognitive deficits in humans, a meta-analysis of treatment studies of schizophrenia patients revealed that, overall, clozapine enhances cognitive performance in schizophrenia patients and specifically two cognitive domains relevant to social discrimination: learning and delayed recall (Woodward et al, 2005). Therefore, it is reasonable to interpret the improvement in BRAT rat social discrimination seen with clozapine as consistent with the clinical experience with clozapine, and thus providing preliminary predictive validity for the BRAT rat model of cognitive deficits. Tests of other antipsychotics will be needed in order to more conclusively establish this contention. However, establishing predictive validity for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia is not a straightforward matter since the benefits of antipsychotics are modest and somewhat controversial. For example, while it is generally accepted that atypical antipsychotics such as clozapine improve cognitive deficits of schizophrenia, there is conflicting evidence regarding whether typical antipsychotics such as haloperidol produce improvements in cognitive deficits of schizophrenia (Green et al, 2002; Harvey et al, 2005; Keefe et al, 2006; Mishara and Goldberg, 2004; Remillard et al, 2005; Rollnik et al, 2002; Medalia et al, 1988; Stip et al, 2005; Lublin et al, 2005). Haloperidol is not effective in some animal models of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia, namely those based on PCP-induced deficits, whereas it does ameliorate the deficits in other preclinical cognitive deficits of schizophrenia models (e.g. Becker & Grecksch, 2006; Dunn and Kilcross, 2006; Besbalov et al, 2007). Thus, it would be harder to interpret the haloperidol findings in regards to their implications for predictive validity and for that reason we chose to test clozapine in this initial study of cognitive deficits in BRAT rats.

In contrast to its effects in BRAT rats, clozapine did not improve social discrimination in LE rats. In fact, it had the opposite effect, negating the natural social discrimination seen in saline-treated LE rats (Table 1). Therefore, the results suggest that the beneficial effects of clozapine on social discrimination were specific to the BRAT rats. This specificity is another aspect that supports validity for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia, since antipsychotics such as clozapine are not considered to be general cognitive enhancers in normal humans or patients that do not have psychotic disorders, and is considered a cognitive impairing drug in humans that do not suffer from schizophrenia or other related psychotic disorders due to its propensity to produce anticholinergic and sedating effects.

PD149163 is an analog of neurotensin, a thirteen amino acid gut-brain peptide. Neurotensin has been proposed to be an endogenous antipsychotic due to, among other things, its ability to inhibit mesolimbic dopamine function (Kinkead and Nemeroff, 2006). While neurotensin does not enter the brain after systemic administration, PD149163 was designed to do so. This compound was developed by chemically modifying the c-terminal hexapeptide fragment, neurotensin(8-13), the smallest fragment that retains the full bioactivity of neurotensin, to make it more resistant to enzymatic degradation (Banks et al, 1995). In previous preclinical studies with PD149163 we found that it produced robust antipsychotic-like effects more similar to atypical than typical antipsychotics. In BRAT rats, a single administration of PD149163 was able to reverse their PPI deficits, as did clozapine but not haloperidol (Feifel et al, 2004). This effect persisted in BRAT rats after chronic administration of PD149163 (Feifel et al, 2007). Recently, Azmi et al, (2006) found that PD149163 administered to rats restored novel object recognition impaired by the anticholingergic scopolamine (Azmi et al, 2006) and that same group also reported that PD149163 increased associative learning in a trace conditioning paradigm (Grimond-Billa et al, 2008). Consistent with those findings and with clozapine effects in this study, acute administration of PD149163 improved social discrimination in this study in BRAT rats, but unlike clozapine the most effective dose of PD149163 in BRAT rats (0.3 mg/kg) also improved social discrimination in LE rats.

The social discrimination test is based upon the social recognition test in which only the familiar juvenile is re-introduced during the testing session and reduction in time spent investigating that juvenile is evidence of intact social memory (Engelmann et al, 1994). The introduction of a second, novel juvenile in the discrimination test was used primarily to control for non-specific reductions in investigation, e.g. sedation, that might affect the second exposure. The improvement in Discrimination Index scores by clozapine and PD149163 in this study was due specifically to decrease in the time investigating the familiar juveniles, whereas investigation time of the new juveniles was not altered. Thus, the effect produced by clozapine and PD149163 of reduced exploration of the familiar juvenile is the classical operational measure of social memory (Engelmann et al, 1994).

The exact mechanisms underlying PD149163's pro-cognitive effects in this and previous studies are not known. PD149163 is selective for the NT1 receptor (Petrie et al., 2004), the neurotensin receptor subtype most implicated in neurotensin's antipsychotic-like effect. Azmi et al speculate the NT1-mediated enhancement of muscarinic transmission may be involved. PD149163 has also been shown to increase neuronal activation in the prefrontal cortex (Azmi et al, 2006) and this activation may contribute to its cognitive effects (Petrie et al, 2004). In any case, these results enhance the candidacy of NT1 agonists as potentially novel antipsychotics that may have benefit for cognitive deficits of schizophrenia. It is interesting that the dose of PD149163 that was most effective in increasing social discrimination in BRAT rats (0.3 mg/kg) also improved social discrimination LE rats. This suggests that PD149163 may facilitate non-impaired memory.

It is noteworthy that both clozapine and PD149163 enhanced social discrimination in test trials despite that both drugs reduced exploration time of LE and BRAT rats during the learning period. This reduction in exploration during the learning period is likely due to a non-specific inhibitory effect on activity since both drugs are known to decrease spontaneous locomotor activity (Wiley, 2008; Norman et al. 2008). Thus, it appears that the reduction in exploratory contact during the learning period produced by clozapine and PD149163 was more than compensated for by enhanced information acquisition during this period in BRAT rats but not LE rats; in essence, clozapine produced more efficient learning in BRAT rats (more retention despite less opportunity to learn). This seemingly paradoxical effect has been reported with PD149163 and atypical antipsychotics in the novel object recognition test which is similar to social discrimination (Azmi et al, 2006; Nagai et al, 2008). Indeed, among rats treated with clozapine and PD149163, there was a inverse relationship between time spent interacting with the juvenile during the learning phase and their subsequent social discrimination performance (Pearson r = −0.182, p=0.06); the more a rat's learning period interaction was suppressed, the better they performed subsequently on social discrimination. Therefore, the mechanism underlying the reduction of spontaneous locomotion produced by these drugs may also be involved in the enhanced cognitive performance. Some rats treated with higher doses of clozapine and PD149163 had to be eliminated from the data analysis altogether, since they displayed a profound reduction in spontaneous locomotion and no interaction at all (zero seconds) with the juvenile during the learning period. It is unlikely that this selective elimination of rats contributed to the positive finding on the social discrimation performance that we observed due to the inverse relationship noted above between interaction suppression and enhancement of social discrimination.

In this study clozapine and PD149163 improved social discrimination after a single administration. This is consistent with most other preclinical tests of antipsychotics and other cognitive enhancing treatments where significant effects are seen following a single dose. It is commonly believed that improvement of cognitive deficits of schizophrenia occurs only with chronic use of antipsychotics and not after a single dose. However, a similar common assumption about the time course of antipsychotic drug benefits on the core symptoms of schizophrenia has recently been proven inaccurate by recent meta-analyses of clinical data that reveal significant benefits begin after a single dose of antipsychotics but require chronic use to achieve optimal effects (Agid et al, 2006; Agid et al, 2008). It is possible therefore that the same holds true for improvement in cognitive deficits of schizophrenia by antipsychotics, but the time course of antipsychotic-induced cognitive deficits of schizophrenia improvements has not been studied. In this regard, we have shown that the natural PPI deficits in BRAT rats are enhanced by a single administration of atypical antipsychotics and this effect increases with chronic administration (Feifel et al, 2007). Future studies should investigate the relative effects of acute and chronic clozapine, PD149163 and other antipsychotics on social discrimination deficits in BRAT rats.

Social discrimination abnormalities in BRAT rats, like the PPI deficits exhibited by this rat strain, may serve as a genetic animal model of schizophrenia cognitive deficits. Further study of this model may aid in elucidating novel genes or pathways implicated in the neuropathology of the disorder. In addition, the results of this study serve as preliminary evidence that the social discrimination deficits in BRAT rats may serve as a predictive model of novel antipsychotic efficacy in cognitive domains.

Footnotes

DISCLOSURE/CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Dr. Feifel serves as a consultant and minor share holder for Argolyn Biosciences, which is developing novel neurotensin analogs for clinical use.

Dr. Feifel has received compensation for professional services (research, speaking, consultant) in the past three years (or anticipates compensation in the next 3 years (for the following companies: Abbott, Addrenex, Alexza, Astra-Zeneca, Eli-Lilly, Janssen, Macneil, Sanofi-Aventis, Shire, Wyeth.

There are no other author conflicts.

REFERENCES

  1. Agid O, Kapur S, Warrington L, et al. Early onset of antipsychotic response in the treatment of acutely agitated patients with psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia research. 2008;102:241–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Agid O, Seeman P, Kapur S. The “delayed onset” of antipsychotic action--an idea whose time has come and gone. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience. 2006;31:93–100. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Azmi N, Norman C, Spicer CH, Bennett GW. Effects of a neurotensin analogue (PD149163) and antagonist (SR142948A) on the scopolamine-induced deficits in a novel object discrimination task. Behav Pharmacol. 2006;17:357–362. doi: 10.1097/01.fbp.0000224382.63744.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Banks WA, Wustrow DJ, Cody WL, Davis MD, Kastin AJ. Permeability of the blood-brain barrier to the neurotensin 8-13 analog NT1. Brain Res. 1995;695:59–63. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00836-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Becker A, Grecksch G. Pharmacological treatment to augment hole board habituation in prenatal Vitamin D-deficient rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2006;166(1):177–83. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bespalov A, Jongen-Rêlo AL, van Gaalen M, et al. Habituation deficits induced by metabotropic glutamate receptors 2/3 receptor blockade in mice: reversal by antipsychotic drugs. The Journal of Pharmacology and Pxperimental Therapeutics. 2007;320:944–50. doi: 10.1124/jpet.106.110684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bielsky IF, Hu SB, Szegda KL, Westphal H, Young LJ. Profound impairment in social recognition and reduction in anxiety-like behavior in vasopressin V1a receptor knockout mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:483–493. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Birkett SD, Pickering BT. The vasopressin precursor in the Brattleboro (di/di) rat. Int J Pept Protein Res. 1988;32:565–572. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-3011.1988.tb01388.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Buchanan RW, Davis M, Goff D, Green MF, Keefe RS, Leon AC, Nuechterlein KH, Laughren T, Levin R, Stover E, Fenton W, Marder SR. A summary of the FDA-NIMH-MATRICS workshop on clinical trial design for neurocognitive drugs for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:5–19. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Dunn MJ, Killcross S. Differential attenuation of d-amphetamine-induced disruption of conditional discrimination performance by dopamine and serotonin antagonists. Psychopharmacology. 2006;188:183–92. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0488-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Engelmann M, Landgraf R. Microdialysis administration of vasopressin into the septum improves social recognition in Brattleboro rats. Physiol Behav. 1994;55:145–149. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90022-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Engelmann M, Wotjak CT, Landgraf R. Social discrimination procedure: an alternative method to investigate juvenile recognition abilities in rats. Physiol Behav. 1995;58:315–321. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(95)00053-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Feifel D, Melendez G, Priebe K, Shilling PD. The effects of chronic administration of established and putative antipsychotics on natural prepulse inhibition deficits in Brattleboro rats. Behav Brain Res. 2007;181:278–286. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.020. 149163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Feifel D, Melendez G, Shilling PD. A systemically administered neurotensin agonist blocks disruption of prepulse inhibition produced by a serotonin-2A agonist. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:651–653. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300083. 149163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Feifel D, Melendez G, Shilling PD. Reversal of sensorimotor gating deficits in Brattleboro rats by acute administration of clozapine and a neurotensin agonist, but not haloperidol: a potential predictive model for novel antipsychotic effects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:731–738. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300378. 149163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Feifel D, Priebe K. Vasopressin-deficient rats exhibit sensorimotor gating deficits that are reversed by subchronic haloperidol. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50:425–433. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01100-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Feifel D, Reza TL, Wustrow DJ, Davis MD. Novel antipsychotic-like effects on prepulse inhibition of startle produced by a neurotensin agonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 1999;288:710–713. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Floresco SB, Geyer MA, Gold LH, Grace AA. Developing predictive animal models and establishing a preclinical trials network for assessing treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:888–894. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Geyer MA, Krebs-Thomson K, Braff DL, Swerdlow NR. Pharmacological studies of prepulse inhibition models of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia: a decade in review. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;156:117–154. doi: 10.1007/s002130100811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gold JM. Cognitive deficits as treatment targets in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;72:21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Grayson B, Idris NF, Neill JC. Atypical antipsychotics attenuate a subchronic PCP-induced cognitive deficit in the novel object recognition task in the rat. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;184:31–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Green AI, Tohen MF, Hamer RM, Strakowski SM, Lieberman JA, Glick I, Clark WS. First episode schizophrenia-related psychosis and substance use disorders: acute response to olanzapine and haloperidol. Schizophr Res. 2004;66:125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Green MF. Cognitive impairment and functional outcome in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2006;67:e12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Green MF, Kern RS, Braff DL, Mintz J. Neurocognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia: are we measuring the “right stuff”? Schizophr Bull. 2000;26:119–136. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Green MF, Marder SR, Glynn SM, McGurk SR, Wirshing WC, Wirshing DA, Liberman RP, Mintz J. The neurocognitive effects of low-dose haloperidol: a two-year comparison with risperidone. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:972–978. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01370-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Grimond-Billa SK, Norman C, G W B, Cassaday HJ. Selectively increased trace conditioning under the neurotensin agonist PD149163 149163 in an aversive procedure in which SR 142948A was without intrinsic effect. J Psychopharmacol. 2008;22:290–299. doi: 10.1177/0269881106081528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hagan JJ, Jones DN. Predicting drug efficacy for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2005;31:830–853. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Harvey PD, Rabinowitz J, Eerdekens M, Davidson M. Treatment of cognitive impairment in early psychosis: a comparison of risperidone and haloperidol in a large long-term trial. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162:1888–1895. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1888. 149163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jentsch JD, Arguello PA, Anzivino LA. Null mutation of the arginine-vasopressin gene in rats slows attentional engagement and facilitates response accuracy in a lateralized reaction time task. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28:1597–605. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Keefe RS, Young CA, Rock SL, Purdon SE, Gold JM, Breier A. One-year double-blind study of the neurocognitive efficacy of olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;81:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kinkead B, Nemeroff CB. Novel treatments of schizophrenia: targeting the neurotensin system. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2006;5:205–218. doi: 10.2174/187152706776359655. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Laycock JF, Gartside IB, Chapman JT. A comparison of the learning abilities of Brattleboro rats with hereditary diabetes insipidus and Long-Evans rats using positively reinforced operant conditioning. Prog Brain Res. 1983;60:183–187. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)64385-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lublin H, Eberhard J, Levander S. Current therapy issues and unmet clinical needs in the treatment of schizophrenia: a review of the new generation antipsychotics. Int Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;20:183–198. doi: 10.1097/00004850-200507000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Medalia A, Gold J, Merriam A. The effects of neuroleptics on neuropsychological test results of schizophrenics. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1988;3:249–271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Mishara AL, Goldberg TE. A meta-analysis and critical review of the effects of conventional neuroleptic treatment on cognition in schizophrenia: opening a closed book. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1013–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nagai T, Murai R, Matsui K, et al. Aripiprazole ameliorates phencyclidine-induced impairment of recognition memory through dopamine D(1) and serotonin 5-HT (1A) receptors. Psychopharmacology. 2008 doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1240-6. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Norman C, Grimond-Billa S, Bennett G, et al. A neurotensin agonist and antagonist decrease and increase activity, respectively, but do not preclude discrete cue conditioning. Journal of psychopharmacology. 2008 doi: 10.1177/0269881108097721. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Petrie KA, Bubser M, Casey CD, Davis MD, Roth BL, Deutch AY. The neurotensin agonist PD149163 increases Fos expression in the prefrontal cortex of the rat. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:1878–1888. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Remillard S, Pourcher E, Cohen H. The effect of neuroleptic treatments on executive function and symptomatology in schizophrenia: a 1-year follow up study. Schizophr Res. 2005;80:99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.07.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rollnik JD, Borsutzky M, Huber TJ, Mogk H, Seifert J, Emrich HM, Schneider U. Short-term cognitive improvement in schizophrenics treated with typical and atypical neuroleptics. Neuropsychobiology. 2002;45:74–80. doi: 10.1159/000048680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Shilling PD, Kinkead B, Murray T, Melendez G, Nemeroff CB, Feifel D. Upregulation of striatal dopamine-2 receptors in Brattleboro rats with prepulse inhibition deficits. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:1278–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.045. 149163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Southam E, Cilia J, Gartlon JE, et al. Preclinical investigations into the antipsychotic potential of the novel histamine H(3) receptor antagonist GSK207040. Psychopharmacology. 2008 doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1310-9. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Stip E, Chouinard S, Boulay LJ. On the trail of a cognitive enhancer for the treatment of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2005;29:219–232. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2004.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Terranova JP, Chabot C, Barnouin MC, Perrault G, Depoortere R, Griebel G, Scatton B. SSR181507, a dopamine D(2) receptor antagonist and 5-HT(1A) receptor agonist, alleviates disturbances of novelty discrimination in a social context in rats, a putative model of selective attention deficit. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;181:134–144. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2268-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Vincent JP. Neurotensin receptors: binding properties, transduction pathways, and structure. Cell Mol Neurobiol. 1995;15:501–512. doi: 10.1007/BF02071313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Wersinger SR, Kelliher KR, Zufall F, Lolait SJ, O'Carroll AM, Young WS., III Social motivation is reduced in vasopressin 1b receptor null mice despite normal performance in an olfactory discrimination task. Horm Behav. 2004;46:638–645. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Wiley JL. Antipsychotic-induced suppression of locomotion in juvenile, adolescent and adult rats. European Journal of Pharmacology. 2008;578:216–21. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2007.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Williams AR, Carey RJ, Miller M. Behavioral differences between vasopressin-deficient (Brattleboro) and normal Long-Evans rats. Peptides. 1983;4:711–716. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(83)90023-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Williams AR, Carey RJ, Miller M. Altered emotionality of the vasopressin-deficient Brattleboro rat. Peptides. 1985;1(6 Suppl):69–76. doi: 10.1016/0196-9781(85)90013-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Winslow JT, Insel TR. The social deficits of the oxytocin knockout mouse. Neuropeptides. 2002;36:221–229. doi: 10.1054/npep.2002.0909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Woodward ND, Purdon SE, Meltzer HY, Zald DH. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological change to clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, and risperidone in schizophrenia. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005;8:457–472. doi: 10.1017/S146114570500516X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES