Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addiction. 2009 Sep 10;104(11):1807–1819. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02691.x

Computer-Delivered Interventions to Reduce College Student Drinking: A Meta-Analysis

Kate B Carey 1, Lori A J Scott-Sheldon 1, Jennifer C Elliott 1, Jamie R Bolles 1, Michael P Carey 1
PMCID: PMC2763045  NIHMSID: NIHMS134586  PMID: 19744139

Abstract

Aims

This meta-analysis evaluates the efficacy and moderators of computer-delivered interventions (CDIs) to reduce alcohol use among college students.

Methods

We included 35 manuscripts with 43 separate interventions, and calculated both between-group and within-group effect sizes for alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. Effects sizes were calculated for short-term (≤ 5 weeks) and longer-term (≥ 6 weeks) intervals. All studies were coded for study descriptors, participant characteristics, and intervention components.

Results

The effects of CDIs depended on the nature of the comparison condition: CDIs reduced quantity and frequency measures relative to assessment-only controls, but rarely differed from comparison conditions that included alcohol content. Small-to-medium within-group effect sizes can be expected for CDIs at short- and longer-term follow-ups; these changes are less than or equivalent to the within-group effect sizes observed for more intensive interventions.

Conclusions

CDIs reduce the quantity and frequency of drinking among college students. CDIs are generally equivalent to alternative alcohol-related comparison interventions.

Keywords: alcohol use, prevention, computer-delivered intervention, college students, meta-analysis

INTRODUCTION

Most college students (80%) drink alcohol, even though the majority cannot legally purchase alcohol; many also report binge drinking, alcohol-related problems, and drinking to get drunk [1]. These drinking patterns have been identified as a public health problem by the Surgeon General [2], Institute of Medicine [3], and the National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse [4], and reducing binge drinking in college students is a goal in Healthy People 2010 [5]. Controlled studies of college alcohol interventions are increasing [6] and, within this literature, computer delivery of prevention interventions is appealing due to its ease of administration and lower cost [7].

The use of computers to deliver alcohol “counseling” offers many appealing features, especially to students. Computer-delivered interventions (CDIs) allow students to access information at a self-determined pace while maintaining privacy. Technology permits personalization of content and the potential for using multimedia, games and virtual simulations to engage emerging adults. Compared to interventions that require counselors, CDIs reach more students [8]; this potential for dissemination affords greater reach to a significant subset of the population.

A 2005 review of CDIs for students described five programs but evaluation data were limited to two randomized controlled trials (RCTs) [9]. By 2008, Elliott, Carey, and Bolles [10] reviewed 17 controlled trials of CDIs for college drinkers, and concluded that CDIs were more efficacious than assessment-only controls, but often equivalent to alternate interventions. The rapid growth of research on CDIs for students now permits a quantitative review of this literature.

The primary purpose of this meta-analytic review is to generate effect sizes that reflect (a) the efficacy of CDIs relative to comparison conditions and (b) within-groups change. A secondary purpose is to explore the (c) efficacy of CDIs relative to active controls, and (d) maintenance of effects over time. We also explore (e) moderators of efficacy of CDIs (e.g., intervention length).

METHODS

Search Strategy and Study Selection

Studies were retrieved from (a) electronic databases (PubMed, PsycINFO, CINAHL, Dissertation Abstracts, ERIC, Cochrane Library, CRISP) using a Boolean search strategy that includes the use of operators (i.e., and, or, not) and truncation (using an asterisk) to link and expand specific keywords (search terms: alcohol/drink*/binge and college/university/ undergraduate* and intervention/prevention and computer/internet/intranet/DVD/email/text*), (b) reference sections of manuscripts, (c) online contents of journals (e.g. Addiction), and (d) responses to listserv requests. Unpublished papers were included to avoid the file-drawer effect (i.e., stronger effects reported in published studies; [11]). Studies were included if they (a) examined an alcohol-related intervention delivered via computer or electronic device (e.g., text messages), (b) sampled undergraduates, (c) assessed behavioral outcomes; and (d) provided sufficient information to calculate effect sizes. When authors reported outcomes in multiple manuscripts, the studies were linked and represented as one study. If a study reported on more than one comparison condition (e.g., standard education and assessment only), the condition with the least contact was used. In some cases, comparisons were made with an active non-CDI when a less-intensive control condition was unavailable. Authors of 17 studies reporting insufficient details were contacted for additional information (83% response rate resulting in the retention of 14 studies). Using these methods, we included 35 manuscripts with 43 separate interventions (Fig. 1).

Study Outcomes

We examined alcohol consumption and problems. Alcohol consumption included: (1) quantity consumed over time (e.g., week, month), (2) quantity per drinking occasion (e.g., Friday night), (3) maximum quantity consumed on one occasion, (4) frequency of heavy drinking (usually defined as ≥5 drinks for men and ≥4 drinks for women [12], and (5) frequency of drinking days. Alcohol-related problems were operationalized using multi-item scales.

Content Coding and Reliability

Study information (e.g., publication year), sample characteristics (e.g., gender, age), target group (e.g., Greek members, freshmen), design and measurement specifics (e.g., recruitment method), and content of intervention and control condition(s) (e.g., number of sessions, content) were coded independently by two of the authors. (Appendix 2 contains a complete list of coding categories.) Methodological quality was assessed using 12 items adapted from validated measures [13, 14]; scores ranged from 0 to 17. Fifteen studies were randomly selected to evaluate interrater reliability. For the categorical dimensions, raters agreed on 56% to 100% of the judgments (mean Cohen’s kappa = .72). Reliability for the continuous variables was calculated using the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC); ICC ranged from .82 to 1.00, with an average of .98 across categories. Disagreements were resolved through discussion.

APPENDIX 2.

Description of studies, samples, intervention, and control conditions.

Study characteristics (k =35)
Year of publication (k = 27)
 Mdn 2005
 Range 2000 – 2008
Year of data collection
 Mdn 2004
 Range 1994–2007
Year of program development
 Mdn 2002
 Range 1993–2006
Published 60%
* Financial Support
 Public 57%
 Private 17%
 None/NR 26%
Region of Sample
 US Northeast 14%
 US Southeast 17%
 US Midwest 6%
 US Southwest 23%
 US Northwest 23%
 Non-US region 9%
 Multiple regions 9%
* Type of institution (k = 33)
 Public university 85%
 Private university 12%
 Multiple institutions 3%
* Size of institution (k = 33)
 <6,000 students 3%
 6,000 – 10,000 students 3%
 >10,000 students 91%
 Multiple 3%
Targeted intervention (% yes) 91%
Target group (k = 32)
 Heavy drinkers 28%
 College freshman 23%
 High-risk drinkers 14%
 Current drinkers 13%
 Alcohol violators 13%
 Upperclass students 3%
 Students turning 21 3%
 Athlete 2%
 Males 2%
Methodological quality score (17 points total)
 Mean (SD) 9.37 (2.52)
 Mdn 10
 Range 3 – 13

Research design and implementation (k = 35)
Recruitment method
 Volunteered 53%
 Recruited 33%
 Mandated 14%
Random Assignment
 Randomized individuals 74%
 Randomized groups 17%
 Nonequivalent control group 9%
Pretest post-test design (% yes) 100%
No. post-intervention assessments
 Mean (SD) 1.63 (0.73)
 Mdn 1
 Range 1 – 3
First post-intervention assessment
 Mean weeks (SD) 5.06 (5.63)
 Mdn 4.33
 Range 0 – 26

Sample Characteristics (k = 35)
Sample Size (N)
 Total 28,621
 Mean (SD) 817.74(3884)
 Mdn 148
Year in school (mean %)
 Freshman 68%
 Sophomore 15%
 Junior 12%
 Senior 5%
 Graduate 1%
Age in years (k = 31)
 Mean (SD) 19.71 (1.09)
 Range 18 – 22
% women
 Mean (SD) 0.50 (0.15)
% Greek members (k = 9)
 Mean (SD) 0.23 (0.15)
% White
 Mean (SD) 0.75 (0.17)
% Black
 Mean (SD) 0.10 (0.12)
% Asian
 Mean (SD) 0.13 (0.12)
% Hispanic
 Mean (SD) 0.08 (0.05)

Intervention Characteristics (k = 55)
No. of intervention conditions
 Mean (SD) 1.36 (0.48)
 Mdn (Range) 1 (1 – 2)
Level of intervention
 Self-directed 86%
 Group delivered 7%
 Computer and group session(s) 5%
 Computer and in-person session 2%
Self-directed computer/web (k = 48)
 No. of sessions (Mdn) 1
 No. of minutes (Mdn) 20
Group-delivered computer/web (k = 4)
 No. of sessions (Mdn) 1
 No. of minutes (Mdn) 120
Computer/web and group session (k = 4)
 No. of computer sessions (Mdn) 31.5
 No. of computer minutes (Mdn) 1
 No. of group sessions (Mdn) 1
 No. of group minutes (Mdn) 30
Computer/web and in-person session (k = 1)
 No. of computer sessions (Mdn) 1
 No. of computer minutes (Mdn) 15
 No. of in-person sessions (Mdn) 1
 No. of in-person minutes (Mdn) 4
Interactive intervention (% yes) 93%
Intervention content tailored
 Individual 89%
 Group 2%
 None/NR 9%
* Intervention Components
 Feedback on consumption 86%
 Alcohol education 77%
 Normative comparisons, generic 77%
 Tailored materials 61%
 Modification strategies 43%
 Challenges/expectancies 39%
 Feedback on problems 36%
 Focus on high-risk situations 34%
 Feedback on risk factors 30%
 Normative comparisons, matching 25%
 General alcohol-related materials 20%
 Writing and/or journaling 16%
 Goal-setting 13%
 Values clarification 7%
 Decisional balance 2%
Delivery mechanism
 Internet 38%
 Intranet 30%
 CD-ROM/Interactive DVD 25%
 Multiple delivery modes 5%
 Portable electronic device 2%
Delivery setting
 On-site 72%
 Off-site 24%
 Unknown 4%
Type of commercially available program used
 None 59%
 Alcohol 101 (Plus) 20%
 College Alc 7%
 Electronic Check-Up to Go 5%
 Check Your Drinking 4%
 My Student Body: Alcohol 4%
 Alcohol Edu 2%
Computer program includes avatar (%) 23%

Comparison Characteristics (k = 54)
Type of control/comparison
 Wait-list/no treatment 43%
 Relevant content, time-matched 31%
 Relevant content, not matched 13%
 Education-only 8%
 Irrelevant content, time-matched 6%
Active comparison conditions (k = 31)
 No. of sessions (Mdn) 1
 Total dose in minutes (Mdn) 35
Delivery mechanism
 Intranet 33%
 Face-to-face 32%
 Internet 23%
 Print materials 13%
Provided alcohol-related materials (% yes) 28%

Note. k = number of interventions; NR, not reported.

*

Multiple categories were possible

Effect Size Derivation

Effect sizes (d) were calculated as the mean difference between the CDIs and control group divided by the pooled standard deviation (SD) [15] or from other statistical information (e.g., independent t-tests) when the means and SDs were unavailable [16]. If authors reported dichotomous outcomes, we calculated an odds ratio and transformed it to d using the Cox transformation [17]. In addition, effect sizes were calculated for time-related change within each group as the mean difference between the post- and pre-test divided by the SD of the paired comparisons [18]. If statistical information could not be obtained from the authors and the study reported no significant group differences, we estimated that effect size to be zero for between-group effect sizes [16]. In calculating d, we controlled for baseline differences when pre-intervention measures were available, and effect sizes were corrected for sample size bias [19]. We calculated multiple effect sizes from individual studies when they had more than one outcome, multiple CDIs, or when outcomes were separated by sample characteristics (e.g., gender). Effect sizes calculated for each intervention and by sample characteristic were analyzed as a separate study [16]. When a study contained multiple measures of the same outcome (e.g., weekly and monthly alcohol consumption), the effect sizes were averaged. A positive sign indicated that the CDIs improved compared to controls or participants improved at post-test relative to pre-test. Two authors independently calculated effect sizes using DSTAT 2.0 [20]; discrepancies were examined for errors and corrected.

Statistical Analysis

Weighted mean effect sizes, d+, were calculated using fixed- and random-effects procedures [16], such that individual studies’ effect sizes were weighted by the inverse of their fixed- or random-effects variance. One study had an extremely large sample size relative to others [21], and these data exerted undue influence on the d+ calculations for three dependent variables (i.e., quantity of alcohol consumed, frequency of heavy drinking, alcohol-related problems at short-term). We retained the study in the analyses by recoding the unrepresentative sample size weights to be equivalent to the next smallest weight within each dependent variable (i.e., windsorizing [16]).

The homogeneity statistic, Q, was computed to determine whether each set of d+s shared a common effect size; a significant Q indicates that additional variability, beyond subject-level sampling error, can be explained [16]. In addition, the I2 index was calculated to assess the proportion of variability in a set of effect sizes attributable to true heterogeneity [22, 23]. Percentages of 25%, 50%, and 75%, are considered low, medium, and high heterogeneity, respectively [24]. If the 95% confidence interval around the I2 index includes zero, the set of effect sizes are considered homogeneous. To explain variability in the effect sizes, the relation between study characteristics and the magnitude of the effects was examined using a modified least squares regression analyses with weights equivalent to the inverse of the variance for each effect size. To examine pre to post differences between groups, we calculated the between-groups-of-studies measure, QB, which is the weighed sum of squares of group mean effect sizes about the grand mean effect size [25]. Analyzes were conducted in Stata 10.0 [26] using macros provided by Lipsey and Wilson [16].

RESULTS

Descriptive Outcomes

Study and participant characteristics, research design, and intervention and comparison condition details are reported in Appendix 1; complete descriptive statistics are reported in Appendix 2. Of the 28,621 participants, 75% were White, 68% first year students, and mean age was 19.71 (SD = 1.09). Half (54%) volunteered; 33% were recruited with targeted efforts and 14% were mandated to participate. Most studies (94%) targeted the intervention to an at-risk group; studies targeted heavy drinkers (28%), freshman (23%), high-risk drinkers (18%), violators of alcohol policy (13%), and current drinkers (13%).

APPENDIX 1.

Descriptive features of the 35 computer-delivered alcohol intervention studies included in the meta-analysis*

Intervention Details
Study N F W University Control Delivery Location Sessions Dose Program Quality Rating
Allison [47] 148 43% 91% US-NE, M public RM CD-ROM (G) Onsite 1 120 A101 3
Barnett et al. [48] 215 51% 76% US-NE, L private RM CD-ROM Onsite 2/1 70/45 A101 13
Bersamin et al. [49]
 Drinkers 139 52% 30% US-SW, L public WL/NT Internet Offsite 2.6 93.6 Other 7
 Non-Drinkers 231
Butler and Correia [50] 84 65% 92% US-SE, L public WL/NT Internet Onsite 1 11.1 Other 7
Carey et al.[43] 192 46% 91% US-NE, L private RNM CD-ROM Onsite 1 60 A101 11
Chiauzzi et al. [51] 212 54% 73% US-NE, multiple RM Internet Offsite 4 80 MSB:A 10
Dimeff & McNeely [52] 33 63% 73% US-NW, L public WL/NT CD-ROM Onsite 2 19 Other 11
Donohue et al. [53] 104 56% 63% US-SW, L public RNM CD-ROM Onsite 1 45 A101 11
Doumas et al. [54] 67 28% 86% US-NW, L public RNM Internet Onsite 1 15 Other 10
Doumas & Anderson [55] 52 41% 79% US-NW, L public WL/NT Internet Onsite 1 15 e-Chug 6
Doumas & Haustveit [56] 52 42% 54% US-NW, L public RM Internet Onsite 1 15 Other 6
Fishburne [57] 213 63% NR US-SE, L public RM Intranet Onsite 1/1 35/35 Other 10
Hunt [58] Low-level: Ppt slideshow 12
158 0 64% US, L public IM High-level: Macromedia Onsite 1 20 Other 9
Kypri et al.[59] 360 52% NR New Zealand EDUC Internet Onsite 1/3 6/18 Other 12
Kypri et al.[60] 94 50% 91% New Zealand EDUC Intranet Onsite 1 7.8 Other 12
Kypri & McAnally [61] 126 51% 75% New Zealand WL/NT Internet Onsite 1 11.8 Other 10
Lao-Barraco & Dunn [62] 103 57% 76% US-SE, L public IRM CD-ROM Onsite 1 105 A101 10
Leffingwell et al.[63] 70 22% 90% US-SW, L public WL/NT CD-ROM Onsite 1 35 Other 12
Lewis et al.[39] 230 52% 100% US-MW, L public WL/NT Intranet Onsite 1 5 Other 11
Lewis & Neighbors [64] 165 55% 97% US-NW, L public WL/NT Intranet Onsite 1 1.5 Other 11
Michael [65] 676 45% 52% US NR CD-ROM Onsite 1 120 A101 5
Mignogna [66]
 Study 1 28 58% 81% US-SW, L public EDUC Multiple Both 28 57 Other 10
 Study 2 19 16% 94% US-SW, L public WL/NT Multiple Both 37 106 Other 11
Miller [67] 445 63% 70% US-NW, L public WL/NT CD-ROM (G) Onsite 2 180 A101 13
Moore et al.[68] 106 58% 70% US-SE, L public RM Internet & Email Offsite 4 40 Other 9
Neighbors et al.[40] 207 59% 80% US-NW, L public WL/NT Intranet Onsite 1 1 Other 12
Neighbors et al.[69] 185 56% 98% US-MW, L public WL/NT Intranet Onsite 1 1.5 Other 12
Neighbors et al.[70] 282 58% 67% US-NW, L public WL/NT Internet Offsite 2 20 Other 11
Saitz et al.[71] 235 55% 81% US-NE, L private RNM Internet Offsite 1 20 Other 8
Steiner et al.[72] 159 61% NR US-SW, L public WL/NT Internet Offsite 1 25 e-Chug 9
Wall [21] 23,127 53% 82% US & Canada WL/NT Internet Unknown 1 150 AEdu 6
Walters et al. [41] 82 48% 73% US-SW, L public WL/NT Internet Unknown 1 25 e-Chug 10
Weitzel et al.[73] 40 55% 77% US-SE, L private WL/NT PED Offsite 10.93 26 Other 8
Wyrick et al.[74] 65 77% 55% US-SE, L public NR Internet Both 3 60 Other 5
*

N, number of participants. F, proportion female. W, proportion White. US = United States. NE = Northeast. SE = Southeast. MW = Midwest. SW = Southwest. S = Small. M = Medium. L = Large. WL/NT, wait-list/no treatment control. PED, portable electronic device. G, group delivery. A101, Alcohol 101 (Plus). MSB:A, My Student Body: Alcohol. e-Chug, electronic check-up to go. AEdu, Alcohol Edu. NR, none/not reported.

Estimated number of minutes of intervention content excluding measurement.

Methodological quality rating score out of a possible 17 points.

The typical intervention (k) was a single-session computerized task delivered via the internet (38%), intranet (30%), or CD-ROM/DVD (25%) lasting a median of 20 minutes. Most CDIs were delivered onsite (72%), whereas 24% of students completed the CDI offsite. Most were investigator-developed; only 41% used commercial programs. Interventions usually included consumption feedback (86%) and normative comparisons (77%), alcohol education (77%) and tailored materials (61%). The comparison condition consisted of wait-list/no treatment control for 43% of the comparisons; 51% presented alcohol-related content (e.g., alcohol education).

The median number of post-intervention assessments was 1 (range = 1 to 3); the first assessment occurred between 0 to 26 weeks, the second between 1 and 52 weeks, and the third between 2 and 52 weeks. To avoid violating the assumption of independence, effect sizes were clustered into (a) short-term (≤ 5 weeks; k = 43), and (b) long-term (≥6 weeks; k = 14).

Intervention Impact Compared with Controls

At short-term follow-up (≤ 5 weeks), students who received a CDI reduced quantity of alcohol consumed on specific intervals/drinking days and the maximum quantity consumed (see Table 1). CDI participants did not differ from controls on quantity of alcohol consumption, frequency of heavy drinking or drinking days, or alcohol-related problems (effects were parallel using fixed- and random-effects assumptions). The hypothesis of homogeneity was rejected only for alcohol-related problems; examination of the I2 index confirmed a moderate level of heterogeneity. Moderator tests were conducted to examine whether study features related to the variability (reported below).

Table 1.

Weighted mean effect sizes and homogeneity statistics for the CDIs by follow-up interval*

Weighed mean d (and 95% CI)
Homogeneity of effect sizes
Outcome k Fixed effects Random effects Q P I2 (95% CI)
Short-term Follow-up (≤5 weeks)
Quantity 20 0.07 (−0.02, 0.16) 0.09 (−0.03, 0.21) 28.71 .070 34% (0%, 61%)
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 25 0.10 (0.01, 0.20) 0.10 (0.01, 0.20) 9.49 .996 0%
Maximum quantity 7 0.16 (0.01, 0.31) 0.16 (0.01, 0.31) 3.30 .770 0%
Frequency of heavy drinking 12 0.10 (0.00, 0.20) 0.13 (−0.01, 0.27) 19.16 .058 43% (0%, 71%)
Frequency of drinking days 17 0.11 (−0.01, 0.23) 0.10 (−0.04, 0.25) 20.61 .194 23% (0%, 57%)
Problems 10 0.10 (−0.02, 0.22) 0.16 (−0.02, 0.34) 18.55 .029 51% (1%, 76%)
Long-term Follow-up (≥6 weeks)
Quantity 12 0.15 (0.05, 0.25) 0.15 (0.05, 0.25) 8.43 .674 0%
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 6 0.08 (−0.05, 0.22) 0.08 (−0.05, 0.22) 3.88 .567 0%
Maximum quantity 7 0.03 (−0.10, 0.16) 0.02 (−0.12, 0.16) 6.85 .335 12% (0%, 55%)
Frequency of heavy drinking 9 −0.10 (−0.20, 0.01) −0.06 (−0.24, 0.11) 20.98 .007 62% (21%, 82%)
Frequency of drinking days 7 0.16 (0.03, 0.29) 0.16 (0.00, 0.32) 9.28 .159 35% (0%, 73%)
Problems 11 0.16 (0.06, 0.25) 0.16 (0.06, 0.25) 6.54 .768 0%
*

CDIs, computer-delivered interventions. k, number of interventions. d, weighted standardized mean difference. CI, confidence interval. Boldface text highlights significant values.

At long-term follow-up (≥ 6 weeks) students receiving a CDI reduced their quantity of alcohol consumed, frequency of drinking days, and alcohol-related problems relative to controls. CDIs and controls were equivalent on measures of quantity at specific intervals/drinking days, maximum quantity, and frequency of heavy drinking (effects were parallel using either fixed- or random-effects assumptions). The hypothesis of homogeneity was rejected only for frequency of heavy drinking (I2 index = 62%, 95% CI 21%–82%); moderator tests were conducted on frequency of heavy drinking at long-term follow-up (reported below).

Supplemental analyses

Controls were categorized as having relevant content (i.e., alcohol-related content including education only) vs. non-relevant content (i.e., wait-list/assessment only or content unrelated to alcohol use). Alcohol-relevant content included alternative delivery (e.g., print or lecture) of the same content (21%), a face-to-face tailored comparison (29%; e.g., brief motivational interview or cognitive behavioral therapy), a non-tailored version delivered via computer (36%), or education-only (14%). Most of the non-relevant controls consisted of wait-list/assessment only conditions (94% at short-term, 100% at long-term).

Compared to participants exposed to a non-relevant control condition, CDI participants reduced their consumption (quantity and quantity at specific intervals/drinking days), frequency of heavy drinking, and frequency of drinking days in the short-term (see Table 2); at long-term they reduced quantity of consumed, and frequency of drinking days. In contrast, compared with control conditions containing alcohol-relevant content, participation in a CDI did not result in less consumption at short-term or long-term. The only comparison favoring participants in the CDIs vs. alcohol-relevant controls was problems at long-term. To explore this finding, we removed alcohol education from “relevant control” and recoded these conditions as “non-relevant control” (3 education-only comparisons), consistent with reviews that demonstrate education alone to be ineffective for changing drinking [27, 28]. After this recoding, the average effect size for CDIs vs. relevant controls at long-term for problems, k = 3, becomes non-significant (d+ = −0.01, 95% confidence interval [CI] = −0.22–0.25), and the corresponding effect size for the CDIs vs. non-relevant controls, k = 8, becomes significant (d+ = 0.20, 95% CI = 0.09–0.30). This pattern corresponds to the other outcomes at long-term follow-up. (Details of this analysis are available from the authors.)

Table 2.

Weighted mean effect sizes for the CDIs by follow-up interval and control type*

CDIs vs. Non-Relevant Controls CDIs vs. Relevant Controls

Outcome k Fixed effects d+ (95% CI) Random effects d+ (95% CI) k Fixed effects d+ (95% CI) Random effects d+ (95% CI)
Short-term Follow-up (≤ 5 weeks)
Quantity 10 0.16 (0.01, 0.31) 0.19 (−0.01, 0.38) 10 0.01 (−0.11, 0.13) 0.01 (−0.14, 0.16)
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 11 0.15 (0.01, 0.28) 0.15 (0.01, 0.28) 14 0.06 (−0.08, 0.20) 0.06 (−0.08, 0.20)
Maximum quantity 1 -- -- 6 0.15 (−0.01, 0.31) 0.15 (−0.01, 0.31)
Frequency of heavy drinking 6 0.21 (0.07, 0.35) 0.29 (0.05, 0.52) 6 −0.01 (−0.15, 0.14) −0.01 (−0.15, 0.14)
Frequency of drinking days 4 0.19 (0.01, 0.37) 0.22 (−0.08, 0.51) 13 0.06 (−0.10, 0.21) 0.06 (−0.10, 0.21)
Problems 6 0.11 (−0.03, 0.26) 0.18 (−0.04, 0.40) 4 0.07 (−0.13, 0.27) 0.14 (−0.24, 0.51)
Long-term Follow-up (≥ 6 weeks)
Quantity 8 0.20 (0.08, 0.31) 0.20 (0.08, 0.31) 4 0.02 (−0.18, 0.22) 0.02 (−0.18, 0.22)
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 1 -- -- 5 0.03 (−0.12, 0.18) 0.03 (−0.12, 0.18)
Maximum quantity 4 0.09 (−0.06, 0.24) 0.08 (−0.07, 0.25) 3 0.14 (−0.40, 0.12) 0.14 (−0.40, 0.12)
Frequency of heavy drinking 1 8 −0.11 (−0.22, 0.01) −0.07 (−0.27, 0.13)
Frequency of drinking days 3 0.28 (0.10, 0.46) 0.28 (0.10, 0.46) 4 0.03 (−0.16, 0.22) 0.04 (−0.18, 0.26)
Problems 5 0.14 (−0.01, 0.28) 0.14 (−0.01, 0.28) 6 0.17 (0.05, 0.30) 0.16 (0.03, 0.30)
*

Non-relevant controls refer to wait-list/assessment only (94% at short-term, 100% at long-term) and irrelevant content unmatched for time (6% at short-term). Relevant controls refer to comparisons with alcohol-related content matched (68% at short-term, 70% at long-term) or unmatched (30% at short-term, 11% at long-term) for time and alcohol-related education (2% at short-term, 20% at long-term). CDIs, computer-delivered interventions. k, number of interventions. d+, weighted standardized mean difference. CI, confidence interval.

Heterogeneous distribution, Ps <.05.

Moderators of Alcohol Consumption at Short- and Long-term Assessment

Potential moderators of intervention impact were examined using weighted regression analyses. Characteristics of the studies (e.g., published vs. unpublished), samples (e.g., gender), and intervention (e.g., length) as well as study quality were entered as predictors of alcohol consumption variables (see Appendix 2 for study, sample, and intervention characteristics used in the analyses). We examined the intervention impact of alcohol-related problems at short-term and frequency of heavy drinking at long-term compared with any type of control condition (i.e., both non-relevant and relevant comparisons are included).

Alcohol-related problems at short-term assessment

CDIs were more successful when the study was published earlier (β = −0.61, p =.01; QResidual (1) = 5.95, P = .01), did not use a commercially available program (β = −0.47, p =.04; QResidual (1) = 4.15, P = .04), included human interaction vs. using the computer alone (β = −0.53, p =.02; QResidual (1) = 5.15, P = .02), and provided general alcohol-related materials (e.g., brochures) to participants (β = 0.53, p =.02; QResidual (1) = 5.15, P = .02).

Frequency of heavy drinking at long-term assessment

CDIs were more successful at reducing heavy drinking frequency at long-term when they were published earlier (β = −0.77, P <.001; QResidual (1) = 12.29, P < .001), included fewer students (β = −0.93, P <.001; QResidual (1) = 18.15, P < .001), and did not provide feedback on alcohol-related problems (β = −0.63, P <.01; QResidual (1) = 8.21, P < .01).

Within-Group Changes Over Time

As shown in Table 3, exposure to a CDI reduced all measured outcomes relative to baseline scores at short-term follow-up (≤ 5 weeks); within-group effect sizes ranged from 0.14 to 0.32. Again, the comparison groups were stratified by relevancy. Participants receiving a non-relevant control tended not to change over time, reducing only their quantity of drinking on specific intervals/drinking days (d+ = 0.22) while increasing their frequency of heavy drinking (d+ = −0.17). In contrast, participants exposed to a relevant control condition showed significant improvement on all outcomes (d+s = 0.19 to 0.34) at short-term.

Table 3.

Efficacy of interventions and controls from pre- to post-test by follow-up interval and control type*

Interventions
Non-Relevant Controls
Relevant Controls
Outcome k d+ (95% CI) k d+ (95% CI) k d+ (95% CI)
Short-term Follow-up (≤ 5 weeks)
Quantity 14 0.17 (0.09, 0.26) 4 0.02 (−0.14, 0.19) 10 0.26 (0.16, 0.35)
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 17 0.32 (0.23, 0.42) 3 0.22 (0.01, 0.44)* 9 0.26 (0.14, 0.38)
Maximum quantity 7 0.26 (0.15, 0.37) 1 -- 6 0.21 (0.10, 0.33)
Frequency of heavy drinking 12 0.14 (0.04, 0.16) 5 0.17 (0.28,0.05) 6 0.28 (0.16, 0.39)
Frequency of drinking days 16 0.17 (0.07, 0.27) 2 0.12 (−0.17, 0.42)* 9 0.19 (0.07, 0.32)
Problems 9 0.14 (0.04, 0.23) 4 0.24 (0.07,0.38) 4 0.34 (0.17, 0.50)
Long-term Follow-up (≥ 6 weeks)
Quantity 8 0.32 (0.21, 0.42) 3 0.05 (−0.09, 0.19) 5 0.33 (0.19, 0.46)
Quantity, specific intervals/drinking days 6 0.08 (−0.02, 0.18) 1 -- 4 0.27 (0.12, 0.42)
Maximum quantity 6 0.22 (0.11, 0.33) 3 0.01 (−0.12, 0.13) 3 0.69 (0.46, 0.91)
Frequency of heavy drinking 5 0.26 (0.13, 0.39) 0 -- 4 0.27 (0.13, 0.42)
Frequency of drinking days 5 0.09 (−0.02, 0.21) 1 -- 3 0.29 (0.12, 0.46)
Problems 5 0.10 (−0.01, 0.20) 3 −0.03 (−0.15, 0.10)* 2 0.09 (−0.09, 0.28)
*

Non-relevant controls refer to wait-list/assessment only. Relevant controls refer to comparisons with alcohol-related content matched (31% at short-term and 100% at long-term) or unmatched (69% at short-term) for time. Weighted mean effect sizes, d+, for which the 95% CI does not include a zero are significant. Effect sizes are based on fixed-effects assumptions; for most outcomes, parallel results were found using random-effects assumptions with the exception of quantity at specific intervals/drinking days for the intervention group, maximum quantity of alcohol consumption, and frequency of heavy drinking, all at long-term follow-up. CDIs, computer-delivered interventions. k, number of interventions. d+, weighted standardized mean difference. CI, confidence interval. Boldface text highlights significant values.

Comparisons between intervention and control type (between-groups Q) is statistically significant at P <.05.

To examine variation in improvement across the CDIs and control groups, we calculated QB. Compared to non-relevant controls, CDI participants reduced quantity consumed (mean = 0.17, SE = 0.04 vs. mean = 0.02, SE = 0.08), frequency of heavy drinking (mean = 0.14, SE = 0.04 vs. mean = −0.17, SE = 0.06), and alcohol-related problems (mean = 0.14, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = −0.24, SE = 0.07), QB = 4.22, 24.00, and 24.94 respectively, Ps ≤ 04. Compared to relevant controls, CDIs were less successful in reducing frequency of heavy drinking (mean = 0.14, SE = 0.04 vs. mean = 0.28, SE = 0.06) and alcohol-related problems (mean = 0.14, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = 0.34, SE = 0.08), QB = 4.94 and 4.42 respectively, Ps <.04.

At long-term follow-up (≥ 6 weeks), receiving a CDI reduced quantity of alcohol consumed, maximum quantity, and frequency of heavy drinking relative to baseline; within-group effect sizes ranged from 0.22 to 0.32. CDI participants did not reduce quantity consumed on specific intervals/drinking days, frequency of drinking, or alcohol-related problems at long-term follow-up. No significant long-term effects were found among participants exposed to a non-relevant control; however, participants in relevant comparison groups reported reductions on all measures of alcohol consumption (d+s = 0.27 to 0.69) except for alcohol-related problems (d+ = 0.09) at long-term.

The between-groups test (QB) again revealed differential improvement at long-term follow-up. Compared with non-relevant controls, CDIs produced greater reductions in quantity (mean = 0.32, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = 0.05, SE = 0.07) and maximum quantity (mean = 0.22, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = 0.01, SE = 0.06), QB = 12.01 and 7.62 respectively, Ps <.01. Compared with relevant controls, CDIs produced less change in quantity at specific intervals/drinking days (mean = 0.08, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = 0.27, SE = 0.07), maximum quantity (mean = 0.22, SE = 0.05 vs. mean = 0.69, SE = 0.11), and frequency of drinking days (mean = 0.09, SE = 0.06 vs. mean = 0.29, SE = 0.08), QB = 5.06, 21.77, and 4.42 respectively, Ps ≤ .04.

DISCUSSION

This meta-analysis summarizes the efficacy of CDIs for college students, across alcohol use outcomes and assessment intervals. We presented both between- and within-group effect sizes to elucidate change resulting from CDIs. Overall, results provide qualified support for the efficacy of CDIs to reduce alcohol use and problems in college students.

CDIs are associated with improvement over time, and produce greater risk reduction than no intervention. Relative to assessment-only controls, CDIs reduced both quantity and frequency measures of consumption; the observed effects are small (0.09 to 0.28) over short- and long-term intervals [15]. In comparison, CDIs evaluated in other populations yield an average effect size of 0.24 at first measurement occasion [29]. Thus, CDIs targeted to students provide effects (relative to no treatment) similar to those found in the general population.

Improvements over time varied across outcomes. Between-groups effects on specific consumption outcomes (quantity per drinking day and/or specific drinking occasions and maximum quantity) were observed at short-term (≤ 5 weeks), but did not persist at the long-term assessment. In contrast, reduction in quantity consumed over a period of time (e.g., weeks, months) was observed only at long-term assessment. Consistent with the Transtheoretical Model [30], delayed reductions in alcohol consumption are likely due to a person’s readiness (and ability) to make necessary changes. Thus, reduction in average quantity consumed may occur incrementally whereas consumption patterns occurring less frequently (e.g., holidays) may be more amenable to immediate change. With respect to alcohol-related problems, CDIs were efficacious at improving long- but not short-term problems, consistent with prior research examining controlled alcohol interventions [6]. The lower base rate of problems (relative to drinking occasions) may require more time for reductions in problems to be observed.

Evidence for the efficacy of CDIs depends upon the comparison condition. Relative to CDI vs. no-treatment controls, CDI vs. active comparisons yielded smaller effects [cf, 31]. In this study, relevant comparison conditions varied with regard to content and intensity; in some cases, the comparison conditions were empirically-validated, counselor-delivered interventions. The small number of comparisons with specific forms of alternative interventions prevents conclusions about the efficacy of CDIs to relative to specific active interventions but supports the conclusion that CDIs did not differ from these alcohol-focused interventions.

Within-group effect sizes afford stronger conclusions: Whereas true controls tended not to change over time, CDI recipients reduced risk over time. Improvement is more consistent at short-term assessments. Research is needed to explain the inconsistent findings at long-term. Not surprisingly, participation in other active alcohol interventions was associated with improvement. In a review of web-based interventions (not restricted to college students), Bewick and colleagues [33] reported a similar pattern: equivalent change over time for both web and alternative intervention conditions. Comprehensive reviews of alcohol interventions reveal that drinking is responsive to motivational [32] and other brief interventions [28]; if they produce equivalent outcomes, CDIs offer a cost-effective alternative to counselor-delivered interventions.

Despite the variability in the CDIs we sampled, few outcomes met formal tests of heterogeneity with enough studies to support moderator analyses. Furthermore, we evaluated many features of the samples, designs, and interventions, but few explained variability in effect sizes. Relative to alternatives, CDIs produced stronger effects in earlier publications, in studies with fewer participants, and when a commercial program was not used. These trends suggest as evaluation trials become larger and greater dissemination is achieved, the likelihood of detecting differences between CDIs and alternatives is reduced (consistent with the transition from efficacy to effectiveness [34]).

Limitations of the Review

Too few studies were available to allow strong inferences regarding (a) trends over time, (b) efficacy of specific CDIs, and (c) moderators of alcohol consumption or problems. First, most studies in the meta-analysis included only one post-intervention assessment, and fewer evaluated outcomes beyond 6 weeks. Thus, fewer comparisons were available for longer follow-ups, limiting opportunities to assess the stability of change. Moreover, variability in the length of follow-ups (i.e., 6 to 52 weeks) may have influenced our findings, making inferences more challenging. That is, it is unclear whether these improvements persist or represent more intermediate change. A prior meta-analytic review of alcohol interventions found reductions in consumption until 6 months whereas improvements in alcohol-related problems persisted longer [6]. Second, CDI refers to delivery mode but other features (e.g., content, structure, interactivity) may be important and varied across studies. The limited comparisons available on any dimension preclude further evaluation of the unique relationship of individual components to efficacy. Finally, the small number of studies did not support multivariate predictor models that would allow evaluation of interactions among moderators.

Future Research

Future research might address issues of study design, optimizing efficacy, and understanding mechanisms of delivery. First, it will be informative to evaluate maintenance of gains associated with CDIs more systematically; studies should include more and longer follow-ups. Drinking varies over the academic year as a function of academic demands and breaks [36], so more assessment occasions will provide greater power to detect change. Second, systematic comparisons of CDIs to alternatives that vary in intensity (e.g., brief vs. intensive face-to-face) and cost would be informative. Such data can provide a clearer picture of the added value of CDIs over alternative interventions. Such comparisons will allow incremental cost-effectiveness analyses (see [37, 38]) to supplement traditional outcome analyses. Third, research evaluating CDIs should identify the components that account for observed outcomes. Several studies provide evidence that reductions in perceived drinking norms mediate the effects of feedback-based CDIs (e.g., [3941]). As recently suggested, computer tailoring technologies range from personalized health assessments with feedback to CDIs in which content is individualized based on complex algorithms [42]. Research on CDIs for college student drinkers might benefit from interventions developed for other health behaviors.

Fourth, studies might be designed to match individuals with intervention modalities. For example, a recent study revealed that females respond more favorably to the counselor-administered intervention whereas males respond equivalently to computer- and counselor-administered interventions [43]. In addition, preference for CDIs and cognitive learning styles might be studied to optimize web-based learning tools [44]. Alternately, low levels of motivation might favor face-to-face interventions, as certain counseling styles can reduce resistance and enhance outcomes [45, 46].

Finally, as the number of evaluation studies increase, meta-analyses will be able to evaluate the delivery mechanisms that moderate the impact of CDIs on alcohol consumption and problems such as on-site vs. off-site, CD-ROM vs. Internet, and single vs. multiple computer-delivered modes. For example, on-site delivery may result in greater intervention compliance than remote delivery.

CONCLUSIONS

CDIs produce significant change over time on measures of quantity and frequency of drinking in college samples. Clear patterns of improvement emerged at short-term (≤ 5 weeks); estimates of long-term improvement are qualified by fewer studies on which to base effect sizes. CDIs are clearly preferable to no intervention, and they are generally equivalent to alternative alcohol-related interventions. Once developed, CDIs are available for modest (or no) cost, providing researchers access to standardized alcohol-related interventions. Despite the availability of CDIs, few standardized programs have undergone rigorous evaluation. Policy decisions will benefit from systematic comparisons of CDIs (commercially available and newly developed) against alternate interventions of varying intensity, determination of active ingredients of CDIs, and attention to person-by-intervention interactions.

Acknowledgments

We thank the following study authors for providing manuscripts and/or additional intervention or statistical information: Nancy P. Barnett, PhD, Christopher J. Correia, PhD, Diana M. Doumas, PhD, William M. Hunt, PhD, Kypros Kypri, PhD, James E. Lange, PhD, Thad R. Leffingwell, PhD, Melissa A. Lewis, PhD, Michelle J. Moore, PhD, Clayton Neighbors, PhD, and Richard Saitz, MD.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: This work was supported by National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Grants K02-AA15574 and R01-AA12518 to Kate B. Carey. The authors have no conflict of interest related to this research.

References

  • 1.Wechsler H, Lee JE, Nelson TF, Kuo M. Underage college students' drinking behavior, access to alcohol, and the influence of deterrence policies. Findings from the Harvard School of Public Health College Alcohol Study. J Am Coll Health. 2002;50:223–36. doi: 10.1080/07448480209595714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. The Surgeon General's Call to Action To Prevent and Reduce Underage Drinking. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Office of the Surgeon General; 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.National Research Council & Institute of Medicine. Reducing underage drinking: A collective responsibility. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2004. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse. Wasting the Best and the Brightest: Substance Abuse at America's Colleges and Universities. New York: Columbia University; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Health People 2010. Washington, DC: Government Printing Office; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Carey KB, Scott-Sheldon LA, Carey MP, DeMartini KS. Individual-level interventions to reduce college student drinking: a meta-analytic review. Addict Behav. 2007;32:2469–94. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Larimer ME, Cronce JM, Lee CM, Kilmer JR. Brief intervention in college settings. Alcohol Res Health. 2004;28:94–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Glasgow RE, Vogt TM, Boles SM. Evaluating the public health impact of health promotion interventions: the RE-AIM framework. Am J Public Health. 1999;89:1322–7. doi: 10.2105/ajph.89.9.1322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Walters ST, Miller E, Chiauzzi E. Wired for wellness: e-interventions for addressing college drinking. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2005;29:139–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Elliott JC, Carey KB, Bolles JR. Computer-based interventions for college drinking: a qualitative review. Addict Behav. 2008;33:994–1005. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.03.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rosenthal R. The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychological Bulletin. 1979;86:638–641. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wechsler H, Davenport A, Dowdall G, Moeykens B, Rimm EB. A gender-specific measure of binge drinking among college students. American Journal of Public Health. 1995;85:982–985. doi: 10.2105/ajph.85.7.982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Jadad AR, Moore RA, Carroll D, et al. Assessing the quality of reports of randomized clinical trials: is blinding necessary? Control Clin Trials. 1996;17:1–12. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(95)00134-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Miller WR, Brown JM, Simpson TL, et al. Handbook of alcoholism treatment approaches: Effective alternatives. 2. Needham Heights, MA US: Allyn & Bacon; 1995. What works? A methodological analysis of the alcohol treatment outcome literature ; pp. 12–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Sanchez-Meca J, Marin-Martinez F, Chacon-Moscoso S. Effect-size indices for dichotomized outcomes in meta-analysis. Psychol Methods. 2003;8:448–67. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Johnson BT, Eagly AH, Reis HT, Judd CM. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. New York, NY US: Cambridge University Press; 2000. Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research ; pp. 496–528. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hedges LV. Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educ Stat. 1981;6:107–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Johnson BT, Wood T. DSTAT 2.0: Software for Meta-Analysis. 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wall A. Evaluating a health education web site: The case of AlcoholEdu. NASPA Journal. 2007;44:692–714. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Huedo-Medina TB, Sanchez-Meca J, Marin-Martinez F, Botella J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol Methods. 2006;11:193–206. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.2.193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj. 2003;327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Hedges LV, Olkin L. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.StataCorp. Stata. College Station, TX: StataCorp; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Larimer ME, Cronce JM. Identification, prevention, and treatment revisited: individual-focused college drinking prevention strategies 1999–2006. Addict Behav. 2007;32:2439–68. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Miller WR, Wilbourne PL. Mesa Grande: a methodological analysis of clinical trials of treatments for alcohol use disorders. Addiction. 2002;97:265–77. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Portnoy DB, Scott-Sheldon LA, Johnson BT, Carey MP. Computer-delivered interventions for health promotion and behavioral risk reduction: a meta-analysis of 75 randomized controlled trials, 1988–2007. Prev Med. 2008;47:3–16. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Prochaska JO, Velicer WF. The transtheoretical model of health behavior change. Am J Health Promot. 1997;12:38–48. doi: 10.4278/0890-1171-12.1.38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Grissom RJ. The magical number .7 +/−. 2: meta-meta-analysis of the probability of superior outcome in comparisons involving therapy, placebo, and control. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1996;64:973–82. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.64.5.973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hettema J, Steele J, Miller WR. Motivational interviewing. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005;1:91–111. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bewick BM, Trusler K, Barkham M, et al. The effectiveness of web-based interventions designed to decrease alcohol consumption--a systematic review. Prev Med. 2008;47:17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hallfors D, Cho H, Sanchez V, et al. Efficacy vs effectiveness trial results of an indicated “model” substance abuse program: implications for public health. Am J Public Health. 2006;96:2254–9. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.067462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Petty RE, Cacioppo JT. The elaboration likelihood model of persusion. In: Berkowitz L, editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1986. pp. 123–205. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal: temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2004;72:155–64. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Olmstead TA, Sindelar JL, Easton CJ, Carroll KM. The cost-effectiveness of four treatments for marijuana dependence. Addiction. 2007;102:1443–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01909.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Pinkerton SD, Holtgrave DR, Johnson-Masotti AP, et al. Cost-Effectiveness of the NIMH Multisite HIV Prevention Intervention. AIDS and Behavior. 2002;6:83–96. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lewis MA, Neighbors C, Oster-Aaland L, Kirkeby BS, Larimer ME. Indicated prevention for incoming freshmen: Personalized normative feedback and high-risk drinking. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2495–2508. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Lewis MA. Targeting Misperceptions of Descriptive Drinking Norms: Efficacy of a Computer-Delivered Personalized Normative Feedback Intervention. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:434–447. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Walters ST, Vader AM, Harris TR. A controlled trial of web-based feedback for heavy drinking college students. Prev Sci. 2007;8:83–8. doi: 10.1007/s11121-006-0059-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Lustria ML, Cortese J, Noar SM, Glueckauf RL. Computer-tailored health interventions delivered over the web: Review and analysis of key components. Patient Educ Couns. 2009;74:156–73. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Carey KB, Henson JM, Carey MP, Maisto SA. Computer versus in-person intervention for students violating campus alcohol policy. :45. doi: 10.1037/a0014281. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cook DA. Learning and cognitive styles in web-based learning: theory, evidence, and application. Acad Med. 2005;80:266–78. doi: 10.1097/00001888-200503000-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Miller M. Distance guidance: Career advice by post. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling. 1993;21:221–229. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Moyers TB, Martin T. Therapist influence on client language during motivational interviewing sessions. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2006;30:245–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2005.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Allison DL. Comparable effects of the use of a computer-assisted, facilitative teaching approach to a traditional, lecture teaching approach on college students' learning about and subsequent use of alcohol. Texas A&M University; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Barnett NP, Murphy JG, Colby SM, Monti PM. Efficacy of counselor vs. computer-delivered intervention with mandated college students. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2529–2548. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bersamin M, Paschall MJ, Fearnow-Kenney M, Wyrick D. Effectiveness of a web-based alcohol-misuse and harm-prevention course among high- and low-risk students. Journal of American College Health. 2007;55:247–254. doi: 10.3200/JACH.55.4.247-254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Butler LH, Correia CJ. Brief alcohol intervention with college student drinkers: Face-to-face versus computerized feedback. Auburn, Alabama: Auburn University; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Chiauzzi E, Green TC, Lord S, Thum C, Goldstein M. My student body: a high-risk drinking prevention web site for college students. J Am Coll Health. 2005;53:263–74. doi: 10.3200/JACH.53.6.263-274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Dimeff LA, McNeely M. Computer-enhanced primary care practitioner advice for high-risk college drinkers in a student primary health-care setting. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice. 2000;7:82–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Donohue B, Allen DN, Maurer A, Ozols J, DeStefano G. A Controlled Evaluation of Two Prevention Programs in Reducing Alcohol Use Among College Students at Low and High Risk for Alcohol Related Problems. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2004;48:13–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Doumas DM, McKinley LL, Book P. Evaluation of a two web-based alcohol interventions for mandated college students. Boise, ID: Boise State University; 2008. p. 34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Doumas DM, Anderson LL. Reducing alcohol use in college freshman: Evaluation of a web-based personalized feedback program. Boise, ID: Boise State University; 2008. p. 38. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Doumas DM, Haustveit T. Reducing heavy drinking in intercollegiate athletes: Evaluation of a web-based personalized feedback program. The Sports Psychologist 2008 [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Fishburne JW. Increasing the self-referencing in social norm interventions. University of Arkansas; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Hunt WM. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering. US: ProQuest Information & Learning; 2005. Effects of participant engagement on alcohol expectancies and drinking outcomes for a computerized expectancy challenge intervention ; pp. 6655–6655. [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kypri K, Langley JD, Saunders JB, Cashell-Smith ML, Herbison P. Randomized controlled trial of web-based alcohol screening and brief intervention in primary care. Arch Intern Med. 2008;168:530–6. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2007.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kypri K, Saunders JB, Williams SM, et al. Web-based screening and brief intervention for hazardous drinking: a double-blind randomized controlled trial. Addiction. 2004;99:1410–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00847.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kypri K, McAnally HM. Randomized controlled trial of a web-based primary care intervention for multiple health risk behaviors. Preventive Medicine. 2005;41:761–766. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lau-Barraco C, Dunn ME. Evaluation of a single-session expectancy challenge intervention to reduce alcohol use among college students. Psychol Addict Behav. 2008;22:168–75. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.2.168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Leffingwell TR, Leedy MJ, Lack CW. Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies (ABCT) Washington, DC: 2005. A multimedia computer-based intervention for college student drinking: Short-term outcomes of a randomized trial. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Lewis MA, Neighbors C. Optimizing Personalized Normative Feedback: The Use of Gender-Specific Referents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:228–237. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Michael ME. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering. US: ProQuest Information & Learning; 2000. Attitudes and perceived behavioral control of first-year college student's alcohol use: A study of an instructional software intervention ; pp. 2495–2495. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Mignogna J. Ru drkn 2nite? Cellular text messaging prompts for alcohol harm-reduction on campus. Oklahoma: Oklahoma State University; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Miller ET. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering. US: ProQuest Information & Learning; 2000. Preventing alcohol abuse and alcohol-related negative consequences among freshmen college students: Using emerging computer technology to deliver and evaluate the effectiveness of brief intervention efforts ; pp. 4417–4417. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Moore MJ, Soderquist J, Werch C. Feasibility and Efficacy of a Binge Drinking Prevention Intervention for College Students Delivered via the Internet Versus Postal Mail. Journal of American College Health. 2005;54:38–44. doi: 10.3200/JACH.54.1.38-44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Neighbors C, Lewis MA, Bergstrom RL, Larimer ME. Being Controlled by Normative Influences: Self-Determination as a Moderator of a Normative Feedback Alcohol Intervention. Health Psychology. 2006;25:571–579. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Neighbors C, Lee CM, Lewis MA, Fossos N, Walter T. Internet-based personalized feedback to reduce 21st birthday drinking: A randomized controlled trial of an event specific prevention intervention. Vol. 49. Seattle: University of Washington; 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Saitz R, Palfai TP, Freedner N, et al. Screening and brief intervention online for college students: The iHealth Study. Alcohol and Alcoholism. 2007;42:28–36. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agl092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Steiner J, Woodall WG, Yeagley JA. The E-Chug: A randomized, controlled study of a web-based binge drinking intervention with college freshman. Paper presented at the Society for Prevention Research; Washington, DC. 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Weitzel JA, Bernhardt JM, Usdan S, Mays D, Glanz K. Using wireless handheld computers and tailored text messaging to reduce negative consequence of drinking alcohol. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs. 2007;68:534–537. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2007.68.534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Wyrick DL, Fearnow-Kenney MD, Wyrick CH, Bibeau DL. College alcohol education and prevention: A case for distance education. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education. 2005;49:5–14. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES