Abstract
With their origins in scientific validation, behavior-analytic applications have understandably been developed with an engineering rather than a crafting orientation. Nevertheless, traditions of craftsmanship can be instructive for devising aesthetically pleasing arrangements—arrangements that people will try, and having tried, will choose to continue living with. Pye (1968) provides suggestions for this, particularly through his distinctions between workmanship of risk versus workmanship of certainty, and the mating of functional precision with effective or otherwise pleasing variability. Close examination of woodworking tools as well as antique machines offers instructive analogues that show, for instance, that misplaced precision can be dysfunctional when precision is not essential to a design. Variability should be allowed or even encouraged. Thus, in the design of behavioral contingencies as well as of practical or purely aesthetic objects, “precise versus variable” is not necessarily a distinction between good and bad. More generally, behavior analysts would do well to look beyond their technical experience for ways to improve the aesthetics of contingency design while continuing to understand the resulting innovations in relation to behavior-analytic principles.
Keywords: aesthetics, acceptability of behavioral techniques, precision, variability, workmanship
Full text
PDFImages in this article
Selected References
These references are in PubMed. This may not be the complete list of references from this article.
- Boyce Thomas E., Hineline Philip N. Interteaching: A strategy for enhancing the user-friendliness of behavioral arrangements in the college classroom. Behav Anal. 2002 Fall;25(2):215–226. doi: 10.1007/BF03392059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branch M. N., Malagodi E. F. Where have all the behaviorists gone? Behav Anal. 1980 Spring;3(1):31–38. doi: 10.1007/BF03392376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catania A. C. The gifts of culture and of eloquence: An open letter to Michael J. Mahoney in reply to his article, "Scientific psychology and radical behaviorism". Behav Anal. 1991 Spring;14(1):61–72. doi: 10.1007/BF03392553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deitz S. M. Understanding cognitive language: The mental idioms in children's talk. Behav Anal. 1986 Fall;9(2):161–166. doi: 10.1007/BF03391942. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hineline P. N. A self-interpretive behavior analysis. Am Psychol. 1992 Nov;47(11):1274–1286. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.47.11.1274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hineline P. N. Can a statement in cognitive terms be a behavior-analytic interpretation? Behav Anal. 1984 Fall;7(2):97–100. doi: 10.1007/BF03391893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hineline P. N. The origins of environment-based psychological theory. J Exp Anal Behav. 1990 Mar;53(2):305–320. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1990.53-305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morris E. K., Higgins S. T., Bickel W. K. Comments on cognitive science in the experimental analysis of behavior. Behav Anal. 1982 Fall;5(2):109–125. doi: 10.1007/BF03392380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuringer Allen. Operant variability: evidence, functions, and theory. Psychon Bull Rev. 2002 Dec;9(4):672–705. doi: 10.3758/bf03196324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- SKINNER B. F. Are theories of learning necessary? Psychol Rev. 1950 Jul;57(4):193–216. doi: 10.1037/h0054367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shimp C. P. Organization in memory and behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1976 Jul;26(1):113–130. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1976.26-113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolf M. M. Social validity: the case for subjective measurement or how applied behavior analysis is finding its heart. J Appl Behav Anal. 1978 Summer;11(2):203–214. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1978.11-203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]