Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2009 Sep 21;106(38):16018–16021. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903207106

Human origins: Out of Africa

Ian Tattersall 1,1
PMCID: PMC2752574  PMID: 19805256

Abstract

Our species, Homo sapiens, is highly autapomorphic (uniquely derived) among hominids in the structure of its skull and postcranial skeleton. It is also sharply distinguished from other organisms by its unique symbolic mode of cognition. The fossil and archaeological records combine to show fairly clearly that our physical and cognitive attributes both first appeared in Africa, but at different times. Essentially modern bony conformation was established in that continent by the 200–150 Ka range (a dating in good agreement with dates for the origin of H. sapiens derived from modern molecular diversity). The event concerned was apparently short-term because it is essentially unanticipated in the fossil record. In contrast, the first convincing stirrings of symbolic behavior are not currently detectable until (possibly well) after 100 Ka. The radical reorganization of gene expression that underwrote the distinctive physical appearance of H. sapiens was probably also responsible for the neural substrate that permits symbolic cognition. This exaptively acquired potential lay unexploited until it was “discovered” via a cultural stimulus, plausibly the invention of language. Modern humans appear to have definitively exited Africa to populate the rest of the globe only after both their physical and cognitive peculiarities had been acquired within that continent.

Keywords: Homo sapiens, evolution, fossil record, symbolic cognition


Africa is in a profound sense the fount of human evolution. Not only did our zoological family Hominidae (Homo sapiens plus its extinct close relatives, often nowadays restricted to the subfamily Homininae; for the purposes of this article the difference is merely notional) originate there ca. 7 Ma (1), but over the past 2 Ma the continent has regularly pumped out new kinds of hominid into other areas of the Old World (2). The genus Homo evolved in Africa at some time ca. 2 Ma [all older contenders to Homo status are debatable (3, 4)], then rapidly spread out of its natal continent to populate Eurasia for the first time (5, 6). The first truly cosmopolitan species of Homo, Homo heidelbergensis, is first known from Africa at ca. 600 Ka (7), before appearing at sites in Europe and eastern Asia from ca. 500 Ka onward. The now-ubiquitous species H. sapiens, to which all living human beings belong, is initially documented in Africa as, somewhat later, is the first material evidence of the symbolic cognitive system that appears to be unique to humans.

Modern H. sapiens Is Highly Derived in Its Osteology

Morphologically, our living species H. sapiens is extremely distinctive. It is not unique among hominids in having a large brain [averaging ≈1350–1400 mL in volume (8)]; but it is unique in the proportions of the skull in which that brain is housed, and in numerous smaller-scale cranial characteristics (4, 9). Among other features not found elsewhere, H. sapiens possesses a short, tall and more or less globular braincase, beneath the front of which a small, anteroposteriorly short and delicately built face is distinctly retracted (10). The orbits are surmounted by individual supraciliary ridges; although not invariably tiny, these are bipartite, with a central portion separated from a lateral plate by an oblique crease (4, 11). In the lower jaw, the H. sapiens chin is not simply a swelling at the external base of the symphysis [which can be found elsewhere among hominids (4, 12)]. Instead, it is a complex structure in the form of an inverted “T,” in which a vertical keel bounded by lateral depressions meets a basal transverse bar running between lateral tubercles (4, 12).

H. sapiens is equally derived in the structure of its postcranial skeleton. For example, in sharp contrast to the recently reconstructed skeleton of Homo neanderthalensis (13), that of modern humans is slender and delicately built; and although the Neanderthal rib cage is conical, tapering distinctly upwards from a broad base that matches the markedly flaring iliac blades of the pelvis, in H. sapiens the thorax is barrel-shaped. It is relatively narrow and tapers inward at the bottom and at the top, while the relatively delicate pelvis below it lacks lateral flare and has notably more vertical iliac blades. The dissimilarity between the two species is striking, and may have affected gait and external appearance (14). Nonetheless, until recently there was room for uncertainty over which thoracic/pelvic condition was derived within the genus Homo.

The two best pelvic specimens reported for species of Australopithecus (15, 16) made it clear that a broad, flaring pelvis is primitive for Hominidae; but whether pelvic flare is also primitive for the genus Homo [defined as those hominids possessing essentially modern body proportions (3)] was less evident. In contrast, the best skeleton of an early Homo (KNM-WT 15000 from West Turkana in Kenya) shows a weakly conical thorax and, as reconstructed, only a modestly wide pelvis (17). Still, it belonged to an immature and thus incompletely developed individual. At the same time, the excellently preserved and widely flaring adult pelvis (SH Pelvis 1) from the Sima de los Huesos at Atapuerca in Spain (18) is that of a Neanderthal relative, and does no more than confirm that this pelvic conformation is primitive for the Neanderthal clade. However, a recently reported adult pelvis from Gona in Ethiopia (19), dated to between 1.4 and 0.9 Ma and attributed by its describers to the same species as the Turkana specimen, joins more limited materials described earlier (20, 21) in showing great robusticity and the broadly flaring conformation. Available evidence thus now strongly suggests that the wide, flat, heavy pelvic morphology is indeed primitive for the genus Homo, in which case, the basic body form of H. sapiens, as well as that of its skull, is highly derived.

The same can also be said for the unique mode of cognition possessed by all living H. sapiens. Alone among organisms, as far as can be told, our species exhibits symbolic mental processes. That is to say, its members deconstruct the world around them into a huge vocabulary of mental symbols. These they combine and recombine in imagination to describe alternate worlds and situations, based on a capacity for generating a potentially infinite array of meanings from a finite set of elements (“discrete infinity”; see ref. 22). As a result, unlike other organisms including such cognitively sophisticated ones as chimpanzees, we live in worlds that are largely of our own making. And despite our manifold physical distinctions from our closest living relatives, it is clearly this cognitive difference that makes us feel so different from them.

Fossil Record of H. sapiens

How and where, then, were the distinctive features of H. sapiens acquired? The relevant fossil record is fairly thin, but it does firmly establish that as an anatomically recognizable entity our species made its first appearance in Africa (23, 24). The “Out of Africa” hypothesis of modern human origins emerged in the mid-1980s, when paleoanthropologists such as Günter Bräuer in Germany (e.g., ref. 25) and Chris Stringer in the U.K. (e.g., ref. 26) began to point out that, sparse as they were, the earliest fossils that resembled members of our species came from southern and eastern Africa. The resultant notion of a “single African origin” for modern humans stood in contrast to “multiregional” interpretations, in which the major modern geographical groups of H. sapiens were seen as having extremely deep roots in time (27). The single African origin notion received an enormous boost from molecular systematics when DNA comparisons (28, 29) began strongly supporting earlier conclusions based on proteins (30) that Africa had been the ultimate source of modern human populations worldwide. Over the last quarter-century, evidence on both the molecular and the fossil fronts has accumulated to the point where there can be little doubt that humankind ultimately originated in Africa.

Beyond this, however, the picture is a little hazy. To some extent, the perspective is complicated by context, for although Homo neanderthalensis, for example, was clearly a member of a larger clade of species united by a suite of readily identifiable characters, the distinctive H. sapiens is largely isolated, bereft of evident close fossil relatives. This isolation was obscured for many years by paleoanthropologists' habit of designating as “archaic H. sapiens” a very motley assortment of relatively large-brained hominids that are geologically fairly recent, but that are clearly not of modern H. sapiens morphology. In Africa, fossils assigned at one time or another to this meaningless wastebasket taxon include the very distinctive crania from Ngaloba, Ndutu, Kabwe, Florisbad and Jebel Irhoud. In blurring the very clear morphological boundaries of our living species the artificial construct of “archaic H. sapiens” served to obscure a picture of considerable morphological and presumably also taxonomic hominid complexity in the later Pleistocene, and it is fortunate that most paleoanthropologists seem now to have recognized its counterproductive nature.

Morphological delineation of H. sapiens is more legitimately complicated by a group of hominid fossils, mostly from South Africa, that possess all or nearly all of the diagnostic skull characters of H. sapiens, except for the bipartite brow and/or the chin. Among such specimens are Border Cave 5, Boskop, Fish Hoek, Klasies River Mouth (except for AP 6222), and maybe Cave of Hearths (4, 31). Most of these hominids are poorly dated; but some, at least, are probably quite recent, and all have traditionally been accepted as modern H. sapiens. More broadly, this group also includes some very early fossils, such as the Omo Kibish 1 specimen from southern Ethiopia recently redated to 195 Ka (23), and most likely also the <160 Ka adult cranium from the northern Ethiopian site of Herto (24) and the >130 Ka Singa partial cranium from Sudan (32). None of these fossils fulfills all of the most stringent morphological criteria applicable to living H. sapiens, but the Herto and Kibish fossils in particular demonstrate quite clearly that the unusual basic morphology evident in our species was established in Africa in the period following 200 Ka. If there is any justification at all for recognizing a form we might call “archaic H. sapiens,” this is where we find it.

Given the radical departure in cranial anatomy of these early African H. sapiens from other hominids known in the same time range, from the same continent (there is very little relevant postcranial record, if any), one reasonable conclusion is that the new morphology already exemplified at Herto and Kibish arose in a single change in gene regulation, with cascading developmental effects throughout the body. Significantly, there is no reason to suppose that the ramifications of this event were limited to the bony modifications observable in the fossils themselves.

Evidence of Behavior

Outside Africa there are no convincing claimants to H. sapiens status before ca. 93 Ka, the date of a clearly modern human skeleton (Qafzeh 9) from the Levantine site of Jebel Qafzeh (33). This find significantly predates any equivalent evidence from eastern Asia and Europe, and molecular dating supports the suggestion that the Qafzeh occurrence represents an initial foray of anatomical H. sapiens out of Africa that was ultimately foiled by climatic vicissitudes (34). What makes the Qafzeh fossil particularly relevant to the origin of modern human behavior and cognition is that, despite its modern morphology, it is associated with Middle Paleolithic stone tools more or less identical with those manufactured by penecontemporaneous Neanderthals in the same region (35).

A recurrent pattern in hominid history has been a temporal disconnect between innovation in the anatomical and behavioral realms. Thus, stone tool making was apparently invented by australopiths, whereas the achievement of modern body form was not accompanied by any detectable advance in material culture (36). The same pattern seems to have held in the case of H. sapiens: Modern human anatomy appears in the record significantly earlier than any evidence of symbolic behaviors. Of course, because behavior does not itself fossilize it has to be inferred from material proxies, many of which are very arguable. Based on various technological and economic indicators (blade production, use of grindstones, pigment processing, hafting) it has been proposed that hints of “modern” human behaviors can be detected as far back as several hundred thousand years ago, and that a sequential accumulation of such indicators reflects a slow, gradual attainment of “modern” behavior patterns over the past 300 to 500 Ka (37, 38). There is no doubt that in such indicators we are seeing evidence of increasing behavioral complexity (even as average hominid brain sizes were tending to increase, albeit not demonstrably in a linear fashion); but it can also convincingly be argued that no strictly technological aspect of Paleolithic behavior can be taken by itself as prima facie evidence of symbolic (as opposed to more generally cognitively complex) behaviors. In this perspective it is legitimate to conclude that symbolic cognitive processes, and their correlates, can be reliably inferred only from the production of overtly symbolic objects (as opposed to those that are merely technologically complex).

Even if we do not adopt the more stringent requirement for inferring the appearance of human symbolic cognition, we can nonetheless remark on the remarkably crude technological contexts in which the first anatomical H. sapiens are found. The few stone tools reported along with the Omo Kibish 1 cranium have been described as “unremarkable” (39), whereas those from the same deposits as the Herto cranium are notably archaic, consisting of some of the latest recorded African handaxes, plus some Middle Stone Age elements, approximately equivalent to the productions of Neanderthals (40). It is only very much later in time that we begin to pick up any artifacts whatever that may convincingly be interpreted as the products of symbolic minds. Earlier potential indicators such as the pigment processing and shellfishing recently reported from the site of Pinnacle Point on the southern African coast at ca. 160 Ka (41) are somewhat wishful markers for “modern” behavior patterns, especially given that both activities are documented for the almost certainly nonsymbolic Homo neanderthalensis (42). And the “symbolic” organization of the living space reported for the ≈100 Ka sites of Klasies River Mouth in South Africa (43) is necessarily inferential.

In light of all this, the most ancient objects that many observers would accept as clearly symbolic come from the younger site of Blombos Cave, also on the southern African coast (44). Recovered from Middle Stone Age layers dated to ca. 77 Ka, they consist of two small ochre plaques bearing engraved geometric designs. The same deposits also yielded small gastropod shells that were apparently pierced for stringing (45). Body ornamentation is widely considered a reliable proxy for symbolic behavior patterns, and the Blombos evidence is supported by similar “beads” found at other African Middle Stone Age sites, including the 82 Ka Grotte des Pigeons in Morocco (46). Interestingly, a possible occurrence of similar kind has been reported outside Africa at the >100 Ka Israeli site of Skhūl (47). This adds a potential hint of complexity to the picture, although biogeographers have generally considered the Levant an extension of the African continent (48). Still, in the record as currently known, the first fully mature expressions of the human capacity do not appear until ca. 35 Ka (4951) in Europe, when an extraordinary artistic flowering testifies to lives that were drenched in symbol. However, there is no reason to expect that all of the dimensions of the new human symbolic capacity should have been exploited at once, and it is at the very least plausible that what we are witnessing in the African Middle Stone Age are the first stirrings of a long process of cognitive discovery that is still continuing today.

It is worth noting that shortly after Blombos times southern Africa experienced an episode of aridification that may have largely or entirely depopulated the area for an extended period (43), implying that early symbolic expression in this region may not have been linearly ancestral to later such expressions elsewhere. Nonetheless, whatever the details of the evidently complex acquisition of symbolic cognition in our species might have been, it seems clear that the Africa provided the stage on which this radically new mode of processing information initially evolved.

Conclusions

Evidently, then, “becoming human” took place in two separate stages. First, the distinctive modern human morphology became established, very clearly in Africa, and probably shortly after 200 Ka. This event involved a radical departure from the primitive Homo body form. Only ca. 100 Ka later, again in Africa, and in a Middle Stone Age industrial context, did modern symbolic behaviors begin to be expressed, underwritten by a new capacity that had most plausibly been present but unexploited in the first anatomical H. sapiens. In evolutionary terms this disconnect was entirely routine, for every new behavior has to be permitted by a structure that already exists: Birds, for example, had feathers for millions of years before coopting them for flight, and tetrapods acquired their limbs in an aquatic context (52).

Symbolic reasoning appears to be qualitatively different from all other forms of cognition, including its own immediate precursor. Its neural substrate continues to be strenuously debated (53, 54); but, whatever it was, that structural innovation was most plausibly acquired as part and parcel of the radical biological reorganization that gave birth to H. sapiens as an anatomically distinctive entity. In which case (like those feathers and limbs) it remained unexploited, at least in the cognitive context, for a very substantial length of time, until its new use was “discovered” by its possessor. How this discovery was made remains a matter for conjecture, but a leading candidate for the necessarily cultural stimulus to symbolic processing of information is the invention of language (55). Language is perhaps the ultimate symbolic activity; and, in contrast to theory of mind, the other leading candidate for the role of releaser (56), it has the advantage of being a communal rather than an internalized attribute. The ability to use language depended, of course, on the presence of the vocal structures required to produce speech; but clearly these had already been exaptively acquired by the earliest anatomical H. sapiens.

Current evidence thus indicates that H. sapiens as we know it today had a dual origin: first as an anatomical entity, and only subsequently as a cognitive one. The clear signal of both the fossil and archaeological records is that both innovations occurred in Africa, from which the first fully modern humans expanded relatively recently to populate the rest of the world.

Acknowledgments.

I thank R.G.K. for his initiative in organizing this significant PNAS Special Feature on a critical aspect of human evolution and for inviting me to contribute to it.

Footnotes

The author declares no conflict of interest

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

References

  • 1.Gibbons A. The First Human: The Race to Discover Our Earliest Ancestors. New York: Doubleday; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tattersall I. Out of Africa again…and again? Sci Am. 2003;13:38–45. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wood BA, Collard M. The human genus. Science. 1999;284:65–71. doi: 10.1126/science.284.5411.65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schwartz JH, Tattersall I. The Human Fossil Record, Vol 4: Craniodental Morphology of Early Hominids (Genera Australopithecus, Paranthropus, Orrorin), and Overview. New York: Wiley-Liss; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.de Lumley H, et al. Datation par la méthode 40Ar/39Ar de la couche de cendres volcaniques (couche VI) de Dmanissi (Géorgie) qui a livré des restes d'hominidés fossils de 1.81 Ma. C R Palevol. 2002;1:181–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Swisher CC, III, et al. Age of the earliest known hominids in Java, Indonesia. Science. 1994;263:1118–1121. doi: 10.1126/science.8108729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clark JD, et al. Palaeoanthropological discoveries in the Middle Awash Valley, Ethiopia. Nature. 1984;307:423–428. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Holloway RL. Brain. In: Delson E, Tattersall I, Van Couvering J, Brooks A, editors. Encyclopedia of Human Evolution and Prehistory. 2nd Ed. New York: Garland; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schwartz JH, Tattersall I. The Human Fossil Record, Vol. 1: Terminology and Craniodental Morphology of Genus Homo (Europe) New York: Wiley-Liss; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lieberman DE, McBratney BM, Krovitz G. Sphenoid shortening and the evolution of modern human cranial shape. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:1134–1139. doi: 10.1073/pnas.022440799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Stringer CB, Hublin JJ, Vandermeersch B. In: The Origin of Modern Humans: A World Survey of the Fossil Evidence. Spencer F, Smith F, editors. New York: Alan R. Liss; 1984. pp. 51–135. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Schwartz JH, Tattersall I. The human chin revisited: What is it and who has it? J Hum Evol. 2000;38:367–409. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1999.0339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sawyer GJ, Maley B. Neanderthal reconstructed. Anat Rec. 2005;283B:23–31. doi: 10.1002/ar.b.20057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rak Y. On the differences between two pelvises of Mousterian context from Qafzeh and Kebara Caves, Israel. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1990;81:323–332. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330810302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Schmid P. Eine Rekonstruktion des Skelettes von A.L. 288–1 (Hadar) und deren Konsequenzen. Folia Primatol. 1983;40:283–306. doi: 10.1159/000156111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kibii JM, Clark RJ. A reconstruction of the StW 431 Australopithecus pelvis based on newly discovered fragments. South African J Sci. 2003;99:225–226. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Walker A, Leakey REF. The Nariokotome Homo erectus Skeleton. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Arsuaga JL, et al. A complete human pelvis from the Middle Pleistocene of Spain. Nature. 1999;399:255–258. doi: 10.1038/20430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Simpson SW, et al. A female Homo erectus pelvis from Gona, Ethiopia. Science. 2008;322:1089–1092. doi: 10.1126/science.1163592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rose MD. A hominine hip bone, KNM-ER 3228, from East Lake Turkana. Am J Phys Anthropol. 1984;63:371–378. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330630404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rosenberg KR, Zune L, Ruff CB. Body size, body proportions, and encephalization in a Middle Pleistocene archaic human from northern China. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:3552–3556. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508681103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hauser MD, Chomsky M, Fitch WT. The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science. 2002;298:1569–1579. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.McDougall I, Brown FH, Fleagle JG. Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature. 2005;433:733–736. doi: 10.1038/nature03258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.White TD, et al. Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature. 2003;423:742–747. doi: 10.1038/nature01669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bräuer G. In: The Origins of Modern Humans: A World Survey of the Fossil Evidence. Smith F, Spencer F, editors. New York: Alan R. Liss; 1984. pp. 327–410. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Stringer CB, Andrews P. Genetic and fossil evidence for the origin of modern humans. Science. 1988;239:1263–1268. doi: 10.1126/science.3125610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Wolpoff MH, Wu X, Thorne AG. In: The Origins of Modern Humans: A World Survey of the Fossil Evidence. Smith F, Spencer F, editors. New York: Alan R. Liss; 1984. pp. 411–483. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cann RL, Stoneking M, Wilson AC. Mitochondrial DNA and human evolution. Nature. 1987;325:31–36. doi: 10.1038/325031a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Stoneking M, Sherry ST, Redd AJ, Vigilant L. New approaches to dating suggest a recent age for the human mtDNA ancestor. Phil Trans R Soc Lond Ser B. 1993;337:167–175. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1992.0094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Nei M, Roychoudhury AK. Genic variation within and between the three major races of man, Caucasoids, Negroids and Mongoloids. Am J Hum Genet. 1974;26:421–443. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tattersall I, Schwartz JH. The morphological distinctiveness of Homo sapiens and its recognition in the fossil record: Clarifying the problem. Evol Anthropol. 2008;17:49–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Schwartz JH, Tattersall I. The Human Fossil Record, Vol. 1: Craniodental Morphology of Genus Homo (Africa and Asia) New York: Wiley-Liss; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Valladas H, et al. Thermoluminescence dating of Mousterian “Proto-Cro-Magnon” remains from Israel and the origin of modern man. Nature. 1988;331:614–616. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fagundes JRF, et al. Statistical evaluation of alternative models of human evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:17614–17619. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708280104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Vandermeersch B. Les Hommes Fossiles de Qafzeh (Israël) Paris: CNRS; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Tattersall I. What happened in the origin of human consciousness? Anat Rec (New Anat) 2004;267B:19–26. doi: 10.1002/ar.b.10041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.McBrearty S, Brooks AS. The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol. 2000;39:453–563. doi: 10.1006/jhev.2000.0435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ambrose SH. Paleolithic technology and human evolution. Science. 2001;291:1748–1753. doi: 10.1126/science.1059487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Klein RG. The Human Career. 2nd Ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Clark JD, et al. Stratigraphic, chronological and behavioural contexts of Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature. 2003;423:747–752. doi: 10.1038/nature01670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Marean CW, et al. Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa in the Middle Pleistocene. Nature. 2008;449:905–909. doi: 10.1038/nature06204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stringer CB, et al. Neanderthal exploitation of marine mammals in Gibraltar. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:14319–14324. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805474105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Deacon H, Deacon J. Human Beginnings in South Africa: Uncovering the Secrets of the Stone Age. Cape Town: David Philip; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Henshilwood C, et al. Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science. 2003;295:1278–1280. doi: 10.1126/science.1067575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Henshilwood C, d'Errico F, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z. Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa. Science. 2004;304:404. doi: 10.1126/science.1095905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bouzouggar A, et al. 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:1964–1969. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703877104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Vanhaeren M, et al. Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science. 2006;312:1785–1788. doi: 10.1126/science.1128139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Tchernov E. In: Neandertals and Modern Humans in Western Asia. Akazawa T, Aoki K, Bar-Yosef Y, editors. New York: Plenum; 1998. pp. 77–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Clottes J. Cave Art. New York: Phaidon; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Conard NJ. A female figurine from the basal Aurignacian of Hohle Fels Cave in southwestern Germany. Nature. 2009;459:248–252. doi: 10.1038/nature07995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Conard NJ, Malina M, Münzel SWC. New flutes document the earliest musical tradition in southwestern Germany. Nature. 2009;460:737–740. doi: 10.1038/nature08169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Shubin N. Your Inner Fish. New York: Pantheon; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lieberman P. The evolution of human speech: Its anatomical and neural bases. Curr Anthropol. 2007;48:39–66. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Coolidge FL, Wynn T. Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambr Archaeol J. 2005;15:5–26. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Tattersall I. An evolutionary framework for the acquisition of symbolic cognition by Homo sapiens. Comp Cogn Behav Revs. 2008;3:99–114. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Dunbar R. Grooming, Gossip and the Origin of Language. London: Faber and Faber; 1996. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES