Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2009 Jul 10;92(4):519–528. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2009.06.012

Impaired Long-term Potentiation and Enhanced Neuronal Excitability in the Amygdala of CaV1.3 Knockout Mice

Brandon C McKinney 1,2, Wilson Sze 4, Benjamin Lee 4, Geoffrey G Murphy 2,3,4
PMCID: PMC2747027  NIHMSID: NIHMS131605  PMID: 19595780

Abstract

Previously, we demonstrated that mice in which the gene for the L-type voltage-gated calcium channel CaV1.3 is deleted (CaV1.3 knockout mice) exhibit an impaired ability to consolidate contextually-conditioned fear. Given that this form of Pavlovian fear conditioning is critically dependent on the basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA), we were interested in the mechanisms by which CaV1.3 contributes to BLA neurophysiology. In the present study, we used in vitro amygdala slices prepared from CaV1.3 knockout mice and wild-type littermates to explore the role of CaV1.3 in long-term potentiation (LTP) and intrinsic neuronal excitability in the BLA. We found that LTP in the lateral nucleus (LA) of the BLA, induced by high-frequency stimulation of the external capsule, was significantly reduced in CaV1.3 knockout mice. Additionally, we found that BLA principal neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice were hyperexcitable, exhibiting significant increases in firing rates and decreased interspike intervals in response to prolonged somatic depolarization. This aberrant increase in neuronal excitability appears to be at least in part due to a concomitant reduction in the slow component of the post-burst afterhyperpolarization. Together, these results demonstrate altered neuronal function in the BLA of CaV1.3 knockout mice which may account for the impaired ability of these mice to consolidate contextually-conditioned fear.

Keywords: L-type voltage-gated calcium channels, afterhyperpolarization, long-term potentiation, conditioned fear, memory mechanisms, spike accommodation

Introduction

We have previously demonstrated that deletion of the L-type voltage-gated calcium channel CaV1.3 disrupts Pavlovian fear conditioning in CaV1.3 knockout mice. Specifically, CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibited normal consolidation of contextually-conditioned fear memories at 1 and 6 hours after training but were unable to retain these memories long-term exhibiting significant impairments 24 hours after conditioning (McKinney and Murphy, 2006). While contextually-conditioned fear is thought to be dependent on both the amygdala as well as the hippocampus (Blanchard, Blanchard, and Fial, 1970; Kim and Fanselow, 1992; Maren, Aharonov, and Fanselow, 1997; Phillips and LeDoux, 1992) the consolidation deficits observed in the CaV1.3 knockout mice likely reflects a disruption of neuronal function within the amygdala proper, as spatial learning in tasks that require intact hippocampal function (e.g. the Morris water maze) are unaltered in these mice (McKinney and Murphy, 2006).

Currently there is general agreement that the basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA), which includes the basal (BA) and lateral (LA) nuclei, is a critical structure for the processing of fear memories (reviewed by Kim and Jung, 2006; Sah, Westbrook, and Lüthi, 2008; Sigurdsson, Doyère, Cain, and LeDoux, 2007) and thus the neurophysiological function of CaV1.3 in this structure is of interest. Voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC) and the L- type voltage gated calcium channels (L-VGCCs) in particular, have been implicated in several putative neurophysiological correlates of learning and memory. However, at present the relative contribution of the specific L-VGCC subtype(s) to these neurophysiological processes remains unknown. Of the presumed cellular mechanisms thought to subserve learning and memory, long-term potentiation (LTP) has been perhaps the most intensively studied.

Long-term potentiation can be induced in vitro in amygdala slice preparations using a variety of induction protocols and has been demonstrated at synapses within the BLA formed by afferent projections from the thalamus (the thalamic pathway) or the cortex (the cortical pathway). L-type VGCCs have been implicated in the induction of LTP at synapses in the thalamic pathway (Bauer, Schafe, and LeDoux, 2002; Weisskopf, Bauer, and LeDoux, 1999) as well as the cortical pathway (Drephal, Schubert, and Albrecht, 2006; Schroeder and Shinnick-Gallagher, 2004; Tsvetkov, Carlezon Jr, Benes, Kandel, and Bolshakov, 2002).

In addition to LTP, L-VGCCs have been demonstrated to play a role in regulating neuronal excitability. In the hippocampus, blockade of L-VGCCs with nimodipine decreases spike accommodation in a concentration dependent manner (Moyer, Thompson, Black, and Disterhoft, 1992). Although similar experiments specifically targeting L-VGCCs have not been performed in the amygdala, non-specific blockade of voltage-gated calcium channels with cadmium has been demonstrated to significantly increase repetitive firing rates in principle neurons of the BLA (Faber, Callister, and Sah, 2001; Faber and Sah, 2002; 2003; Washburn and Moises, 1992). The increased excitability that results from blockade of VGCCs is thought to be a direct result of a reduction in the post-burst afterhyperpolarization or AHP. The AHP is a hyperpolarizing voltage deflection that follows a burst of action potentials and serves to limit firing to a sustained depolarizing input (Alger and Nicoll, 1980; Hotson and Prince, 1980; Madison and Nicoll, 1984). The AHP is often described as having 3 components: a fast, medium and a slow AHP. The fast AHP (fAHP) occurs immediately after individual action potentials and lasts only 1–5 ms. The medium AHP (mAHP) is typically observed after a burst of action potentials and has a decay constant of approximately 100 ms. The slow AHP (sAHP) has a time constant of 1–5 seconds and is voltage-independent (Faber and Sah, 2007). Several studies using L-VGCC antagonists have shown that blockade of these channels in hippocampal pyramidal neurons leads to a significant reduction in the currents underlying the AHP, suggesting that the AHP is generated by calcium influx via L-VGCCs (Lima and Marrion, 2007; Marrion and Tavalin, 1998; Power, Wu, Sametsky, Oh, and Disterhoft, 2002; Shah and Haylett, 2000). While it has been previously demonstrated that blockade of voltage-gated calcium channels in the amygdala can significantly reduce the AHP (Faber et al., 2001; Faber and Sah, 2002; 2003; Washburn and Moises, 1992) to what extent that L-type voltage-gated calcium channels contributed to the generation of the AHP is unknown as is the relative contribution of the specific subtypes.

In the present study, we sought to determine to what extent the L-type calcium channel subtype CaV1.3 contributed to amygdala LTP and neuronal excitability. We found that CaV1.3 is critical for LTP induced in an in vitro horizontal slice preparation using a HFS induction protocol. In addition, we found that neurons in the BLA of the CaV1.3 knockout mice were hyperexcitable, exhibiting substantial increases in repetitive firing rates and decreased accommodation which were accompanied by a substantial decrease in the sAHP. Together, these results suggest that neuronal function is impaired in the BLA of CaV1.3 knockout mice and this may account for the impaired ability of these mice to consolidate contextually-conditioned fear.

Materials and Methods

Mice

The Cav1.3 knockout mice were generated by introducing a neomycin cassette into exon 2 of the gene for the pore-forming subunit of the Cav1.3 calcium channel (Platzer, Engel, Schrott-Fischer, Stephan, Bova, Chen, Zheng, and Striessnig, 2000). The null allele was maintained on a C57BL/6NTac background by successively crossing (> 12 generations) heterozygous null offspring with C57BL/6NTac wild-type mice purchased from Taconic Farms (Hudson, NY). Experimental mice were bred onto a 129S6B6F2/Tac hybrid genetic background. To obtain experimental mice, 129S6B6F1/Tac hybrid mice were generated first by crossing heterozygous null C57BL/6NTac mice from the maintenance cross with wild-type 129S6/SvEvTac mice purchased from Taconic Farms (Hudson, NY). The Cav1.3 heterozygous null 129S6B6F1/Tac mice were subsequently intercrossed to obtain experimental mice. For all experiments approximately equal numbers of young (2–6 mos) male and female mice were used. All comparisons were made between Cav1.3 knockout mice and wild-type littermates and the experimenter was kept blind as to the genotype throughout the experiment. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the guidelines set forth by the University of Michigan Committee on Use and Care of Animals.

Slice Preparation

The “horizontal slice preparation” was utilized for all of the in vitro slice experiments. We elected to use this particular preparation because the external capsule (EC) of the horizontal slice preparation contains intact afferents from brain regions including the hippocampus, perirhinal as well as entorhinal cortices (von Bohlen und Halbach and Albrecht, 1998; 2002) and it has been previously demonstrated that in the horizontal slice preparation, LTP in the lateral amygdala that results from stimulation of the EC can be induced in the absence of GABA antagonists such as picrotoxin (Drephal et al., 2006). Mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and decapitated. The brain was removed and horizontal slices were prepared as described in Drephal et al. (2006) and Stoop and Pralong (2000). Briefly, the brain was divided at the midline and each hemisphere was positioned flat on its medial surface. The olfactory bulb, the cerebellum, and the brain stem were subsequently removed and the dorsal side of the brain was cut along a plane which was tilted at a 10° posterosuperior-anteroinferior angle. The exposed dorsal side of the brain was glued onto a cutting block, the brain was covered with ice-cold (<1°C) oxygenated sucrose-based cutting solution containing the following (in mM): 2.8 KCl, 1 MgCl2, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 1 CaCl2, 206 sucrose, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, 0.40 ascorbic acid and horizontal slices (400 μm) were prepared with a vibrating blade microtome (VT1000S, Leica Microsystems). These slices contained the ventral hippocampus, the medial and lateral divisions of the entorhinal cortex, the perirhinal cortex, and the amygdaloid complex (see Figure 1A). Slices were transferred to a holding chamber filled with oxygenated artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) containing the following (in mM): 124 NaCl, 2.8 KCl, 2 MgSO4, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 10 D-glucose, 0.40 ascorbic acid at room temperature and remained there for at least 1 hour before being individually transferred to a submersion chamber and continuously perfused (~1.5 ml/min) with oxygenated ACSF heated to 31°C.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Synaptic transmission in the EC-LA pathway does not differ between CaV1.3 knockout and wild-type mice. (A) A picture of a typical horizontal amygdala slice with stimulating and recording electrode placement depicted (EC; external capsule, LA; lateral amygdala). (B) Input/output curve plotted as a function of field potential amplitude (in mV) recorded in the LA verses amount of current delivered to the EC (in μA). Amplitude of field potentials recorded in the LA increases and plateaus with increasing stimulation strength in both CaV1.3 knockout and wild-type mice. All data are presented as mean ± S.E.M.

Electrophysiology

All recordings were made using a Dagan BVC-700A amplifier in bridge mode and filtered at 1–3 kHz. Recordings were digitized using an Axon Instruments 1440 DigiData A/D converter at a rate of 10 kHz and acquired to a Dell Optipex GX 620 desktop computer with pClamp 10 (Axon Instruments). Field potential recordings were made using glass pipettes made from Clark Borosilicate Standard Wall glass (Warner Instruments), pulled with a P-97 Flaming-Brown pipette puller (Sutter Instruments), filled with ACSF (tip resistances ~ 1 MΩ), and placed in the lateral nucleus of the BLA. Bipolar (Pt/Ir) stimulation electrodes were used to stimulate the EC. Stimulation of the EC with square wave pulses (100 μs) resulted in a compound waveform that contained the summation of both EPSPs and synchronized action potentials (Doyere, Schafe, Sigurdsson, and LeDoux, 2003; Watanabe, Saito, and Abe, 1995). Because the slope measure of this waveform in the LA is more sensitive to variability and noise (Doyere et al., 2003), we analyzed the amplitude of field potentials in the present study.

Basal synaptic transmission was assessed by examining the input-output relationship within the LA. Input-output response curves were constructed by varying the stimulation intensity and averaging 5 responses for each stimulation intensity. For experiments that examined LTP, the stimulus intensity that evoked a field potential amplitude equal to ~50% of the maximal response was then used for HFS as well as single-pulse stimulations that preceded and followed the HFS. A stable baseline of responses was obtained for at least 5 minutes and then a HFS consisting of two trains at 100 Hz (1 s duration; 30 s apart) was administered. Subsequent responses to single stimuli were recorded for 60 min, and their amplitude quantified as change in percentage with respect to baseline. Stimulus frequency pre- and post-HFS was 0.067 Hz.

To investigate the impact of CaV1.3 deletion on intrinsic neuronal properties, whole-cell recordings were made from principal neurons in the amygdala horizontal slice preparation using the “blind” patch method (Blanton, Lo Turco, and Kriegstein, 1989). While the initial recording electrode position for these experiments was within the lateral nucleus of the BLA it is possible that as the recording electrode was advanced through the horizontal slice, the final recording location may have been in the ventral portion of the basal nucleus and therefore, hereafter we refer to these as principal neurons of the BLA without distinguishing the subnuclei.

Patch-pipettes were made from Clark Borosilicate Standard Wall glass (Warner Instruments), pulled using a P-97 Flaming-Brown pipette puller (Sutter Instruments) with resistances of 3–6 MΩ, and filled with the following internal solution (in mM): 120 potassium methylsulfate, 20 KCl, 10 HEPES, 4 Na2-ATP, 2 MgCl2, 0.3 Tris-GTP, 0.2 EGTA, 7 phosphocreatine. Seal resistances of >1 GΩ were achieved prior to rupturing into whole-cell mode. Principal neurons within the BLA were distinguished from interneurons based on 1) spike width, 2) frequency accommodation, 3) the presence of a post burst afterhyperpolarization (Mahanty and Sah, 1998), and were considered healthy if they had a resting membrane potential of −58 mV or below and exhibited action potentials that overshot 0 mV. Single action potentials were evoked by a depolarizing square current step (5 ms) of increasing amplitude delivered via the head stage. Single action potentials were measured from rest and analyzed for spike threshold, amplitude and width (measured at ½ of the action potential amplitude). Repetitive spiking was examined at rest in these neurons using a series of 500 ms current steps of increasing intensity (−0.05 nA to 0.35 nA, .05 nA steps). Spike accommodation was assessed at the highest level of depolarization in each group (0.35 nA). To record the post burst AHP neurons were held at 5 mV below action potential threshold and the AHP was evoked using a 100 ms current step sufficient to elicit 5 action potentials. AHP measurements were made from the average of 10 successive traces from each neuron.

All recordings were analyzed off-line using Clampfit 10.0 (Axon Instruments). Sample sizes refer to number of slices in the synaptic physiology experiments and the number of cells in the whole-cell experiment and all values are expressed as mean ± S.E.M. Statistical analysis was performed using student t-tests or repeated measures ANOVA using post hoc comparisons (Fisher’s PLSD) where appropriate.

Results

CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibit intact synaptic transmission, but impaired LTP

The in vitro horizontal slice preparation of the amygdala is pictured in Figure 1A. An input- output response curve was constructed to assess basal synaptic transmission in CaV1.3 knockout mice (Figure 1B). A repeated measures ANOVA revealed that field potential amplitudes increased with EC stimulus intensity (F(6, 150) = 90.3, p < 0.05), but there was no significant difference (F(1, 25) = 0.16, p > 0.05) between wild-type (15 slices from 8 mice) and CaV1.3 knockouts (12 slices from 7 mice).

Having established that basal EC-LA synaptic transmission was unaltered in CaV1.3 knockout mice, we sought to investigate the ability of CaV1.3 knockout mice to exhibit LTP following HFS delivered via the EC. Figure 2 shows the effect of HFS on field potential amplitude in the LA. Paired t-tests reveal that average field potential 55–60 minutes post-HFS was significantly potentiated over the average field potential during the five-minute baseline in wild-type (12 slices from 9 mice; t(1, 11) = 5.2, p < 0.05) and CaV1.3 knockout mice (10 slices from 7 mice; t(1, 9) = 2.8, p < 0.05). However, field potential amplitudes were significantly less potentiated (t(1, 20) = 2.8, p < 0.05; unpaired t-test) in CaV1.3 knockout mice (112.7 ± 4.6 %) than wild-type mice (144.3 ± 8.6 %). These results demonstrate that deletion of CaV1.3 results in a significant decrease in long-term potentiation of EC-LA synapses.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Long-term potentiation of LA field potentials (LA-LTP) induced by high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the EC is reduced in CaV1.3 knockout mice. (A) Representative traces of field potentials evoked by EC stimulation before and after HFS stimulation in wild-type mice (left traces) and CaV1.3 knockout mice (right traces). Scale bar 0.5 mV/5 msec. (B) Group data from all experiments. Significant LA-LTP is induced by HFS in slices prepared from both wild-type and CaV1.3 knockout mice, but LA-LTP was significantly reduced in CaV1.3 knockout mice. Data points represent averaged amplitudes normalized with respect to baseline. All data are presented as mean ± S.E.M.

BLA neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibit enhanced excitability and decreased post-burst AHP

To investigate the role of CaV1.3 in regulating intrinsic excitability, whole-cell current clamp recordings were made from BLA principal neurons in slices prepared from CaV1.3 knockout mice (14 cells from 6 mice) and wild-type mice (16 cells from 8 mice). There was no significant difference in resting membrane potential between CaV1.3 knockout mice and wild- type mice (−63.6 ± 1.2 & −65.4 ± 1.5 mV, respectively; unpaired t-tests, p > 0.05). Similarly, there was no significant difference in input resistance between CaV1.3 knockout mice and wild-type mice (147.1 ± 13.9 & 173.3 ± 10.3 MΩ, respectively; unpaired t-tests, p > 0.05). In addition, BLA principal neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice were not significantly different from wild-type BLA principal neurons when membrane properties measured during a single evoked spike were examined (Figure 3). We did not observe any significant differences between CaV1.3 knockout mice and wild-type mice with regard to action potential threshold, width, or peak amplitude (Figure 3A, B & C respectively; unpaired t-tests for all measures, p > 0.05).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Singe spike properties of BLA principal neurons recorded from CaV1.3 knockout mice were similar to those recorded in wild-type mice. (A) Representative single action potentials from a wild-type (A1) and CaV1.3 knockout (A2) BLA principle neuron. Scale bars: 10 mV/20 ms. (B) Action potential threshold, defined as the amount of membrane depolarization from resting membrane potential (RMP) required to elicit an action potential, was not significantly different in neurons from CaV1.3 mice when compared to wild-type neurons. Action potential height (C) and action half-width (D) was similar in neurons recorded from CaV1.3 knockout and wild-type mice. All data are presented as mean ± S.E.M.

While deletion of CaV1.3 did not significantly alter membrane properties at rest or during a single action potential, loss of CaV1.3 resulted in substantial changes in firing properties in response to prolonged depolarization (Figure 4). As the representative traces presented in Figure 4A indicate, prolonged current steps (0.35 nA; 500 ms) consistently generated more spikes in BLA principal neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice when compared with wild-type mice. A repeated measures ANOVA of the group data for the average number of spikes for the various current levels (Figure 4B) revealed a significant main effect of genotype (F(1, 28) = 6.5; p < 0.05) as well as a significant interaction between genotype and average spike number (F(6, 168) = 5.5; p < 0.05). Post-hoc comparisons of the average number of spikes at the individual current levels indicated that the BLA neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice fire more action potentials than BLA neurons from wild-type mice at all levels of depolarization except the two lower depolarizing steps (0.05 & 0.10 nA; Figure 4B).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

BLA principal neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibit greater excitability than neurons from wild-type mice. (A) Representative traces from BLA principal neurons recorded in horizontal amygdala slices prepared from a wild-type (A1) or CaV1.3 knockout (A2) mouse during a 500 ms, 0.35 nA current injection. Scale bar: 20 mV/100 ms. (B) Current steps of increasing magnitude produced an increase in the average number of spikes in both groups; however BLA principal neurons recorded from CaV1.3 knockout fired significantly more action potentials as compared to wild-type BLA principle neurons for current steps larger than 0.10 nA. (C) Spike accommodation, as measured by the average duration of the interspike interval (ISI), was significantly reduced in BLA neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice for ISI 3, 4 and 5. All data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. (*) p < 0.05 (Fisher’s PLSD posthoc comparison).

In addition to firing more action potentials, neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibited a significant reduction in spike accommodation (Figure 4C). To assess spike accommodation, the duration of the interspike interval (ISI) for the first six spikes was measured during repetitive spiking evoked by a 500 ms, 0.35 nA current step in BLA principal neurons from CaV1.3 knockout mice (14 cells from 6 mice) and wild-type mice (15 cells from 8 mice; one neuron could not be driven to fire six spikes). While neurons from both groups of mice exhibited spike accommodation, the average duration of the ISI was significantly less for CaV1.3 knockout mice when compared to wild-type mice (F(1,27) = 6.1; p < 0.05; main effect of genotype). From the group data it appears that deletion of CaV1.3 has a greater impact upon spiking that occurs later in the train of action potentials; an observation that is confirmed by post-hoc comparisons that were only significant for ISI 3–5. Taken collectively these data indicate that deletion of CaV1.3 leads to an aberrant increase in neuronal excitability within BLA principal neurons.

The firing frequency of principal neurons in the BLA is modulated in part by calcium- activated potassium currents that underlie the post burst AHP (Faber and Sah, 2002; 2005; Power and Sah, 2008; Washburn and Moises, 1992). Therefore, we hypothesized that increased neuronal excitability observed in the BLA of CaV1.3 knockout mice was due to a decrease in the AHP. Indeed, the AHP that resulted after 5 action potentials was significantly smaller in the CaV1.3 knockout mice in overall area (5552.8 ± 762.2 mV • ms) when compared with the AHP recorded in wild-type mice (10003.8 ± 1249.6.2 mV • ms; t(1, 28) = 2.9, p < 0.05; Figure 5). A change in the overall area of the AHP could be the result of alterations in either the mAHP or sAHP. To determine whether the mAHP was affected in CaV1.3 knockout mice, we measured the amplitude of the AHP at its peak (~ 60 ms after current stimulus offset) and 200 ms after current stimulus offset. The peak amplitude of the AHP did not differ between wild-type (−9.9 ± 0.6 mV) and CaV1.3 knockout mice (−9.6 ± 0.5 mV; t(1, 28) = 0.6, p > 0.05; Figure 6A). There was a strong, but statistically non-significant, trend for a smaller AHP amplitude at 200 ms after current stimulus offset in CaV1.3 knockout mice (−4.8 ± 0.4 mV) compared with wild-type mice (−6.0 ± 0.5 mV; t(1, 28) = 2.0 p = 0.06; Figure 6B). To assess if the sAHP was reduced in CaV1.3 knockout mice, we measured the amplitude of the AHP at 1000 ms after current stimulus offset. The AHP amplitude at 1000 ms was significantly smaller in CaV1.3 knockout mice (−1.7 ± 0.3 mV) than in wild-type mice (−2.9 ± 0.3 mV; t(1, 28) = 2.4, p < 0.05; Figure 6C) suggesting that deletion of CaV1.3 selectively impacts the generation of the sAHP. Additionally, AHP duration is significantly shorter in CaV1.3 knockout mice (4.4 ± 0.7 seconds) than in wild-type mice (6.7 ± 0.6 seconds, t(1, 28) = 2.5; p < 0.05, Figure 6D).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Area of the AHP is significantly smaller in CaV1.3 knockout mice. (A) Representative traces of AHP recorded from a BLA neuron of a wild-type mouse (A1) and CaV1.3 knockout mouse (A2). In both traces the action potentials are cropped to increase the scale of the AHP. Scale bar: 1 mV/1000 ms. (B) Average AHP area area of the AHP in CaV1.3 knockout mice is ~50% of that in wild-type mice. Data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. (*) p < 0.05 (unpaired t-test).

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Later components of the AHP are decreased in CaV1.3 knockout mice. (A) AHP peak amplitude does not differ between wild-type and CaV1.3 knockout mice. (B) While there a trend for a smaller AHP amplitude at 200 ms after stimulus offset in CaV1.3 knockout than wild-type mice this difference was not statistically significant (p = 0.06). (C) The AHP amplitude at 1000 ms after stimulus offset is significantly smaller in CaV1.3 knockout mice than wild-type mice. (D) The duration of the AHP is significantly shorter in CaV1.3 knockout mice than wild-type mice. All data are presented as mean ± S.E.M. (*) p < 0.05 (unpaired ttest).

Discussion

The primary finding of the current study is that deletion of the voltage-gated calcium channel CaV1.3 results in altered neuronal function within the amygdala. Specifically, we have demonstrated that LTP is significantly reduced in the amygdala of CaV1.3 knockout mice. In addition, we report that principal neurons in the BLA of CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibit enhanced excitability as measured by an increase in repetitive firing and a decrease in the post-burst AHP.

Our experiments using the horizontal slice preparation demonstrate that LTP induced by HFS of the afferent fibers of the EC is significantly reduced in mice that lack the CaV1.3 calcium channel. Importantly, the deficit in HFS LTP was observed in the absence of any alteration in basal synaptic transmission. Long-term potentiation in the amygdala is dependent upon activation of either NMDA receptors, L-type voltage gated calcium channels or both. The relative contribution of L-VGCCs to the induction of LTP in the amygdala appears dependent on the type of slice preparation, the specific stimulation protocol used to induce LTP and the afferents that are stimulated (Sah et al., 2008). Specifically, LTP induced in horizontal slices using 2 bouts of 100Hz stimulation (1 sec duration; 30 sec apart) of the external capsule can be blocked by either NMDA receptor antagonists or calcium channel blockers such as nifedipine (Drephal et al., 2006). Thus our results are consistent with the notion that the bulk of the LTP that is blocked by nifedipine is mediated by the L-type calcium channel CaV1.3. To limit our assumptions regarding which neuronal subpopulations (i.e. excitatory or inhibitory) were or were not impacted by the deletion of CaV1.3 we elected to use the horizontal slice preparation because LTP in this preparation can be induced without blocking inhibitory transmission (Drephal et al., 2006). Thus, while it seems likely that the observed deficit in LTP reflects an alteration in calcium signaling postsynaptically, it is possible that deletion of CaV1.3 disrupts plasticity in inhibitory interneurons such that there is an increase in inhibition after LTP induction in the CaV1.3 knockout mice. In addition to permitting LTP induction in the absence of GABA antagonists, the EC of the horizontal slice preparation contains intact afferents from the hippocampus, perirhinal and entorhinal cortices (von Bohlen und Halbach and Albrecht, 1998; 2002), brain regions critical for encoding contextual information (Burwell, Saddoris, Bucci, and Wiig, 2004; Kim and Fanselow, 1992) of the type with which CaV1.3 knockout mice are impaired in forming associations (McKinney and Murphy, 2006).

Deletion of CaV1.3 did not significantly alter the passive membrane properties of the principle neurons in the BLA. We did not observe any statistically significant changes in resting membrane potential or input resistance in the CaV1.3 knockout mice. Similarly, there was no change in the spike threshold, height or half width of single action potentials recorded in CaV1.3 knockout BLA principle neurons. Neuronal excitability in the amygdala has been reported to be regulated by voltage-gated calcium channels. Repetitive firing in response to a prolonged depolarizing step is significantly increased in the presence of cadmium (Faber et al., 2001; Faber and Sah, 2002; Gean and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1989; Washburn and Moises, 1992). Consistent with these reports, we observed an increase in neuronal excitability as measured by a significant increase in the firing frequency and a decrease in the interspike interval in response to prolonged current steps in the CaV1.3 knockout mice. It is worth noting that although the difference in input resistance between wild-types and knockouts was not statistically significant, there was a trend toward a decrease in the CaV1.3 knockout mice. This modest decrease in input resistance would be predicted to result in less membrane depolarization in response to somatic current injection making our finding of increased excitability even more striking. At present we do not know the cellular mechanism that underlies this putative change in input resistance but it is not unreasonable to hypothesize that this may be a compensatory mechanism which would functionally oppose the increased neuronal excitability observed in the CaV1.3 knockout mice.

Previous reports have suggested that VGCC modulation of neuronal excitability is regulated at least in part by the AHP (Faber et al., 2001; Faber and Sah, 2002; Gean and Shinnick- Gallagher, 1989; Washburn and Moises, 1992). Indeed, the CaV1.3 knockout mice exhibited significant reductions in the overall area of the AHP as compared to wild-type littermates. Deletion of CaV1.3 significantly reduced the overall area of the AHP as well as the AHP amplitude measured at 1000 ms after current stimulus offset. We did not observe a significant change in the peak amplitude of the AHP and although there was a trend towards a difference, the AHP amplitude measured at 200 ms was not significantly different in CaV1.3 knockout mice. In addition, the AHP duration is significantly shorter in CaV1.3 knockout mice than in wild-type mice. Taken collectively we conclude that CaV1.3 is selectively involved in the generation of the sAHP.

Though significantly reduced, a residual AHP (measured by overall area and amplitude at 1000 ms) is present in the principal BLA neurons of CaV1.3 knockout mice. At present the calcium source for the residual AHP is not known. In the hippocampus, where the putative role that L-VGCCs play in sAHP generation has been studied in more detail, it appears that blockade of L-VGCCs with dihydropyridines does not completely abolish the underlying calcium-activated potassium current that underlies the sAHP (Power et al., 2002; Shah and Haylett, 2000; Tanabe, Gahwiler, and Gerber, 1998). In these studies the residual calcium-activated potassium current might reflect the relatively poor blockade of L-VGCCs by dihydropyridines (Helton, Xu, and Lipscombe, 2005; Xu and Lipscombe, 2001). Clearly, in the present study this is not the case. One possibility is that the calcium source for the residual AHP is calcium influx through the other brain-expressed L-VGCC, CaV1.2. Although we have not examined the role of CaV1.2 in the generation of the sAHP in BLA principle neurons, we have recently demonstrated that deletion of CaV1.2 in CA1 pyramidal neurons within the hippocampus does not significantly reduce the sAHP while deletion of CaV1.3 reduced the sAHP by approximately 50% (Kuo, McKinney, White, and Murphy, 2006).

Alternatively one might imagine that calcium from intracellular calcium stores may be responsible for the residual sAHP. In hippocampal pyramidal neurons, disruption of calcium-induced calcium release (CICR) from intracellular calcium stores has been shown to reduce sIAHP—the current that underlies the AHP (Borde, Bonansco, de Sevilla, Le Ray, and Buno, 2000; Shah and Haylett, 2000; Tanabe et al., 1998; Torres, Arfken, and Andrade, 1996; van de Vrede, Fossier, Baux, Joels, and Chameau, 2007). While similar experiments have not been carried out in projection neurons in the amygdala it has recently been reported that calcium released from intracellular stores in response to cholinergic stimulation does not activate calcium-activated potassium channels that give rise to the sIAHP suggesting that CICR likely does not significantly contribute to the sAHP (Power and Sah, 2008). In addition, it has recently been demonstrated that calcium loading of the endoplasmic reticulum in BLA projection neurons can be achieved with subthreshold depolarization and it has been suggested that this calcium influx is gated by CaV1.3 (Power and Sah, 2005). Therefore if CICR is responsible for the residual sAHP, our data would suggest that the source of calcium that triggers the calcium release from internal stores does not require CaV1.3 and that the internal calcium stores can be replenished in the absence of CaV1.3.

A final explanation for the residual sAHP is the potential contribution of other voltage-gated calcium channels. While there has not been a systematic examination of which voltage-gated calcium channels mediate the sAHP in the amygdala, it is clear that voltage-gated calcium channels play a critical role in sAHP. The sAHP (or the underlying sIAHP) is almost completely abolished by cadmium (Faber et al., 2001; Faber and Sah, 2002; 2003; Washburn and Moises, 1992) and high concentrations of nickel (Faber and Sah, 2002; 2003). In support of the suggestion that CaV1.3 operates in concert with other VGCCs is the observation that the N-type calcium channel blocker ω-conotoxin-GVIA significantly reduces sIAHP in cultured rat hippocampal pyramidal neurons (Shah and Haylett, 2000). Whatever the other source of calcium may be, the data presented here suggest that CaV1.3 plays a key role in the generation of the sAHP in amygdala principle neurons.

The fact that BLA-LTP and neuronal excitability, two hypothesized neurobiological substrates for learning and memory, are altered in Cav1.3 knockout mice makes it tempting to speculate that one or both of the abnormalities leads to the impaired ability of Cav1.3 knockout mice to consolidate contextual fear learning. Within the BLA, a strong case can be made that LTP is necessary for fear learning. Sensory information about many potential conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, including auditory, contextual, and somatosensory information, converge on the BLA (Romanski, Clugnet, Bordi, and LeDoux, 1993; von Bohlen und Halbach and Albrecht, 1998; 2002) and LTP has been demonstrated in each of these afferent pathways (Bauer et al., 2002; Chapman, Kairiss, Keenan, and Brown, 1990; Drephal et al., 2006; Huang, Martin, and Kandel, 2000; Rogan, Staubli, and LeDoux, 1997; Weisskopf et al., 1999). Additionally, fear learning has been shown to modify synaptic strength of afferents on to BLA neurons in a way similar to experimentally-induced LTP (McKernan and Shinnick-Gallagher, 1997). Further, fear learning and BLA-LTP share similar stimulus contingencies (Bauer, LeDoux, and Nader, 2001) and molecular mechanisms (Rodrigues, Schafe, and LeDoux, 2004). Together, these observations suggest that an LTP-like mechanism underlies fear learning in the BLA. An example of the shared molecular mechanisms of fear learning and BLA-LTP which is consistent with our findings comes from a study in which Bauer et al (2002) demonstrated that the L-VGCC antagonist blocks BLA-LTP as well as long-term fear memory when a L-VGCC antagonist was infused into the BLA. Based on these studies it would be logically consistent to hypothesize that the deficits in contextual fear conditioning consolidation observed in the Cav1.3 knockout mice is due to a reduction in BLA-LTP, however this hypothesis may be overly simplistic.

There is a significant disparity in time course between the BLA-LTP experiments and the deficits observed in the Cav1.3 knockout mice with regard to the consolidation of contextual fear conditioning (see McKinney and Murphy 2006, Figure 2). This disparity has been reported frequently in the literature. Many manipulations (genetic and pharmacological) that impair long-term but not short-term memory block LTP within minutes after induction. The work of Bauer et al. (2002) provides a relevant example. Application of the L-VGCC antagonist verapamil impaired BLA-LTP induced by paired stimulation of the EC immediately, whereas intra-amygdala infusion of the same L-VGCC antagonist before fear learning resulted in impairment performance 24 hours later, but not at earlier time points. Some have argued that this disparity in time course may reflect a difference in the way in which LTP is engaged naturally in vivo and the way it is studied in vitro. These differences include patterns of stimulation and modulation by other areas of the brain (Schafe, Nader, Blair, and LeDoux, 2001). Alternatively, the LTP underlying fear memories may not be induced during training, but rather at a later time point, for example, during sleep. There is evidence that consolidation of memories may occur via the replaying of newly acquired patterns of activity during sleep (Axmacher, Mormann, Fernández, Elger, and Fell, 2006) and thus replaying of specific patterns of neuronal activity during sleep may induce LTP.

Like LTP, changes in neuronal excitability have been proposed as a neurobiological substrate of learning. It has been suggested that enhanced excitability is not part of the encoding of learning per se, but rather serves a permissive function for synaptic modification (Zhang and Linden, 2003). It is proposed that when a neuron is more excitable, the threshold for LTP is lower, thus information encoding is facilitated. Following this line of reasoning, one would expect animals with more excitable neurons to be better capable at encoding information and thus more efficient learners. Our results suggest this is not the case in Cav1.3 knockout mice. That is, though Cav1.3 knockout mice exhibit enhanced excitability in principal neurons of the BLA (the present study) and in CA1 neurons of the hippocampus (Kuo et al., 2006), neither hippocampus-dependent learning (Morris water maze) nor BLA-dependent learning (contextual fear conditioning) is enhanced (McKinney and Murphy, 2006). It could be that enhanced excitability does facilitate learning, but Cav1.3 knockout mice have other deficits that interfere with learning (e.g., impaired BLA-LTP). Further behavioral characterization of Cav1.3 knockout mice may reveal that there are some learning tasks on which they perform better than wild-type mice. Our data, however, are consistent with other data in which manipulations that enhance excitability rescue learning and memory deficits in aged animals (Disterhoft, Thompson, Moyer, and Mogul, 1996; Murphy, Fedorov, Giese, Ohno, Friedman, Chen, and Silva, 2004), but lead to impairments in young animals (Giese, Storm, Reuter, Fedorov, Shao, Leicher, Pongs, and Silva, 1998).

Voltage-gated calcium channels have previously been implicated in amygdala neurophysiology, including LTP and neuronal excitability, but these data are the first to implicate the Cav1.3 subtype specifically. While it seems likely that the disruption of BLA- LTP or the aberrant increase in neuronal excitability could account for the impaired ability of Cav1.3 knockout mice to consolidate conditioned fear, a thorough understanding of the relative contributions of these two neurophysiological changes to the memory deficits observed in the Cav1.3 knockout mice awaits further study.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Fredrick Henry for his help in the preparation of this manuscript. This work was supported by grants from The National Institute of General Medical Sciences 5T32GM008322 (to B.C.M.) and The National Institute on Aging RO1AG028488 (to G.G.M.)

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Alger BE, Nicoll RA. Epileptiform burst afterhyperpolarization: calcium-dependent potassium potential in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal cells. Science. 1980;210:1122–1124. doi: 10.1126/science.7444438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Axmacher N, Mormann F, Fernández G, Elger CE, Fell J. Memory formation by neuronal synchronization. Brain Research Reviews. 2006;52:170–182. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bauer EP, LeDoux JE, Nader K. Fear conditioning and LTP in the lateral amygdala are sensitive to the same stimulus contingencies. Nature Neuroscience. 2001;4:687–688. doi: 10.1038/89465. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauer EP, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. NMDA receptors and L-type voltage-gated calcium channels contribute to long-term potentiation and different components of fear memory formation in the lateral amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:5239–5249. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-12-05239.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Fial RA. Hippocampal lesions in rats and their effect on activity, avoidance, and aggression. The Journal of comparative and physiological psychology. 1970;71:92–101. doi: 10.1037/h0028958. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blanton MG, Lo Turco JJ, Kriegstein AR. Whole cell recording from neurons in slices of reptilian and mammalian cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1989;30:203–210. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(89)90131-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Borde M, Bonansco C, de Sevilla DF, Le Ray D, Buno W. Voltage-clamp analysis of the potentiation of the slow Ca2+-activated K+ current in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus. 2000;10:198–206. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:2<198::AID-HIPO9>3.0.CO;2-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Burwell RD, Saddoris MP, Bucci DJ, Wiig KA. Corticohippocampal contributions to spatial and contextual learning. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:3826–3836. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0410-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chapman PF, Kairiss EW, Keenan CL, Brown TH. Long-term synaptic potentiation in the amygdala. Synapse. 1990;6:271–278. doi: 10.1002/syn.890060306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Disterhoft JF, Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Jr, Mogul DJ. Calcium-dependent afterhyperpolarization and learning in young and aging hippocampus. Life Sciences. 1996;59:413–420. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(96)00320-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Doyere V, Schafe GE, Sigurdsson T, LeDoux JE. Long-term potentiation in freely moving rats reveals asymmetries in thalamic and cortical inputs to the lateral amygdala. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;17:2703–2715. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02707.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Drephal C, Schubert M, Albrecht D. Input-specific long-term potentiation in the rat lateral amygdala of horizontal slices. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2006;85:272–282. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2005.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Faber ES, Callister RJ, Sah P. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of principal neurons in the rat lateral amygdala in vitro. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2001;85:714–723. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Faber ES, Sah P. Physiological role of calcium-activated potassium currents in the rat lateral amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:1618–1628. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-05-01618.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Faber ES, Sah P. Ca2+-activated K+ (BK) channel inactivation contributes to spike broadening during repetitive firing in the rat lateral amygdala. Journal of Physiology. 2003;552:483–497. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.050120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Faber ES, Sah P. Independent roles of calcium and voltage-dependent potassium currents in controlling spike frequency adaptation in lateral amygdala pyramidal neurons. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;22:1627–1635. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04357.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Faber ES, Sah P. Functions of SK channels in central neurons. Clinical and Experimental Pharmacology and Physiology. 2007;34:1077–1083. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2007.04725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Gean PW, Shinnick-Gallagher P. The transient potassium current, the A-current, is involved in spike frequency adaptation in rat amygdala neurons. Brain Research. 1989;480:160–169. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)91578-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Giese KP, Storm JF, Reuter D, Fedorov NB, Shao LR, Leicher T, Pongs O, Silva AJ. Reduced K+ channel inactivation, spike broadening, and after-hyperpolarization in Kvβ1.1-deficient mice with impaired learning. Learning and Memory. 1998;5:257–273. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Helton TD, Xu W, Lipscombe D. Neuronal L-type calcium channels open quickly and are inhibited slowly. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:10247–10251. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1089-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hotson JR, Prince DA. A calcium-activated hyperpolarization follows repetitive firing in hippocampal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1980;43:409–419. doi: 10.1152/jn.1980.43.2.409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Huang YY, Martin KC, Kandel ER. Both protein kinase A and mitogen-activated protein kinase are required in the amygdala for the macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term potentiation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:6317–6325. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-17-06317.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kim JJ, Fanselow MS. Modality-specific retrograde amnesia of fear. Science. 1992;256:675–677. doi: 10.1126/science.1585183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kim JJ, Jung MW. Neural circuits and mechanisms involved in Pavlovian fear conditioning: A critical review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2006;30:188–202. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kuo AG, McKinney BC, White JA, Murphy GG. Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner. Atlanta, GA: Society for Neuroscienc; 2006. Voltage-gated L-type calcium channel subtype CaV1.3 contributes significantly to the generation of the AHP in CA1 pyramidal neurons. Online., Program No. 131.6. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lima PA, Marrion NV. Mechanisms underlying activation of the slow AHP in rat hippocampal neurons. Brain Research. 2007 doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Madison DV, Nicoll RA. Control of the repetitive discharge of rat CA 1 pyramidal neurones in vitro. Journal of Physiology. 1984;354:319–331. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Mahanty NK, Sah P. Calcium-permeable AMPA receptors mediate long-term potentiation in interneurons in the amygdala. Nature. 1998;394:683–687. doi: 10.1038/29312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Maren S, Aharonov G, Fanselow MS. Neurotoxic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus and Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 1997;88:261–274. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(97)00088-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Marrion NV, Tavalin SJ. Selective activation of Ca2+-activated K+ channels by co-localized Ca2+ channels in hippocampal neurons. Nature. 1998;395:900–905. doi: 10.1038/27674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. McKernan MG, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature. 1997;390:607–611. doi: 10.1038/37605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McKinney BC, Murphy GG. The L-Type voltage-gated calcium channel CaV1.3 mediates consolidation, but not extinction, of contextually conditioned fear in mice. Learning & Memory. 2006:584–589. doi: 10.1101/lm.279006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Moyer JR, Jr, Thompson LT, Black JP, Disterhoft JF. Nimodipine increases excitability of rabbit CA1 pyramidal neurons in an age- and concentration-dependent manner. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1992;68:2100–2109. doi: 10.1152/jn.1992.68.6.2100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Murphy GG, Fedorov NB, Giese KP, Ohno M, Friedman E, Chen R, Silva AJ. Increased neuronal excitability, synaptic plasticity, and learning in aged Kvβ1.1 knockout mice. Current Biology. 2004;14:1907–1915. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Phillips RG, LeDoux JE. Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1992;106:274–285. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.106.2.274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Platzer J, Engel J, Schrott-Fischer A, Stephan K, Bova S, Chen H, Zheng H, Striessnig J. Congenital deafness and sinoatrial node dysfunction in mice lacking class D L-Type Ca2+ channels. Cell. 2000;102:89–97. doi: 10.1016/s0092-8674(00)00013-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Power JM, Sah P. Intracellular calcium store filling by an L-type calcium current in the basolateral amygdala at subthreshold membrane potentials. Journal of Physiology. 2005;562:439–453. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.076711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Power JM, Sah P. Competition between calcium-activated K+ channels determines cholinergic action on firing properties of basolateral amygdala projection neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:3209–3220. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4310-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Power JM, Wu WW, Sametsky E, Oh MM, Disterhoft JF. Age-related enhancement of the sow outward calcium-activated potassium current in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons In vitro. Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22:7234–7243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-07234.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Rodrigues SM, Schafe GE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms underlying emotional learning and memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron. 2004;44:75–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rogan MT, Staubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Romanski LM, Clugnet MC, Bordi F, LeDoux JE. Somatosensory and auditory convergence in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1993;107:444–450. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.3.444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Sah P, Westbrook RF, Lüthi A. Fear conditioning and long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1129:88–95. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schafe GE, Nader K, Blair HT, LeDoux JE. Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: a cellular and molecular perspective. Trends in Neurosciences. 2001;24:540–546. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01969-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schroeder BW, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear memories induce a switch in stimulus response and signaling mechanisms for long-term potentiation in the lateral amygdala. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;20:549–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03517.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shah M, Haylett DG. Ca2+ channels involved in the generation of the slow afterhyperpolarization in cultured rat hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2000;83:2554–2561. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.2554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sigurdsson T, Doyère V, Cain CK, LeDoux JE. Long-term potentiation in the amygdala: A cellular mechanism of fear learning and memory. Neuropharmacology. 2007;52:215–227. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Stoop R, Pralong E. Functional connections and epileptic spread between hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and amygdala in a modified horizontal slice preparation of the rat brain. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;12:3651–3663. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00253.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Tanabe M, Gahwiler BH, Gerber U. L-Type Ca2+ channels mediate the slow Ca2+-dependent afterhyperpolarization current in rat CA3 pyramidal cells in vitro. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1998;80:2268–2273. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.5.2268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Torres GE, Arfken CL, Andrade R. 5-hydroxytryptamine4 receptors reduce afterhyperpolarization in hippocampus by inhibiting calcium-induced calcium release. Molecular Pharmacology. 1996;50:1316–1322. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Tsvetkov E, Carlezon WA, Jr, Benes FM, Kandel ER, Bolshakov VY. Fear Conditioning Occludes LTP-Induced Presynaptic Enhancement of Synaptic Transmission in the Cortical Pathway to the Lateral Amygdala. Neuron. 2002;34:289–300. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00645-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. van de Vrede Y, Fossier P, Baux G, Joels M, Chameau P. Control of IsAHP in mouse hippocampus CA1 pyramidal neurons by RyR3-mediated calcium-induced calcium release. Pflugers Archive. 2007;455:297–308. doi: 10.1007/s00424-007-0277-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. von Bohlen und Halbach O, Albrecht D. Tracing of axonal connectivities in a combined slice preparation of rat brains--a study by rhodamine-dextran-amine-application in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 1998;81:169–175. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(98)00026-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. von Bohlen und Halbach O, Albrecht D. Reciprocal connections of the hippocampal area CA1, the lateral nucleus of the amygdala and cortical areas in a combined horizontal slice preparation. Neuroscience Research. 2002;44:91–100. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(02)00092-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Washburn MS, Moises HC. Muscarinic responses of rat basolateral amygdaloid neurons recorded in vitro. Journal of Physiology. 1992;449:121–154. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1992.sp019078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Watanabe Y, Saito H, Abe K. Nitric oxide is involved in long-term potentiation in the medial but not lateral amygdala neuron synapses in vitro. Brain Research. 1995;688:233– 236. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)00563-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Weisskopf MG, Bauer EP, LeDoux JE. L-type voltage-gated calcium channels mediate NMDA-independent associative long-term potentiation at thalamic input synapses to the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:10512–10519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10512.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Xu W, Lipscombe D. Neuronal CaV1.3 α1 L-type channels activate at relatively hyperpolarized membrane potentials and are incompletely inhibited by dihydropyridines. Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:5944–5951. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-05944.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Zhang W, Linden DJ. The other side of the engram: experience-driven changes in neuronal intrinsic excitability. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2003;4:885–900. doi: 10.1038/nrn1248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES