Abstract
Stick balancing at the fingertip is a powerful paradigm for the study of the control of human balance. Here we show that the mean stick balancing time is increased by about two-fold when a subject stands on a vibrating platform that produces vertical vibrations at the fingertip (0.001 m, 15–50 Hz). High speed motion capture measurements in three dimensions demonstrate that vibration does not shorten the neural latency for stick balancing or change the distribution of the changes in speed made by the fingertip during stick balancing, but does decrease the amplitude of the fluctuations in the relative positions of the fingertip and the tip of the stick in the horizontal plane, A(x,y). The findings are interpreted in terms of a time-delayed “drift and act” control mechanism in which controlling movements are made only when controlled variables exceed a threshold, i.e. the stick survival time measures the time to cross a threshold. The amplitude of the oscillations produced by this mechanism can be decreased by parametric excitation. It is shown that a plot of the logarithm of the vibration-induced increase in stick balancing skill, a measure of the mean first passage time, versus the standard deviation of the A(x,y) fluctuations, a measure of the distance to the threshold, is linear as expected for the times to cross a threshold in a stochastic dynamical system. These observations suggest that the balanced state represents a complex time–dependent state which is situated in a basin of attraction that is of the same order of size. The fact that vibration amplitude can benefit balance control raises the possibility of minimizing risk of falling through appropriate changes in the design of footwear and roughness of the walking surfaces.
Introduction
The maintenance of balance while standing and during locomotion arises from complex interactions between the walker and their environment. So robust are the control mechanisms that the occurrence of a fall is a cause of great concern to the walker and often to the medical profession as well. Current hypotheses for the control of balance are motivated by considerations of the stabilization of a pendulum in the inverted position; a classic problem in control theory [1]. The control problem arises because the upright position of the pendulum is unstable and hence even the slightest perturbation is sufficient to cause it to fall over. Consequently, for over 20 years, it has been assumed that human balance represents an equilibrium that is stabilized by the interplay between the biomechanical properties of the musculo–skeletal systems [2]–[4] and by time–delayed negative neural feedback [5], [6]. Recently this view of human balance control has been challenged [7]–[9]. A growing number of experimental observations emphasize that the upright position is not a stable equilibrium, but a more complex and bounded time–dependent state [4], [10]–[16]. Moreover, control in two paradigms of human balance control, namely stick balancing at the fingertip [7], [17] and postural sway during quiet standing [8], [9], [18], [19], is intermittent not continuous.
A direct way to explore the nature of the balanced state is to examine the effects of parametric excitation on the ability of a subject to balance a stick at their fingertip. The term ‘parametric excitation’ refers to the fact that when the pivot point of an inverted pendulum is vibrated the effects enter the equations of motion through a time varying parameter [20], [21]. It is well known that if the upright position is an equilibrium then it can be stabilized by vibrating the pivot point in the vertical direction using frequencies that exceed
(1) |
where is the critical frequency (cycles/sec) at which the upright position is stabilized, is the peak–to–peak amplitude of the oscillation, is the length of the pendulum, and is the acceleration due to gravity [20]. However, this effect requires that the pivot point be physically attached to the pendulum in order that downward accelerations exceed gravity [21]: this is not possible for stick balancing since the stick sits on the fingertip but is not bonded to it. An alternate hypothesis for balance control, referred to herein as “drift and act”, is that the desired upright position is one in which the dynamics are confined within a small basin of attraction: inside the basin of attraction trajectories “drift”; however, whenever trajectories exceed the basin boundaries, corrective actions (“act”) are taken to redirect the trajectories back into it [7], [13], [16], [22]–[27]. The observed statistical properties of human stick balancing, namely the Weibull–type survival functions [28], [29], the power laws that describe the times between successive corrective movements [17], and the Lévy distributions which describe the changes in speed made by the fingertip [30], [31], point to an underlying dynamical system that is tuned near enough to the edge of stability that critical control parameters can be noisily forced back and forth across the stability boundary. In the setting of drift and act control any amplitude lowering effect due to parametric excitation would be stabilizing since it biases the fluctuations away from the stability, or basin, boundary and hence prolongs the first passage time.
The organization of our discussion is as follows. First, we demonstrate that the mean stick balancing time is increased when the subject stands on a vibrating platform (Figure 1). The purpose of the vibrating platform is to introduce periodic vertical vibrations at the fingertip (parametric excitation) in a manner that does not influence the freedom of the balancing arm and hand movements. Second we show that whole body vibration does not decrease the neural latency for stick balancing or alter the changes in speed made by the fingertip during stick balancing. Third it is shown that the relative movements of the position of the fingertip and tip of the stick exhibit an oscillatory relationship in the horizontal plane and that vibration decreases the amplitude of these fluctuations. Fourth, it is shown that a plot of the logarithm of the vibration-induced increase in the mean stick balancing time, a measure of the mean first passage time, versus is the standard deviation of these fluctuations, a measure of the distance to the threshold, is linear as expected for the times to cross a threshold in a stochastic dynamical system [32], [33]. Finally, we illustrate that for a generic class of “drift and act”–type mathematical models parametric excitation can produce a lowering of the amplitude of limit cycle oscillations.
Results
Subjects balanced a stick on their fingertip while standing on a vibrating platform (Figure 1). Figure 2 shows the effect of a 50 Hz, 0.001 m vertical vibration at the fingertip on the stick survival curve for one subject. The stick survival curves have the characteristic shape expected for a failure time process [28], [29]. The effect of the vibration is to shift the stick survival curve to the right. For the subject in Figure 1 the mean stick survival time, , a measure of balancing skill, is increased by –fold (, Mann-Whitney U test).
The effects of 15–50 Hz vibration on stick balancing skill for 9 subjects having a range of skill levels are summarized in Figure 3 and Table 1. Two types of vibrating platform were used (Table 1): vertical vibrating platforms that produce periodic vertical vibrations at the fingertip (Physioplate, iTonic, Powerplate) and a vibrating platform that produces aperiodic vibrations mainly in the horizontal plane (Soloflex). Figure 3a shows that for 19/21 experiments using the vertical–type vibrating platforms, vibration produced a statistically significant improvement in stick balancing skill (; in 11/19 experiments the level of significance was ). In 2/21 experiments (open ) the increase in mean survival time with vibration did not reach the level of significance (). These experiments involved two subjects vibrated at 25 Hz: in each case a statistically significant increase in stick balancing skill was obtained when the vibration frequency was increased to 50 Hz.
Table 1. Vibration characteristics and stick balancing skill.
Vibration Amplitude (mm) | Relative Survival | |||
Vibration Source | Sole | Fingertip (still) | Fingertip (Balancing) | |
None (18) | - | - | - | 1.0 (0.8–1.2) |
Physioplate | ||||
15 Hz (3) | 2.3 | 0.3 | 0.1 | 2.6 (1.4–4.1) |
25 Hz (3) | 1.2 | 0.2 | 0.1 | 2.1 (1.5–2.6) |
50 Hz (3) | 0.9 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 2.2 (1.5–3.1) |
iTonic/Powerplate | ||||
25 Hz (6) | 1.1 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 1.5 (1.0–2.1) |
50 Hz (6) | 1.2 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 2.1 (1.4–3.4) |
Soloflex (6) | 0.3 | 0.1 | UD | 1.0 (0.9–1.2) |
Number of subjects given in brackets.
Mean (Minimum value - Maximum value).
Relative survival is the mean stick survival time measured in the presence of vibration divided by the mean stick survival time measured in the absence of vibration.
UD is undetectable.
In contrast, Figure 3b shows that when 7 subjects performed stick balancing while standing on a vibrating platform that produced undetectable vertical vibrations at the fingertip, no statistically significant enhancement of skill was observed ( for all subjects). Taken together, these observations strongly indicate that the vibration–enhancement of stick balancing skill is not simply due the effects of whole body vibration per se, for example on vision [34], but are primarily related to vertical vibrations at the fingertip.
Vibration and neural latency
Modeling studies of an inverted pendulum controlled by time–delayed negative feedback indicate that a necessary, but not sufficient, condition for stabilization is that the neural latency, , is less than a critical delay, , given by [5], [15], [35]. For m, s which is longer than estimates of s for low to moderate skill stick balancers [30]. To test the possibility that the beneficial effects of vibration on stick balancing skill were related to its effects on neural response times, we measured the cross–correlation, , between the position of the tip of the stick at time and the corrective movements made by the fingertip at time , i.e. [30]. The shift in from gives an estimate of the response time, or neural latency, for stick balance control.
Figure 4 shows the effect of vibration on for two subjects. It can be seen that vibration shifts to the right by s. Thus the vibration enhancement of stick balancing skill is not due to a shorter neural latency, i.e. a faster neural response time. Increases in neural latency towards as stick balancing skill increases with practice have been observed previously [30], and have been interpreted as reflecting a decreased role for active neural control. It should be noted that since a vibratory input to the fingertip necessarily effects the position of the reflective markers at both ends of the stick equally, it cannot itself produce a shift in . Consequently the effects of vibration are superimposed on . Differences between the prominence of the vibratory component to (compare Figure 4c and d) presumably reflects differences in the low–pass filtering characteristics of different bodies and postures on the vibratory input applied at the sole of the foot and were not investigated further.
Vibration and fingertip speed
Previous studies have shown that the distribution of the changes in speed, , made by the fingertip during stick balancing, is Lévy–distributed [30], [31]. Increases in stick balancing skill over the first few days of practice are mirrored by a broadening of the tails of . In other words, skilled stick balancers are able to make, or tolerate, larger 's. Whole body vibration can alter motor performance through its effects on skeletal muscle [36], muscle spindles [37]–[39] and motor cortex excitability and voluntary drive [40]. Thus it is important to determine whether the beneficial effects of vibration on stick balancing are manifested through its effects on .
Figure 5 compares the effects of m, 25 Hz and 50 Hz vibration on for one subject. Clearly vibration produces no significance change in and, in particular, does not broaden the tails of the distribution. The same observations were obtained for two other subjects (one having a higher skill level and the other a lower skill level than the subject shown in Figure 5 (data not shown)). Thus the beneficial effects of vibration on stick balancing skill are not related to changes in .
Vibration and fingertip movements
An often under–appreciated aspect of stick balancing is the oscillatory relationship between the relative movements of variables related to the controlled variable, e.g. the vertical displacement angle, , and variables related to the controller, e.g. the position of the fingertip. Experimentally this oscillatory relationship is most easily appreciated by viewing stick balancing from above looking downwards. In this view information concerning the vertical extent of the movements is necessarily lost, but the oscillatory relationship between the movements of the fingertip and the tip of the stick in their respective horizontal planes is clearly seen. We represented these movements by the calculating the length, , of the position vector to the fingertip or the tip of stick from a common reference point, , where . Figure 6a shows the oscillatory relationship between the movements of fingertip and tip of the stick represented in this manner (see legend for more details). This oscillatory relationship between controlled and controller is not unique to stick balancing at the fingertip but arises in mechanical stick balancing (Figure 6b), virtual stick balancing (Figure 6c), and even human postural sway [2]. The oscillatory movements are thought to be related to intrinsic difficulties in controlling both the position of the fingertip and the vertical displacement angle, , [15] and the lag arises because these paradigms in essence represent a time–delayed pursuit–escape task.
Figure 7 shows the effects of vibration on the movements of the fingertip and the vertical displacement angle, , of the balanced stick. By comparing sufficiently long balancing trials of approximately the same length (see figure legend for details) it is seen that the effect of vibration is to concentrate the movements of the fingertip over a smaller area in the horizontal plane (compare Figure 7a and c). The difference between the solid and dashed lines in Figure 6a is defined as . Figure 7b shows that the standard deviation of is decreased in the presence of vibration and Figure 7d shows that is biased towards vertical.
Figure 8 summarizes the relationship between the vibration–induced decrease in the fluctuations in and the increase in stick balancing skill. Since the changes in are not precisely periodic, we treated them as a stochastic signal and characterized the amplitude of the fluctuations using the standard deviation. Clearly the larger the reduction in the standard deviation of , the greater the vibration–induced enhancement in stick balancing skill (Figure 8a). These observations can be re–interpreted in terms of the Kramers rate theory for the escape of a particle from a potential well [32], [33]. According to this theory the mean first passage time, , i.e. the mean time that it takes a particle to exceed the height of the potential barrier, is related to the barrier height by
If we identify with the relative survival (RA), and the decrease in the fluctuations with a vibration–induced increase in barrier height, then Figure 8b shows that a plot of the logarithm of RA versus the % decrease of the amplitude fluctuations is linear. Thus the effects of vibration of stick balancing skill can be well understood from the effects of vibration on increasing the effective barrier height of a potential well by decreasing the amplitudes of the fluctuations.
Discussion
Our observations demonstrate that stick balancing skill can be enhanced by periodically vibrating the fingertip in the vertical direction. The frequency of these vibrations (15–50 Hz) is much less that required for the stabilization of an inverted pendulum by vibrating an attached pivot point ( Hz for a = 0.001 m and 0.55 m). The whole body vibration enhancement of the mean stick balancing time is observed only when vertical vibrations are produced at the fingertip, is associated with a small increase in neural latency, and produces no changes in the distribution of the changes in speed made by the fingertip. Taken together these observations suggest that the skill enhancement is due to vertical vibrations at the fingertip and not to the effects of vibration on the nervous or musculo–skeletal system. We suggest that a simple explanation for this unexpected observation is to hypothesize that the upright balanced position is not a simple equilibrium, but represents a complex bounded time–dependent state that is confined within a basin of attraction whose size is of the same order [7]–[9], [15]. Consequently, for sufficiently large fluctuations, trajectories can escape the basin of attraction, and the stick subsequently falls. In this setting, any mechanism that biases the fluctuations generated by this time-dependent state away from the basin boundary enhances stick balancing skill. The experimentally observed exponential relationship between the vibration–induced increase in stick balancing skill and the decrease in the amplitude of the fluctuations the the fingertip-stick movements supports this interpretation.
Although, the use of parametric excitation to control the amplitude of limit cycle oscillations has been described previously [41]–[46], little attention has been previously given to the possible implications of this mechanism for human balance control. Recent control theoretic arguments for the control of an unstable fixed point in the presence of time delayed feedback and random perturbations (“noise”) have emphasized the need for switch–like controllers in which for small displacements the variable “drifts” with active control (“act”) taken only once the variable exceeds certain thresholds [7], [10], [13], [22], [23], [25]–[27]. A one–dimensional generic model with “drift–and–act” control of human balance with parametric excitation takes the form
(2) |
where is a constant, is the forcing frequency, is the time delay, are, respectively, the values of the controlled variable at times and , and describes white additive noise with variance . The feedback function, , has the step–like shape shown in Figure 9a. Models of this type have been successfully employed, for example, to obtain insights into the properties of the two–point correlation functions observed for human postural sway [7], [13], [16]. Figure 9b illustrates that in the absence of noise the amplitude of a limit cycle oscillation can be lowered using parametric excitation. The attractiveness of drift and act, and related controllers, is that they are robust, inexpensive to implement, and optimal for finite corrective actions [47]. However, it may also be possible to gain further insights into our observations by examining the effects of parametric excitation on recently developed models for balancing that are based on an inverted pendulum controlled by nonlinear, time–delayed feedback [8], [9], [15], [17], [35], [48], [49].
Measurements of the frequency and amplitude dependence of the vibration–enhancement of stick balancing skill provide the direction for future model development. However, there are two intrinsic limitations of our experimental design. The first limitation arises because we use whole body vibration to introduce vibration at the fingertip. Consequently the range of frequencies available for testing is limited because frequencies less than Hz are considered harmful for humans [50], [51] and those greater than Hz were reported by our participants to be extremely uncomfortable. Similarly the range of amplitudes generated at the fingertip by vibrating the feet is limited by the fact that for vibrating frequencies above Hz the human body with knees flexed behaves as a powerful low–pass filter [50], [51]. In order to overcome these limitations it will be necessary to develop experimental techniques suitable for introducing vibrations directly to the fingertip without hindering the movements of the balancing arm and hand; possibly techniques that involve appropriately designed puffs of air.
The second, and perhaps more important, limitation is related to the assessment of stick balancing skill. Using the mean stick survival time, , determined from at least 25 stick balancing trials as a measure of skill level corresponds to trials per vibration condition (see METHODS) and translates into minutes accumulated exposure to whole body vibration for a subject with s. Thus in order to minimize fatigue, each experiment was done on a different day. However, since stick balancing is a voluntary goal–directed task, skill level increases each day with practice [52]. These changes in skill level are not detectable over the time it takes to determine , but are readily apparent when is compared from one day to the next. For example, we observed that 6 out of 11 subjects who practice stick balancing 30–60 minutes a day achieve stick balancing times s within 14 days of consecutive practice. In view of these considerations our experiments focused on subjects who had relatively low stick balancing skill levels (e.g. s). The observations in Figures 7a and c suggest that it might eventually be possible to assess skill from measurements made on a single, sufficiently long stick balancing trial (see legend).
It is tempting to speculate that drift–and–act control strategies might also be involved in the maintenance of postural balance. Since falls for adult humans are rare events compared to stick falls during stick balancing, the basin of attraction for posture is larger that that for stick balancing. Experimental evidence in favor of drift–and–act type human postural balance control include the intermittent nature of the corrective movements [17]–[19], the persistence and anti–persistence properties of the two–point correlation functions for postural sway [10], [13], [15], [16], and the ankle–hip–step strategies used by humans to maintain balance in response to increasingly large perturbations [53]. Indeed we have confirmed that vibration applied to bilateral Achilles' tendons during quiet standing produces a reduction in the amplitude of the fluctuations in the center of pressure during postural sway as predicted by (2) (unpublished observations).
Human movements and balance control take place in a randomly fluctuating environment. The anticipation that random fluctuations can improve balance control has already been verified [54]–[57]. Here we have shown that introducing vibrations to the body has functional benefit, namely the vibrations enhance stick balancing skill. Our observation that the amplitude of the vibrations is important for stabilizing balance raises the possibility that falls are not always simply related to “slips and trips”, but may be encouraged by modern day society's efforts to filter out effects of surface–induced vibrations through shoe and walking surface design. Thus, in view of the impending epidemic of falling due to aging demographics [58], it may be possible that changes in walking shoe and surface design may help reduce the risk of falling in this population.
Materials and Methods
Subjects
Data was collected and analyzed for 11 females and 7 males ages 18–59 years who were free of balance disorders. This study was approved by the institutional review board at Claremont McKenna College and A. T. Still University in accordance with the currently applicable U. S. Public Health Service Guidelines. All participants provided written informed consent for all research testing.
Stick balancing at the fingertip
Stick balancing was performed while the subject stood on a vibratory platform in stocking feet with knees slightly flexed and their back against a vertical support (Figure 1). Sticks were wooden dowels with diameter mm and length m. For each stick balancing trial we used a coin flip we used a coin flip to determine whether the vibration was on or off. This procedure was continued until we had accumulated at least 25 trials for each condition, a process that took min to complete depending on the skill of subject. The time that the stick remained balanced at the fingertip was timed using a stop watch. Control studies ( in Figure 3) mimicked this procedure except that the subject was not subjected to whole body vibration and the coin flip was used to randomly assign each trial to one of two groups.
Stick balancing skill was measured by estimating quantities related to the first passage time, i.e. the time elapsed until the balanced stick falls [28], [29]. The survival function, , for stick balancing has the form of a Weibull survival function, i.e. , where . The mean stick balancing time, , calculated using a minimum of 25 consecutive trials, was used as a measure of skill level. Participants for this study were selected from a group of subjects who had practiced stick balancing for a few days. We selected those subjects who had achieved a low to moderate skill level (s; Figure 3). Approximately 50% of subjects achieve much higher skills levels within 10 days of practice, e.g. min) and were excluded since the time to complete the required trials would have been so long (e.g. at least 8 hours) that fatigue would have become a factor.
Vibrating platforms were commercially available: Physioplate (Globus Sport and Health technologies, LLC), iTonic (Freemotion Fitness), Powerplate (Powerplate North America, Northbrook, Illinois) and Soloflex (Soloflex, Inc.). The frequency and vertical amplitude of the vibrations were measured at the platform surface and at the fingertip using a three camera motion capture system (Qualisys Oqus 300, sampling frequency 500 Hz). Reflective markers were firmly attached to the vibrating platform and to each each of the stick using Epoxy cement. Measurements of the vibration amplitude were made while the stick was held in the outstretched hand and at the fingertip during stick balancing. These measurements are summarized in Table 1. The range of frequencies and amplitudes of the fingertip vibration are well within the range of responses recorded for human mechano–receptors [59]. We allowed the subjects to adjust their comfort level by self–selecting the degree of flexion at their knee (Figure 1).
Virtual stick balancing measurements involved using a paradigm developed previously that involves the interplay between a human and a computer [60], [61]. Briefly, the subject views a target and a dot on a computer screen. The dot reflects the movements controlled by the computer mouse and the movements of the target are controlled by the computer. The task is for the subjects to keep the dot and target as close together as possible while avoiding escape of either off the screen. The analogy to real stick balancing is made by programming the computer to move the target within a parabolic potential that is centered on the mouse position (see [60], [61] for more details). Computer programs were written in Python using VisionEgg, a high level interface between Python and OpenGL [62].
Mechanical stick balancing measurements involved using a paradigm that incorporates a dc–motor–operated plotter [15]. The pendulum is attached to a slider by means of a pivot: the pendulum can rotate freely in the –plane and the cart is confined to move along the plotter rail in the –direction. A potentiometer placed at the fulcrum of the pendulum detects . A dc servomotor drives the slider on the rail using a timing belt, and the position of the slider is detected by using a second potentiometer. Although it is possible to use separate proportional–integral–derivative (PID) controllers to stabilize and the position of the slider, we controlled only (see [15] for more details). The time delay was introduced by first digitizing the analog signal from the potentiometer and writing this information to a static random access memory (RAM). The contents of the RAM were read out after a time delay, , and converted to analogue to produced the output signal.
Statistical and mathematical analyses
Since stick survival times are Wiebull–distributed we used non–parametric statistics, specifically a Mann-Whitney U test (Wilcoxon rank sum test), to test for statistical significance between stick survival curves. Cross–correlation functions, were converted to white noise by calculating the difference between consecutive time points [63] (‘diff’ function in MATLAB). The vertical displacement angle, , was calculated from the horizontal coordinates of the two ends of the balanced stick, i.e.
where the subscripts denote, respectively, the top and bottom of the stick. The change in fingertip speed, , was calculated as follows [30], [31]: The change in the position of the marker, , in one time step, is
where the notation denotes the position vector measured from a common reference supplied by the Qualisys measurement system. The magnitude of the mean speed, , is
where the notation denotes the norm. Hence
where . All computer simulations were performed using XPPAUT [64].
Acknowledgments
We acknowledge useful discussions with J. Bélair, S. A. Campbell, A. Landsberg, B. Mortimer, F. Moss, A. Straw and A. Ruina and the assistance of S. Lee in preparing Figure 1. J. Lippai implemented the virtual stick balancing task into Python using VisionEgg and S. Tajima constructed the mechanical stick balancing apparatus. We thank S. Goings, K. Lee and L. Liu for assistance in performing the experiments. The vibration platforms were provided by Globus Sport and Health Technologies LLC, Soloflex, Inc. and FreeMotion Fitness. In experiments using the Powerplate vibrating platform were performed at the Clinical Exercise Testing Center (USC; Davis Erceg).
Footnotes
Competing Interests: One of the authors, Toru Ohira, is an employee of a research laboratory at Sony. Toru is a former student of mine and we have worked together on the mathematics of systems with delay and noise for over 19 years (he is presently a consultant on my NSF grant). There is no connection between the work he does for Sony and the work contributed for this project.
Funding: National Science Foundation (NSF-0617072) and Instituto de Investgaciones Cinetifica (IVIC-141). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Morris K. New York: Harcourt/Academic Press; 2001. An Introduction to Feedback Controller Design. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Winter DA, A E Patla EP, Ishac M, Gielo-Perczak K. Stiffness control of balance during quiet standing. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:1211–1221. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.80.3.1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Loram ID, Kelly SM, Lakie M. Human balancing of an inverted pendulum: is sway size controlled by ankle impedance. J Physiol. 2001;532:879–891. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0879e.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Masani K, Vette AH, Kawashima N, Popovic MR. Neuromusculoskeletal torque–generation process has a large destabilizing effect on the control mechanism of quiet standing. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100:1465–1475. doi: 10.1152/jn.00801.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stepan G. New York: Wiley & Sons; 1989. Retarded dynamical systems: Stability and charactertistic functions. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Stepan G. Delay effects in the human sensory system during balancing. Phil Trans R Soc A. 2009;367:1195–1212. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2008.0278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Milton JG, Cabrera JL, Ohira T. Unstable dynamical systems: Delays, noise and control. EPL. 2008;83:48001. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bottaro A, Yasutake Y, Nomura T, Casidio M, Morasso P. Bounded stability of the quite standing posture: An intermittent control model. Human Movement Science. 2008;27:473–495. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2007.11.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Asai Y, Tasaka Y, Nomura K, Nomura T, Casidio M, et al. A model of postural control in quiet standing: Robust compensation of delay–induced instability using intermittent activation of feedback control. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e6169. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Collins JJ, Luca CJD. Random walking during quiet standing. Phys Rev Lett. 1994;73:764–767. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.73.764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Yamada N. Chaotic swaying of the upright posture. Human Movement Science. 1995;14:711–726. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Newell KM, Slobouinov SM, Slobounova ES, Molenaar PC. Stochastic processes in postural center–of–pressure profiles. Exp Brain Res. 1996;113:158–164. doi: 10.1007/BF02454152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Eurich CW, Milton JG. Noise–induced transitions in human postural sway. Phys Rev E. 1996;54:6681–6684. doi: 10.1103/physreve.54.6681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kawasoe Y, Ikura Y, Uchiyama K, Kaise T. Chaos–entropy analysis and acquisition of individuality and proficiency of human operator's skill using a neural controller. Journal System Design Dynamics. 2008;2:1351–1363. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Milton J, Cabrera JL, Ohira T, Tajima S, Tonosaki Y, et al. The time–delayed inverted pendulum: Implications for human balance control. Chaos. 2009;19:026110. doi: 10.1063/1.3141429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Milton J, Townsend JL, King MA, Ohira T. Balancing with positive feedback: the case for discontinuous control. Phil Trans R Soc A. 2009;367:1181–1193. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2008.0257. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cabrera JL, Milton JG. On–off intermittency in a human balancing task. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;89:158702. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.158702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Loram ID, Maganaris CN, Lakie M. Active, non–spring–like muscle movements in human postural sway: how might paradoxical changes in muscle length be produced? J Physiol. 2005;564:281–293. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.073437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Loram ID, Maganaris CN, Lakie M. Human postural sway results from frequent, ballistic bias impulses by soleus and gastrocnemius. J Physiol. 2005;564:295–311. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.076307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Acheson D. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998. From Calculus to Chaos: An introduction to dynamics. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pippard AB. The inverted pendulum. Eur J Phys. 1987;8:203–206. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hoffman ER. Fitts' law with transmission delay. Ergonomics. 1992;35:37–48. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Collins JJ, Luca CJD. Open–lopp and closed–loop control of posture: A random–walk analysis of center–of–pressure trajectories. Exp Brain Res. 1993;95:308–318. doi: 10.1007/BF00229788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Gawthrop P, Loram I, Lakie M. Predictive feedback in human simulated pendulum balancing. Biol Cybern. 2009;101:131–146. doi: 10.1007/s00422-009-0325-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Guckenheimer J. A robust hybrid stabilization strategy for equilibria. IEEE Trans Automatic Control. 1995;40:321–326. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Stépán G, Insperger T. Stability of time–periodic and delayed systems - a route to act–and–wait control. Ann Rev Control. 2006;30:159–168. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Insperger T. Act–and–wait concept for continuous–time control systems with feedback delay. IEEE Trans Control Syst Technol. 2006;14:974–977. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cabrera JL, Milton J. Stick balancing: On–off intermittency and survival times. Nonlinear Studies. 2004;11:305–317. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Cabrera JL, Luciani C, Milton J. Neural control on multiple time scales: Insights from human stick balancing. Condensed Matter Physics. 2006;9:373–383. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cabrera JL, Milton JG. Human stick balancing: Tuning Lévy flights to improve balance control. Chaos. 2004;14:691–698. doi: 10.1063/1.1785453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cluff T, Balasubramaniam R. Motor learning characterized by changing Lévy distributions. PLoS ONE. 2009;4:e5998. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kramers HA. Brownian motion in a field of force and the diffusion model of chemical reactions. Physica. 1940;7:284–304. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Foss J, Moss F, Milton J. Noise, multistability, and delayed recurrent loops. Phys Rev E. 1997;55:4536–4543. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.76.708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Peli E, Garcia-Perez MA. Motion perception during involuntary eye vibration. Exp Brain Res. 2003;149:431–438. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1383-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Landry M, Campbell SA, Morris K, Aguilar CO. Dynamics of an inverted pendulum with delayed feedback control. SIAM J Appl Dyn Sys. 2005;4:333–351. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Proske U, Morgan DL, Gregory JE. Thixotropy in skeletal muscle and in muscle spindles: A review. Prog Neurobiology. 1993;41:705–721. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(93)90032-n. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kossev A, Sigglekow S, Kapels K, Dengler R, Rollnik JD. Crossed effects of muscle vibration on motor–evoked potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001;112:453–456. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00473-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jordan MJ, Norris SR, Smith DJ, Herzog W. Vibration training: an overview of the area, training consequences, and future considerations. J Strength Cond Res. 2005;19:459–466. doi: 10.1519/13293.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Luo J, McNamara B, Moran K. The use of vibration training to enhance muscle strength and power. Sports Med. 2005;35:23–41. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200535010-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sigglekow S, Kossev A, Schubert M, Kappels HH, Wolf W, et al. Modulation of motor evoked potentials by muscle vibration: the role of vibration frequency. Muscle Nerve. 1999;22:1544–1548. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4598(199911)22:11<1544::aid-mus9>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Chin C, Nayfeh AH, Mook DT. The response of a nonlinear system with a non–semisimple one–to–one resonance to a combination resonance. Int J Bifurc Chaos. 1995;5:971–982. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Atay FM. van der Pol's oscillator under delayed feedback. J Sound Vib. 1998;218:333–339. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Lumbantobing H, Haaker TI. On the parametric excitation of some nonlinear aeroelastic oscillators. J Fluids Structure. 2004;19:221–237. [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ibrahim RA, Castravete SC. Flutter suppression of a plate–like wing via parametric excitation. Nonlinear Dynamics. 2006;46:387–426. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Maccari A. Vibration amplitude control for a van der Pol–Duffing oscillator with time delay. J Sound Vib. 2008;317:20–29. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Tang J, Han F, Xiao H, Wu X. Amplitude control of a limit cycle in a coupled van der Pol system. Nonlinear Analysis. 2009;71:2491–2496. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Flügge-Lotz I. New York: McGraw–Hill; 1968. Discontinuous and Optimal Control. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Sieber J, Krauskopf B. Complex balancing motions of an inverted pendulum subject to delayed feedback control. Physica D. 2004;197:332–345. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Sieber J, Krauskopf B. Bifurcation analysis of an inverted pendulum with delayed feedback conteol near a triple–zero eigenvalue singularity. Nonlinearity. 2004:85–103. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rasmussen G. Human body vibration exposure and its measurement. J Acoust Soc Amer. 1983;73:2229. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Griffin MJ. Fundamentals of human responses to vibration. In: Fahy F, Walker J, editors. Fundamentals of Noise and Vibration. New York: Taylor & Francis; 1998. pp. 179–224. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Milton JG, Small SS, Solodkin A. On the road to automatic: Dynamic aspects in the development of expertise. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;21:134–143. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200405000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH. New York: Williams & Wilkins; 2001. Motor Control: Theory and Practical Applications. [Google Scholar]
- 54.Priplata A, Niemi J, Salen M, Harry J, Lipsitz LA, et al. Noise–enhanced human balance control. Phys Rev Lett. 2002;89:238101. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.238101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Priplata AA, Niemi JB, Harry JD, Lipsitz LA, Collins JJ. Vibratory insoles and balance control in elderly people. Lancet. 2003;362:1123–1124. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)14470-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Priplata AA, Patritti BL, Niemi JB, Hughes R, Gravelle DC, et al. Noise–enhanced balance control in patients with diabetes and patients with stroke. Ann Neurol. 2006;59:4–12. doi: 10.1002/ana.20670. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hijmans JM, Geertzen JHB, Zijlstra W, Hof AL, Postema K. Effects of vibrating insoles on standing balance in diabetic neuropathy. Journal of Rehabilitative Research & Development. 2008;45:1441–1450. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Moss F, Milton JG. Balancing the unbalanced. Nature. 2003;425:911–912. doi: 10.1038/425911a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Roll JP, Roll R. From eye to foot: a proprioceptive chain involved in postural control. In: Amblard B, Berthoz A, Clarac F, editors. Posture and gait: Development, adaptation and modulation. New York: Elsevier; 1988. pp. 155–164. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bormann R, Cabrera JL, Milton JG, Eurich CW. Visuomotor tracking on a computer screen: an experimental paradigm to study the dynamics of motor control. Neurocomputing. 2004;58-60:517–523. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cabrera JL, Bormann R, Eurich C, Ohira T, Milton J. State–dependent noise and human balance control. Fluct Noise Lett. 2004;4:L107–L117. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Straw A. An open–source library for realtime visual stimulus generation. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2008;11 (4) doi: 10.3389/neuro.11.004.2008. doi: 10.3389.neuro.11.004.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Jenkins GM, Watts DG. San Francisco: Holden–Day; 1968. Spectral Analysis and its Applications. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ermentrout B. Philadelphia: SIAM; 2002. Simulating, Analyzing, and Animating Dynamical Systems: A guide to XPPAUT for researchers and students. [Google Scholar]