ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHIC (EEG) SLOW WAVES ARE THE EPITOME OF DEEP SLEEP. THE PREVALENCE AND AMPLITUDE OF SLOW WAVES IN THE SLEEP EEG are typically quantified as slow-wave activity (SWA; power density in the 0.75 - 4.5-Hz range). SWA appears to be a marker of sleep need and to be an expression of a homeostatic sleep-regulatory process.1 But it is unclear whether SWA is simply an EEG epiphenomenon or whether it directly serves important functions. In this issue of SLEEP, Landsness et al2 tested the hypothesis that SWA is responsible for the consolidation of visuomotor learning.
Many recent studies have linked SWA to learning and synaptic plasticity. SWA is not the only aspect of sleep that has been related to such processes, but it has specifically been implicated in declarative memory,3 as well as in overnight gains in perceptual4 and visuomotor performance.5 SWA appears to be use dependent such that cortical circuits that were particularly active during wakefulness produce more SWA during subsequent sleep,5,6 whereas underused circuits produce less SWA.7 Increases in SWA that were induced either by practicing a learning task during prior wakefulness5 or by transcranial direct-current stimulation during sleep3 were found to correlate positively with the magnitude of sleep-dependent learning effects.
Whereas much of the earlier evidence was correlative, a recent study using an acoustic SWA-suppression paradigm provided support for a causal relationship between SWA and sleep-dependent gains in perceptual learning.8 The power of the SWA-suppression paradigm is that— if applied carefully—it allows for a sizable reduction of SWA, while total sleep time and REM sleep remain unaffected. Using this type of paradigm, Landsness et al2 examined the role of SWA in rotation adaptation, a well-characterized form of visuomotor learning. The authors show that reduction of SWA by an average of 31% prevented overnight improvement in visuomotor performance and that changes in SWA over the right parietal cortex, as measured with high-density EEG, correlated with changes in performance. Despite its strength, the acoustic SWA-suppression paradigm also has limitations: it is difficult to know whether the behavioral effects of the stimuli are attributable solely to the decrease in SWA or additionally to an increase in arousals. Well aware of these limitations, Landsness et al2 systematically pursued several new strategies to overcome them.
First, they included a control acoustic stimulation condition (CAS), in which subjects were exposed to stimuli during sleep but only when slow waves were absent. In contrast to slow wave deprivation (SWD), subjects exhibited significant learning in the CAS condition, thus ruling out the possibility that the acoustic stimulation procedure per se prevented learning. Next, the authors compared the number of clinically defined arousals. Although they found that the arousal index was slightly higher in the SWD than in the CAS conditions, multiple regression analysis with SWA and arousal index as independent variables revealed that only SWA predicted next-day visuomotor performance. Finally, the authors introduced a new automated arousal index that was based on an event-related spectral perturbation analysis of the EEG response to auditory stimuli. The basic idea was to identify EEG responses to stimuli that may not meet the criteria for clinically defined arousals but may represent a more subtle kind of sleep fragmentation that may differ between the SWD and CAS conditions. At the end of an exhaustive analysis, it was demonstrated, however, that it was indeed SWA and not a subtle type of EEG lightening or sleep fragmentation that predicted visuomotor performance.2 The analysis validated the acoustic SWA-suppression paradigm and strengthened its interpretational power.
In summary, Landsness and colleagues2 provide strong evidence for a causal role of SWA in visuomotor learning. Despite the clarity of these results, one should keep in mind that some other studies have failed to find effects of SWA suppression on learning9,10 or on other waking functions.11,12 It is possible that methodologic differences may have played a role, and, therefore, it will be important in the future to examine such differences more systematically. It is also reasonable to assume that waking functions differ in the degree to which they depend on SWA. The investigation of such differences may reveal clues about the functional role of SWA. The fact that the largest behavioral effects of acoustic SWA suppression so far have been found in learning2,8 is consistent with a role for SWA in plasticity.
DISCLOSURE STATEMENT
Dr. Aeschbach has received research support from Cephalon and has served on the advisory board of Zeo, Inc.
REFERENCES
- 1.Borbély AA, Achermann P. Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. WB Saunders Company. Philadelphia, PA: Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine; 2000. pp. 377–390. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Landsness EC, Crupi D, Hulse BK, et al. Sleep-dependent improvement in visuo-motor learning: a causal role for slow waves. Sleep. 2009;32:1273–84. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.10.1273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Marshall L, Helgadottir H, Molle M, Born J. Boosting slow oscillations during sleep potentiates memory. Nature. 2006;444:610–613. doi: 10.1038/nature05278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stickgold R, Whidbee D, Schirmer B, Patel V, Hobson JA. Visual discrimination task improvement: a multi-step process occurring during sleep. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2000;12:246–254. doi: 10.1162/089892900562075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, Tononi G. Local sleep and learning. Nature. 2004;430:78–81. doi: 10.1038/nature02663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kattler H, Dijk DJ, Borbely AA. Effect of unilateral somatosensory stimulation prior to sleep on the sleep EEG in humans. J Sleep Res. 1994;3:159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00123.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Huber R, Ghilardi MF, Massimini M, et al. Arm immobilization causes cortical plastic changes and locally decreases sleep slow wave activity. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:1169–1176. doi: 10.1038/nn1758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aeschbach D, Cutler AJ, Ronda JM. A role for non-rapid-eye-movement sleep homeostasis in perceptual learning. J Neurosci. 2008;28:2766–2772. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5548-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Karni A, Tanne D, Rubenstein BS, Askenasy JJM, Sagi D. Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science. 1994;265:679–682. doi: 10.1126/science.8036518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Genzel L, Dresler M, Wehrle R, Grozinger M, Steiger A. Slow wave sleep and REM sleep awakenings do not affect sleep dependent memory consolidation. Sleep. 2009;32:302–310. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.3.302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bonnet MH. Performance and sleepiness following moderate sleep disruption and slow wave sleep deprivation. Physiol Behav. 1986;37:915–918. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Walsh JK, Hartman PG, Schweitzer PK. Slow-wave sleep deprivation and waking function. J Sleep Res. 1994;3:16–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.1994.tb00099.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]