Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Oct 10.
Published in final edited form as: Neuropsychol Rev. 2008 Sep 24;18(3):179–183. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9068-8

Critical issues in cultural neuropsychology: profit from diversity

Jennifer J Manly 1
PMCID: PMC2759971  NIHMSID: NIHMS125825  PMID: 18814033

Addressing cultural diversity in cognitive test performance is an unavoidable future of neuropsychological research and practice

There is widespread agreement that many neuropsychological measures do not have acceptable diagnostic accuracy when used among people who are not Caucasian, well-educated, native English-speaking, and middle to upper class (Ardila, Rodriguez-Menendez, & Rosselli, 2002; Boone, Victor, Wen, Razani, & Pontón, 2007; Brickman, Cabo, & Manly, 2006; Loewenstein, Arguelles, Arguelles, & Linn-Fuentes, 1994; Manly, 2005). A review of scholarly works reveals that not only is this an area of controversy in our field, but also that neuropsychological testing among culturally and linguistically diverse people is an area of critical vulnerability in the theoretical and empirical foundation for neuropsychological practice.

The current status of neuropsychological practice among culturally and linguistically diverse people raises ethical questions. Guidelines for ethical decision-making within the neuropsychological assessment of ethnically and linguistically diverse people has been discussed in detail in several prior publications (Artiola i Fortuny & Mullaney, 1997; Brickman et al., 2006; Harris, Cullum, Bush, & Drexler, 2002; T. M. Wong, 2006), making ethics one of the more comprehensively documented “subtopics” in cultural neuropsychology. The American Psychological Association Ethics Code (“Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct,” 2002) states that it is unethical to use inappropriate measures among culturally different individuals. Yet refusing to assess a large and growing segment of the population, many of whom may potentially benefit from neuropsychological services, would also violate the ethics of our field (Brickman et al., 2006; Harris et al., 2002). It is our ethical obligation to safeguard the welfare of people who come under our care, to take cultural background into account and to do our best to eliminate bias (“Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct,” 2002). The quality and accuracy of current assessment of culturally diverse people may be “the very best we can do” but whether this level of practice is justified entirely depends on the setting of the assessment, what is at stake, the evidence base for the methods used, and the level of cultural competency of the neuropsychologist.

Despite recent proliferation in the number of publications that directly address ethical, theoretical, and practical issues in cultural neuropsychology, the explosive growth in the number of culturally and linguistically diverse people in the United States has exposed our lack of preparation. We are playing a game of catch-up that is, in part responsive to the pressure of a national research agenda of reducing health disparities in the United States; cognitive function has been implicated as a critical predictor and outcome of gaps in health across the life course (see Glymour and Manly, this issue), and therefore accurate assessment is tantamount to addressing these disparities.

The facts on America’s growing cultural diversity are essential knowledge for neuropsychologists in the in the United States. Although 66% of the population is non-Hispanic White in 2008, this “majority” group will contribute far less of the total population growth in coming years. The non-Hispanic White share of the U.S. population has fallen steadily from 74% in 1995 to 66 percent in 2008, and it is projected to decline to 50% in 2050(Minority Population Tops 100 Million, 2007). This increasing diversity will be reflected in the people who we serve, and hopefully, our students and our colleagues. Neuropsychologists must be responsive to the rapidly changing and diverse nature of the population by taking into account cultural, linguistic, and educational background of the people we assess in all aspects of our practice, including not only the measures and normative standards that we use, but also the languages in which we are competent to assess, the educational materials we provide, and the recommendations we make. In preparing neuropsychologists for the linguistic needs of the population, consideration should be given not only to first language and language spoken at home, but also immigration age (if relevant), acculturation, and bilingualism (Llorente, 2007).

The US Census(United States Bureau of the, 2001)projects that after 2016, more non-Hispanic Blacks will be added to the population each year than non-Hispanic Whites, and African Americans are already the largest minority group in 22states. Multiple groups (an admittedly incomplete group are cited here) have provided critical information about neuropsychological testing among African Americans (Campbell et al., 1996; Fillenbaum, Heyman, Huber, Ganguli, & Unverzagt, 2001; Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004; Lichtenberg, Brown, Jackson, & Washington, 2004; Lucas et al., 2005; Manly, 2005; Nabors, Evans, Strickland, Fletcher-Janzen, & Reynolds, 2000; Unverzagt et al., 2007).

The racial/ethnic groups with the highest rates of population increase in coming years will be Latinos/Hispanics and Asian and Pacific Islander populations. Due to the insight and dedication of several researchers, there are multiple resources available to neuropsychologists who will be faced with the tremendous growth of the Asian and Pacific Islander population over the next several decades (Fujii & Wong, 2006; Teng et al., 1994; Teng, Yeo, & Gallagher-Thompson, 1996; T. Wong & Fujii, 2004; T. M. Wong, 2000). The numbers are striking: although the general population of the US will increase 49% from 2000 to 2050, the Asian population will increase 213%, and between 2005 and 2006, the percentage growth of the Native Hawaiian and Other Pacific Islander population was the highest of any race group except for Asians. The US population of those who self-identified as Asian was 14.9 million in 2006, and concentrated in California, New York, and Texas, with Chinese-Americans are the largest Asian group, followed by Filipinos, Asian Indians, Vietnamese, Koreans, and Japanese. Chinese is the second most common non-English language spoken at home.

It is projected that by 2010, Latinos/Hispanics will become the second-largest ethnic group (this is already the case in 19 states) and after2020, the Hispanic population will add more people to the United States every year than will all other race/ethnic groups combined. Concentrated in California, Texas, New Mexico, New York, and Florida, but growing rapidly in other regions, current census information indicates that the majority (64%) of Latino/Hispanics in the United States describe themselves as having Mexican background, 10%are Puerto Rican, Cuban, Salvadoran and Dominican are 3% each, and the remainder describe themselves as having other Central American, South American or other Hispanic or Latino origins. I will not attempt to comprehensively cite all the neuropsychological literature concentrating on Latino American participants across different regions of the US, but it is critical to recognize that this work is relevant to English-speaking, Spanish-speaking, and bilingual people (Ardila, Rosselli, & Puente, 1994; Artiola i Fortuny, Heaton, & Hermosillo, 1998; Artiola i Fortuny & Mullaney, 1997; Boone et al., 2007; Cherner et al., 2007; Echemendia, Harris, Congett, Diaz, & Puente, 1997; Heaton et al., 2001; Judd& Beggs, 2005; Llorente, 2007; Llorente, Ponton, Taussig, & Satz, 1999; Loewenstein, Arguelles, Barker, & Duara, 1993; Lopez & Taussig, 1991; Mungas, Reed, Marshall, & Gonzalez, 2000; Ponton & Ardila, 1999; Ponton et al., 1996; Rey, Feldman, Rivas-Vazquez, Levin, & Benton, 1999; Uzzell, Ponton, & Ardila, 2007)(Rivera-Mindt et al., this issue). The tremendous body of literature in Spanish-speaking countries should also be required reading for clinicians and researchers (Artiola i Fortuny et al., 1998; Barraquer-Bordas, 1999; Fernandez & Marcopulos, 2008; Matute, Rosselli, Ardila, & Morales, 2004; Ostrosky-Solis, Ardila, & Rosselli, 1999; Rosselli & Ardila, 2003).

Research and available clinical tools and guidelines for neuropsychological assessment of the country’s American Indian and Alaska Native population are grievously deficient, although several research teams have been recently making progress in recruiting and providing information about cognitive test performance in these populations (Ferraro & McDonald, 2005; Verney et al., 2008; Whyte et al., 2005). Given that American Indians and Alaska Natives are currently the largest race or ethnic minority group in Alaska, Arizona, Idaho, Montana, New Mexico, North Dakota, Oklahoma, South Dakota and Wyoming, emphasis on empirical information in this cohort, with appreciation of its extraordinary heterogeneity, is critical.

Seeking a higher level scientific language for cultural neuropsychology

Readers of this special issue will recognize that the manuscripts within do not focus on particular ethnic, racial, and linguistic groups, but instead address issues that underlie neuropsychological assessment and research on cognition across culturally diverse people. This is consistent with efforts to explore the role of culture in cognitive test performance and to find basic principles that can be useful in improving the accuracy and utility of neuropsychological measures across all cultural and linguistic groups, not just minorities. In research within the fields of economics (Card & Krueger, 2003; Jencks & Brown, 1975), public health (Cooper & Kaufman, 1998; Kaufman & Cooper, 2001; Kaufman, Cooper, & McGee, 1997), and epidemiology (Krieger, 1999, 2000; D. R. Williams, 1997; D. R. Williams, Neighbors, & Jackson, 2003), there has been substantial progress in conceptualizing the role of race and culture in determining meaningful health and social outcomes, as well as the effect of background factors on measurement of these outcomes. Each of the manuscripts in this special issue reflects an approach to neuropsychological research methodology and assessment that is informed by these approaches outside the field of neuropsychology. Each of these manuscripts reflect an approach that takes advantage of the complex experiences of diverse people, as well as the creative, necessary solutions to methodological challenges that arise in the assessment of culturally diverse people, to improve the quality and utility of neuropsychological measures.

Whether our field can become, on the whole, useful to improve health outcomes among culturally diverse people depends on our understanding of diagnostic validity, causation, and employing correct statistical methods in analyzing data. Pedraza and Mungas discuss measurement issues in cultural neuropsychology, frame issues of diagnostic validity in this area, and review advanced psychometric methodologies that can improve the reliability and validity of neuropsychological measures when used in diverse people. Glymour, Weuve, and Chen discuss causation and construct validity from the perspective of researchers attempting to understand the role of race and culture on cognitive test performance and also explore the link between culture and cognitive health outcomes.

Byrd and colleagues review the status of neuropsychological assessment of culturally diverse children and the implications of this work on our understanding of the role of culture on cognitive development.

Glymour and Manly explore the way in which epidemiologic approaches to understanding the health effects of racial, economic, cultural, and educational experiences over the life course can help advance our understanding of affect cognitive aging and performance on neuropsychological tests among ethnic minorities.

Rivera-Mindt, Gollan and colleagues take on one of the most exciting topics in neuropsychology, the study of cognition and cognitive test performance among bilinguals. They review the theoretical underpinnings of this area of study and also review the practical approaches to assessment that have been offered in the literature.

Given the history of attempts to assess intellectual functioning across cultures (Baker, 1998; Gould, 1996; Hilliard, 1979; Neisser et al., 1996; R. L. Williams, 1974), it is understandable that neuropsychologists would want to side-step this controversy entirely. However, if neuropsychologists can engage with these issues in research and clinical settings, we can embrace the opportunity to benefit from the lessons of the past, and further establish our field as an integral and methodologically sound component of health care. The articles in this special issue do not comprehensively cover the rich topic of cultural neuropsychology, but I am confident that they will challenge conventions and stimulate further exploration and discussion.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by National Institute on Aging grants AG16206, AG028786, and the Alzheimer’s Association.

References

  1. Ardila A, Rodriguez-Menendez G, Rosselli M. Current issues in neuropsychological assessment with Hispanics/Latinos. In: Ferraro FR, editor. Minority and cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002. pp. 161–179. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ardila A, Rosselli M, Puente AE. Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Spanish Speaker. New York: Plenum; 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Artiola i Fortuny L, Heaton RK, Hermosillo D. Neuropsychological comparisons of Spanish-speaking participants from the U.S.-Mexico border region versus Spain. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1998;4:363–379. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Artiola i Fortuny L, Mullaney H. Neuropsychology with Spanish speakers: Language use and proficiency issues for test development. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1997;19:615–622. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baker L. From Savage to Negro: Anthropology and the Construction of Race, 1896–1954. Berkeley: University of California Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  6. Barraquer-Bordas L. Neuropsychological society of Catalana: past, present and future. Rev Neurol (Paris) 1999;29:1268–1270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boone KB, Victor TL, Wen J, Razani J, Pónton M. The association between neuropsychological scores and ethnicity, language, and acculturation variables in a large patient population. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22(3):355–365. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brickman AM, Cabo R, Manly JJ. Ethical Issues in Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology. Applied Neuropsychology. 2006;13(2):91–100. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1302_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Campbell A, Rorie K, Dennis G, Wood D, Combs S, Hearn L, et al. Handbook of Tests and Measurement for Black Populations. Vol. 2. Berkeley: Cobb and Henry; 1996. Neuropsychological assessment of African Americans: Conceptual and methodological considerations; pp. 75–84. [Google Scholar]
  10. Card D, Krueger B. School resources and student outcomes: An overview of the literature and new evidence from North and South Carolina. The Journal of Economic Perspectives. 2003;10:31–50. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cherner M, Suarez P, Lazzaretto D, Fortuny LAi, Mindt MR, Dawes S, et al. Demographically corrected norms for the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-revised and Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-revised in monolingual Spanish speakers from the U.S.-Mexico border region. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007;22(3):343–353. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cooper R, Kaufman JS. Race and hypertension: science and nescience. Hypertension. 1998;32:813–816. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.32.5.813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Echemendia RJ, Harris JG, Congett SM, Diaz ML, Puente AE. Neuropsychological training and practices with Hispanics: a national survey. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1997;11:229–243. [Google Scholar]
  14. Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. American Psychologist. 2002;57(12):1060 –1073. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernandez A, Marcopulos B. A comparison of normative data for the Trail Making Test from several countries: Equivalence of norms and considerations for interpretation. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2008;49(3):239–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2008.00637.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ferraro FR, McDonald L. More Culturally Sensitive Neuropsychological Tests (and Normative Data) Needed. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2005;53:53. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000165754.85135.1d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fillenbaum GG, Heyman A, Huber MS, Ganguli M, Unverzagt FW. Performance of elderly African American and White community residents on the CERAD Neuropsychological Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2001;7:502–509. doi: 10.1017/s1355617701744062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fujii D, Wong T. Neuropsychological Assessment with Asian-American Immigrants: Recommendations for Meeting Daubert Standards. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology. 2006;4:3 –31. [Google Scholar]
  19. Gould SJ. The Mismeasure of Man. New York: W.W. Norton & Company; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  20. Harris JG, Cullum CM, Bush S, Drexler M. Ethical issues in clinical neuropsychology. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002. Ethical decision making with individuals of diverse ethnic, vultural, and linguistic backgrounds; pp. 223–241. [Google Scholar]
  21. Heaton RK, Miller SW, Taylor MJ, Grant I. Revised Comprehensive Norms for an Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically Adjusted Neuropsychological Norms for African American and Caucasian Adults. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  22. Heaton RK, Taylor M, Manly J, Tulsky D, Chelune GJ, Ivnik I, et al. Clinical Interpretations of the WAIS-II and WMS-III. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2001. Demographic effects and demographically corrected norms with the WAIS-III and WMS-III; pp. 181–210. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hilliard AG. Standardization and Cultural Bias Impediments to the Scientific Study and Validation of “Intelligence”. Journal of Research and Development in Education. 1979;12(2):47–58. [Google Scholar]
  24. Jencks C, Brown M. The effects of desegregation on student achievement: Some new evidence from the equality of educational opportunity survey. Sociology of Education. 1975;48:126–140. [Google Scholar]
  25. Judd T, Beggs B. Cross-cultural forensic neuropsychological assessment. In: Barrett K, George WH, editors. Race, Culture, Psychology and Law. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Press; 2005. pp. 141–162. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kaufman JS, Cooper RS. Considerations of use of racial/ethnic classification in etiologic research. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;154:291–298. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Kaufman JS, Cooper RS, McGee DL. Socioeconomic status and health in blacks and whites: the problem of residual confounding and the resilience of race. Epidemiology. 1997;8:621–628. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Krieger N. Embodying inequality: a review of concepts, measures, and methods for studying health consequences of discrimination. International Journal of Health Services. 1999;29:295–352. doi: 10.2190/M11W-VWXE-KQM9-G97Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Krieger N. Refiguring “race”: epidemiology, racialized biology, and biological expressions of race relations. International Journal of Health Services. 2000;30:211–216. doi: 10.2190/672J-1PPF-K6QT-9N7U. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lichtenberg PA, Brown DR, Jackson JS, Washington O. Normative Health Research Experiences Among African American Elders. Journal of Aging and Health. 2004;16:78S–792. doi: 10.1177/0898264304268150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Llorente AM. Principles of Neuropsychological Assessment with Hispanics: Theoretical Foundations and Clinical Practice. New York: Springer; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  32. Llorente AM, Ponton M, Taussig IM, Satz P. Patterns of American immigration and their influence on the acquisition of neuropsychological norms for Hispanics. Archives of Neuropsychology. 1999;14:603–614. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Loewenstein DA, Arguelles T, Arguelles S, Linn-Fuentes P. Potential cultural bias in the neuropsychological assessment of the older adult. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:623–629. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Loewenstein DA, Arguelles T, Barker WW, Duara R. A comparative analysis of neuropsychological test performance of Spanish-speaking and English-speaking patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 1993;48:142–149. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.3.p142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lopez SR, Taussig IM. Cognitive-intellectual functioning of Spanish-speaking impaired and nonimpaired elderly: Implications for culturally sensitive assessment. Psychological Assessment. 1991;3:448–454. [Google Scholar]
  36. Lucas JA, Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Ferman TJ, Willis FB, Petersen RC, et al. Mayo’s Older African American Normative Studies: Normative data for commonly used clinical neuropsychological measures. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2005;19:162–183. doi: 10.1080/13854040590945265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Manly JJ. Advantages and disadvantages of seperate norms for African Americans. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2005;19:270–275. doi: 10.1080/13854040590945346. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Matute E, Rosselli M, Ardila A, Morales G. Verbal and Nonverbal Fluency in Spanish-Speaking Children. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2004;26(2):647–660. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2602_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Minority Population Tops 100 Million 2007. 2007 August 9; Retrieved August 1, 2008, 2008, from http://www.census.gov/Press-Release/www/releases/archives/population/010048.html.
  40. Mungas D, Reed BR, Marshall SC, Gonzalez HM. Development of psychometrically matched English and Spanish language neuropsychological tests for older persons. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:209–223. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nabors NA, Evans JD, Strickland TL, Fletcher-Janzen E, Reynolds C. Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology. New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2000. Neuropsychological Assessment and Intervention with African Americans; pp. 31–42. [Google Scholar]
  42. Neisser U, Boodoo G, Bouchard TJJ, Boykin AW, Brody N, Ceci SJ, et al. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist. 1996;51:77–101. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ostrosky-Solis F, Ardila A, Rosselli M. NEUROPSI: A brief neuropsychological test battery in Spanish with norms by age and educational level. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1999;5:413–433. doi: 10.1017/s1355617799555045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Ponton MO, Ardila A. The future of neuropsychology with Hispanic populations in the United States. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1999;14:565–580. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ponton MO, Satz P, Herrera L, Ortiz F, Urrutia CP, Young R, et al. Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1996;2:96–104. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700000941. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rey GJ, Feldman E, Rivas-Vazquez R, Levin BE, Benton A. Neuropsychological Test Development and Normative Data on Hispanics. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1999;14(7):593–601. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosselli M, Ardila A. The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain and Cognition. 2003;52:326–333. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00170-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Teng EL, Hasegawa K, Homma A, Imai Y, Larson E, Graves A, et al. The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI): a practical test for cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia. International Psychogeriatrics. 1994;6:45–58. doi: 10.1017/s1041610294001602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Teng EL, Yeo G, Gallagher-Thompson D. Ethnicity and the Dementias. Washington DC: Taylor and Francis; 1996. Cross-cultural testing and the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument; pp. 77–85. [Google Scholar]
  50. United States Bureau of the, C. Census 2000 Summary File 1, United States. United States Census Bureau 2001 [Google Scholar]
  51. Unverzagt FW, Sujuan G, Lane KA, Callahan C, Ogunniyi A, Baiyewu O, et al. Mild Cognitive Dysfunction: An Epidemiological Perspective With an Emphasis on African Americans. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2007;20(4):215–226. doi: 10.1177/0891988707308804. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Uzzell B, Ponton M, Ardila A, editors. International Handbook of Cross-Cultural Neuropsychology. New York: Psychology Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  53. Verney SP, Jervis LL, Fickenscher A, Roubideaux Y, Bogart A, Goldberg J. Symptoms of depression and cognitive functioning in older American Indians. Aging& Mental Health. 2008;12(1):108 –115. doi: 10.1080/13607860701529957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Whyte SR, Cullum CM, Hynan L, Lacritz L, Rosenberg R, Weiner M. Performance of Elderly Native Americans and Caucasians on the CERAD Neuropsychological Battery. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 2005;19:74 –78. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000165508.67993.a3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Williams DR. Race and health: basic questions, emerging directions. Annals of Epidemiology. 1997;7:322–333. doi: 10.1016/s1047-2797(97)00051-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Williams DR, Neighbors H, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93:200–208. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Williams RL. Scientific racism and IQ: The silent mugging of the black community. Psychology Today. 1974;7(12):32–41. [Google Scholar]
  58. Wong T, Fujii D. Neuropsychological Assessment of Asian Americans: Demographic Factors, Cultural Diversity, and Practical Guidelines. Applied Neuropsychology. 2004;11(1):23 –36. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1101_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Wong TM. Neuropsychological assessment and intervention with Asian Americans. In: Fletcher-Janzen E, Strickland T, Reynolds C, editors. Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum; 2000. pp. 43–53. [Google Scholar]
  60. Wong TM. Ethical Controversies in Neuropsychological Test Selection, Administration, and Interpretation. Applied Neuropsychology. 2006;13(2):68 –76. doi: 10.1207/s15324826an1302_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES