Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2009 Mar 19;19(1):12–19. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.001

Spatial representation across species: Geometry, language, and maps

Barbara Landau 1, Laura Lakusta 2
PMCID: PMC2745626  NIHMSID: NIHMS130902  PMID: 19303766

Summary

We review growing evidence that the reorientation system-- shared by both humans and non-human species-- privileges geometric representations of space and exhibits many of the characteristic features of modular systems. We also review evidence showing that humans can move beyond the limits of non-human species by using two cultural constructions, language and explicit maps. We argue that, although both of these constructions are uniquely human means of enriching the spatial system we share with other species, their representational formats, functions and developmental trajectories are quite different, yielding distinctly different tools for empowering human spatial cognition.

Introduction

Recent studies have shown that many species reorient using geometric representations of spatial layouts, often ignoring highly salient non-geometric information. These findings have led to the hypothesis that the reorientation mechanism is modular, in Fodor's [1] sense [2, 3, 4], i.e. that the mechanism underlying reorientation is designed to compute geometric layout, and is blind—or impenetrable—to salient non-geometric information about layout. The capacity to reorient using geometry is present in humans by the age of 18 months, but it undergoes significant change over development, with increasing ability to reorient using a combination of geometric representations of the layout together with non-geometric information. This developmental change raises intriguing questions about the possible role of language in building new, more powerful devices to represent space. Here, we review the special role of geometric representations in reorientation across a variety of species, including humans. We then evaluate several hypotheses about the role of language in human reorientation, and suggest that the formalism of language offers humans a unique tool to strengthen an existing capacity to integrate geometric and non-geometric information. Finally, we compare the role of language to another cultural construction: the map. We argue that, although both language and maps are uniquely human capacities that enrich the spatial system we share with other species, the representational formats, functions, and developmental trajectories of the two systems are quite different, yielding distinctly different tools for empowering human spatial cognition.

Geometry is special: A capacity for reorientation that is shared across species

The idea of a geometric module for reorientation was first proposed by Cheng [2] and Gallistel [3] on the basis of experiments in which rats were familiarized to a small rectangular environment whose corners had been baited with food, were then disoriented and finally returned to the environment, where they were allowed to search for the food. Search patterns showed an unusual signature: rats tended to split their searches between the correct corner and its geometric equivalent, ignoring the salient markings of the corners (Figure 1). This pattern indicates that the rat's reorientation mechanism engages a representation of the overall geometric structure of the environment—the lengths of the walls, their angles of intersection, and their sense relations-- but ignores salient surface markings. The signature geometric search pattern has since been replicated in many species, including chicks [5], fish [6], rhesus monkeys [7], pigeons [8], and human children and adults [4, 9, 10]. However, many of these studies also show that, under a variety of conditions, non-geometric information can be used along with the geometric representation, leading to search at the uniquely correct corner of the space. For some, this has challenged the idea of a geometric module, whose support has largely depended on the single criterion of impenetrability—i.e. blindness to non-geometric features [11]. More crucially for our purpose, the findings have raised new questions about the nature of the reorientation system that is shared by all species as well as the differences that may uniquely distinguish humans from other species.

Figure 1. Illustration of the experimental set-up and geometric pattern of search reported in [2].

Figure 1

In this testing environment, the location of the food is fully specified by the surfaces’ salient visual features and odors. However, after being familiarized to the testing environment, removed and disoriented, and then returned to the testing environment to search for the food, rats searched primarily the correct corner and the geometrically equivalent corner (as depicted by the ‘x’ in the illustration) (recreated from Cheng, 1986).

Considerable evidence now suggests that reorientation is a specialized system that privileges geometric representations of spatial layout. First, the geometric shape of layouts is primary, in the sense that it is used across a wide variety of species and does not depend on experience with layouts having particular lengths of surfaces or angles of intersection [12]. Whether chicks are raised in a rectangular or a circular environment, they spontaneously encode the geometric properties of both environments during later tests of reorientation. Second, geometric representations are engaged under functionally specific contexts, with more use of geometry in tasks requiring locomotion to a target than reaching to a target in tabletop arrays, which does not require locomotion to a goal [13, 14]. Geometry is used more often when people are inside a space than when they view the space as an observer from the outside, and performance is better when the viewer moves within the array than when the array moves [15, 16]. Third, the effective array must have extended surfaces: even a 30 cm high surrounding surface elicits geometric responses among children, but a geometrically equivalent array composed of large individual columns does not, nor does an equivalent structure formed by tape on the floor [17]. Fourth, although most tests of geometric reorientation have used spaces that are characterized by rectangular enclosures—which differ in relative lengths of the four walls—geometric responding may be computable with values on other relative scales, including different dot size, but not different colors [18]. This finding suggests that relative comparisons along a single spatial scale may be sufficient to induce geometric responding.

Finally, reorientation using geometric representations of layouts appears to have neural specificity, with the hippocampus as a potential site. Rats [19] and pigeons [20] with hippocampal lesions are impaired in using geometry for reorientation, with specific impairment in rats linked to encoding the relative lengths of walls [21]. The right and left hippocampal areas play different roles in an animal's use of geometric vs. non-geometric properties of space [22, 23, 24], consistent with evidence from humans [25]. Hippocampal place cells have long been implicated in the representation of spatial layout in the rat [26], and recent studies show that these place cells come to gradually and incrementally respond to the overall shape of an environment (e.g. round vs. square) during learning trials, with specific shape-related response patterns persisting over time [27]. Thus place cells appear to “learn” the shapes of layouts, raising the intriguing possibility that this is part of the neural substrate for the mechanism of reorientation, Another contributor may be recently discovered “grid cells” in the medial entorhinal cortex, which appear to encode distributions of regularly spaced locations across the environment [28]. Understanding the relationship between grid cells and place cells will be critical to developing theories of the specific neural mechanisms underlying the construction and use of cognitive maps [29, 30].

Although the evidence for neural specificity in humans is sparse at present, lesion studies suggest that topographical disorientation (the inability to orient in the environment) can occur with damage to a variety of regions of the brain, including the hippocampus, parahippocampal regions and parietal regions [31, 32]. Recent studies suggest that hippocampal functions play a primary role in the formation of cognitive maps, both accounting for variation in the speed of acquisition of new maps among normal individuals [33] and selective impairment in acquiring new maps in a case of developmental disorientation [34]. Studies of people with Williams syndrome, a genetic deficit giving rise to severe spatial impairments and damage to both parietal and hippocampal regions [35, 36, 37] show severely impaired use of geometry in the reorientation task (L Lakusta, B Dessalegn, B Landau, Cognitive Development Society, Santa Fe, October, 2007) (see also [38] for evidence of selective impairment in geometry-based but not feature- based reorientation in neglect patients). Because Williams syndrome is a genetic disorder, scientists are attempting to link specific genes with aspects of the spatial disorder. Although we are far from understanding this complex causal chain, intriguing studies show that Limk1 knockout mice are impaired in the classic Morris water maze, which tests place learning [39]. These mice also have abnormalities of hippocampal neurons, despite overall grossly normal development of the nervous system.

These results clearly implicate the role of the hippocampus in reorientation among humans, but considerably more research will be needed to distinguish effects of hippocampal damage per se from damage to other areas contributing to the reorientation mechanism, such as the parietal and parahippocampal regions. The use of animal models to probe the relative contributions of hippocampus and other structures in the formation and use of shape-based representations of the environment should be helpful in guiding specific hypotheses about humans.

It is worth noting that some of these properties fall within the criteria that Fodor [1] proposed for modular systems, in particular, domain-specificity, localization, ontogenetic invariance, and characteristic breakdown patterns. Although much debate over the modularity of reorientation has focused on the criterion of penetrability, Fodor's criteria were intended to be characteristic—not defining-- properties of modules. The properties described earlier suggest that the reorientation system clearly has some properties of modular systems (but see [11] for a different view). Understanding whether and how these properties change over development and across species could shed significant light on the nature of modular systems across species more generally.

Language and its role in reorientation

The privileged nature of geometry in reorientation contrasts to an interesting degree with the incorporation of non-geometric information. The conditions under which this occurs appear to be more variable, depending on settings such as the size of the space [6, 10, 11, 40, 41], the proximal vs. distal location of the feature within the space [11], and the location of the target relative to the feature [11]. Increasingly consistent integration of geometric and non-geometric properties over development has suggested to some that language could play a crucial role in this change. One hypothesis is that language provides a representational system which allows information from separate modular systems—in this case, the reorientation system and the object representation system-- to be combined in some common format [4, 42, 43, 44]. On this proposal, non-verbal species and human toddlers who search geometrically (and do not use non-geometric information) would be unable to combine properties that are the output of separate modules (geometric structure/ sense and object/color); whereas older children and adults would be able to do so by virtue of having the representational apparatus of language, which could produce a new representation of the target's location such as [[LEFT-OF [BLUE WALL]]. In its strong form, the hypothesis suggests that language is both necessary and sufficient to change the computations carried out during the reorientation task. If true, this would result in a vast restructuring of human spatial representation, conferring enormous additional power to a system that would otherwise only be capable of producing outputs from each system without combining them.

Considerable evidence casts doubt on this strong version of the language hypothesis. Empirical findings show that non-verbal species can integrate geometry and features [6, 7, 8, 11, 41, 45], so language is clearly not necessary for integration. Moreover, evidence supporting the role of language among children and adults has been equivocal: Verbal shadowing interferes with the use of features in some studies [42] but not others [46] while spatial shadowing interferes in other studies [46, 47]. Finally, evidence from 4−5 year olds showed that the production of “left” and “right” – but not comprehension of these terms, nor a range of other variables-- was correlated with the use of geometry and surface features [44]. It is unclear why production but not comprehension would be correlated if language were necessary to provide the appropriate (combinatorial) representational format. Being able to produce the expressions “X left of Y/ right of Y” requires a representation that should also underlie comprehension.

A different hypothesis about the role of language is that the reorientation system allows combination of geometry and non-geometrical properties, but that some combinations are either unstable or impoverished, leading to difficulty forming the representation or rapid forgetting, for example. The combinatorial properties of syntax and semantics could then be used on-line to enhance this representation or boost its natural life in working memory. This hypothesis of Momentary Interaction [48, 49] suggests flexible and powerful enhancement of spatial representations through language, but not radical restructuring.

To see how this could work requires two steps. First, the presence of the geometric error pattern among toddlers suggests that they already possess a non-linguistic representation of both relative wall length (or other properties along a scale [18]), and sense relations between pairs of wall lengths. This representation would include [X, LONG WALL], [Y, SHORT WALL], and [X LEFT-OF Y], [X RIGHT OF Y], where the latter relationships are defined by the relationship between the body's axes and those of the layout. Combining these, one can represent [[LONG WALL] [[LEFT-OF] [SHORT WALL]]]. This representation would be sufficient to locate the target in one of two geometrically equivalent corners, yielding the geometric error. Human toddlers can also represent objects and their non-geometric properties, e.g. [BLUE WALL] and indeed, can use these features to locate targets when they are not disoriented [4]. Language is therefore not necessary to encode the sense relationship between the two walls, nor is it necessary to encode object/color. If one assumes that non-linguistic representations are compositional, then language would not be necessary to combine the two either. However, language can express the two together in a single phrase, and this linguistic format might be more enduring and stable than the corresponding non-linguistic representation. The linguistic representation [[LEFT-OF [BLUE WALL]], could in this way facilitate search using both geometric and non-geometric properties.

Insights into how this would work can be drawn from studies examining the more general problem of how humans combine location and surface color information. Studies in perception and attention show that color- location conjunctions are represented in fragile form by the visual system, that is, binding of the conjoined features requires focused attention and is subject to disintegration under conditions of attentional impairment [50, 51, 52]. Recent studies further suggest that, over development, language plays an increasingly powerful role in promoting the strength, stability, and durability of representations for this kind of conjunction [49]. Four year-olds were shown a target square that was split in half by color (e.g. vertically split with red on the left and green on the right) and had to match it to its identity after a 1-second delay. Test items included the target, its reflection (e.g. red right, green left), and a square with a different geometric split (e.g. a diagonal split, half red and half green, see Figure 2). Children performed above chance, but they chose the target only about 60% of the time (with a chance level of 33%). Errors were predominantly the reflection items—consistent with robust representation and retention of the geometry (the vertical split) but quite fragile retention of the color-location combination. Follow-up experiments showed that this pattern of performance was robust, even when non-linguistic attention manipulations were used to draw attention to the location of the red part, e.g., flashing it on and off, growing and shrinking it, or having the child point to it before it disappeared. In contrast to these non-linguistic manipulations, introducing the square with the instruction “The red is to the left of the green” enhanced children's ability to match after delay, raising their accuracy to about 80%. Comparison with other linguistic manipulations revealed two critical variables: The syntax of the sentence, which provides an asymmetrical frame establishing which part of the display is figure and which part is reference object [53, 54], and the directional value of the predicate [X LEFT-OF Y] that relates the figure and reference object and establishes an asymmetrical direction (i.e. if X is left of Y, then Y cannot be left of X). Sentences such as “The red is touching the green”—while very similar in structure, did not enhance performance, showing that the asymmetrical or directional value of the predicate [LEFT-OF] was crucial. Importantly, post-tests showed that children knew that “left” and “right” referred to locations along the horizontal axis of a display, but did not know which term mapped onto which direction, ruling out the idea that they had retrieved a complete and correct meaning of [X LEFT-OF Y] from their long-term lexical memory. Rather, the instruction “The red is to the left of green” helped 4 year-olds represent the correct color-location pairings in the moment of the task, essentially telling them which part was figure and which was reference object, and which direction was called “left” (or “right”). The explicit representation of the color-location information through the instructional sentence was sufficient to improve 4 year-olds’ performance, suggesting the power of momentary, on-line interactions between language and visual attention, consistent with recent evidence from adults [55] (B Dessalegn, B. Landau, unpublished). This interaction has a developmental profile, with three year-olds unable to take advantage of these linguistic structures to improve performance in the task, and 6 year-olds automatically recoding the task into language, shown by ceiling performance even without explicit instruction (B Dessalegn, B. Landau, unpublished1).

Figure 2. Illustration of the matching task used by [49].

Figure 2

Four-year-old children were presented with a target stimulus located in the top, center portion of a computer screen. The target then disappeared, there was a 1-second delay, and children then viewed three test items at the bottom of the computer screen (for example, from left to right: the target, a square with a different geometric split, and the target's mirror reflection). Children were asked to choose the item that was exactly the same as the one he/she first saw.

The results provide an intriguing parallel to the evidence on reorientation and suggest a specific mechanism whereby language could enhance humans’ ability to reorient by strengthening a unified representation of geometric and non-geometric properties of space. Specifically, the conjunction of color and geometric information does not require language for its encoding, but language can boost its strength—either during encoding or in immediate memory. The nature of the linguistic support is rich, involving both syntax and semantics, but is ideally suited to recoding relationships that may otherwise be represented in fragile form. In the case of reorientation, geometric layout and color could be combined in fragile form without language (as in the case of human toddlers and non-linguistic species), but receive a boost when recoded in linguistic format. If this is true, the use of linguistic encoding should speed and facilitate accurate search, as suggested by results of shadowing experiments [42], and consistent with reorientation performance among first generation deaf Nicaraguan signers who encode [LEFT] and [RIGHT] unsystematically (J Pyers, A Shusterman, A Senghas, K Emmorey, E. Spelke, Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, March). Related findings show more accurate searches among children who hear the location or reward value of the colored wall—but not among children who only hear the wall mentioned [56]. This too suggests that children may be recoding the layout in terms of the syntax and semantics that encode asymmetrical spatial relationships.

The momentary recoding hypothesis also fits well with recent evidence from the color domain that shows developmentally increasing lateralization for rapid between-category color judgments. Adults show an advantage of right visual field presentation of stimuli (projected to the left hemisphere), whereas infants show the opposite effect [57, 58]. The switch to the left hemisphere increases as children learn their color terms [58, 59], consistent with an increasingly powerful and automatic role for language in human cognitive function. However, as with reorientation, the role of language is not to create new kinds of representations, but rather, to provide a format which might speed, enhance, or facilitate performance.

Maps: The permanent record of our geometric knowledge

While language may provide a mechanism for momentarily strengthening the storage of geometric and non-geometric information, maps are the symbolic system par excellence for encoding and permanently retaining spatial information. Although language and maps are both created and used by humans alone to communicate information to others, the differences stop there. Unlike language, maps are symbolic devices whose explicit purpose is to encode spatial information about layout. Unlike language, the format of maps is roughly analog in nature, capturing the spatial layout of an environment by spatial transformations that preserve what is essential to finding things—most often, the geometric properties of layouts. Although language can encode spatial information, there are striking limits on how well it can represent complex spatial information. For example, surface textures, facial expressions, or the detailed contours of an overall spatial layout are difficult to represent in precise form by language; for these properties, a picture (map) is worth a thousand words. In contrast, maps are explicitly designed to preserve geometric information, providing an analogue representation that can articulate even the most complex and detailed aspects of spatial layout. And unlike language—which is largely mastered by the time of schooling-- the developmental timetable for achieving mastery in map use begins around age 3 but extends throughout the lifetime [60], with significant individual differences in mastery.

The capacity to use maps includes several core components. First is understanding of basic correspondences-- that the map is a representation having a purpose separate from its status as an object, that symbols on the map represent objects and places in space, and that the spatial layout on the map corresponds to a real physical layout [60]. The symbolic function of map-like objects emerges abruptly as early as 2−1/2 or 3 years [61] and children can use simple maps to locate objects as early as age 3 [62, 63, 64]. However, since maps can vary quite widely in the specific graphic devices used to represent landmarks and layout, as well as the choice of which properties are preserved, enhanced, and/or omitted, proficient map use has a long developmental trajectory tied to learning the specifics of map symbolism, perhaps by analogy with learning the specifics of different writing scripts. For example, children initially assume that properties such as color or exact line size correspond with fidelity to the objects they represent, leading to erroneous conclusions about the real dimensions of roads, bridges, etc. [60].

A second core component of map use is the mechanism whereby a representation of the map (its layout) is aligned with a representation of the layout in the real world. This can be accomplished by mentally aligning one with the other, or by physically aligning the map so that the geometric structures of the map and layout are viewed from the same perspective. Mental alignment is likely to build on the capacity to reorient oneself in an environment; formally, reorientation requires aligning a representation of layout that has been stored in memory with a representation of the currently observable layout [3].

Appreciation of core spatial properties of maps is species-specific, emerges quite early in development and emerges without special tutoring. Studies of chimps’ use of map-like objects have been limited, but evidence shows that they can use scale models to locate an object in the real space, but fail in even modest tests of generalization [65, 66]. By contrast, 4 year-old children can use maps without any prior specific experience. A congenitally blind 4 year-old who had never experienced explicit map-like representations of space used the angles among objects on a simple map to find targets in a room, and did so even when the entire map was displaced laterally or rotated vertically, destroying any direct alignment between symbols on the map and corresponding targets in the room [62]. By the age of four, and with no special exposure to maps, children can use both angular and distance relationships among symbols [62, 63, 64, 67] suggesting that they immediately understand these basic geometric correspondences between maps and real space. The use of distance and angle mappings requires no explicit tutoring, nor does it require extensive experience with symbolic devices, as the Munduruku, an isolated Amazonian group having no experience with graphic symbols, were found to immediately use distance and angle information represented on a simple map to locate objects in a nearby space [68]. In contrast to the ease of using angle and distance information, sense (right/left) relationships appear to be more challenging, with 4 year-olds showing considerable difficulty in using this property to disambiguate two identical targets [64]. This is consistent with the idea that left-right relationships are represented in unstable fashion by humans.

Although the findings suggest that children appreciate the correspondence between the map and the space it represents quite early and with little or no previous experience, other aspects of map use can be difficult and require both training and specific experience. For example, alignment of maps that are rotated in the horizontal plane (thus destroying viewpoint correspondence) present particular challenges to children [69] as well as adults. Learning to interpret particular graphic devices can also be a life-long process [60].

Concluding Remarks

The capacity to locate objects as we move through space is fundamental to the survival of many species. It is perhaps not surprising, therefore, that we share with many other species basic systems of navigation and reorientation. Where we differ from other species is in additional, powerful representational systems that allow us to share with others our representations of space. Both language and maps allow us to do this. However, there are crucial differences between the systems. Language may provide the tools to more stably represent some aspects of spatial organization that are otherwise fragile (such as sense relations); and through its combinatorial nature and infinite productivity, it provides us with the means to describe in infinite detail a given layout and the routes through it. But such descriptions take much time, can be hard for humans to process, and are severely limited in the extent to which they can result in a spatial representation of sufficient quality and resolve to guide navigation. In contrast, maps are portable representations of space; they relieve us of the burden of remembering detailed aspects of layout that might be discarded in the representations we use for reorientation, and provide a relatively immediate display of the spatial information we need to guide action. Using maps depends on having basic mechanisms of reorientation in place, but maps take us beyond the present, supporting our ability to know and understand spatial layouts without having experienced them firsthand.

The two systems of representation--both uniquely human, both emerging early in development with no formal tutoring-- have the capacity to extend our spatial capacities beyond those of other species. They provide the means for stably representing aspects of space that might be otherwise difficult to maintain in memory, and they allow us to explicitly communicate about space with each other. The capacity to communicate about space supports the development of additional tools of remarkable power. These tools are what ultimately transport human cognition far beyond the bounds of other species. With these tools, we can move backwards in time to reconstruct the layouts of bygone cities from ancient maps and forwards in time to explore lands not yet encountered. This freedom from the bounds of the present is the deepest signature of human understanding.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Megan Chen who helped with preparing the figures. This research was supported by NIH grant, NINDS RO1 NS 050876 to Barbara Landau.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

1

There is an important difference between the error types in the color/ location task and those in the reorientation task. In the former, children err by mistakenly assigning color to location (e.g. red on left of green instead of red on right of green). In the reorientation task, errors of this sort would lead to a pattern of search in which children search at either the corner that is LEFT-OF the red wall or the one that is RIGHT-OF the red wall. This does not happen. What does happen in the geometric error pattern is that children altogether ignore wall color. So, although it may be generally true that it is difficult to combine sense relations with color, the reorientation task additionally suggests that there is special difficulty in specifying the wall color in the representation. It is possible that reorientation is the extreme case in which color slides wantonly over surfaces.

Contributor Information

Barbara Landau, Johns Hopkins University.

Laura Lakusta, Montclair State University.

References

  • 1.Fodor JA. The modularity of mind. MIT Press; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cheng K. A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation. Cognition. 1986;23:149–178. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90041-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gallistel R. The organization of learning. MIT Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hermer L, Spelke E. Modularity and development: The case of spatial reorientation. Cognition. 1996;61:195–232. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(96)00714-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Vallortigara G, Zanforlin M, Pasti G. Geometric modules in animals’ spatial representations: A test with chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1990;104:248–254. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.3.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces. Animal Cognition. 2007;10:47–54. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0029-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Gouteux S, Thinus-Blanc C, Vauclair J. Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2001;130:505–519. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kelly DM, Spetch ML, Heth CD. Pigeons’ encoding of geometric and featural properties of a spatial environment. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 1998;112:259–269. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Hermer L, Spelke E. A geometric process for spatial representation in young children. Nature. 1994;370:57–59. doi: 10.1038/370057a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Learmonth AE, Newcombe NS, Huttenlocher J. Toddlers’ use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2001;80:225–244. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2001.2635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11*.Newcombe N, Ratliff K. Explaining the development of spatial reorientation: Modularity-plus-language versus the emergence of adaptive combination. In: Plumert J, Spencer J, editors. The Emerging Spatial Mind. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 53–76. [A review of evidence from humans and other species on reorientation, and presentation of the adaptive combination hypothesis, an alternative to modularity.] [Google Scholar]
  • 12**.Chianetti C, Vallortigara G. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment. Animal Cognition. 2008;11:139–146. doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0099-y. [This study provides compelling evidence for an innate geometric system. Chicks were reared in a rectangular or circular shaped environment and then placed in a rectangular shaped environment, with salient featural cues, where they underwent a reorientation task. Both groups of chicks showed that they spontaneously encoded the geometry, suggesting that experience with particular geometric properties of layouts is not necessary for later geometric encoding.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gouteux S, Vauclair J, Thinus-Blanc C. Reorientation in a small-scale environment by 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old children. Cognitive Development. 2001;16:853–869. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hupbach A, Nadel L. Reorientation in a rhombic environment: No evidence for an encapsulated geometric module. Cognitive Development. 2005;20:279–302. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M. How toddlers represent enclosed spaces. Cognitive Science. 2003;27:749–766. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lourenco SF, Huttenlocher J. How do young children determine location? Evidence from disorientation tasks. Cognition. 2006;100:511–529. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17**.Lee SA, Spelke ES. Children's use of geometry for reorientation. Developmental Science. 2008;11:743–749. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00724.x. [This study explores the conditions under which humans use geometry to reorient. Four-year-old children used the geometry of the environment to reorient when extended, three-dimensional, 30 centimeter high surfaces defined the shape of the room. Children did not reorient using geometry when the shape of the room was defined by two-dimensional lines on the floor or by several large, stable columns.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18*.Huttenlocher J, Lourenco SF. Coding location in enclosed spaces: Is geometry the principle? Developmental Science. 2007;10:741–746. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00609.x. [Eighteen- to 24-month old children performed a reorientation task in a square room. When the adjoining walls had circles of different sizes, children searched the correct corner where the toy was hidden (34.4%) and the corner diagonally opposite the correct corner (36.5%). However, this pattern was not found when the adjoining walls had different colors (red and blue), or when two of the walls were plain and two were patterned. The authors conclude that relative features on the same scale, rather than geometry per se, may guide reorientation.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.McGregor A, Hayward AJ, Pearce JM, Good MA. Hippocampal lesions disrupt navigation based on the shape of the environment. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2004;118:1011–1021. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.1011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Vargas JP, Petruso EJ, Bingman VP. Hippocampal formation is required for geometric navigation in pigeons. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20:1937–1944. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03654.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21*.Jones PM, Pearce JM, Davies VJ, Good MA, McGregor A. Impaired processing of local geometric features during navigation in a water maze following hippocampal lesions in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007;121:1258–1271. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1258. [When performing a navigation task to a hidden goal, rats with hippocampal lesions were impaired at discriminating a right-angle corner from its mirror reversal corner and were impaired at discriminating a short vs. long wall. However, they were able to use salient features (an acute angle; a colored wall) that highlighted the direction of search.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Nardi D, Bingman VP. Asymmetrical participation of the left and right hippocampus for representing environmental geometry in homing pigeons. Behavioural Brain Research. 2007;178:160–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Vallortigara G, Pagni P, Sovrano VA. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: Evidence from a lopsided animal brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16:390–400. doi: 10.1162/089892904322926737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Tommasi L, Gagliardo A, Andrew RJ, Vallortigara G. Separate processing mechanisms for encoding geometric and landmark information in the avian hippocampus. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;17:1695–1702. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Epstein R, Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature. 1998;392:598–601. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.O'Keefe J, Nadel L. The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Clarendon; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lever C, Wills T, Cacucci F, Burgess N, O'Keefe J. Long-term plasticity in hippocampal place-cell representation of environmental geometry. Nature. 2002;416:90–94. doi: 10.1038/416090a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature. 2005;436:801–806. doi: 10.1038/nature03721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jeffery KJ, Burgess N. A metric for the cognitive map: found at last? Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2006;10:1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hayman RM, Jeffery KJ. How heterogeneous place cell responding arises from homogenous grids – a contextual gating hypothesis. Hippocampus. 2008;18:1301–1313. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Aguirre GK, D'Esposito M. Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain. 1999;122:1613–1628. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bohbot VD, Allen JJB, Nadel L. Memory deficits characterized by patterns of lesions to the hippocampus and parahippocampal cortex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2000;911:355–368. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06737.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Iaria G, Lanyon LJ, Fox CJ, Giaschi D, Barton JJS. Navigational skills correlate with hippocampal fractional anisotropy in humans. Hippocampus. 2008;18:335–339. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Iaria G, Bogod N, Fox CJ, Barton JJS. Developmental topographical disorientation: Case one. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Kohn P, Mervis CB, Kippenhan JS, Olsen RK, Morris CA, Berman KF. Neural basis of genetically determined visuospatial construction deficit in Williams Syndrome. Neuron. 2004;43:623–631. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Meyer-Lindenberg A, Mervis CB, Sarpal D, Koch P, Steele S, Kohn P, Marenco S, Morris CA, Das S, Kippenhan S, Mattay VS, Weinberger DR, Berman KF. Functional, structural, and metabolic abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in Williams syndrome. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 2005;115:1888–1895. doi: 10.1172/JCI24892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37*.Landau B. The organization and development of spatial cognition: Insights from Williams syndrome. In: Burack J, editor. Handbook of Intellectual Disabilities and Development. Oxford University Press; 2009. [A proposal for the organization of spatial cognition, its development in humans, and its susceptibility to impairment, using the framework of the dorsal and ventral streams of the brain.] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Guariglia C, Coriale G, Cosentino T. TENS modulates spatial reorientation in neglect patients. Neuroreport. 2000;11:1945–1948. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200006260-00027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Osborne LR. Molecular basis of a multisystem disorder. In: Morris CA, Lenhoff HM, Wang PP, editors. Williams-Beuren Syndrome: Research, Evaluation, and Treatment. JHU Press; 2006. pp. 18–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Learmonth AE, Nadel L, Newcombe NS. Children's use of landmarks: Implications for modularity theory. Psychological Science. 2002;13:337–341. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2002.00461.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41*.Sovrano VA, Vallortigara G. Dissecting the geometric module: A sense linkage for metric and landmark information in animals’ spatial reorientation. Psychological Science. 2006;17:616–621. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01753.x. [This paper explores effects of space size observed in non-human animals that are similar to those observed in human children, and proposes a hypothesis to explain the effects of space size across both species.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hermer-Vazquez L, Spelke E, Katsnelson A. Source of flexibility in human cognition: Dual task studies of space and language. Cognitive Psychology. 1999;39:3–36. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Carruthers P. The cognitive functions of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2002;25:657–726. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x02000122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hermer-Vazquez L, Moffet A, Munkholm P. Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: The case of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition. 2001;79:263–299. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00120-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Sovrano VA, Bisazza A, Vallortigara G. Modularity and spatial reorientation in a simple mind: Encoding of geometric and nongeometric properties of a spatial environment by fish. Cognition. 2002;85:B51–B59. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00110-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46*.Ratcliff KR, Newcombe NS. Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms. Cognitive Psychology. 2008;56:142–163. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.06.002. [In Experiment 1, adults performed a reorientation task in a rectangular room with one colored wall, while undergoing verbal interference. Adults who received vague task instructions showed impaired performance, while those that received explicit task instructions searched the correct corner. In Experiment 2, non-verbal, spatial interference (visualization task) also impaired performance. The authors argue that spatial language is not necessary for use of features when reorienting.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hupbach A, Hardt O, Nadel L, Bohbot VD. Spatial reorientation: Effects of verbal and spatial shadowing. Spatial Cognition and Computation. 2007;7:213–226. [Google Scholar]
  • 48*.Landau B, Dessalegn B, Goldberg A. Language and space: Momentary interactions. In: Evans V, Bergen B, Zinken J, editors. Language, cognition, and space: The state of the art and new directions. Advances in Cognitive Linguistics Series. Equinox Publishing; 2008. [Reviews a range of evidence supporting the view that language plays a momentary role in modulating visual and spatial representations.] [Google Scholar]
  • 49**.Dessalegn B, Landau B. More than meets the eye: The role of language in binding visual properties. Psychological Science. 2008;19:189–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02066.x. [This study provides an alternative to strong Whorfian and non-Whorfian claims. The authors show that language can exert powerful, yet momentary, effects on visual-spatial representations. See text for details.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Treisman A, Gelade G. A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology. 1980;12:97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Treisman A, Schmidt H. Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology. 1982;14:107–141. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Arguin M, Joanette Y, Cavanagh P. Visual search for feature and conjunction targets with an attention deficit. Journal of cognitive neuroscience. 2003;5:436–452. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.4.436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Talmy L. How language structures space. In: Pick H, Acredolo L, editors. Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research and Application. Plenum Press; 1983. pp. 225–282. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Gleitman LR, Gleitman H, Miller C, Ostrin R. Similar, and similar concepts. Cognition. 1996;58:321–376. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(95)00686-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Spivey MJ, Tyler MJ, Eberhard KM, Tanenhaus MK. Linguistically mediated visual search. Psychological Science. 2001;12:282–286. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Shusterman A, Spelke E. Language and the development of spatial reasoning. In: Carruthers P, Laurence S, Stich S, editors. The Structure of the Innate Mind. Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gilbert AL, Regier T, Kay P, Ivry RB. Whorf hypothesis is supported in the right visual field but not the left. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:489–494. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509868103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58*.Franklin A, Drivonikou GV, Bevis L, Davies IRL, Kay P, Regier T. Categorical perception of color is lateralized to the right hemisphere in infants, but to the left hemisphere in adults. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:3221–3225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0712286105. [By showing different category effect patterns for adults and pre-linguistic infants, this paper provides strong evidence that language affects color perception. Adults discriminate between category colors faster than within category colors and this effect is strongest when the stimuli are presented in the right visual field (left hemisphere processing). However, for 4−6 month-old-infants, the advantage for between category discrimination is greater for the left visual field (right hemisphere processing).] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59*.Franklin A, Drivonikou GV, Clifford A, Kay P, Regier T, Davies IRL. Lateralization of categorical perception of color changes with color term acquisition. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105:18221–18225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0809952105. [Follow-up to their earlier study on lateralization, showing that this changes developmentally as children acquire color terms.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60*.Liben L, Myers L. Developmental changes in children's understanding of maps: What when, and how? In: Plumert J, Spencer J, editors. The Emerging Spatial Mind. Oxford University Press; 2007. pp. 193–218. [Review of the cognitive requirements for map use and the developmental changes in children's understanding of these.] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.DeLoache J. Early understanding and use of symbols: The model model. Current Perspectives in Psychological Science. 1995;4:109–113. [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Landau B. Early map use as an unlearned ability. Cognition. 1986;22:201–223. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(86)90015-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Huttenlocher J, Newcombe N, Vasilyeva M. Spatial scaling in young children. Psychological Science. 1999;10:393–398. [Google Scholar]
  • 64**.Shusterman A, Lee SA, Spelke ES. Young children's spontaneous use of geometry in maps. 2008;11:F1–F7. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00670.x. [This study is the first to show that, without training, 4-year-old children use the geometry in a map to guide their search for a hidden object. Children used landmarks, angle, and distance information, however they did not use left-right sense information, as shown by their tendencies to confuse the two geometrically equivalent corners in an isosceles triangle.] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Premack D, Premack A. The mind of an ape. W.W. Norton; 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kuhlmeier V, Boysen S. The effect of response contingencies on scale model task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 2001;115:300–306. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Huttenlocher J, Vasilyeva M, Newcombe N, Duffy S. Developing symbolic capacity one step at a time. Cognition. 2008;106:1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68**.Dehaene S, Izard V, Pica P, Spelke ES. Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. Science. 2006;311:381–384. doi: 10.1126/science.1121739. [Several experiments examined the use of geometric representations by individuals from a remote culture having little to no experience with symbolic devices. Children older than 6 and adults had no difficulty interpreting and using simple maps to guide search in an adjacent space.] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bluestein M, Acredolo L. Developmental changes in map reading skills. Child Development. 1979;50:691–697. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES