Abstract
Background
Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST) is a semantic treatment that aims to improve lexical retrieval of content words in sentence context by promoting systematic retrieval of verbs (e.g., measure) and their thematic roles (i.e., agent (doer of the action, e.g., carpenter, chef)) and patient (receiver of the action, e.g., lumber, sugar)). VNeST is influenced by Loverso and colleagues (e.g., Loverso, Selinger, and Prescott, 1979), who used “verb as core” treatment to improve sentence production with encouraging results, and McRae and colleagues, who showed that verbs prime typical agents (e.g., pray-nun) and patients (arrest-criminal) (Ferretti, McRae, & Hatherell, 2001) and vice-versa (McRae, Hare, Elman, & Ferretti, 2005).
Aims
There are four specific questions in this study. Does training a set of verbs using VNeST generalize to the ability to produce 1) an agent (carpenter), trained verb (measure), and patient (stairs) in response to novel picture stimuli and 2) an agent (nurse), untrained semantically related verb (weigh), and patient (baby) in response to novel picture stimuli? 3) Are generalization effects maintained?, and 4) Does VNeST generalize to the ability to retrieve nouns and verbs not directly related to treatment items in single word naming, picture description and connected speech tasks?
Methods & Procedures
Four participants with aphasia participated. Participants received VNeST, which involves retrieval of agent-patient pairs (e.g., chef/sugar, surveyor/land) related to trained verbs (e.g., measure), two times per week. A single subject, repeated probe, multiple baseline experimental design was used. Generalization to sentence production for sentences containing trained verbs and untrained semantically related verbs was tested weekly.
Outcomes & Results
Results demonstrated generalization to lexical retrieval of content words in sentences with trained and untrained verbs across participants. Additionally, pre- to post-treatment generalization was observed on single verb and noun naming and lexical retrieval in sentences across a variety of tasks across participants. Generalization to connected speech was observed for 3 of 4 participants.
Conclusions
Though preliminary, these results indicate that VNeST may be effective in promoting generalization from single word naming to connected speech in persons with moderate aphasia. A number of clinical implications related to treatment efficiency are discussed.
Keywords: aphasia, semantic therapy, verb, generalization, connected speech, thematic roles
Many studies have now shown that speech-language therapy has a significant and in some cases, quite large treatment effect in subjects with aphasia. However, most of these studies involve some variation of “naming therapy” in which the subject is taught to name a set of objects and the outcome measure is performance improvement on this set (Nickels, 2002). However, because there is usually no relationship between word meaning and word sound, naming treatments are intrinsically non-generalizing. Knowledge gained tends to be limited to the words actually trained, and there is at best very modest improvement in naming performance with untrained words (Kiran & Thompson, 2003; McNeil, 1997). Expectably, subjects generally show minimal improvement in their ability to communicate in daily life. Achieving large-scale generalization is essential if speech-language therapy is going to improve the daily communicative lives of people with aphasia.
Unlike naming therapy directed to lexical semantic deficits, therapy directed to semantic deficits might be expected to intrinsically generalize to the extent that trained items share features with untrained items (Plaut, 1996). Counter-intuitively, training on a spectrum of unusual exemplars of a category can be more effective in inducing generalization than training on typical exemplars (Plaut, 1996). This is because unusual exemplars convey information about both the core regularities defining the category (which help to distinguish it from other categories) and the full range of regularities that are crucial in distinguishing all the different within-category exemplars from each other. Recent clinical work has confirmed this concept (Kiran & Thompson, 2003). Semantic treatments potentially have broad applicability to aphasia rehabilitation because most left hemisphere strokes either damage cortices supporting semantic representations or the interface between these cortices and perisylvian language cortex (Roth, Nadeau, Hollingsworth, Cimino-Knight, & Heilman, 2006). Furthermore, knowledge represented in association cortices in the right hemisphere may usefully contribute to aphasia recovery, but further training of right hemisphere semantic networks may be required for this to happen.
A variety of approaches to semantic therapy have been employed with some success (Raymer & Rothi, 2000). These have included: 1) word-picture matching tasks using semantically related foils; 2) answering yes-no questions about semantic features of pictured objects; 3) semantic sorting of objects; 4) variously cued matching of semantic associates as the number and relatedness of semantic foils is increased; 5) correction of naming errors by provision of additional semantic information that distinguishes the erroneous response from the correct response; and 6) systematic training in the semantic features of objects. Generally these approaches might be characterized as “item-centric” in that the focus is on enabling the subject to differentiate the item in question from near neighbors in its domain. Knowledge of particular semantic domains (e.g., animals) might usefully be fleshed out using item-centric techniques, but semantic knowledge that spans the breadth of daily life is difficult to achieve with this approach.
In a series of studies, Loverso and colleagues introduced a treatment conceptually similar to that of the current study (Loverso, Prescott, & Selinger, 1988; Loverso, Selinger, & Prescott, 1979; Loverso, Prescott, Selinger, Wheeler, & Smith, 1985). The treatment, originally known as “verb as core” (Loverso et al., 1979) and later known as Cueing Verbs Treatment (CVT) (Loverso et al., 1988) was originally developed from theories in cognitive psychology and generative semantics that proposed that the verb is the predicate core of all simple sentences and specifies the relationships between concepts (e.g., Filmore, 1968). Participants moved through levels of the treatment, and the levels and tasks were refined over the series of studies. Tasks included generating, copying, writing and repeating the agent and patient for the presented verbs and answering “wh” questions about them. Outcome measures showed pre- to post-treatment improvement in performance on the Porch Index of Communicative Abilities (PICA, Porch, 1973) (Loverso et al., 1979; Loverso et al., 1988) and improved ability to generate agents and patients for trained and untrained verbs (Prescott et al., 1982).
The current treatment, Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST), focuses on what we will call the predicative components of the distributed semantic representation of concepts, which are expressed linguistically as verbs. Predicative components are features of noun concept representations that add meaning explicitly sustained dynamically over a period of time, in contrast to adjectival components, which are static and enduring. Thus, running, leaping, barking and whining can be predicative components of a dog concept representation, whereas black, shaggy, large, or friendly can be adjectival components. Both adjectival and predicative components can be features of many noun concept representations. In addition, because verbs can have more than one thematic role (corresponding to arguments), a given predicative component can simultaneously be a feature of two or even three noun concept representations, and can link these representations to each other. Thus, measuring is something that can be done by carpenters, chefs, surveyors, and designers and to lumber, sugar and land (Figure 1) and therefore it constitutes a predicative component of each of these concept representations. Consistent with these theoretical principles, McRae and colleagues have shown bidirectional priming/co-activation of verbs and their thematic roles, so that a verb primes typical agents (e.g., arresting/policeman), patients (arresting/criminal), and instruments (e.g., cutting/scissors) (Ferretti, McRae, & Hatherell, 2001) and vice versa (McRae, Hare, Elman, & Ferretti, 2005), indicating that the meaning of a verb is not separate from its thematic roles but is dependent on them (Druks, 2002; Ferretti et al., 2001, Jackendoff, 1972). From the perspective of parallel distributed processing networks (Rumelhart, Smolensky, McClelland, & Hinton, 1986), the principle of content addressable memory posits that a predicative component can engage, at least to some degree, all the various concepts that can incorporate that component as a feature.
As figure 1 illustrates, a given verb can be a predicative component to an extraordinarily diverse array of concepts (whether as agent or patient). Furthermore, because VNeST requires the subject to generate agents and patients for verbs, it may plausibly encourage her to focus on the full spectrum of plausible predicative components for each agent and patient, in short, the full action milieu of a carpenter, chef or surveyor. To the extent that the participant is successful in engaging these various concept representations, as reflected in success in retrieving them, there is an opportunity for Hebbian learning to occur within the neural networks supporting the concepts, thereby providing the basis for more successful retrieval in the future.
VNeST may take advantage of another potentially important mechanism. Engaging the semantic representation of a verb (which can become a feature of the multi-component distributed representation underlying a noun concept, as discussed) can partially engage the semantic representations of other verbs that share features with that verb (e.g., weigh and measure in Figure 1) but are predicative components of somewhat different sets of concepts. This mechanism could potentially expand the extent of the semantic field influenced by the therapy.
The basic task of VNeST is to generate agent and patient pairs to a target verb (e.g., chef/sugar, carpenter/lumber, surveyor/land for measure) with the intent of strengthening the connections between the verb and its thematic roles. The anticipation was of a broad semantic impact that would be reflected not only in improved ability to retrieve trained verbs and their thematic nouns but also untrained verbs and their thematic roles. The specific questions posed in this study were: 1) Does training a set of verbs using VNeST generalize to the ability to produce 1) an agent (carpenter), trained verb (measure), and patient (stairs) in response to novel picture stimuli and 2) an agent (nurse), untrained semantically related verb (weigh), and patient (baby) in response to novel picture stimuli? 3) Are generalization effects maintained?, and 4) Does VNeST generalize to the ability to retrieve nouns and verbs not directly related to treatment items in single word naming, picture description and connected speech tasks?
Method
Participants
Four participants with aphasia were recruited from Austin, TX area hospitals and the University of Texas Speech and Hearing Clinic. Participants met several selection criteria: 1) aphasia due to single left hemisphere stroke 9+ months before participation, 2) monolingual English speaking, 3) impaired lexical access for nouns and verbs, 4) no other neurological or learning disorder, 5) right-handed prior to stroke, 6) adequate hearing, vision, and comprehension to engage fully in testing and treatment. (See Table 1 for demographic information).
Table 1.
Participant | M/F | Age | Education (years) |
Occupation | Site of lesion |
MPO | Type of aphasia |
WAB AQ |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | M | 52 | 10 | Body mechanic | Left MCA | 10 | TMA | 76.4 |
2 | F | 63 | 16 | Computer programmer | Left MCA | 96 | TMA | 78.5 |
3 | F | 75 | 16 | Retired school teacher | Left MCA | 22 | Conduction | 73.8 |
4 | F | 56 | 14 | Musician | Large Left MCA | 21 | Conduction | 70.6 |
M: Male; F: Female; MCA: Middle Cerebral Artery; MPO: Months post onset; WAB AQ: Western Aphasia Battery Aphasia Quotient; TMA: Transcortical motor aphasia
Participants who met inclusion criteria underwent several specific assessments to characterize aphasia type and severity and lexical retrieval impairments. (See Table 2 for details). On the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB, Kertesz, 1982), Participants 1 and 2 (P1 and P2) exhibited moderate transcortical motor aphasia with no more than mild-moderate apraxia of speech (Dabul, 2001) whereas Participants 3 and 4 (P3 and P4) exhibited moderate conduction aphasia.
Table 2.
Participant 1 | Participant 2 | Participant 3 | Participant 4 | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
pre-tx | post-tx | pre-tx | post-tx | pre-tx | post-tx | pre-tx | post-tx | |
TESTS AND TASKS WITH INCLUSION CRITERIA |
||||||||
Western Aphasia Battery (AQ) | 76.4 | 82.5 | 78.5 | 86.4 | 73.8 | 81.2 | 70.6 | 82.3 |
Information | 7 | 9 | 8 | 9 | 9 | 9 | 8 | 9 |
Fluency | 5 | 6 | 4 | 6 | 8 | 9 | 7 | 8 |
Comprehension | 8.6 | 8.15 | 9.35 | 9.9 | 8.1 | 8.9 | 7.7 | 9.85 |
Repetition | 9.4 | 9.8 | 9.6 | 9.5 | 3.9 | 6.2 | 4.9 | 6.4 |
Naming | 8.2 | 8.3 | 8.3 | 8.8 | 7.9 | 7.5 | 7.7 | 7.9 |
Northwestern Verb Productin Battery | ||||||||
Sentence production without verb provided*(N = 22) |
22 (54.5%) |
19 (86.4%) |
13 (59.1%) |
18 (81.8%) |
16 (72.7%) |
19 (86.4%) |
22 (31.8%) |
22 (63.6%) |
TESTS AND TASKS TO EVALUTE PRE-POST CHANGES |
||||||||
Boston Naming Test (N = 60) |
43 (71.7%) |
50 (83%) |
49 (81.7%) |
54 (90%) |
27 (45%) |
33 (55%) |
27 (45%) |
41 (68.3) |
NVPB (Single verbs) (N = 22) |
16 (72.7%) |
19 (86.4%) |
16 (72.7%) |
16 (72.7%) |
18 (81.8%) |
20 (90.9) |
11 (50.0%) |
13 (59.1%) |
tx: treatment
The Northwestern Verb Production Battery protocol requires showing and reading aloud the verbs for each sentence to be produced. However, the verbs were not seen or heard by the participants in this study during administration of the test in order to evaluate better noun and verb retrieval changes
Single word lexical retrieval was evaluated with the Boston Naming Test (BNT, Goodglass, Kaplan, & Weintraub, 1983) and the Northwestern Verb Production Battery (NVPB, Thompson, 2002). The NVPB consists of 25 pictures that show people or animals engaging in a common action (e.g., barking, climbing, putting). Verbs identical to treatment verbs were omitted (e.g., fry, write). Participants exhibited a range of abilities on the BNT (Goodglass et al., 1983) with the participants with nonfluent aphasia performing better than those with fluent aphasia. The predominant error type on the BNT for P1 (N = 17 errors) was semantic (70.6%) followed by unrelated (17.6%) and No Response/I don’t know (NR/IDK) (11.8%) errors. P2’s predominant errors (N = 11 errors) were also semantic (54.5%) followed by NR/IDK (36.4%) and phonemic (9.1%) errors. Error types were more varied for participants 3 and 4. P3’s predominant error type (N = 27 errors) was phonemic (39.4%) followed by NR/IDK (33.3%), semantic (12.1%), neologisms (9.1%) and unrelated words (6.1%). P4’s predominant error type (N = 27 errors) was NR/IDK (45.5%) followed by neologisms (24.2%), phonemic (21.2%), semantic (6.1%), and unrelated words (3.0%).
Verb naming accuracy on the NVPB was also variable across participants with no predictable pattern across aphasia type. With respect to verb naming errors, Participants 1 (N = 6 errors) and 3 (N = 4 errors) produced only semantic errors (100%). P4 (N = 11 errors) predominantly produced semantic errors as well (54.5%) followed by NR/IDK (36.4%) and unrelated words (9.1%). The predominant error for P2 (N = 6 errors) was NR/IDK (66.7%) followed by semantic (33.3%).
Lexical retrieval in a sentence context was also evaluated with the NVPB (Thompson, 2002). Picture stimuli elicit sentences with the same verbs used in the single word context (e.g., The dog is barking., The boy is climbing a tree., The woman is putting the box on the shelf.). This test’s protocol typically involves showing and reading the verb to the participant. However, in order to evaluate the participants’ abilities without verb assistance, the verb was not shown to or heard by the participants. Since lexical retrieval was of primary interest, participants were given credit for a sentence if all the required lexical items were present regardless of word order or inflection of the verb. Accuracy varied across participants with a range of 31.8 to 72.7%.
Connected Speech Samples
Connected speech samples were collected prior to the initiation of treatment to evaluate participants’ lexical retrieval abilities in discourse. Two picture description tasks, the WAB picnic picture (Kertesz, 1982) and cookie theft picture (Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983), and a Cinderella narrative were used. For picture description, participants were instructed to describe what was happening in the pictures and to use complete sentences. For the Cinderella task, participants reviewed the story by looking at the pictures in a book (without words), and then they told the story without the book. No prompts were provided except occasional encouragement. The responses for all three connected speech tasks were combined into one sample. They were transcribed by the first author and a trained Communication Sciences and Disorders undergraduate employing Quantitative Production Analysis (QPA: Saffran, Berndt, & Schwartz, 1989).
A standard measures analysis in Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts (SALT) (Language Analysis Lab, 1984–2006) was conducted to determine total number of utterances and mean length of utterance (MLU). However, of primary interest was whether the participants showed changes in the proportion of utterances consisting of two elements: 1) lexical retrieval of an appropriate agent and verb (and patient when necessary) and 2) relevant meaning to a listener aware of the topic. These elements were examined because it was expected that the ability to retrieve the content words of a sentence would improve and that the words retrieved would contain more semantic specificity (i.e., relevance). Hereafter, utterances containing a relevant agent + verb + (object) will be referred to as complete utterances. Acceptable errors included grammatical, morphological, reference, and phonemic errors as well as circumlocutions since these aspects of connected speech were not targeted in treatment. However, a complete utterance could not be an abandoned utterance or fragment, even if part of the utterance was correct. Likewise, a perfectly formed utterance not related to the stimuli was not considered a complete utterance.
The following are examples of utterances that represent all possible scoring combinations. The end of each utterance is marked with a + or − to indicate whether an utterance contained required content words (+/−SV) and relevant meaning (+/− RELEVANT).
The tree is open [+SV][−RELEVANT]. (Not complete utterance — Irrelevant)
The son is flying is a kite [+SV][+RELEVANT]. (Complete utterance)
To walk through the step [−SV][−RELEVANT]. (Not complete utterance — Incomplete subject and verb combination and irrelevant)
A little guy with some sand on the shore with his hands the sand [−SV][+RELEVANT]. (Not complete utterance — missing main verb)
After the utterances were coded, the number of complete utterances was divided by total number of utterances to determine the percentage of complete utterances. Before treatment, participants showed deficits in producing complete utterances (range = 50.4% to 62.5% of total utterances).
After initial scoring by the first author, a licensed speech-language pathologist familiar with speech output of persons with aphasia re-scored one complete, randomly chosen transcript for each participant. Reliability (expressed as agreement between each scorer’s count of complete utterances) was 93% for coding of complete utterances.
Stimuli Development/Validation
Sentence Elicitation Pictures
Twenty-four pictures were developed for baseline and treatment probes. All sentences elicited an agent, verb, and patient. The agents and patients portrayed had specific titles in most cases (e.g., nurse, carpenter) to promote specific language use instead of use of generic words (e.g., lady, man). Sentences were divided into two sets (verb set 1 and verb set 2) (Table 3). Sets were created so that each verb in one set was semantically related to a verb in the other set (e.g., measure/weigh). As determined by Student’s t-test, there was no significant difference in frequency (Coltheart, 1981) between the agents and patients for verb set 1 (t(24) = 0.35, p = 0.73) or verb set 2 (t(23) = 0.57, p = 0.57) or between the agents (t(25) = 1.36, p = 0.19) and patients (t(22) = 0.48, p = 0.63) in the two verb sets. All pictures designed for this study were hand-drawn by the same artist and were colored with similar color complexity. The drawings were approximately 4” × 6” and centered on 8–1/2” × 11” white paper. A healthy younger group (10 females and 1 male; average age = 36.4 years; average education = 17.2 years) filled out a questionnaire that required them to rate verb pairs on a scale of 1 (not related) to 7 (highly related) for semantic relatedness. Only verb pairs with a rating of > 4.5 were selected for this study (average rating = 5.53 (SD = 0.94)). (See Table 3).
Table 3.
Verb Set 1 | Verb Set 2 | Sem Rel (Avg./SD) |
Target sentence | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Item | Freq | Image | Fam | Item | Freq | Image | Fam | Verb set 1 | Verb set 2 | |
Bake | 12 | 495 | 549 | Fry | 2 | n/a | n/a | 5.54/0.52 | The chef is baking a pie. | The cook is frying eggs. |
Throw | 42 | 477 | 548 | Kick | 16 | 551 | 563 | 5.18/0.75 | The tennis player is throwing his racquet. | The soccer player is kicking the ball. |
Sew | 6 | 478 | 517 | Knit | 10 | n/a | n/a | 5.82/0.87 | The tailor is sewing a jacket. | The mother is knitting a sweater. |
Read | 173 | 499 | 568 | Write | 106 | 548 | 560 | 5.54/1.13 | The student is reading a book. | The policeman is writing a ticket. |
Fly | 33 | 582 | 537 | Drive | 105 | n/a | n/a | 5.0/0.77 | The pilot is flying an airplane. | The chauffeur is driving a limousine. |
Measure | 91 | 379 | 555 | Weigh | 4 | 411 | 536 | 5.64/1.21 | The carpenter is measuring the stairs. | The nurse is weighing a baby. |
Scrub | 9 | n/a | n/a | Wash | 37 | 522 | 632 | 7.0/0.0 | The maid is scrubbing the floor. | The zoo keeper is washing an elephant. |
Watch | 81 | 525 | 576 | Examine | 33 | 341 | 549 | 5.0/1.41 | The audience is watching the play. | The veterinarian is examining the dog. |
tow | 1 | 406 | 468 | Push | 37 | 341 | 549 | 4.64/1.80 | The farmer is towing the (horse) trailer. | The gardener is pushing the lawnmower. |
chop | 3 | 575 | 487 | Slice | 13 | 507 | 540 | 6.0/0.89 | The boy (scout) is chopping wood. | The father is slicing a ham. |
Deliver | 18 | n/a | n/a | Send | 74 | n/a | n/a | 4.7/1.27 | The mailman is delivering the letter. | The woman is sending a package. |
Mix | 13 | n/a | n/a | Shake | 17 | n/a | n/a | 6.27/0.65 | The bartender is mixing the drink. | The baby is shaking the rattle. |
Avg | 45.1 | 490.7 | 533.9 | Avg | 36.3 | 460.1 | 561.3 | 5.53/0.94 | ||
SD | 55.3 | 67.7 | 36.5 | SD | 38.7 | 93.8 | 32.7 |
freq: frequency (range = 0 – 69971); image: imageability (range = 100 – 700); fam: familiarity (range = 100 – 700), n/a: not available, Sem Rel: Semantic relatedness ratings for verb pairs on 1–7 scale.
Present progressive form not required.
In addition to semantic relatedness, each verb pair shared at least one verb feature class (e.g., measure and weigh are both Measure verbs (major class) and Register verbs (subclass)) (Levin, 1993). Optional 2-place (e.g., bake), obligatory 2-place (e.g., examine), and optional 3-place (e.g., deliver) verbs were employed. Semantically “heavy” verbs with relatively specific semantic meanings (e.g., bake, drive) were used rather than “light” verbs (e.g., make, go). Verb sets were also matched for frequency (t(11) = 0.17, p = 0.87), imageability (t(5) = 0.68, p = .53), familiarity (t(5) = 0.90, p = 0.40), and number of syllables: Range = 1–3 syllables, t(11) = 0.32, p = .75) (Colheart, 1981), although a number of words did not have imageability and frequency ratings. (See Table 3 for details.)
Control task
A single word adjective retrieval task was developed as a non-verb control task that would rule out the possibility that improvements occurring during treatment reflected a nonspecific effect on semantic knowledge underlying concept representations. Adjectives were chosen rather than verbs because a sufficient number of verbs that were unrelated to treatment verbs and balanced across retrieval variables (e.g., frequency) could not be generated. Participants were expected to complete a sentence by providing a synonym to the adjective provided in the sentence. For example, Someone who is sick is also said to be _____ (target is ill). The adjectives were matched to the 24 verbs included in the probe stimuli on frequency (t(34) = 0.18, p = 0.86), imageability (t(24) = 0.71, p = 0.48), and familiarity (t(22) = 1.22, p = 0.23).
A group of older adults (8 females, 2 males; average age = 59.6 years; average education = 15.70 years) produced sentences for the sentence elicitation pictures and the also completed the adjective control task. The sentences elicitation pictures chosen for use in this study were produced with the intended target with a minimum of 90% accuracy, and the adjective control items were completed with the correct target with a minimum of 85% accuracy.
Experimental design
A multiple baseline across behaviors and subjects design (Connell & Thompson, 1986; McReynolds & Kearns, 1983) was used to evaluate the effects of VNeST. The design consisted of three phases: 1) baseline, 2) treatment of trained items with weekly administration of generalization and control probes to monitor effects of treatment on untrained tasks/stimuli, 3) maintenance (1 month post-treatment when possible). Stable baselines (no greater than 20% variability across baseline probes) were established for generalization and control probes (but please see comments about P4’s control baselines in results section) prior to treatment administration and weekly during the treatment phase.
To answer questions 1 and 2 (Does training a set of verbs using VNeST generalize to the ability to produce 1) an agent (carpenter), trained verb (measure), and patient (stairs) in response to novel picture stimuli and 2) an agent (nurse), untrained semantically related verb (weigh), and patient (baby) in response to novel picture stimuli?), weekly probes of pictures depicting an agent (carpenter/nurse), action (measure/weigh), and patient (stairs/baby) were administered. To answer question 3 (“Are generalization effects maintained following treatment?”) generalization and control probes were administered 1 month post-treatment. Finally, to answer question 4 (“Does VNeST treatment generalize to the ability to retrieve nouns and verbs not directly related to treatment items in single word naming, picture description and connected speech tasks?”) pre- and post-treatment tests including the BNT (Goodglass, Kaplan, & Weintraub, 1983), NVPB (Thompson, 2002) and various connected speech tasks were administered.
Generalization was defined as 40 percentage points over the highest baseline (see Edmonds & Kiran, 2006; Kiran & Thompson, 2003; Thompson, Shapiro, Kiran, & Sobecks, 2003).
Baseline, treatment, and maintenance probe measures
During baseline, treatment, and maintenance probe sessions, 20 pictures and the adjective control task were administered. During sentence production, pictures were pseudo-randomly presented so that verbs in the same semantic category (e.g., bake and fry) were not sequential. Participants were instructed to “Make a sentence and include him, the action, and this” (while pointing to the agent, verb, patient). Generally, no prompts were provided. However, if a participant produced a general word for the target (e.g., cut instead of slice or man instead of carpenter), he was prompted to use a more specific word. Additionally, if the participant produced a relevant target that was not the intended target (e.g., The landscaper is mowing the grass instead of The landscaper is pushing the lawnmower), the action or relevant item was pointed to for clarification. The first response produced after the prompt was recorded. These same prompts were given to the normal older group when the pictures were normed as well.
Responses to the pictures were scored as correct or incorrect based on success in lexical retrieval. A correct response included the agent, verb, and patient. Grammatical and/or morphological errors were not considered since these were not targeted in treatment. A few alternative responses were considered correct based on responses from the normal older group (e.g., pull was accepted for tow). In the event of multiple attempts, the best sentence was scored.
For the adjective control task, participants were expected to complete a sentence by providing a synonym to the adjective provided in the sentence. For example, Someone who is sick is also said to be _____ (target is ill). To be scored as correct, a response had to be an adjective consistent with responses provided by the normal group. In the event of multiple attempts, the final adjective produced was scored. One phonological error (e.g., omission, addition) per lexical item was allowed for all tasks.
Treatment
Stimuli consisted of: 1) 10 cards containing the names of the 10 trained verbs (verb set 1) (e.g., measure), 2) 6–8 cards for each verb containing 3–4 agents and 3–4 patients that form 3–4 pairs related to each verb (e.g., chef/sugar, carpenter/lumber, surveyor/land, designer/room for the verb measure) chosen to represent a range of possibilities to maximally expand the variety of scenarios related to each verb, 3) 5 cards containing the following words (who, what, where, when, why), and 4) 12 sentences used for semantic judgment (heard but not seen by participants). The 12 sentences contained the target verb broken into four categories: a) correct (The designer measures the room.), b) inappropriate agent (The infant measures the lumber.), c) inappropriate patient (The chef measures the television.), d) thematic reversal (The room measures the designer) (See step 4 of the training, Appendix A).
Appendix A.
Step | Objective | Therapy step | Examples |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Generation of 3 agents or patients for verb. | Clinician says to pt: Tell me who (or what, to be alternated throughout a session) can verb (be verbed). Clinician puts down card with the word who or what written on it as well as a card with the verb written on it. For each word generated, a card with the word written on it is put under the word who or what. (See Appendix B for examples.). Participants are encouraged to provide personal responses. A minimum of 3 words are required before moving to next step. |
Agents for verb measure: Chef Carpenter Body mechanic (personal example) |
If pt cannot produce 3 words, the pt receives cards with appropriate responses and 3 foils written on them and chooses those that go with the verb and rejects those that do not. | |||
2 | Generation of corresponding agent or patient to complete agent-patient pairs | Clinician asks pt to generate another 3 words that correspond to the agents or patients generated in step 1. For each word generated, a card with the word written on it is put under the word who or what, as appropriate. (See Appendix B.) |
Patients to match agents: (Chef)/sugar (Carpenter)/lumber (Body mechanic)/bumper |
As in step 1, if pt cannot produce 3 words, the pt receives words with appropriate responses along with 3 foils and chooses those which are appropriate and rejects those that are not. All word pairs are read aloud by pt. | |||
3 | Answer wh-questions about agent-patient pair | The pt chooses one agent-patient pair that he/she wants to discuss in more detail (different pair each week). That pair of words is moved out of the lists of agents and patients to focus attention. (See Appendix B.). The clinician then asks the pt to answer wh-questions (where, when, and why) about that pair. Complete sentences were not required for their responses. |
When does a chef measure sugar? “in the morning” Why... “correct amount” Where... “kitchen” |
4 | Semantic judgment of sentences | All cards are removed from the table. Clinician reads 12 sentences containing the target verb (4 correct, 4 with inappropriate agent, 4 with inappropriate patient, 4 with agent and patient switched). The pt indicates whether the sentences make sense or not. | Inappropriate agent: The dentist measures the door. (It’s possible but not the job of a dentist.) |
5 | Generation of 3 agent-patient pairs (repeat steps 1–2) | a) The clinician asks the same question asked in step 1. Clinician says to pt: Tell me who (or what) can verb (be verbed). Pt encouraged to produce up to 3 items. b) Clinician asks for words that correspond to those produced in step 5a. No cards are used during this step. General feedback was provided (“Good work,” etc.) |
a) Who measures things? “Chef” “Mechanic” b) What does a chef (then mechanic) measures. “sugar” “I don’t know.” |
pt: participant
VNeST was administered two times per week for 2 hour sessions. The first hour of the second session each week was dedicated to probes. Participants performed five treatment steps that aimed to strengthen the semantic meaning of the target verb and to promote stronger associations between the verb and related agents and patients. Essentially, the participants were asked to produce orally 3–4 thematic role pairs (e.g., carpenter and lumber) for a provided verb (e.g., measure). When they were unable to produce a word, they were provided with written options on cards (some appropriate and some foils). They were encouraged to provide at least one personal pair, and the responses could change from week to week. After generating a list of items and reading them aloud, they chose a pair and answered wh-questions about it. See Appendix A for detailed treatment steps with examples and Appendix B for an example of what was generated and seen by the participant for treatment steps 1–3.
Appendix B.
Treatment was terminated when participants produced a minimum of 24 agent-patient pairs (80% accuracy) during treatment step #1 (e.g., for measure, acceptable pairs would include chef/sugar, surveyor/land, designer/room). Since there were 10 treatment items and 3 opportunities for agent/patient pairs, a total of 30 pairs in one week was possible. Thus, 24 correct pairs (80%) in one week met treatment termination criterion. All participants achieved the 80% accuracy criterion before the last administered probe except for P2 who achieved criterion before the penultimate probe.
Treatment Reliability
In order to ensure that the treatment protocol was conducted consistently within and across participants, a trained Communication Sciences and Disorders undergraduate observed 25% of the sessions live. The treatment protocol was followed with a reliability of 98% (determined by comparing the observed treatment steps to the written protocol). Reliability on scoring of the weekly probes was conducted by the same individual on 25% of the probes. Agreement in responses and scoring was 95%.
Results
Weekly probes
Results of weekly probes for all participants are presented in Figure 2 to 5 in multiple baseline format. These results address research questions 1 and 2, which were: Does training a set of verbs using VNeST generalize to the ability to produce 1) an agent (carpenter), trained verb (measure), and patient (stairs) in response to novel picture stimuli and 2) an agent (nurse), untrained semantically related verb (weigh), and patient (baby) in response to novel picture stimuli? For the top two graphs of each figure, credit was given only if participants produced all content words (agent, verb, and patient).
Participant 1
Correct production of agent, verb and patient in description of pictures depicting trained actions increased from a baseline maximum of 0% to a high of 80%. For pictures depicting untrained actions, correct production of agent, verb and patient increased from a baseline maximum of 10% to 90%. Maintenance probes were 30% and 50% greater than the highest baseline value for sentences with trained and untrained verbs, respectively. There was no evidence of improvement on the adjective control task.
Participant 2
Correct production of agent, verb and patient in description of pictures depicting trained actions increased linearly from a baseline maximum of 40% to a high of 80%. For pictures depicting untrained actions, correct production of agent, verb and patient increased from a baseline maximum of 40% to 90%. Maintenance probes were 20% and 30% over the highest baseline for sentences with trained and untrained verbs, respectively. There was no evidence of improvement on the adjective control task.
Participant 3
Correct production of agent, verb and patient in description of pictures depicting trained actions increased linearly from a baseline maximum of 30% to a high of 60%. For pictures depicting untrained actions, correct production of agent, verb and patient increased from a baseline maximum of 30% to 90%. Maintenance probes were 50% and 60% over the highest baseline for sentences with trained and untrained verbs, respectively. There was no evidence of improvement on the adjective control task during the treatment phase. Improvement on the control task during the baseline phase reflects a growing understanding of the task by the participant (which she expressed). However, there was no improvement from the baseline phase to the treatment phase, so experimental control was maintained. See Peach and Wong (2004) for a similar observation.
Participant 4
Correct production of agent, verb and patient in description of pictures depicting trained actions increased linearly from a baseline maximum of 0% to a high of 40%. For pictures depicting untrained actions, correct production of agent, verb and patient increased from a baseline maximum of 0% to 50%. Maintenance probes were not administered for P4 due to her unavailability. There was no evidence of improvement on the adjective control task.
Pre- and post-treatment language measures
The results for tests administered pre- and post-treatment are presented in Table 2. These results address research question 3, “Does VNeST generalize to nouns and verbs not directly related to treatment items in single word naming, picture description and connected speech tasks?”
Noun retrieval improved an average of 13.2 percentage points (range = 8.3 to 23.3) on the Boston Naming Test (Goodglass et al., 1983), and verb retrieval improved an average of 8.0 percentage points (range = 0 to 13.7%) on the NVPB (Thompson, 2002). Noun and verb retrieval in sentence production on the NVPB improved on average 25 percentage points (range = 13.7 to 31.9). All participants improved at least 5 points, (range = 6.1 to 12. 8 points) on the Western Aphasia Battery (WAB, Kertesz, 1982), an indication of clinically significant improvement (Katz & Wertz, 1997). However, improvement on naming was not observed for any participant, as would be expected. Rather, the nonfluent participants showed improvement on spontaneous speech (information and fluency) while the fluent participants improved in comprehension and repetition. These improvements reflect improvements in the relative weaknesses of the aphasia types and are consistent with various tasks involved in the VNeST protocol (e.g., comprehension of wh-questions and sentence judgment) and other pre- to post-treatment improvements (connected speech tasks).
Participants 1, 2, and 3 improved 16.9% to 30.1% on connected speech samples as measured by their production of complete utterances (defined by presence of an appropriate agent, verb, and patient, and relevance to the topic). Participant 4 (P4) showed no improvement. See Figure 6. All participants produced more utterances (P1: 25 pre-treatment to 29 post-; P2: 53 to 59; P3: 48 to 65; P4: 121 to 159). Change in mean length of utterance (MLU) was variable (P1: 4.79 to 6.29; P2: 5.48 to 5.98; P3: 8.62 to 8.71; P4: 6.99 to 6.90). See Appendix C for an example of P3’s pre- and post-treatment cookie theft picture description.
APPENDIX C.
Utt | Pre-treatment | Post-treatment |
---|---|---|
1 | A mother is working in the kitchen. | Mother and two of her children were in the kitchen. |
2 | While she has her back to them her two children (xxx). | The children were not looking at mother. |
3 | The boy is standing on the top (of a xxx). | They were trying to find some cookies. |
4 | (But) he’s standing up. | The girl was standing on the floor. |
5 | It’s tilting. | She was reaching with one hand to reach a cookie. |
6 | He’s almost starting to fall. | Her brother was handing her a cookie. |
7 | He’s the top of the cookie (car car) jar (xxx). | The boy was standing on a stool. |
8 | He has a cookie xxx. | It was beginning to fall. |
9 | Looking for a cookie while his sister is reaching (xxx). | (As) he hit with two hands. |
10 | His last cookie. | One hand was leaning over. |
11 | While they’re doing that the mother is drying a plate. | He was beginning to fall. |
12 | He’s not paying attention to the water that’s coming. | With his other hand he was reaching for the cookie jar. |
13 | The sink and pouring over the sink. | He had the cupboard. |
14 | And he has a (xx xx) for get that. | He had it opened. |
15 | The mother is standing with curtains folded on each side. | (So) the top part was beside it. |
16 | (So) she can look through the window. | He was picking up a cookie with the other hand. |
17 | To walk through the step. | He had one door that he had opened of the cabinet. |
18 | There’s some plants near there. | While he was on that, mother was standing on the kitchen where the sink was. |
19 | Back in the kitchen again he’s standing with two cups and a plate over there. | Evident she was not looking. |
20 | n/a | Water was pouring from the sink and pouring over all the way to the floor. |
21 | n/a | The mother was drying a (a) plate. |
22 | n/a | And he had one pool in front. |
23 | n/a | He was wearing an apron. |
24 | n/a | He possibly was looking toward the window. |
25 | n/a | It looked like the bottom window was open. |
26 | n/a | The other side you could see the window and part of a house and some plants beside that. |
27 | n/a | He had curtains on the side of the window. |
28 | n/a | Beside the kitchen was two cups. |
29 | n/a | There was one glass. |
Utt: Utterance; n/a: not applicable
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to investigate the effects of Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST), a novel treatment inspired by the work of Loverso and colleagues (Loverso, Prescott, & Selinger, 1988; Loverso, Selinger, & Prescott, 1979; Loverso, Prescott, Selinger, Wheeler, & Smith, 1985) and McRae and colleagues (Ferretti et al., 2001; McRae et al., 2005), on the lexical retrieval of nouns and verbs in sentences containing trained and untrained verbs as well as in other tasks ranging from single word naming to connected speech.
Research question 1 and 2 asked whether VNeST would result in generalization of lexical retrieval of agent, verb, and patient in a picture description task with stimuli that included trained and semantically related untrained verbs. For sentences containing trained verbs, 3 of the 4 participants met the generalization criterion, with Participant 3 missing criterion by 10 percent. All participants achieved generalization criterion for sentences containing untrained verbs. Overall, these generalization patterns are encouraging since the picture stimuli were never seen in treatment, and the particular agent-verb-patient combinations tested in probes were never trained. Further, many errors during the treatment and maintenance phases were either semantic errors (e.g., fly → drive) or consisted of good sentences (e.g., The policeman is writing a ticket. → The policeman gave a ticket to the owner of the car.), representing highly communicative responses, that did not pass our strict scoring criteria.
Research question 3 asked whether generalization measured during the treatment phase would be maintained one month post-treatment. The results overall were encouraging with Participants 1 and 3 showing a maintenance of 30 to 60 percentage points above the maximum baseline value and Participant 2 with 20 to 30 percentage points above maximum baseline value. Interestingly, P3 showed improvement in sentences with trained verbs in the maintenance probe relative to her performance at conclusion of treatment. This improvement was due to a resolution of semantic errors observed during the treatment phase.
Research question 4 asked whether noun and verb retrieval would improve in single word production and sentence production with stimuli that did not explicitly include trained or semantically unrelated verbs. All participants showed gains in single noun retrieval, and 3 of 4 participants improved on single word verb retrieval. All participants improved on agent-verb-(patient) retrieval in sentence production as evidenced by improved sentence production on the NVPB (Thompson, 2002) stimuli. Widespread generalization was hypothesized because Verb Network Strengthening Treatment (VNeST) focuses on increasing semantic knowledge underlying noun and verb representations and the connections between their neural substrates. It takes advantage of the fact that many verbs allow a very broad range of possibilities for both agent and patient, thereby providing an opportunity for highly diverse modifications of the neural networks underlying cerebral representations of potential agents and patients. In this way, the therapy (in which 6–8 nouns are retrieved per verb) vastly expands the spectrum of semantic features that are incorporated in the training. Further, the therapy exploits the semantic representation of verbs as a contributor to the semantic representations of agents and patients.
Question 4 also asked whether lexical retrieval of nouns and verbs improved in constrained connected speech tasks. Three of the participants showed improvement in the ability to produce utterances containing a relevant subject, verb, and object with an increase in number of utterances overall. Participant 4 did not show improvements in connected speech on any measure. In fact, she became less efficient, producing more total utterances with no increase in complete utterances. A post hoc examination of participant 4’s sentence production revealed difficulty constructing a sentence frame and substantial paragrammatism not observed in P3, the other fluent participant. Thus, the increased ability in lexical retrieval observed in single words and constrained sentence production did not translate to less constrained speech tasks, perhaps due to her overall difficulty in sentence construction.
The breadth of generalization observed in this study has not generally been reported in studies of semantic treatment of aphasia (see Raymer & Gonzalez-Rothi, 2000). However, in studies targeting verb retrieval, there has been evidence of generalization to semantically related verbs and their potential thematic roles in sentences (Prescott et al., 1982; Raymer & Ellsworth, 2002) and connected speech (Edwards, Tucker, & McCann, 2004). VNeST is likely more conducive to widespread generalization because of the extensive networks of concepts that are engaged in treatment. Specifically, the primary task of VNeST is systematic retrieval of nouns that vary in semantic category, animacy, frequency, and typicality and that represent plausible agents and recipients of target verbs. Because verbs can be predicative components of noun concept representations (whether agent or recipient), verbs prime potential thematic roles and vice versa (Ferretti et al., 2001; McRae et al., 2005). This priming occurs through the capacity for content addressable memory that is intrinsic to neural networks supporting parallel distributed processing, as in the brain. Verbs should also, in principle, prime the representations of other verbs with which they share features. Thus, centering a semantic therapy on verbs provides the basis for engaging large expanses of semantic networks during treatment and through Hebbian learning, strengthening the neural basis for a large variety of concept representations, and increasing the probability that those representations can elicit corresponding word production. The initial success with VNeST provides tentative evidence in support of this hypothesis.
The mechanism for improvement on tests involving words not directly related to treatment items (BNT, NVPB) is not clear, particularly because the failure to improve on the adjective control task suggests that the treatment did not have a nonspecific effect on semantic knowledge underlying concept representations. It is possible that there was such a nonspecific effect but that the adjective task represented a poor control because it was more difficult than the tasks we used involving verbs. However, this seems unlikely because, with the possible exception of subject 2, there was no evidence of a potential floor effect in adjective task performance. It is conceivable that improvements in performance on completely unrelated words reflected the effect of other generalization mechanisms than semantic generalization (Nadeau & Kendall, 2006). The data collected in this study do not enable us to draw further conclusions, but future studies will examine this issue.
Because this is a phase 1 treatment study involving a limited number of subjects, the current results must be interpreted conservatively and further studies are needed. In particular, longer baseline phases, treatment phases and more maintenance data points are needed to establish baseline stability and to better evaluate treatment results. Our results also need to be replicated in subjects with a greater spectrum of aphasia types. However, the results are compelling given that the improvement on all dependent variables was evident across all four participants. Additionally, this treatment offers a more expansive, yet systematic approach, to semantic treatment than more traditional approaches that focus on single items. Finally, because this treatment allows participants to draw exemplars from daily life, it enhances participant interest and increases the likelihood that therapeutic gains will be personally relevant and potentially directly applicable to daily communicative life. Ecologically valid outcome measures examining functional changes are needed in future studies to evaluate treatment effects on participation and communicative success in functional situations.
Acknowledgements
The authors would first like to thank the four individuals who participated in this study. Additional thanks go Laura K. Allred for creating the artwork for developed stimuli and Michela Palmieri and Adam Jacks for assistance with reliability.
References
- Colheart M. The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1981;33A:497–505. [Google Scholar]
- Connell PJ, Thompson CK. Flexibility of single subject experimental designs. Part III: Using flexibility to design or modify experiments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 1986;51:214–225. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5103.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dabul BL. Apraxia Battery for Adults (ABA-2) Dedham, MA: AliMed Inc; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Druks J. Verbs and nouns—a review of the literature. Journal of Neurolinguistics. 2002;15:289–315. [Google Scholar]
- Edmonds LA, Kiran S. Effect of semantic naming treatment on crosslinguistic generalization in bilingual aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2006;49:729–748. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/053). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edwards S, Tucker K, McCann C. The contribution of verb retrieval to sentence construction: A clinical study. Brain and Language. 2004;91:78–79. [Google Scholar]
- Ferretti TR, McRae K, Hatherell A. Integrating verbs, situation schemas, and thematic role concepts. Journal of Memory and Language. 2001;44:516–547. [Google Scholar]
- Filmore C. The case for case. In: Back E, Harms R, editors. Universals in Linguistic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1968. [Google Scholar]
- Goodglass H, Kaplan E. Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination. 2nd ed. Media, PA: Williams and Wilkins; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Goodglass H, Kaplan E, Weintraub S. Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff R. Semantic interpretation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1972. [Google Scholar]
- Katz RC, Wertz RT. The efficacy of computer-provided reading treatment for chronic aphasic adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 1997;40:493–507. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4003.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kertesz A. Western Aphasia Battery. Austin: Pro-ed; 1982. [Google Scholar]
- Kiran S, Thompson CK. The role of semantic complexity in treatment of naming deficits: Training semantic categories in fluent aphasia by controlling exemplar typicality. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research. 2003;46(4):773–787. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/061). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Language Analysis Lab. Software for Analyzing English and Spanish Language Samples. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin; 1984–2006. [Google Scholar]
- Levin B. English Verb Classes and Alternations: A Preliminary Investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Loverso FL, Selinger M, Prescott TE. Application of verbing strategies to aphasia treatment. Clinical Aphasiology Conference: Clinical Aphasiology Conference; 1979. pp. 229–238. BRK Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Loverso FL, Prescott TE, Selinger M. Cueing verbs: a treatment strategy for aphasic adults (CVT) Journal of Rehabilitation Research. 1988;25(2):47–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loverso FL, Prescott TE, Selinger M, Wheeler KM, Smith RD. The application of microcomputers for the treatment of aphasic adults. Clinical Aphasiology Conference: Clinical Aphasiology Conference; 1985. pp. 189–195. BRK Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall J, Chiat S, Pring T. An impairment in processing verbs thematic roles: a therapy study. Aphasiology. 1997;11:855–876. [Google Scholar]
- McNeil MR. Clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. New York: Thieme; 1997. [Google Scholar]
- McRae K, Hare Elman, Ferretti TR. A basis for generating expectancies for verbs from nouns. Memory and Cognition. 2005;33(7):1174–1184. doi: 10.3758/bf03193221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McReynolds LV, Kearns KP. Single subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. Baltimore, MD: University Park Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Nadeau SE, Kendall DL. Significance and possible mechanisms underlying generalization in aphasia therapy: semantic treatment of anomia. Brain and Language. 2006;99:10–11. [Google Scholar]
- Nickels L. Therapy for naming disorders: Revisiting, revising, and reviewing. Aphasiology. 2002;16(1011):935–979. [Google Scholar]
- Peach RK, Wong PCM. Integrating the message level into treatment for agrammatism using story retell. Aphasiology. 2004;18(567):429–441. [Google Scholar]
- Plaut DC. Relearning after damage in connectionists networks: Toward a theory of rehabilitation. Brain and Language. 1996;52:25–82. doi: 10.1006/brln.1996.0004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Porch BE. The Porch Index of Communicative Abilities: Administration, scoring, and interpretation. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1973. [Google Scholar]
- Prescott TE, Selinger M, Loverso FL. An analysis of learning, generalization, and maintenance of verbs by an aphasic patient. Clinical Aphasiology Conference: Clinical Aphasiology Conference; 1982. pp. 178–182. BRK Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Raymer AM, Ellsworth TA. Response to contrasting verb retrieval treatments: A case study. Aphasiology. 2002;16(1011):1031–1045. [Google Scholar]
- Raymer AM, Rothi LJG. The semantic system. In: Nadeau SE, Rothi LJG, Crosson B, editors. Aphasia and Language: Theory to Practice. New York: Guilford: 2000. pp. 108–132. [Google Scholar]
- Roth HL, Nadeau SE, Hollingsworth AL, Marie Cimino-Knight A, Heilman KM. Naming concepts: Evidence of two routes. Neurocase. 2006;12(1):61–70. doi: 10.1080/13554790500502892. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rumelhart DE, Smolensky P, McClelland JL, Hinton GE. In: Parallel distributed processing: A handbook of models, programs and exercises. McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE, editors. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Saffran EM, Berndt RS, Schwartz MF. The quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: procedure and data. Brain and Language. 1989;37(3):440–479. doi: 10.1016/0093-934x(89)90030-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK. The Northwestern Verb Production Battery. 2002 Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson CK, Shapiro LP, Kiran S, Sobecks The role of syntactic complexity in treatment of sentence deficits in agrammatic aphasia: the complexity account of treatment efficacy (CATE) Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 2003;46(3):591–607. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2003/047). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]