Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Oct 13.
Published in final edited form as: J Neurother. 2008 Dec 1;12(4):185–204. doi: 10.1080/10874200802502144

Event-Related Potential Study of Executive Dysfunctions in a Speeded Reaction Task in Cocaine Addiction

Estate Sokhadze 1, Christopher Stewart 1, Michael Hollifield 1, Allan Tasman 1
PMCID: PMC2760844  NIHMSID: NIHMS73400  PMID: 19830263

Abstract

This study used a flanker task with NoGo elements to investigate frontal executive function deficits in 19 cocaine abusers. The executive functions of interest in this study were cortical inhibition or ability to withhold motor response, the ability to select an appropriate response among several competing ones, the ability to inhibit inappropriate responses, and the ability to detect error and exercise corrective control. These processes were evaluated with specific frontal and parietal event-related potentials (ERP) registered during performance on this speeded reaction time task with conflicting motor response demands. Specifically we used behavioral response measures, stimulus-locked anterior (frontal N200, N450) ERP markers of conflict detection, response inhibition (NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3), and response-locked error-related negativity (ERN) that represent different time points of signal classification, motor response conflict detection, response inhibition, and error monitoring processes. The results revealed that the higher-level executive motor control attributed to the prefrontal cortex is hypoactive in cocaine abusers, and therefore is incapable to effectively resolve response conflicts arising between the competing motor response alternatives. It was also demonstrated that the mesial frontal structures, such as the anterior cingulate cortex, implicated in motor response conflict detection and error monitoring functions were also compromised in addicts. It is reasonable to propose that a ‘hypofunctional’ prefrontal and midfrontal processing results in a diminished ability to effectively override strong habitual automated response tendencies controlled by the lower-level neural mechanisms triggered by the external stimuli. The results propose a neurobiological basis for the understanding why cocaine abusers are facing difficulties in controlling their drug-seeking and drug-taking behaviors, and why their drug-related habitual behavior is so vulnerable to be triggered by external (e.g., drug-related items and environment) cues.

Keywords: ERP, cocaine abuse, executive functions, prefrontal cortex

Introduction

Patients with substance use disorder (SUD) show cognitive deficiencies affecting normal behavioral functioning. Cognitive functional diagnostic tools are needed to reveal cortical origin of the observed executive impairments and the underlying abnormalities in neural mechanisms in this disorder presented with frontal “top-down” control deficiency symptoms. In general executive processes are shown to be more negatively affected in cocaine addiction (Goldstein et al., 2001). Current cocaine addiction theories consider this form of SUD as a complex neural and behavioral process where inability to exercise cognitive control to override strong drug seeking and drug taking behaviors is considered to be a very important factor (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Hester & Garavan, 2004). These theories also emphasize the role of executive dysfunction in cocaine addiction, and the negative psychopharmacological effects of cocaine abuse on brain structures that are involved in the cognitive control of behavior (Lyvers, 2000)

Functional abnormalities of cocaine-dependent patients vs. controls have been observed in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), insula, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), basal ganglia, and limbic-related regions (Volkow et al., 2003, 2004). Another major target for this drug of abuse is the striatum, a subcortical brain region important for the habit formation and storing of fixed behavioral patterns (Koob & Le Moal, 2001). The neural structural deficits within prefrontal and limbic structures contribute to the deficits in behavioral inhibition which are the characteristics of chronic cocaine users. Chronic abuse of cocaine is associated with structural and functional abnormalities in the brain, particularly in prefrontal and midline mesial frontal structures known to be involved in executive control, and chronic cocaine users consistently display neuropsychological impairments on tests of executive function (Di Sclafani et al., 2002; Franklin et al., 2002; Matochik et al., 2003; Miller and Cohen, 2001).

Neurocognitive tests aimed at testing specific domains of cognitive functioning (attention, working memory, set shift, etc.) are usually assessed using neuropsychological test batteries with reaction time and accuracy being the main outcomes. Electrophysiological evaluations that use along with behavioral data also electroencephalographic (EEG) and/or even-related potential (ERP) measures allow more accurate and in-depth analysis of specific cognitive functions, in particular executive functions, known to be compromised in SUD. Among cognitive processes subsumed under the ‘executive functions’ term, cortical inhibition deserves special attention in cocaine addiction.

Cortical inhibitory state in ERP can be assessed by use of a continuous performance test in which a motor response is required to one signal (“Go”) but must be suppressed to others (“No-Go”). In normal individuals the anterior P300 waveform to the “Go” is larger than to the “No-Go” condition for this task. In contrast, cocaine addicts have lower P300 overall (Biggins et al., 1997; Fein, Biggins, & McKay, 1996; Kouiri, Lukas, & Mendelson, 1996; Polich et al., 1994) and they may do not show any increase in the anterior P300 to the “Go” condition. The frontal activation is larger in the “No-Go” than in the “Go” condition and is presumed to reflect the inhibition that is required for response suppression. In cocaine addicts the frontal activation during “No-Go” task is lower than in normal individuals, an indication that frontal lobe control of response inhibition is reduced (Hester & Garavan, 2004; Strik et al., 1998). Two major ERP components have been identified as the markers for response inhibition: first, the “No-Go-N2”, a negative deflection with a fronto-central maximum around 200–300 ms, and second, referred to as “No-Go-P3”, an augmented positive-going peak usually peaking between 300 and 600 ms (Falkenstein, Hoormann, & Hohnsbein, 1999). The ERP markers of response inhibition (hereafter referred to as NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3) represent different time points of response inhibition process and can be used as sensitive cortical inhibition indices in addiction research.

One important executive function is the ability to select a contextually appropriate response among several competing ones, whereas another important function of executive control is the ability to inhibit contextually inappropriate responses. These processes can be evaluated with specific frontal and parietal ERP waves registered during processing of conflicting response tendencies manipulated by experimental task. Other executive deficits in SUD are observed during performance on speeded reaction time tasks and are manifested in deficiencies related to response error monitoring and motor response conflict detection. These deficits are evaluated by assessment of response-locked fronto-centrally distributed ERP such as error-related negativity (ERN). Other ERP parameters related to response conflict detection and processing can be extracted from such fronto-central ERP components as N200 (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004) and N450 (West, Bowry, & McConville, 2004; West, 2003) components. Neuroimaging studies (Goldstein & Volkow, 2002; Hester & Garavan, 2004) showed that the higher-level executive motor control attributed to the PFC is hypoactive in SUD, and therefore is incapable to effectively resolve response conflicts arising between the conflicting motor response alternatives. It is reasonable to propose that a ‘hypofunctional’ PFC processing results in a diminished ability to effectively override strong habitual automated response tendencies controlled by the lower-level neural mechanisms (e.g., premotor areas, basal ganglia).

Objectives and aims of the study

The objective of this exploratory study was to investigate the timing and character of the interaction between the cortical areas-of-interest, specifically the PFC, ACC, and premotor areas and posterior attention systems in the cognitive tests with demanding motor response task. This study used a speeded forced choice task (Eriksens' flanker test) with response inhibition (NoGo) trials to infer dysfunctions of executive control analyzing overt behavioral responses (e.g., reaction time and errors) and dense-array event-related potentials (ERP). The study used stimulus-locked ERPs to distinguish the neural processes related to stimulus identification and categorization, response selection, inhibition of inappropriate conflicting responses, and execution of correct responses. Specific ERP, such as the response-locked error-related negativity (ERN) was used to index output response monitoring. The same test was conducted in two groups of subjects: (1) a group of patients with cocaine abuse/dependence (N=19), and (2) age and gender matched group of healthy controls (N=15). Each subject enrolled in the study participated in a flanker experiment described below in the method section.

One of the aims was to examine behavioral and ERP measures of the processes related to a direct inhibition of responses in a response inhibition (“NoGo” condition) trials of the task in patients with cocaine addiction and controls. To determine which neural processes are dysfunctional in this form of SUD, we compared behavioral and ERPs measures between patients with cocaine addiction and healthy controls. We predicted that the patient groups (compared to controls) will exhibit less inhibitory efficiency than controls in the ‘NoGo’ trials of this task. Patient group differences in inhibition efficiency were predicted to be reflected in a lower amplitude of the fronto-central ERP indices of the inhibition (so called anterior ‘NoGo-N2’ and ‘NoGo-P3’), since attenuated ERP indices of inhibition will be reflecting deficient PFC inhibitory activity.

Another aim was to determine the differences in ERP indices of visual signal processing and action monitoring in a forced choice Eriksens' flanker task (as the most commonly used speeded task with a higher rate of behavioral errors) in patients with cocaine addiction and healthy controls. In this experiment, we specifically used the fronto-central N200 and N450 components of stimulus-locked ERPs and the response-locked error-related negativity (ERN) as measures of response conflict and action monitoring in this speeded reaction time (RT) task with interferences. Therefore, we used the fronto-central N200, N450, and ERN as indices of response conflict and monitoring system functioning. From the numerous studies (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004; West, 2003; West et al., 2004) it is known that dipole sources of these frontal ERPs (N200, N450, ERN) are localized to different subdivisions of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC).

The study was based on a model of movement preparation, execution, and response conflict monitoring processes in which prefrontal, medial frontal, parietal, and primary motor areas are differentially impaired in patients with cocaine abuse. In this speeded forced-choice experiment with motor response execution and inhibition demands we used dense-array ERP and behavioral response measures to test the hypothesis that higher-order motor control and cognitive functions are impaired in cocaine abusers. The goal of this chronopsychophysiological methodological approach was to determine if executive motor control impairments in SUD are the result of an abnormal interaction between the prefrontal (PFC), mesial frontal (ACC), premotor and motor cortices accompanied by a diminished executive cortical control over movements in a speeded motor task with flanker distracters.

The study used the temporal resolution of event-related brain potential recording techniques to detect sequential activation of functionally connected cortical areas involved in movement preparation and inhibition and to identify functional abnormalities leading to executive movement impairment and cognitive deficits in cocaine addiction.

Methodological background

Event-related potential measures of signal processing, response selection, and error monitoring

P300

The most widely studied cognitive ERP component is P300 (P3), a wave of positive polarity that peaks within 300 - 500 ms (Polich & Kok, 1995; Pritchard, 1981). P300 amplitude is inversely related to stimulus probability and directly related to the information-processing engaged by the stimulus (Johnson, 1986; Pritchard, 1981). P300 varies in latency as well as amplitude. P300 latency has proven to be a useful adjunct to reaction time (RT) in studies of mental chronometry, as it tends to be more reflective of manipulations of stimulus-evaluation time than the time consumed by subsequent response-related processes (Pritchard et al., 1999; Verleger, 1997). Thus, for example, if a certain experimental manipulation shortens RT but does not affect P300 latency, then it can be concluded that the manipulation acts to shorten RT by primarily affecting response-related processes rather than processes related to stimulus evaluation.

N200

The negative ERP component (N200), located over centro-parietal and posterior scalp locations occurs between about 190 and 320 ms post-stimulus (Näätännen et al., 1978, 1993). This ERP component is associated with stimulus categorization, perceptual closure, and attention focusing, therefore signaling that a completed perceptual representation has been formed (Wijers et al., 1996; Potts et al., 2004). The anterior fronto-central N200 component is thought to be related to conflict monitoring (Donkers & van Boxtel, 2004) in tasks with interferences (Stroop test). In a Go-NoGo task this component is often interpreted as reflecting inhibitory executive functions (Heil et al., 2000; Kopp et al., 1996). Van Veen & Carter (2002) used the Eriksen flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974), in which participants have to respond to a centrally presented target while trying to ignore simultaneously presented flanker stimuli. Such a task activates the ACC that has been shown to respond to conflict between simultaneously active, incompatible response tendencies. The fronto-central N200 was used in this study as an index of the ACC activity.

Error-related negativity (ERN) and ERP indices of response selection and conflict monitoring

In speeded RT tasks (e.g., Stroop or flanker tests) immediately after errors (50-150 ms), a negative component appears over the fronto-central areas, the error-related negativity (ERN) (Herrmann et al., 2004). The ERN has been attributed to cognitive operations of detecting errors or response conflict (Gehring & Knight, 2000). Dipole modeling has localized ERN sources to the ACC (Gehring & Knight, 2000; Herrmann et al., 2004; Van Veen & Carter, 2002; West, 2003). In psychiatric studies, a decreased ERN is typically related to increased severity of psychomotor poverty symptoms (Bates et al., 2004). On the contrary, the ERN is found enhanced in anxiety, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), obsessive-compulsive disorder, and phobia (Hajcak et al., 2003; Rauch et al., 1996).

The ERN is generally accepted as a neural index of response-monitoring processes. The ERP studies of the neural correlates of conflict processing using Stroop and similar interference tasks have revealed the frontal N450 (400-500 ms) negative wave that was associated with conflict detection and thought to originate from the activity in the ACC (Markela-Lerenc et al., 2004; West, 2003, West et al., 2004). The N450 is modulated by the degree of conflict, being higher when conflict is high (Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; Van Veen & Carter, 2002; West et al., 2004). The generators of the N450 and the ERN reflect activity of the ACC and serve as neural correlates of conflict processing and response monitoring processes.

Experimental test and procedures to test cortical inhibition

Behavioral measures in response inhibition tasks

In laboratory settings, response inhibition is traditionally tested using stop-signal and Go-NoGo task paradigms (Verbruggen et al., 2004). The participant is instructed to perform a task of pressing a button in response to a specific stimulus (“Go”) and to inhibit a button pressing in response to another (“NoGo”), or to inhibit response when a stop-signal sign flashes. The NoGo task is functionally equivalent to a stop-signal task in which the respond and stop-signals are presented simultaneously (Band et al., 2003; Logan, 1994). For “Go-NoGo” tasks, the behavioral index of inhibitory control is the number of errors on “NoGo” trials. Deficiency of inhibitory control is inferred on the basis of the pattern of errors. Omission errors are interpreted as reflecting deficits in ability to process target and initiate motor act, either due to deficient attention or failed motor preparedness. Commission errors are interpreted as reflecting direct response inhibition deficits in “NoGo” trials.

ERP measures of response inhibition

ERPs reflect sensory, cognitive, and motor processes and are useful to find correlates of inhibition mechanisms in the classical “Go-NoGo” tasks. As it was mentioned above, there are several ERP components that are considered as correlates of the motor inhibitory processes in NoGo trials. The studies using visual NoGo-tasks reported two effects in NoGo-vs. Go-ERPs: a negative wave at midline frontal sites with a latency of 150–400 ms, the ‘NoGo-N2’, and a positive wave with maximum at the midline fronto-central area with a latency of 300–500 ms, the ‘NoGo-P3’ (Falkenstein et al., 1999). The NoGo-N2 is thought to reflect a frontal inhibition mechanism which is active on “NoGo” trials (Bekker et al., 2004; Falkenstein et al., 2002; Roberts et al., 1994; Strik et al., 1998). The generators of the visual NoGo-N2 have been localized to inferior-lateral prefrontal cortex (Falkenstein et al., 1999, 2002). According to Falkenstein et al (2002), NoGo-P3 could reflect a closure of a preceding inhibition process, whereas inhibition itself is reflected in the NoGo-N2. On the assumption that NoGo trials and zero delay stop-signal are functionally equivalent, successful inhibition in stop-signal trials should exhibit the frontal N200 and P300 similar to the NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 potentials reflecting inhibition localized to the PFC (Kok et al., 2004). Though most investigators agree that the NoGo-N2 and –P3 components are related to frontal inhibition seen during “NoGo” paradigms (reviewed in [Falkenstein et al., 1999, 2002], yet others have shown that the anterior P300 is not only due to response inhibition [Salisbury et al., 2004]). In a “NoGo” task, frontal NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 are generally considered to be ERP correlates of the frontal inhibitory process.

Experimental task to test response conflict and action monitoring: Flanker Task

The flanker test is a type of selective attention task that requires perceptual or cognitive suppression of competing information (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1979; Coles et al., 1985). This test is similar to Stroop and Go-NoGo tests of executive functions, except information is spatially distinct (Nigg, 2005). For example, the subject views a target area in the center of a computer screen, with an instruction to press the corresponding key depending on whether the “R” or “L” appears in the center. Immediately adjacent to the center letter are two “flanking” distracter letters that are to be ignored. The flankers can be incompatible (LLRLL) or neutral (e.g., RRRRR). It takes longer to respond to incongruent than to congruent trials because in the first instance the flanker is a possible response that must be suppressed. Even though subjects are instructed to ignore the distracters, the presence of incongruent flankers in the stimulus array is associated with longer RTs and higher error rates. This speeded forced choice RT task requires motor responses to congruent and incongruent stimuli, is known to evoke response conflict (e.g. Coles et al., 1985; Kopp et al., 1996), and is often used to assess response error monitoring function. Decrement in performance in the flanker task is thought to result from activation of the conflicting response by the incongruent flankers (Coles et al., 1985).

Model of the prefrontal-cingulate interaction in motor response control

The activation of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and the ACC occurs in diverse demanding cognitive tasks, including tasks requiring motor response inhibition (Gehring & Knight, 2000; Gehring et al., 2000). Several studies outlined that the ACC deals with relatively simple conflict monitoring (Botvinick et al., 1999; Bunge et al., 2001; Bush et al., 2002; Carter et al., 1998; Devinsky et al., 1995); whereas the PFC deals with more complex aspects including selection of actions (Corr, 2002; Faw, 2003). The PFC is not critical for performing simple, automatic movements. Such motor acts are controlled by the lower level premotor areas. By contrast, the PFC is important when ‘top-down’ processing is required, and when mapping between sensory inputs, action plans and memory must be integrated and coordinated (Miller & Cohen, 2001). Whereas the automatic aspects of motor behavior and learned responses may have been relegated to lower structures, the more schematic representations of action, as well as the general rules of motor tasks, remain represented in prefrontal networks (Damasio, 1996; Elliott, 2003; Fuster, 2001). It is suggested that the ACC is involved in detecting conflict and that the PFC is involved in resolving it (Aron et al., 2004; Gehring & Knight, 2000). There is a model in which the PFC detects the need for executive control and signals the ACC to perform the control function (Markela-Lerenc et al., 2004; Turken & Swick, 1999). Another model postulates the reverse arrangement, in which the ACC detects the problems and communicates with the PFC structures that implement the executive control (Gehring et al., 2000). A more complex model (Gehring & Knight, 2000), points out that the ACC monitors for errors and conflicts, but depends on the PFC for processing necessary to implement error correction or conflict resolution. The PFC maintains action representation necessary for decision-making. Without such decision the ACC is unable to distinguish correct from incorrect responses. The model suggests that a system other than the ACC or the PFC (e.g., basal ganglia) implements corrective action, but it operates under the strong modulatory frontal influences.

The neuroanatomical basis of executive function appears to be the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Coordinated activation of the PFC and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) serve diverse executive functions, including those directly involved in motor control through the access to the premotor areas. This study was guided by the following hypothetical assumptions: 1) the PFC is essential in maintaining task-relevant motor action representation in cognitive tasks; 2) the ACC is essential to detect response-related conflict and errors during cognitive tasks, and once a conflict occurrence is detected, signals the PFC for resolution; 3) the left dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC) is critical for the resolution of conflict by selecting an appropriate response according to task demands, while the right inferior PFC is involved in inhibition of the inappropriate responses; 4) the PFC controls motor action planning in the fronto-striatal loops, and modulates movement preparation in the supplementary motor areas (SMA), premotor area (MI), and basal ganglia; 5) the premotor cortical structures and basal ganglia control and program motor function in the absence of response conflict, when the prefrontal executive control is not recruited.

Our hypothesis posits that the hypofunctional prefrontal processes in cocaine addiction can result in the inability to effectively override habitual response and implement a corrective modulation of the premotor structures. This model assumes that cocaine addicted subjects will perform worse when engagement of PFC is required for effective corrective action after errors. On the other hand, globally lower functionality of mesial frontal structures implicated in response conflict detection and error signaling (i.e., ACC and pre-SMA) in cocaine addition will result in lower ability to detect conflicting response tendencies and committed response errors, therefore providing less efficient feedback needed for executive prefrontal involvement for response conflict resolution and error corrections.

Testing the model by comparing behavior and ERP indices of inhibition in SUD patients and controls

To test these hypotheses we examined behavioral output and spatio-temporal pattern of brain activation during speeded choice reaction time task with response inhibition trials using dense-array scalp EEG recordings and analysis of ERP components. To determine which neural processes are dysfunctional in SUD, we compared behavioral performance and ERP measures between cocaine addicted patients and controls. We assumed that the effects related to inhibition (“NoGo-N2”, “NoGo-P3”) have generators in the PFC, whereas effects related to error and correct response monitoring (ERN) and motor response conflict (frontal N200 and N450) have generators in the ACC on the base of numerous studies that used dipole source localization techniques for these ERP measures (Falkenstein et al., 1999, 2002; Kok et al., 2004; Nieuwenhuis et al., 2003; Van Veen & Carter, 2002; West et al., 2004).

The “Go-NoGo” part of the experiment tested the prefrontal deficits in direct inhibition of motor response in the trials with ‘NoGo’ task. We predicted that patients compared with controls will commit disproportionately more commission errors in these ‘NoGo’ trials. We predicted that the patients will show less inhibitory efficiency than controls and will exhibit behavioral and ERP deficits in withholding responses. Patient differences in inhibition efficiency were expected to be reflected in a lower amplitude of the fronto-central NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3. We also used the ERN as response monitoring index to test the hypothesis that behavioral error monitoring system is under-responsive to occurring errors in SUD patients during inhibition of motor responses, and thus contributes to the executive functioning deficits observed in cocaine addiction. We predicted that if the executive action monitoring is hypofunctional in cocaine addiction, then the failure to inhibit response after “NoGo” target stimulus will not lead to an enhanced ERN.

The second (flanker experiment with congruent and incongruent flankers) integral part of the experiment tested the hypothesis that in patients with cocaine addiction, as compared to healthy controls, the frontal conflict monitoring system has lower capacity to detect and respond to response interferences, while action monitoring is under-responsive to errors. The experiment utilized a flanker task, in which interfering flanking letters are used to induce errors to a critical central letter indicating response side (left vs. right key press). Patients with cocaine abuse were predicted to have an attenuated amplitude and a prolonged latency of the anterior N200 and N450 ERP components in an incongruent flanker condition indicating a low reactivity to potential response conflict, and a smaller ERN on error trials compared to controls. We predicted globally slower RTs but less pronounced behavioral and ERP interference effects in congruent trials in patients with cocaine addiction compared to controls since less response conflict and interference occur in such congruent trials and PCF involvement is not crucial.

Methods

Subjects: Recruitment and screening procedures

Male and female patients of any race over the age of 18 meeting inclusion and not exclusion criteria were eligible for this study. The protocol of the study and recruitment advertisements were approved by the local IRB. The advertisements were posted at the local hospital and drug and alcohol rehabilitation center. Inclusion criteria were meeting DSM-IVTR criteria for cocaine abuse/dependence (APA, 2000). In addition, eligible participants must be (1) judged to be in generally good physical health except for possible acute or chronic drug-related problems; (2) willing and be able to participate in cognitive lab tests. Exclusion criteria were (a) current diagnosis of other Axis I psychiatric disorder, other than cocaine dependence and PTSD (since most cocaine abusers in our population have high PTSD scores); (b) current psychiatric symptoms requiring medication; (c) severe medical or psychiatric impairments that preclude from the cooperation with the study protocol; (d) substance withdrawal symptoms requiring immediate medical attention; (e) inability to read, write, or speak English; (f) neurological disorders that may affect EEG recording (e.g., epilepsy). Major patient recruitment sites were Ambulatory Clinic of University of Louisville Hospital, Jefferson County Drug and Alcohol Treatment Center (JADAC, located in one block from the lab). The patients were referred to Drs. Stewart and Hollifield for psychiatric assessment and determination of eligibility to enroll in this study. Each subject with SUD was evaluated and screened by Dr. Stewart (board certified addiction specialist) to confirm cocaine abuse or cocaine dependence diagnosis. The patients with cocaine dependence diagnosis history were referred as well from the other psychiatric units in Louisville metro area.

Psychiatric status questionnaires, drug use and psychosocial functioning screening

The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV ([SCID I], First et al., 2001) was used for Axis I diagnoses. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) was assessed using The Post-traumatic Symptom Scale-Self Report (PSS-SR) (Foa et al., 1989) questionnaire. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 (HSCL-25) (Derogatis et al., 1974) was used to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. Handedness of patients was assessed using the Edinburgh inventory (Oldfield, 1971). Scores from the Addiction Severity Index (ASI) were used to measure problem severity in the areas of medical, employment, drug abuse, legal, family/social, and psychiatric difficulties (McLellan et al., 1980). Cocaine Negative Consequences Checklist (Michalec et al., 1996) was used to assess short- and long-term adverse effects resulting from cocaine use. Psychosocial adjustment will be assessed using the Social Adjustment Scale (SAS) (Weissman & Bothwell, 1976). These psychiatric assessments were important part of our outpatient subjects' clinical evaluations at the intake stage since most of them expressed willingness to enroll in an integrated behavioral treatment trial based on neurofeedback and motivational interviewing.

Drug screens

Qualitative urine toxicology screens (DrugCheck 4, NxStep, Amedica Biotech Inc., CA) was conducted in each subject to confirm cocaine use. In addition, qualitative urine toxicology screens for amphetamines, opiates and marijuana was performed to assess for additional abused substances. Positive test for marijuana was not considered exclusion criteria. Qualitative Saliva drug test (ALCO SCREEN, Chematics, IN) was also used to rule out current alcohol use.

Control subjects

Control subjects were recruited by the advertisement posted at different University of Louisville Health Science Center locations.

Subjects in the study

Nineteen cocaine abusing/dependent subjects (7 females, mean age, 42.1 ±5.5, range 32-56 years, 40% Afro-Americans) [SUD group] and 15 non-drug using control subjects (8 females, mean age, 37.0 ± 9.4, range, 29-64 years) [CNT group] participated in this study. Sixteen subjects in SUD group tested positive for cocaine, 8 of them also tested positive for marijuana use. Thee subjects in SUD who did not tested positive were recovering addicts enrolled in this study after the inpatient JADAC rehabilitation course with abstinence period of less than 60 days. Therefore majority of our outpatient population consisted from current cocaine users. Most preferred form of administration of the drug was smoking crack cocaine. One SUD subject in this study used cocaine intravenously. Sixteen of cocaine addicts reported also regular use of nicotine/smoking. None of the subjects in SUD group were in any treatment program other than attending Narcotics Anonymous (NA) or Alcoholic Anonymous (AA) meetings. All of the subjects except two patients from SUD group and one from control group were right-handed. All control participants reported no current or past history of neurological or psychiatric disorders or dependence on any substances other than nicotine or caffeine. Subjects were fully informed about the nature of this research and signed informed consent form approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Louisville (Protocol #240.06). For the specimen collection (urine drug screen) subjects signed a separate consent form also approved by the IRB within the same study protocol.

Instrumentation

EEG data acquisition and signal processing

All EEG data are acquired with a 128 channel Electrical Geodesics Inc. (Eugene, OR) system (v. 200) consisting of Sensor Net electrodes, Net Amps, and Net Station software (v. 4.0.1) running on a Macintosh G4 computer. EEG data are sampled at 500 Hz, 0.1 - 100 Hz analog filtered, referenced to the vertex. The Sensor Net is a lightweight elastic thread structure containing silver/silver chloride electrodes housed in a synthetic sponge on a pedestal. The sponges are soaked in a potassium chloride solution to render them conductive. EEG data are recorded continuously. Stimulus-locked EEG data are segmented off-line into 1000 ms epochs spanning 200 ms pre-stimulus to 800 ms post-stimulus around the critical stimulus events: e.g., “Go” and “NoGo” stimuli. For the response-locked ERN data are segmented from 500 ms pre- and 500 ms post-response. Data are digitally screened for artifact (eye blinks or movements etc.), and following additional visual, contaminated trials removed using both built-in artifact rejection tools. If data contains a significant number of trials contaminated with lateral eye movements, eye movement artifact correction algorithm will be applied. Remaining data are sorted by condition and averaged to create the ERPs. Prior to averaging ERP data are digitally filtered at 20 Hz lowpass to remove residual high-frequency noise. Averaged ERP data are baseline corrected over a 200 ms baseline period relative to segment start, and re-referenced into an average reference frame (Dien, 1998). The subject ERPs are averaged together to produce the grandaverage waveform across subjects.

Stimulus presentation and behavioral response collection

All stimulus presentation and behavioral response collection is controlled by a Dell computer running E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, PA). Visual stimuli are presented on a 15″ flat-panel display. Manual responses are collected with a 4-button keypad. In all experiments, subjects are seated in an adjustable chair with their chin in a chinrest. The chinrest is placed so that subject's eyes are 50 cm from the center of the flat panel screen. Subjects are instructed to remain as still as possible with their eyes on the fixation mark. Subjects are requested to refrain from blinking during trials. Breaks are provided every 3 - 4 minutes so that subjects can rest their eyes.

Stimuli and timing: Eriksens' flanker test with NoGo trials

This study uses a flanker task, in which one critical center letter is flanked to the right and left by distracter letters (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974, 1979). In some cases, the flankers match the central letter (congruous, no interference) and sometimes they match the other letter (incongruous, interference). Subjects respond with a two-choice, forced-alternative, pressing a left-hand key to one central letter, a right-hand key to the other. The letters are “R” and “L”. Congruent trials look as “LLLLL” or “RRRRR”, while incongruent as “LLRLL” or “RRLRR”. In another block representing a modification of the initial flanker task by adding NoGo-trials, subjects should not respond to a central target stimulus in a form of N-letter regardless of which letter is flanked on each side (e.g. “RRNRR” and/or “LLNLL” string requires no response). The stimulus is presented at the screen for only 100 ms. There are 960 trials in this test. Probability of NoGo trials (NNNNN, LLNLL, RRNRR) is 20% (half of them are congruent), while probabilities of Go trials is 80% (half of them congruent). There are 8 blocks (120 trials/block) in this task. This flanker test takes 45 minutes to complete. Figure 1 illustrates stimulus material in this version of flanker task.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Stimuli material used in this modification of Eriksens' flanker test with “NoGo” trials. The letters are white on a black background at the screen of monitor.

Dependent measures and data analysis

Behavioral measures

Behavioral measures mean reaction time, accuracy (percent of correct responses), number of commission (pressing key to a “NoGo” stimulus, or pressing wrong key) and omission errors (missed response to a “Go” stimulus).

Electrophysiological (ERP)

Adaptive mean amplitude and latency of ERP peak (e.g., N200, P300) within a temporal window across a region-of-interest (ROI) channel group. Each ROI contains at least 4 electrodes. For the flanker experiments list of ERP dependent variables includes amplitude and latency of the anterior (fronto-central) N200 (300-400 ms), anterior N450 (400-500 ms), posterior N200 (N2b, 240-320 ms), anterior (frontal P300) and posterior P300 (P3b, 350-600 ms) in successful and unsuccessful Go and NoGo trials, and correct and error choice (flanker) trials; and the frontal ERN (50-200 ms post-response) in error trials only. Difference waves are calculated for NoGo-N2 and –P3 (e.g. NoGo-N2-minus-Go-N2) in NoGo flanker trials. Response-locked ERN is recorded in trials with commission errors only. Topography of the ROIs and time windows for each ERP component is adjusted after visual inspection of the averaged waveforms.

Data analysis

Statistical analyses are performed on the subject-averaged behavioral and ERP data with the subject averages being the observations. The primary analysis model is the repeated measures ANOVA, with dependent EEG variables being all ERP and ERN component's amplitude at selected time windows. Data for each EEG-based dependent variable are analyzed using repeated-measures ANOVA (SPSS 14.0). Within-subject factors are: Stimulus (Go-target, NoGo), Congruence (congruent, incongruent), and Error (commission, omission) type for the ERN. Between subject factors is Group (SUD, Controls). The waveform and topographic plots and the dipole analyses are performed on the grandaverage data. Topographic maps are created using spherical spine interpolation. A-priori hypotheses are tested with single-tailed Student's t-tests for groups with unequal variance. In all ANOVAs, Greenhouse-Geisser corrected p-values is employed where appropriate.

Results

Behavioral responses

Reaction times in both congruent and incongruent “Go” trials were globally slower in SUD group (435.5 [Mean] ± 54.2 [Standard Deviation] in SUD vs. 381.0 ± 66.0 ms in CNT group, one way ANOVA, F=5.13, p=0.033) and was more pronounced in congruent “Go” trials (429.0 ± 67.4 vs. 362.5 ± 65.6 ms, F=6.09, p=0.021). Patients in SUD group did more commission and omission errors in congruent trials compared to controls (11.3 ± 11.0 vs. 2.39± 2.23 %, F=6.92, p= 0.16), but difference was not significant for incongruent “Go” trials.

Centro-parietal ERP components: N2b

The amplitude of N2b (averaged across 4 centro-parietal EEG recording sites, N=19 in SUD, and N=15 in CNT group) was lower (i.e., less negative) in SUD group compared to controls in congruent and incongruent “Go” (-0.27± 0.98 vs. -1.06± 0.91 mV, F=4.89, p=0.035), and “NoGo” trials (-0.28± 0.98 vs. -1.85± 0.94 μV, F=5.45, p=0.026). The latency of N2b component was delayed in SUD group compared to controls both in “Go” (268.1 ± 33.9 vs. 235.3 ± 39.7 ms, F=6.67, p=0.015) and “NoGo” (262.6 ± 32.3 vs. 232.6 ± 38.1 ms, F=6.16, p=0.018) conditions. There were no any other interactions effects for amplitude and latency of the N2b.

P3b

The amplitude of P3b was lower in SUD group compared to controls both to congruent and incongruent “Go” stimuli (2.22 ± 1.62 vs. 4.00 ± 2.75 μV, F=5.18, p=0.03), but only to incongruent “NoGo” stimuli (1.80± 1.39 vs. 3.28± 2.44 μV, F=4.68, p=0.039). The latency of P3b was not statistically different in SUD compared to CNT group (across all “Go” stimuli, 473.2 vs. 466.7 ms, not significant) but showed a significant Congruence (congruent, incongruent) × Group (SUD, CNT) effect (F=5.12, p=0.031). This interaction can be described as a shorter P3b latency to incongruent stimuli, while longer latency to congruent stimuli in addicts compared to controls.

Frontal ERP components: N200 (N2a)

The amplitude of this component did not show any group differences, but the latency of N2a was globally longer in cocaine addiction group in all conditions (congruent and incongruent “Go”, 281 vs. 239 ms, F=6.96, p=0.013; congruent and incongruent “NoGo”, 276 vs. 238 ms, F=7.35, p=0.011). This might be indicative of slower processing of response conflict in SUD group.

N450

We could not find any amplitude or latency group differences for N450 component.

Anterior difference waves

For the assessment of cortical inhibition we analyzed difference waves (NoGo-minus-Go) both for N200 (NoGo-N2) and P300 (NoGo-P3) window ranges.

Frontal N200 differences wave (NogGo-N2)

One way ANOVA showed significant differences between SUD and control groups only in congruent NoGo-N2 waves. Amplitude of the difference wave was lower in SUD group compared to controls (0.32 ± 0.85 vs. -0.65 ± 1.56 μV, F=4.90, p=0.035). Furthermore, we found a marginal Congruence × Group interaction (F=4.22, p=0.05) which can be described as a significantly more pronounced frontal NoGo-N2 difference wave in incongruent trials in controls, without any between-group differences for congruent trials. Considering that NoGo-N2 was used as a cortical inhibition index, it may point that higher efforts to inhibit response to more difficult incongruent trials were exposed by CNT group subjects, even though main effect of congruence on this parameter was not reaching significance level. This effect was observed at the FCz site and at the ROI which included 5 neighboring fronto-central channels.

Anterior frontal NoGo-P3 difference wave

This frontal difference wave showed between group difference being higher in controls compared to SUD group (1.38 ± 1.30 vs. 0.41 ± 1.10 μV, F=4.36, p=0.047). There were no group interactions with congruence for this frontal difference wave.

Error-related negativity (ERN)

This stimulus-locked parameter was calculated only for those subject who committed more than 12 commission errors. Only 6 subjects from control group and 6 subjects from the SUD group met this criteria. The amplitude of ERN during commission errors was significantly more negative in controls compared to addicts (-5.71 ± 2.76 vs. -2.20 ±1.52 μV, F=7.42, p=0.021) which shows higher effectiveness of error detection and error monitoring in control subjects. The latency of ERN did not show any group differences.

Discussion

Our results show poorer performance on the speeded flanker task with response inhibition demands in cocaine addicts compared to controls. Cocaine abusers had significantly slower reaction time and higher error rate globally, but behavioral responses showed more differences in congruent rather than incongruent “Go” trials. More omission than commission errors were committed by addicts which did not support our initial expectation of higher rate of commission errors in this group.

Posterior (centro-parietal) measures of attention (N2b, P3b) also showed lower magnitude of response both to congruent and incongruent “Go” and “NoGo” trials in addicts compared to controls. Another interesting and unexpected finding was longer latency of P3b to congruent stimuli rather than incongruent ones in addicts. However, P3b decrements are found in most of SUD and alcoholism, not only cocaine use disorder (Bauer & Hesselbrok, 2001; Carlson, Iacono, & McGue, 2002; O'Connor et al., 1994; Polich, Pollock, & Bloom, 1994; Porjesz & Begleiter, 1998). The P3b attenuation effect is also typical for many psychiatric disorders that often are comorbid with SUD, such as conduct disorder, ADHD, PTSD, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, etc. (Bauer & Hesselbrock, 1999, 2001; Bauer, 1997; O'Connor et al., 1994; Friedman & Squires-Wheeler, 1994). Reduced P3b might be reflecting as well a predisposition (“risk factor”) for development of substance use disorder in general, rather than direct consequence of drug abuse.

In this regard, it was more valuable to detect frontal NoGo-N2 and NoGo-P3 amplitude and latency differences between cocaine addicts and controls, because reduced anterior ERP components during response withholding could be related to prefrontal brain dysfunction and point that a deficit in frontal inhibitory control is an underlying mechanism shared by different psychiatric conditions (Bauer & Hesselbrock, 1999; Clark, Parker, & Lynch, 1999; Tarter et al., 2003). Once again, in a similar manner as for behavioral responses and posterior ERPs measures, our cocaine addicted subjects showed significantly less pronounced frontal NoGo-N2 difference wave in incongruent “NoGo” trials rather than controls. Considering that the NoGo-N2 was used as a cortical inhibition index in this study, we can suggest that more efforts to inhibit response on relatively more conflicting incongruent trials were exercised by the control subjects but not by the addicts. Error-related negativity was significantly larger in controls compared to cocaine addicts during commission errors and met our prediction of under-reactivity to committed errors in cocaine addicts.

Unfortunately, we could not detect any significant differences either in amplitude or latency of the frontal component N450, which is also thought to be originating from the ACC during tasks with conflicting response demands (e.g., flanker or Stroop test). One of the explanations for this could be sought in the methodology of the component scoring used in our current study. The N450 component is exhibited as a negative deflection immediately after the peak of P300 or on a descending front of P300. Considering that the SUD group showed globally attenuated P300, a different approach to baseline correction method or a different calculation of the N450 parameters (e.g., magnitude or integral of the wave) might happen to be more sensitive to detect group differences, and we plan to recalculate this index using more appropriate scoring techniques.

In general, the results of the present study indicate that the difficulties that cocaine dependent subjects experience during flanker task and response inhibition are expressed more when corrective actions should be detected and signaled (but are not) by the medial frontal conflict and error detection system (i.e., ACC) and when more executive (“top-down”) control engagement should be requested. Under-reactive motor response conflict detection and error monitoring system along with the “hypofunctional” prefrontal control might be the main reason for the lower capacity for corrective action need detection and actual implementation of cognitive control necessary for the better performance on this task.

In accordance with the view of hierarchical organization of central motor control, different brain areas are involved in functionally separate mechanisms of advanced movement preparation such as response selection and motor act programming. The frontal lobes control motor acts during performance on reaction task through the planning (e.g., PFC via associated basal ganglia circuits), preparation (pre-supplementary motor area [pre-SMA], supplementary motor area [SMA] and their basal ganglia and cerebellum loops), and execution of movements (primary motor area [Ml] and sub-cortical areas) (Brunia, 1999; Brunia & van Boxtel, 2001; Faw, 2003; Fuster, 1999, 2001; Miller & Cohen, 2001; Thaler et al., 1988).

Anatomically the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is known to be connected to the premotor cortex by two main routes. The first one connects PFC with premotor areas via fronto-striatal connections and thalamus. The second route includes direct cortico-cortical connections of the PFC with the mesial premotor areas. Several frontomesial cortical areas are involved in motor control: the anterior pre-SMA, the posterior SMA, and motor areas of the ACC. Externally triggered movements (i.e., those used in our task) are thought to be mainly mediated by the lateral premotor cortex and MI (Jenkins et al., 2000; Kaiser et al., 2000). These premotor areas seem more involved in selection of movements based on external cues or prompts (Faw, 2003; Passingham, 1995).

Activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in externally triggered tasks is associated with working memory and other executive “top-down” control processes (Wiese et al., 2004). The MI area has control over limb movements based on external cues, while the SMA is based on internal cues or working memory (Jahanhashi et al., 1995; Passingham, 1995). The DLPFC is contributing to the “movement readiness potential” approximately 1 s prior to movement, and only then the MI and SMA areas show the “readiness potential” (500 ms prior to action), thus preparing extra-pyramidal motor commands, while motor cortex sends basic pyramidal commands (Faw, 2003). The left DLPFC is the most important area in the programming strategies, control of executive functions, and motor responses (Miller & Cohen, 2001).

It was suggested that among neural substrates mediating avoidance behavior, the ACC deals with relatively simple expectations of conflict and conflict monitoring (Botvinick et al., 1999; Carter et al., 1998; Devinsky et al., 1995), whereas prefrontal cortex deals with more complex aspects including selection of actions (Faw, 2003). The PFC is not critical for performing simple, automatic movements, such as orienting which relies on ‘bottom-up’ processing. In our condition with a congruent (easy, no interferences) flanker stimuli the response is practically automatic, and worse performance by addicts taps at generally more poor psychomotor functioning rather than frontal executive differences. By contrast, the PFC is important when ‘top-down’ processing is required (Heyder et al., 2004). For example, it is needed when a goal-directed behavior must be guided in complex situations when mapping between sensory inputs, action plan and memory should be integrated and coordinated (Miller & Cohen, 2001). In a case of incongruent flanker stimuli, normally functioning medial frontal network for motor conflict prediction and interference detection (indexed by N2a and N450), as well as timely and correct identification of an error (ERN) is crucial for triggering this “top-down” control to adjust motor response to task demands. Poorly functioning response conflict and error monitoring system in cocaine addicts expressed in a lower frontal N2a and ERN may result in an ignorance (or neglect) both of potential motor conflicts (interference) and committed errors, which may partially explain relatively less errors during incongruent flankers in this group.

Furthermore, even when conflicting response interferences and errors are detected and signaled for executive control, a lowered capacity for the inhibitory prefrontal control over pre-motor structure tends to fail, thus letting these lower level premotor structures be guided by external signals without significant prefrontal control and corrective override. When working memory load is not too demanding (as it is a case in our study, where the level of errors is relatively low) this unrestricted following initial pattern of response to stimuli and underestimation of potential response conflict may result in a certain advantage for the cocaine addiction group during incongruent trials.

In a real life environment it is well known that cocaine addicts have difficulties to inhibit their own actions and behaviors aimed at drug-seeking and drug-taking. The sequelae of lowered frontal executive and inhibitory control over behavioral pattern results in over-reactivity of behavioral responses to drug-related external cues, and internal craving and drug-related ruminations. It is reasonable to propose that a ‘hypofunctional’ PFC executive control (probably due to neurotoxicity of cocaine) results in a diminished ability to effectively override strong habitual automated response tendencies controlled by the lower-level neural mechanisms (e.g., premotor areas, basal ganglia).

Drug addiction leads to frontal “top-down” control deficits. Deficient inhibitory control results in an inability to exert corrective actions over strong over-learned habitual responses, thus allowing more automatic external salient stimuli (drug cues and environmental situations) and pathological craving to drive behavior. Individuals predisposed to behavioral disinhibition are more vulnerable to impulsive drug abuse. Reduced prefrontal inhibitory control results as well in a diminished capacity to override stress responses and generally poor stress coping skills typically found in cocaine addicts.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Frontal and fronto-central region-of-interest [ROI] (F2, FCz, and EGI channel 5 located between them, 0.5 cm left from FC2) grandaverage ERP waveforms during congruent and incongruent “Go” trials in controls (N=15) and cocaine addicts (N=19). The controls group as compared to cocaine addicts shows more pronounced frontal negativity differences between congruent and incongruent “Go” trials.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Frontal and fronto-central (F2 and Fcz) grandaverage ERP waveforms during all “Go” and “NoGo” conditions. The group of cocaine addicts compared to controls shows less frontal negativity (N200, N450) differences between congruent and incongruent “NoGo” trials.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Typical frontal P300 “ánteriorization” effect observed in the group of control subjects (N=15) in this study. During both congruent and incongruent “NoGo” trials the P300 component is better visible at more anterior parts of the topographic maps. The amplitude of this positive component is larger and is expressed more during conflicting incongruent “NoGo” trials at time window peaking around 400 ms post-stimulus. The red color reflects higher amplitude of the wave.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Group of cocaine addicts (N=19) compared to control group (N=15) shows less differentiated congruent vs. incongruent “NoGo” anterior frontal P300 amplitude on topographic maps (400 ms post-stimulus), and globally lower amplitude of the component during motor response withholding.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

Error-related negativity (ERN) during commission errors in controls (N=6) and cocaine addicts (N=6). The amplitude of ERN is larger in controls than addicts even through the latency did not show significant differences. The window selected for these fronto-central ROI containing 4 EEG recording sites (AFz, Fz, and 2 EGI channels located close to F1 and F2) is highlighted by the light blue color and shows a negative peak between 50 – 150 ms post- commission error.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the ISNR Research Committee grant and the NIDA R03DA021821 grant to Tato Sokhadze.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) 4th. Washington, D.C.: 1994. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8:170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Band GP, van der Molen MW, Logan GD. Horse-race model simulation of the stopsignal procedure. Acta Psychologica. 2003;112:105–142. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(02)00079-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bates AT, Liddle PF, Kiehl KA, Ngan ETC. State dependent changes in error monitoring in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2004;38:347–356. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2003.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauer LO. Frontal P300 decrement, childhood conduct disorder, family history, and predisposition of relapse among abstinent cocaine abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 1997;44:1–10. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(96)01311-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauer LO, Hesselbrock VM. P300 decrements in teenagers with conduct problems Implications for substance abuse risk and brain development. Biological Psychiatry. 1999;46:263–272. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00335-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bauer LO, Hesselbrock VM. CSD/BEM localization of P300 sources in adolescents “at risk” : Evidence of frontal cortex dysfunction in conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;50:600–608. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01066-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bekker EM, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN. Electrophysiological correlates of attention, inhibition, sensitivity and bias in a continuous performance task. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115:2001–2013. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Biggins CA, MacKay S, Clark W, Fein G. Event-related potential evidence for frontal cortex effects of chronic cocaine dependence. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42:472–485. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00425-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Botvinick M, Nystrom LE, Carter CS, Cohen JD. Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature. 1999;402:179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Brunia CHM. Neural aspects of anticipatory behavior. Acta Psychologica. 1999;26:165–178. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00006-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brunia CHM, van Boxtel GJM. Wait and see. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2001;43:59–75. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(01)00179-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bunge SA, Ochsner KN, Desmond JE, Glover GH, Gabrieli JD. Prefrontal regions involved in keeping information in and out of mind. Brain. 2001;124:2074–2086. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.10.2074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, et al. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proceedings National Academy Sciences of the USA. 2002;99:523–528. doi: 10.1073/pnas.012470999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carlson SR, Iacono WG, McGue M. P300 amplitude in adolescent twins discordant and concordant for alcohol use disorder. Biological Psychology. 2002;61:203–227. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(02)00059-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science. 1998;280:747–749. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark D, Parker A, Lynch K. Psychopathology of substance related problems during early adolescence: a survival analysis. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 1999;28:333–341. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp280305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Coles MGH, Gratton G, Bashore TR, Eriksen CW, Donchin E. A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1985;11:529–553. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.11.5.529. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Corr PJ. J.A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli. Personality Individual Differences. 2002;32:1247–1253. [Google Scholar]
  20. Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions Royal Society London: Biological Sciences. 1996;351:1413–1420. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1996.0125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Derogatis LR, Lipman RS, Rickels K, Uhlenhuth EH, Covi L. The Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL): a self-report symptom inventory. Behavioral Sciences. 1974;19:1–15. doi: 10.1002/bs.3830190102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Devinsky O, Morrell MJ, Vogt BA. Contributions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain. 1995;118:279–306. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.1.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Di Sclafani V, Tolou-Shams M, Price LJ, Fein G. Neuropsychological performance of individuals dependent on crack-cocaine, or crack-cocaine and alcohol, at 6 weeks and 6 months of abstinence. Drug Alcohol Dependence. 2002;66:161–171. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(01)00197-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dien J. Issues in the application of the average reference: Review, critiques, and recommendations. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 1998;30:34–43. [Google Scholar]
  25. Donkers FCL, van Boxtel GJM. The N2 in go/no-go tasks reflects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain Cognition. 2004;56:165–176. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Elliott R. Executive functions and their disorders. British Medical Bulletin. 2003;65:49–59. doi: 10.1093/bmb/65.1.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of target letters in a non-search task. Perception and Psychophysics. 1974;16:143–149. [Google Scholar]
  28. Eriksen CW, Eriksen BA. Target redundancy in visual search. Perceptual Psychophysics. 1979;26:195–205. [Google Scholar]
  29. Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks and their relation to inhibition. Acta Psychologica. 1999;101:267–291. doi: 10.1016/s0001-6918(99)00008-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Falkenstein M, Hoormann J, Hohnsbein J. Inhibition-related ERP components: Variation with modality, age, and time-on-task. Journal Psychophysiology. 2002;16:167–175. [Google Scholar]
  31. Faw B. Pre-frontal executive committee for perception, working memory, attention, long-term memory, motor control, and thinking: A tutorial review. Consciousness Cognition. 2003;12:83–139. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8100(02)00030-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fein G, Bigging C, MacKay S. Cocaine abusers have reduced auditory P50 amplitude and suppression compared to both normal controls and alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;39:955–965. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00299-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV-TR axis I disorders - patient edition (SCID - I/P) New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  34. Franklin TR, Acton PD, Maldjian JA, Gray JD, Croft JR, Dackis CA, O'Brien CP, Childress AR. Decreased gray matter concentration in the insular, orbitofrontal, cingulate, and temporal cortices of cocaine patients. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51:134–142. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01269-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Friedman D, Squires-Wheeler E. Event-related potentials (ERPs) as indicators of risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 1994;20(1):63–74. doi: 10.1093/schbul/20.1.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe. 3rd. Lippincott-Raven; Philadelphia: 1997. 1997. [Google Scholar]
  37. Fuster JM. Cognitive functions of the frontal lobes. In: Miller BL, Cummings JL, editors. Human frontal lobes. Guilford; New York: 1999. pp. 187–195. [Google Scholar]
  38. Fuster JM. The prefrontal cortex- an update: time is of the essence. Neuron. 2001;30:319–333. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00285-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Gehring WJ, Knight RT. Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience. 2000;3:516–520. doi: 10.1038/74899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gehring WJ, Himle J, Nilsenson L. Action monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological Science. 2000;11:1–6. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Goldstein R, Volkow ND. Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. American Journal Psychiatry. 2002;159:1642–1652. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.10.1642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hajcak G, McDonald N, Simons RF. Anxiety and error-related activity. Biological Psychology. 2003;64:77–90. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(03)00103-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Herrmann MJ, Remmler J, Ehlis AC, Heindrich A, Fallgatter AJ. Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe) Cognitive Brain Research. 2004;20:294–299. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Heil M, Osman A, Wiegelmann J, Rolke B, Hennighausen E. N200 in the Eriksen-task: inhibitory executive processes? Journal of Psychophysiology. 2000;14:218–225. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hester R, Garavan H. Executive dysfunctions in cocaine addiction: evidence for discordant frontal, cingular, and cerebellar activity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:11017–11022. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3321-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Heyder K, Suchan B, Daum I. Cortico-subcortical contributions to executive control. Acta Psychologica. 2004;155:271–289. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Jahanshahi M, Jenkins IH, Brown RG, Marsden CD, Passingham RE, Brooks DJ. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements - I. An investigation using measurement of regional blood flow with PET and movement-related potentials in normal and Parkinson's disease subjects. Brain. 1995;118:913–933. doi: 10.1093/brain/118.4.913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jenkins IH, Jahashahi M, Jueptner M, Passingham R, Brooks DJ. Self-initiated versus externally triggered movements. II. The effect of movement predictability on regional blood flow. Brain. 2000;123:1216–1228. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.6.1216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Johnson R. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:367–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00649.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Kaiser J, Lutzenberger W, Preissl H, Mosshammer D, Birmbaumer N. Statistical probability mapping reveals high-frequency magnitoencephalographic activity in supplementary motor area during self-paced finger movements. Neuroscience Letters. 2000;283:81–84. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(00)00921-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kok A, Ramautar JR, de Ruiter MB, Band GP, Ridderinkhof KR. ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology. 2004;42:9–20. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-8986.2003.00127.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2001;24:97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Kopp B, Rist F, Mattler U. N200 in the flanker task as a neurobehavioral tool for investigating executive control. Psychophysiology. 1996;33:282–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb00425.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kouri EM, Lukas SE, Mendelson JH. P300 assessment of opiate and cocaine abusers: effects of detoxification and bupernorphine treatment. Biological Psychiatry. 1996;40:617–628. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00468-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Logan G. On the ability to inhibit thoughts and action. In: Dagenbach D, Carr TH, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention. Academic Press; NY: 1994. pp. 189–239. [Google Scholar]
  56. Lyvers M. “Loss of Control” in alcoholism and drug addiction: a neuroscientific interpretation. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2000;8:225–249. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.8.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Markela-Lerenc J, Ille N, Kaiser S, Fiedler P, Mundt C, Weisbrod M. Prefrontal-cingulate activation during executive control: Which comes first? Cognitive Brain Research. 2004;18:278–287. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.10.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Matochik JA, London ED, Eldreth D, Cadet JL, Bolla KI. Frontal cortical tissue composition in abstinent cocaine abusers: a magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage. 2003;19:1095–1102. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00244-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. McLellan AT, Luborsky L, Woody GE, O'Brien CP. An improved diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: The Addiction Severity Index. Journal of Nervous Mental Disorders. 1980;168:26–33. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198001000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Michalec EM, Rohsenow DJ, Monti PM, Varney SM, Martin RA, Dey AN, Myers M, Sirota AD. A Cocaine Negative Consequences Checklist: development and validation. Journal of Substance Abuse. 1996;8:181–193. doi: 10.1016/s0899-3289(96)90238-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review Neuroscience. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Näätänen RA, Gaillard WK, Mäntysalo S. Early selective attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica. 1978;2:313–329. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(78)90006-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Näätänen R, Schröger E, Karakas S, Tervaniemi M, Paavilainen P. Development of a memory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. Neuroreport. 1993;4:503–506. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199305000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Nieuwenhuis S, Yeung N, van den Wildenberg W, Ridderinkhof KR. Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulate function in a Go/No-Go task: effects of response conflict and trial-type frequency. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3:17–26. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Nigg JT. Neuropsychologic theory and findings in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: the state of the field and salient challenges for the coming decade. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1424–1435. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. O'Connor S, Bauer LO, Tasman A, Hesselbrock VM. Reduced P3 amplitudes of ERPs are associated with both a family history of alcoholism and antisocial personality disorder. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 1994;18:1307–1321. doi: 10.1016/0278-5846(94)90095-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Passingham R. Frontal lobes and voluntary action. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  69. Polich J, Kok A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: an integrative review. Biological Psychology. 1995;41:103–146. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Polich J, Pollock VE, Bloom FE. Meta-analysis of P300 from males at risk for alcoholism. Psychological Bulletin. 1994;115(1):55–73. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Potts GF, Patel SH, Azzam PN. Impact of instructed relevance on the visual ERP. International Journal Psychophysiology. 2004;52:197–209. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Porjesz B, Begleiter H. Genetic basis of event-related potentials and their relationship to alcoholism and alcohol use. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 1998;15:44–57. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199801000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Pritchard WS. Psychophysiology of P300. Psychological Bulletin. 1981;89:506–540. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Pritchard WS, Houlihan ME, Robinson JH. P300 and response selection: A new look using independent-components analysis. Brain Topography. 1999;12:31–37. doi: 10.1023/a:1022277506517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rauch SL, van der Kolk BA, Fisler RE, Alpert NM, Orr SP, et al. A symptom provocation study of posttraumatic stress disorder using positron emission tomography and script-driven imagery. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1996;53:380–387. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830050014003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Roberts LE, Rau H, Lutzenberger W, Birbaumer N. Mapping P300 waves onto inhibition: Go/Nogo discrimination. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1994;92:44–55. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90006-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Robertson EM, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Studies in cognition: the problems solved and created by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15:948–960. doi: 10.1162/089892903770007344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Salisbury DF, Griggs CB, Shenton ME, McCarly RW. The NoGo P300 ‘anteriorization’ effect and response inhibition. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2004;115:1550–58. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.01.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Strik WK, Fallgatter AJ, Brandies D, Pascual-Marqui RD. Three-dimensional tomography of event-related potentials during response inhibition: evidence for phasic frontal lobe activation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 1998;108:406–413. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(98)00021-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Tarter RE, Kirisci L, Mezzich A, Cornelius J, Pajer K, Vanyukov MM, Gardner T, Blackson D, Clark D. Neurobehavior disinhibition in childhood predicts early age at onset of substance disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1078–1085. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Thaler DE, Rolls ET, Passingham RE. Neuronal activity of the supplementary motor area (SMA) during internally and externally triggered wrist movement. Neuroscience Letters. 1988;93:264–269. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(88)90093-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Turken AU, Swick D. Response selection in the human anterior cingulate cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:920–924. doi: 10.1038/13224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Valls-Sole J, Vallderiola F. Neurophysiological correlate of clinical signs in Parkinson's disease. Clinical Neurophysiology. 2002;113:792–805. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(02)00080-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Van Veen V, Carter CS. The timing of action-monitoring process in the anterior cingulate cortex. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience. 2002;14(4):593–602. doi: 10.1162/08989290260045837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Verleger R. On utility of P3 latency as an index of mental chronometry. Psychophysiology. 1997;34:131–156. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1997.tb02125.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Verbruggen F, Liefooghe B, Vandierendonck A. The interaction between stop signal inhibition and distractor interference in the flanker and Stroop task. Acta Psychologica. 2004;116:21–37. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain: insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigations. 2003;111:1444–1451. doi: 10.1172/JCI18533. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ. The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology. 2004;47:3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2004.07.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Weissman MM, Bothwell S. Assessment of social adjustment by patient self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1976;33:1111–1115. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770090101010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. West R. Neural correlates of cognitive control and conflict detection in the Stroop and digit-location tasks. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41:1122–1135. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00297-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. West R, Bowry R, McConville C. Sensitivity of medial frontal cortex to response and nonresponse conflict. Psychophysiology. 2004;41:739–748. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00205.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Wiese H, Stude P, Nebel K, de Greiff A, et al. Movement preparation in self-initiated versus externally triggered movements: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroscience Letters. 2004;371:220–225. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.08.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Wijers AA, Mulder G, Gunter TC, Smid HGOM. Brain potential analysis of selective attention. In: Neumann O, Sanders AF, editors. Handbook of perception and action. Vol. 3. Tullamore, Ireland: Academic Press; 1996. pp. 333–387. Attention. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES