Abstract
Objectives
To examine how prenatal drug exposure (PDE) and the caregiving environment relate to cognitive, academic, and behavioral performance at ages 6 and 7.
Methods
A longitudinal follow-up was conducted of 111 children with PDE and a community cohort (CC) of 62 non-drug-exposed children. Children completed standardized tests of cognition (Stanford-Binet IV, SB-IV) and academic performance (Wide Range Achievement Test-3, WRAT-3). Caregivers completed ratings of child behavior problems (Child Behavior Checklist, CBCL).
Analysis
Multivariate analyses were conducted adjusting for gender, prenatal tobacco exposure, number of caregiver placement changes, and three caregiver variables assessed at age 7 including depressive symptoms, employment status, and public assistance status.
Results
After adjusting for perinatal and environmental variables, there were no significant exposure group differences in cognition, academic performance, or behavior problems. In comparison with males, females had higher scores on overall IQ and 4 of 8 SB-IV subtests, fewer caregiver-reported attention and aggression problems, and higher reading achievement scores. There were no significant gender by group interactions.
Conclusion
When analyses were adjusted for perinatal and environmental variables, most associations between PDE and cognitive-behavioral functioning were attenuated. Regardless of drug exposure history, males performed more poorly than females on multiple cognitive-behavioral indices. Both exposed and non-exposed children were from low-income families and obtained scores substantially below normative expectations, demonstrating the insidious effects of poverty on children’s development.
Keywords: prenatal drug exposure, school age children, gender, low-income
Drug abuse among women of child-bearing age is a serious public health problem. Most of the research on the effects of prenatal drug exposure (PDE) has been conducted among young children; findings on performance during the school-age years have been mixed. Some investigators have found no associations between PDE and cognitive performance,1,2 play behavior,3 academic achievement,2,4 attention, or teacher-rated classroom behavior.2 In contrast, others have found associations between PDE and behavior problems,1,5 symptoms of oppositional defiant disorder and ADHD,6 aggression,7 task persistence and attention problems,8,9 and language performance.10 The inconsistent findings may be partially attributed to methodological inconsistencies and to failure to control for confounders, ranging from prenatal tobacco and alcohol exposure,11–14 to parental and family variables, such as mental health, education, intelligence, and income. To ensure that variables contributing to children’s functioning were identified and controlled, this investigation was guided by developmental-ecological theory,15 utilizing a bidirectional model whereby children are influenced by their proximal environment, including their family, peers, and schools, and in turn, impact their proximal environment through their behavior.
Investigators have reported that effects of PDE on children’s cognitive and behavioral functioning are moderated by gender, with males displaying more adverse 67 behavioral and academic outcomes than females. 7,16–19 Males are typically exposed to more violence than females, and socially approved male role models are often aggressive, suggesting that social learning may exert an influence in the development of behavioral difficulties.20 Neuroimaging studies have described gender-specific differences in children’s brain development thought to be guided by genetic and hormonal changes as early as the second trimester.21 Differences include overall volumetrics, right > left frontal asymmetry, and white and gray matter ratios.22, 23 These differences may explain the lag that males experience in verbal development and their risk for language-related learning disabilities.24
This study examines children with confirmed PDE, defined by positive toxicology and self-report of frequent use, and a non-exposed group of children from the same community. We hypothesize that children with PDE have worse scores on scales assessing cognitive, academic, and behavioral performance compared to children with no exposure. We also hypothesize that males are more vulnerable to the negative effects of PDE than females, as evidenced by worse scores.
METHODS
Study Design and Participants
The participants were part of a longitudinal randomized, controlled trial of a home-based intervention among drug-using women and their infants. Recruitment procedures and the home intervention protocol have been reported previously.25 Women were recruited from a University Hospital that serves a largely inner city, African American population. Eligibility criteria included positive maternal or infant urine toxicology screen at delivery or history of substance abuse, gestational age ≥ 32 weeks, birth weight ≥ 1750 grams, and no congenital or medical problems requiring admission to the neonatal intensive care unit. These criteria were imposed so we could evaluate the impact of an early intervention program on children’s development without considerations imposed by severe intrauterine growth restriction, congenital problems, or the need for other interventions. Recruitment began in 1991 and continued for 30 months. Women with a history of drug use were approached shortly after delivery; 265 completed the baseline evaluation 2 weeks post-delivery and were randomized into intervention or control groups. The intervention group received biweekly developmentally oriented home visits by a community-experienced outreach worker for one year, based on the Infant Health and Development Program.26 The control group received brief monthly tracking visits. Mothers and children were followed for evaluation visits at regular intervals. Data were collected by research assistants blind to intervention status. Mothers were paid for evaluation visits..
At age 7 years, 128 children (48.3%) were available for assessment. Causes of attrition were: death 8 (3.0%), foster care placement 37 (14.0 %), moved out of state or family withdrew 9 (3.3 %), and noncompliance 83 (31.3%). Women lost to follow-up were younger than women who were retained (26.2 vs 27. 7 years, p=.01). There were no differences in neonatal characteristics, maternal drug use, urine toxicology, or other demographic variables.
To ensure that children in the PDE group were exposed to illegal substances prenatally, we used self-report and toxicology screens. Children were assigned to the PDE group if their mother admitted using cocaine and/or heroin at least twice/week for the final six months of pregnancy or if they or their mother had a positive toxicology screen for heroin or cocaine. Children of mothers who reported infrequent drug use throughout pregnancy and who did not have a positive toxicology screen were excluded from analyses (n=15). Two children within the PDE group were HIV-infected and were excluded. Of the final eligible sample of 111 children with a history of PDE, 12.5% did not have a positive toxicology screen for cocaine or heroin, but admitted to frequent use during pregnancy. Of those with positive toxicology screens: 33.3% were positive for cocaine only, 16.2% were positive for heroin only, 50.5% were positive for both cocaine and heroin (Table 1).
Table 1.
Drug-Exposure Group | Gender | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neonatal Characteristics | Prenatally Drug- Exposed (n=111) | Community Cohort (n=62) | p-value* | Male (n=78) | Female (n=95) | p-value* |
Birth Weight (g)+ | 2780 (400) | 3380 (570) | <.001 | 2990 (540) | 2930 (530) | .49 |
Length (cm)+ | 48.0 (2.4) | 50.4 (2.6) | <.001 | 48.9 (2.5) | 48.6 (2.9) | .48 |
Head circumference (cm)+ | 32.7 (1.4) | 34.5 (1.6) | <.001 | 33.4 (1.6) | 33.1 (1.7) | .28 |
Weight for gestational age (z-score) + | −.89 (.7) | −.03 (.9) | <.001 | −.56 (.9) | −.61 (.9) | .74 |
Prenatal drug exposure type | ||||||
No prenatal drug exposure (%) | -- | 100% | 37.2% | 34.7% | ||
Cocaine only (%) | 33.3% | -- | -- | 19.2% | 23.2% | .55 |
Heroin only (%) | 16.2% | -- | 15.4% | 6.3% | ||
Cocaine and Heroin (%) | 50.5% | -- | 28.2% | 35.8% | ||
Tobacco use during pregnancy (%) | 84.7% | 27.4% | <.001 | 64% | 65% | .90 |
Alcohol use during pregnancy (%) | 42.3% | 30.6% | .14 | 36% | 40% | .67 |
| ||||||
Caregiver Characteristics | ||||||
| ||||||
Mother’s age at 1st pregnancy + | 18.6 (4.0) | 19.5 (4.2) | .30 | 19.0 (4.6) | 18.1 (3.6) | .24 |
Caregiver age+ | 40.3 (9.9) | 31.0 (5.5) | <.001 | 36.2 (8.0) | 37.6 (10.9) | .36 |
Primary caregiver % | ||||||
Birth mother | 54.9% | 100.0 % | 70.9% | 70.8% | ||
Non-maternal relative care | 44.2% | 0% | <.001 | 29.1% | 28.5% | .46 |
Non-relative care | 0.9% | 0% | 0% | 0.6% | ||
Caregiver education+ | 11.4 (1.6) | 11.7 (1.1) | .19 | 11.5 (1.5) | 11.5 (1.4) | .89 |
Caregiver K-BIT composite score+ | 81.2 (12.3) | 81.7 (10.5) | .78 | 83.0 (10.5) | 80.6 (12.5) | .11 |
Caregiver depressive symptoms (CESD) + | 11.7 (9.8) | 12.9 (9.9) | .48 | 12.0 (9.1) | 12.2 (10.4) | .89 |
Caregiver married, % | 11.5% | 19.4% | .18 | 16.5% | 12.5% | .52 |
Caregiver public assistance, % | 55.8% | 45.2% | .21 | 54.4% | 50.0% | .65 |
At least one caregiver employed, % | 65.5% | 91.9% | <.001 | 78.5% | 71.9% | .38 |
Number of caregiver changes (birth to age 7)+ | 1.1 (1.1) | .03 (.2) | <.001 | .7 (1.0) | .7 (1.1) | .96 |
Ongoing alcohol use, % | 61.9% | 59.7% | .24 | 67.1% | 56.3% | .16 |
Ongoing tobacco use, % | 71.7% | 33.9% | <.001 | 57.0% | 59.4% | .76 |
Ongoing cocaine/heroin use, % | 34.5% | 1.6% | <.001 | 29.1% | 17.7% | .10 |
Note. Mean and standard deviation
p-values are for t-statistics when variable is continuous and the chi-square statistic when variable is categorical
Participants in a non-exposed Community Cohort (CC) group served as a community standard for comparison. They were recruited from the University primary care clinic when they were 5 years-old. Records were reviewed to identify children who had been born in the University Hospital, both the mother and infant had negative toxicology screens (administered routinely at all deliveries), and had no history of drug use. We identified 120 eligible participants and 70 (58%) enrolled. There were no differences in demographic characteristics between those who did and did not enroll. Participants resided in the same community as participants from the PDE group and were matched for socio-economic status, age of first pregnancy, and race. Sixty-two of the 70 children (89%) from the CC group who were assessed at age seven.
Children in the PDE group had significantly lower birth weight, length and head circumference, had more neonatal problems, and stayed longer in the hospital compared to the CC group (Table 1). The groups differed in exposure to both illicit (e.g., heroin, cocaine) and legal substances (e.g., tobacco).
Caregivers in the PDE group were significantly older than caregivers in the CC group, although there was no difference in age at 1st pregnancy, caregiver education, or caregiver IQ (Table 1). In comparison with mothers in the CC group, caregivers in the PDE group were less likely to be employed, less likely to be married, and more likely to receive public cash assistance. Though groups did not differ in reported current alcohol use, caregivers in the CC group reported lower rates of current tobacco and cocaine/heroin use. At age 7, 45% of the PDE children were living with non-maternal caregivers (Table 1); all CC children resided with biological mothers.
Child Measures
Children’s cognitive, academic, and behavioral performance were measured by standardized scales with excellent psychometric properties.
Cognition
The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (SB-IV) was administered to children at 6 years.27 The SB-IV assesses intelligence and cognitive abilities and provides an overall test composite score and standard age scores in four areas: Verbal, Quantitative, Abstract/Visual Reasoning, and Short-term Memory. Raw scores, based on the number of correct items, are converted into standard scores (M=100, SD=16).
Academic Achievement
The Wide Range Achievement Test-3 (WRAT-3) was administered to children at 7 years and measures basic skills in reading, arithmetic and spelling.28 Raw scores are converted into standard scores (M=100, SD=15).
Behavior
The Child Behavior Check List (CBCL) was administered to caregivers when children were 7 years.29–30 The CBCL consists of 120 items related to behavior problems, which are scored on a 3-point scale ranging from not true to often true. Raw scores are converted to T-scores (M=50, SD=10). The CBCL produces a total behavior problem T-score, internalizing and externalizing scales, and several narrow band t-scores (e.g., Anxious/Depressed, Withdrawn, Somatic problems, Social problems, Thought problems, Attention problems, Aggressive behaviors, and Delinquent behaviors).
Caregiver Measures
The Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)31 was used to measure intellectual ability among caregivers. The K-BIT generates a composite score (M=100, SD=15), comprised of verbal and nonverbal abilities. The convergent validity of the K-BIT has been established in a range of populations, including urban, African American populations.
The Center for Epidemiological Studies–Depression (CES-D) Scale was used to measure depressive symptoms.32 The 20-item scale addresses six aspects of depression: depressed mood, guilt/worthlessness, helplessness/hopelessness, lethargy, loss of appetite, and sleep disturbance. Respondents rate the frequency of symptoms from 0 “rarely or never” to 3 “most or all the time.” Higher summed scores indicate more symptoms.
At each evaluation, respondents reported changes in primary caregiver. The number of changes was summed, providing a score representing caregiver changes. Caregivers provided information on their level of education, employment status, whether they were receiving public cash assistance (e.g., Aid To Families with Dependent Children, WIC, or unemployment benefits), marital status, and current substance use derived from the Addiction Severity Index. 33
Data Analysis
To identify confounding variables, we examined the intercorrelations among prenatal tobacco and alcohol exposure, infant birth weight for gestational age, number of caregiver changes, and several caregiver variables recorded at the 7-year visit: caregiver education, public assistance status, employment status, depressive symptoms, and ongoing drug use and their associations with PDE and the cognitive-behavioral outcome variables (Table 2). Covariates were selected based on significant associations with independent and dependent variables of interest. In analyses involving cognitive and academic outcomes, we controlled for gender, prenatal tobacco exposure, number of caregiver changes, public assistance status, employment status, and caregiver depressive symptoms. In analyses on the CBCL, we controlled for gender, prenatal tobacco exposure, number of caregiver changes, and caregiver depressive symptoms. We examined intervention status within the PDE group and found no effects of intervention or intervention by covariate interactions on the cognitive-behavioral variables. Therefore intervention status was not included in the analyses.
Table 2.
Variable | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. PDE status | -- | .58*** | .12 | −.47*** | .48*** | .48*** | −.09 | .11 | −.28*** | −.06 | .37*** | −.15+ | −.13+ | −.12 | −.11 | .17* | .02 |
2. Prenatal tobacco exposure | -- | .26*** | −.38*** | .30*** | −.34*** | −.15* | .05 | −.22** | .03 | .30*** | −.16* | −.18* | −.18* | −.15* | .16* | .08 | |
3. Prenatal alcohol exposure | -- | −.12 | .05 | −.08 | −.18* | .08 | .01 | .11 | .13+ | −.02 | −.02 | −.05 | −.04 | .07 | .02 | ||
4. Infant birth weight for gestational age | -- | −.29*** | .24** | .08 | −.14+ | .23** | .06 | −.22** | .09 | .07 | .07 | −.02 | .04 | .04 | |||
5. Number of primary caregiver changes (birth to age 7) | -- | −.63*** | −.21** | .10 | −.23*** | .01 | .17* | −.23*** | −.19* | −.13+ | −.07 | .13+ | .02 | ||||
6. Placement in non-maternal care | -- | .09 | −.12 | .26*** | .14+ | .06 | .19* | .08 | .06 | .05 | −.11 | −.03 | |||||
7. Caregiver education | -- | −.19* | .21** | −.20** | −.12 | .12 | .06 | .07 | .09 | −.05 | −.04 | ||||||
8. Caregiver receives public assistance | -- | −.29*** | .10 | .20* | −.17* | −.12 | −.14+ | −.10 | −.02 | .08 | |||||||
9. Caregiver employment status | -- | −.01 | −.29*** | .17* | .02 | .06 | .06 | .02 | −.02 | ||||||||
10. Caregiver depressive symptoms (CES-D) | -- | .09 | −.13+ | −.12 | −.22** | −.20** | .26*** | .41*** | |||||||||
11. Caregiver ongoing drug use | -- | −.09 | −.12 | −.11 | −.02 | .07 | .03 | ||||||||||
12. Child SB-IV total composite standard score | -- | .56*** | .55*** | .59*** | −.15+ | −.16* | |||||||||||
13. Child WRAT-3 Reading Achievement | -- | .88*** | .69*** | −.29*** | −.25** | ||||||||||||
14. Child WRAT-3 Spelling Achievement | -- | .66*** | −.26** | −.22** | |||||||||||||
15. Child WRAT-3 Arithmetic Achievement | -- | −.23** | −.28*** | ||||||||||||||
16. Child Behavior Checklist Externalizing behavior problems | -- | .67*** | |||||||||||||||
17. Child Behavior Checklist Internalizing behavior problems | -- |
Note. p< .05
p< .01
p <.001
N = 173
To test the first hypothesis that children with PDE would have lower scores on measures of cognitive, academic, and behavioral performance at age 6 and 7 than non-exposed children, we conducted multivariate analyses of variance, followed by univariate analyses (ANOVA) to identify differences within specific subtests. This strategy reduces the likelihood of a Type 1 error that might result from conducting multiple ANOVAs. PDE and CC were the independent variable. We began with unadjusted analyses, followed by analyses adjusted for covariates.
To test the second hypothesis, that the effects of PDE were modified by gender, we repeated the analyses including a gender by group interaction term, comparing children in the PDE and CC groups. Finally, we analyzed gender as a main effect to examine whether males were more vulnerable than females. According to Cohen,34 a sample size of 128 is required to detect group differences with a medium effect size for a power of .80. Thus, this study (n = 173) has adequate power to detect medium and large effect sizes.
RESULTS
Cognition
Unadjusted multivariate analyses indicated that children in the PDE group scored lower than children in the CC group on the SB-IV, F (8, 165) = 2.27, p<.05, specifically on two subtests: Absurdities, F (1, 172) = 5.59, p<.05, and Memory for Sentences, F (1, 172) = 14.38, p<.001.
After adjusting for covariates, there were no significant differences by exposure group on the overall SB-IV composite score, the four area scores, or on individual subtests (Table 3). The gender by group interaction was not significant.
Table 3.
Drug-Exposure Group | Gender | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Characteristics | Prenatally Drug- Exposed (n=111) | Community Cohort (n=62) | p-value* | Male (n=78) | Female (n=95) | p-value* |
Stanford-Binet IV (age 6) | ||||||
| ||||||
Vocabulary | 43.7 (6.4) | 45.4 (6.5) | .45 | 43.3 (5.9) | 45.2 (6.8) | .04 |
Comprehension | 47.1 (6.1) | 48.3 (5.7) | .53 | 45.5 (5.3) | 49.2 (6.0) | <.001 |
Absurdities | 43.7 (5.1) | 45.5 (4.3) | .49 | 43.8 (4.9) | 44.8 (4.9) | .14 |
Pattern Analysis | 42.2 (5.4) | 43.0 (6.4) | .99 | 41.4 (4.9) | 43.3 (6.3) | .02 |
Copy | 35.1 (3.9) | 35.7 (3.3) | .98 | 35.3 (3.4) | 35.2 (4.0) | .87 |
Quantitative | 44.7 (6.6) | 46.4 (8.5) | .80 | 44.6 (7.4) | 45.9 (7.3) | .21 |
Bead Memory | 41.2 (7.1) | 41.0 (8.3) | .58 | 40.2 (7.0) | 41.9 (7.9) | .15 |
Memory for Sentences | 43.2 (4.9) | 46.1 (4.7) | .07 | 43.3 (5.0) | 45.0 (4.9) | .01 |
| ||||||
Verbal | 88.4 (11.0) | 91.3 (11.2) | .46 | 87.0 (9.6) | 91.4 (12.0) | .01 |
Abstract/Visual Reasoning | 73.8 (9.0) | 75.4 (9.3) | .97 | 73.2 (7.7) | 75.4 (10.1) | .13 |
Quantitative Reasoning | 89.4 (13.2) | 92.7 (16.9) | .80 | 89.2 (14.8) | 91.8 (14.6) | .24 |
Short-term Memory | 81.6 (11.7) | 84.7 (12.6) | .71 | 80.6 (11.5) | 84.5 (12.3) | .03 |
| ||||||
Composite Standard Score (IQ) | 80.1 (10.1) | 83.3 (11.2) | .81 | 79.2 (9.5) | 83.0 (11.2) | .01 |
| ||||||
Wide Range Achievement Test-3 (age 7) | ||||||
| ||||||
Reading | 93.3 (15.9) | 97.6 (16.1) | .86 | 92.2 (16.1) | 96.9 (15.9) | .05 |
| ||||||
Spelling | 93.9 (14.7) | 97.9 (17.8) | .95 | 93.2 (16.4) | 97.0 (15.5) | .10 |
| ||||||
Arithmetic | 87.4 (17.1) | 91.4 (17.0) | .60 | 86.8 (17.3) | 90.5 (16.9) | .14 |
Note. All multivariate comparisons between PDE and CC groups controlled for gender, prenatal tobacco exposure, number of caregiver changes, caregiver depressive symptomatology, employment status, and public assistance status. In an examination of cognitive differences on the SB-IV, there was not a significant prenatal drug exposure status × gender interaction nor was there a significant main effect for prenatal drug exposure status, F (8, 165) = 1.16, p =.33. Only child gender was significantly associated with cognitive functioning in the final model, F (8, 165) = 2.88, p =.005. An examination of academic achievement differences on the WRAT-3 indicated that there was not a significant prenatal drug exposure status × gender interaction nor was there a significant main effect for prenatal drug exposure status, F (8, 165) = .30, p =.82, or child gender, F (8, 165) = 1.26, p =.28. Caregiver depressive symptomatology, F (8, 165) = 5.30, p = .002, was a significant multivariate predictor of child academic achievement in the final model.
There was a main effect for gender (see Table 3) such that males had significantly lower performance than females on the overall SB-IV Test Composite, two of four area scores (Verbal Reasoning, Short-term Memory), and four of the eight subtests (Vocabulary, Comprehension, Pattern Analysis, and Short-term Memory).
Academic Achievement
There were no significant associations between exposure group and Reading, Spelling, or Arithmetic achievement using unadjusted or adjusted comparisons (Table 3). The gender by exposure group interaction was not significant. Although there was not a significant multivariate effect for gender on academic achievement, males had significantly lower Reading achievement scores than females.
Behavior
Unadjusted multivariate analyses indicated that children in the PDE group were rated as having more behavior problems than children in the CC group on the CBCL, F (8, 165) = 3.30, p<.01, specifically on Aggression, F (1, 172) = 5.35, p<.05, and on Externalizing behavior problems, F (1, 172) = 4.95, p<.05.
After adjusting for covariates, there was a significant multivariate main effect for exposure group on caregiver-reported behavior problems on the CBCL (Table 4); however, there were no significant differences on any of the individual subscales or any of the broadband behavioral domains. There was not a gender by exposure group interaction.
Table 4.
Drug-Exposure Group | Gender | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CBCL t-scores (age 7) | Prenatally Drug- Exposed (n=111) | Community Cohort (n=62) | p-value* | Male (n=78) | Female (n=95) | p-value* |
Aggressive behavior | 51.1 (10.6) | 47.5 (8.3) | .09 | 50.9 (10.6) | 49.2 (9.4) | .04 |
Anxious/Depressed | 50.5 (10.9) | 47.9 (7.7) | .13 | 50.3 (9.6) | 49.4 (9.3) | .73 |
Attention problems | 51.0 (10.8) | 48.7 (8.8) | .51 | 51.9 (11.7) | 48.5 (8.2) | .006 |
Delinquent behavior | 50.6 (10.8) | 48.3 (7.5) | .84 | 51.2 (9.7) | 48.7 (8.2) | .08 |
Social problems | 50.9 (10.3) | 48.7 (9.1) | .60 | 51.6 (10.8) | 48.5 (8.2) | .47 |
Somatic complaints | 49.5 (9.4) | 50.5 (10.3) | .75 | 49.3 (9.6) | 50.5 (10.3) | .21 |
Thought problems | 50.2 (10.0) | 50.1 (11.0) | .86 | 50.2 (8.6) | 49.9 (10.7) | .61 |
Withdrawn behavior | 49.1 (9.7) | 51.2 (10.8) | .15 | 51.2 (10.6) | 49.2 (9.2) | .11 |
| ||||||
Internalizing Problems | 49.8 (10.2) | 49.4 (9.3) | .84 | 50.4 (9.2) | 49.6 (9.4) | .22 |
Externalizing Problems | 51.0 (10.8) | 47.6 (8.0) | .19 | 51.0 (10.5) | 49.0 (9.1) | .04 |
| ||||||
Total Behavior Problems | 50.7 (10.4) | 48.3 (8.8) | .28 | 51.2 (10.8) | 48.7 (8.9) | .06 |
Note. Multivariate comparisons between PDE and CC groups controlled for gender, prenatal tobacco exposure, number of caregiver changes, and caregiver depressive symptoms at age 7 visit. Separate analyses were run for subscales, internalizing and externalizing scores, and the total CBCL score. There was not a significant prenatal drug exposure status × gender interaction; there was a significant main effect for prenatal drug exposure status, F (8, 165) = 2.48, p =.02. Child gender, F (8, 165) = 2.29, p = .02, and caregiver depressive symptoms, F (8, 165) = 4.85, p < .001, were significant predictors of child behavior problems in the final model.
Males were rated as having more Externalizing behavior problems than females (Table 4). Males had significantly higher Aggression and Attention behavior problem ratings than females as well as marginally higher levels of Delinquent behavior problems.
DISCUSSION
This study yielded three major contributions to findings related to low-income, urban children with a history of PDE. First, with the inclusion of perinatal and environmental covariates, there were few differences in cognitive, academic, and behavioral performance scores between the children based on PDE history. These findings are striking because not only did we use maternal affirmation plus positive toxicology screens to confirm PDE status, but we recruited a community comparison group that represented families who resided in the same low-income communities as the PDE children and had experienced many of the same environmental challenges associated with poverty, but had not experienced PDE or the early caregiver disruptions that frequently occur among drug using families. 35 Our analyses suggested that the perinatal, maternal, and family covariates, selected on the basis of developmental-ecological theory, explained more variance in early child functioning than a history of PDE.
Some of the controversial findings in the field of PDE may be related to inconsistent attention to potential confounders. The negative consequences of prenatal exposure to alcohol and tobacco are well-known,13,14 yet many investigators have not adjusted for them in their analyses. In our bivariate findings, prenatal tobacco use occurred more often in the PDE group than in the CC group and was related to children’s lower functioning in multiple domains at ages 6 and 7. Thus, ignoring prenatal tobacco exposure could have led us to attribute more negative effects to PDE than warranted.
Substance using women are at risk for mental health problems, including depressive symptoms, which may interfere with their caregiving ability. 36 In our analyses, we found associations between caregiver depressive symptoms and measures of children’s academic performance and caregiver-reported behavior problems. Again, investigators who have not considered caregiver depressive symptoms may have attributed children’s performance patterns to PDE, rather than to caregiver depressive symptoms, particularly if they relied on caregiver-report measures.
Children who were born with PDE had lower scores than children in the CC group on the total score of caregiver-reported behavior problems, even after adjusting for perinatal and environmental factors. However, the lack of differences on the internalizing or externalizing scales or on any of the narrow band scales, suggests that there may have been subtle differences that only reached significance when all behaviors were considered together.
Caution is warranted when interpreting the present findings because there is evidence that PDE may be a risk factor for subtle, specific neurodevelopmental deficits, rather than global deficits. Arousal and attentional systems appear to be particularly vulnerable to the effects of PDE.37 Richardson and colleagues reported that even when there were no differences between PDE children and a matched comparison group at age 6 years on intellectual ability, academic performance, or teacher ratings, PDE children had deficits in sustained attention.2 Bendersky and colleagues have provided evidence that exposure to cocaine in utero has a negative effect on inhibitory control functioning and is associated with aggressive behavior problems at age five.7, 38 Although other investigations have also found that PDE children display more externalizing and total behavior problems than children with similar backgrounds who were not exposed to drugs prenatally, covariate adjustment has varied.5, 6, 16
These findings suggest that the effects of PDE must be considered in the context of the home environment.3, 4, 7, 15 In an analysis among PDE children at 18 months, we showed that parenting stress and child abuse potential were higher for caregivers with five or more risk factors compared to caregivers with fewer risk factors.39 Although at that time children’s developmental status did not differ by caregiver risk status, it is possible that sustained exposure to caregivers who find parenting stressful and have an inclination towards abuse or harsh parenting could eventually result in behavioral and developmental problems. Both cocaine-exposed and non-exposed fourth grade children in low-income families have better cognitive functioning and academic performance when they are raised in better functioning homes.4 However, in keeping with developmental-ecological theory, findings should be interpreted from a bidirectional perspective. That is, children are not only influenced by their environment, but children influence their environment through their behavior.
A second finding is that males demonstrated more vulnerability than females in 4 of 8 subtests of the SB-IV, the aggression and attention subscales from the CBCL, and reading scores from the WRAT-3. Effect sizes related to cognitive functioning ranged from small to medium (Cohen’s d .3–.6) and were small for behavioral and academic findings (Cohen’s d .2–.3). 34 These data are consistent with findings regarding vulnerability among males in general, including those prenatally exposed to illegal substances,7,16,17,40,41 with deficits commonly noted in sustained attention, concentration, self-regulation, and working memory, skills that are associated with activation of the prefrontal cortex.
Gender differences in brain development occur as early as 18 weeks gestation when males begin producing testosterone, which leads to several hormone-related changes in the brain. Males develop greater hemispheric asymmetry (right hemisphere larger than left), slightly larger overall brain volume, proportionately less gray matter relative to white matter, a thinner corpus callossum, and more cerebrospinal fluid surrounding the brain than females. Although such neuroanatomical differences may contribute to certain advantages for males in spatial abilities and a propensity towards physical action, they may also put males at an increased risk for attention, language, and information processing deficits, because males tend to share information between hemispheres less efficiently than females.24, 42
In our data, the absence of gender by exposure interactions indicates that the effects of gender and prenatal drug exposure were not synergistic. In other words, males had worse performance than females across several measures of cognition and behavior, regardless of PDE status.
Finally, the children’s low cognitive and academic achievement scores, regardless of PDE history, are consistent with findings from other samples of low-income children with and without PDE.4,43 Poverty has an insidious effect on multiple aspects of child functioning, particularly when it occurs in the context of maternal depressive symptoms.44 Recent evidence from the NICHD Early Child Care Research Network,45 has shown that 9-year-old children in chronically impoverished families had lower cognitive performance and more behavior problems than children who were not exposed to poverty, partially explained by a lack of enriching parenting behaviors and home experiences.
Methodological Limitations
There are several methodological limitations that should be considered in interpreting the data. First, although we relied on toxicology screens and self-report of frequent drug use to determine the level of substance exposure, it is possible that there may have been some misclassification among women who used substances early in their pregnancy, but not at the time of delivery and failed to report substance use. Second, due to the limited sample size and the high prevalence of polysubstance use, we could not detect small, drug-specific differences and we could not examine whether the severity of exposure was related to children’s behavior and development. However, most drug-using women use multiple substances which makes this a fairly representative sample.14,25 Third, we may have eliminated the highest risk infants by including only infants who were relatively healthy at birth. Thus, findings do not generalize to infants with intrauterine growth restriction, prematurity, or congenital problems. Fourth, although the gender differences were consistent with other reports, they were relatively small, raising questions about their clinical significance.
Finally, we limited our control of the caregiving environment to demographic, psychological, and self-report variables. Although we included multiple confounders based on developmental-ecological theory, it is likely that additional environmental variables play a substantial role in children’s behavior and academic performance.
Future Directions
In summary, once theoretically and empirically-derived confounders were included in the analyses, most differences in the children’s cognitive, behavioral, or academic performance at ages 6 and 7 year were attenuated. Future investigations of PDE children should adjust for potential confounders to ensure that attributions to PDE are accurate. In addition, it may be useful to examine subtle aspects of neurocognitive functioning that may be more sensitive to PDE than global assessments of functioning. It is also possible that performance differences related to PDE could occur as children age and use alternative cognitive and problem solving strategies.
Low-income children, especially males, are at risk for poor cognitive and behavioral functioning. Interventions are needed to ensure that children in low-income families, regardless of their history of PDE, receive developmentally enriching opportunities early in life to avoid the cognitive and behavioral consequences that can undermine subsequent success.
Acknowledgments
This study was supported by the National Institute of Drug Abuse (R01-DA07432 and R01-DA021059)
Footnotes
None of the authors has a conflict of interest.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Chasnoff IJ, Anson A, Hatcher R, et al. Prenatal exposure to cocaine and other drugs: Outcome at four to six years. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1998;846:314–328. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Richardson GA, Conroy ML, Day NL. Prenatal cocaine exposure: Effects on development of school-age children. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 1996;18:627–634. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(96)00121-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Accornero VH, Morrow CE, Bandstra ES, et al. Behavioral outcome of preschoolers exposed prenatally to cocaine: Role of maternal behavioral health. J Pediatr Psychol. 2002;27:259–269. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/27.3.259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hurt H, Brodsky NL, Roth H, et al. School performance of children with gestational cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27:203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Bada HS, Das A, Bauer CR, et al. Impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on child behavior problems through school age. Pediatr. 2007;119:e348–e359. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Linares TJ, Singer LT, Kirchner HL, et al. Mental health outcomes of cocaine-exposed children at 6 years of age. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31:85–97. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Bendersky M, Bennett D, Lewis M. Aggression at age 5 as a function of prenatal exposure to cocaine, gender, and environmental risk. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31:71–84. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bandstra ES, Morrow CE, Anthony JC, et al. Longitudinal investigation of task persistence and sustained attention in children with prenatal cocaine exposure. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2001;23:545–559. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(01)00181-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ornoy A, Segal J, Bar-Hamburger R, Greenbaum C. Developmental outcome of school-age children born to mothers with heroin dependency: Importance of environmental factors. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2001;43:668–75. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201001219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Beeghly M, Martin B, Rose-Jacobs R, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure and children’s language functioning at 6 and 9.5 years: Moderating effects of child age, birthweight, and gender. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31:98–115. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsj028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brown JV, Bakeman R, Coles CD, et al. Prenatal cocaine exposure: A comparison of 2-year-old children in parental and nonparental care. Child Dev. 2004;75:1282–1296. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00739.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Frank DA, Augustyn M, Knight WG, et al. Growth, development, and behavior in early childhood following prenatal cocaine exposure: A systematic review. JAMA. 2001;285:1613–1625. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.12.1613. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Cornelius MD, Ryan CM, Day NL, et al. Prenatal tobacco effects on neuropsychological outcomes among preadolescents. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22:217–225. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200108000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jacobson JL, Jacobson S. Methodological issues in research on developmental exposure to neurotoxic agents. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27:395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2005.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bronfenbrenner U, Ceci SJ. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychol Rev. 1994;101:568–86. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Delaney-Black V, Covington C, Norstron B, et al. Prenatal cocaine: Quantity of exposure and gender moderation. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2004;25:254–263. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200408000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nordstrom BB, Sood BG, Sokol RJ, et al. Gender and alcohol moderate caregiver reported child behavior after prenatal cocaine. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2005;27:191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2004.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Chatterji M. Reading achievement gaps, correlates, and moderators of early reading achievement: Evidence from the early childhood longitudinal study (ECLS) kindergarten to first grade sample. J Educ Psychol. 2006;98:489–507. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Smith JR, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. The consequences of living in poverty for young children’s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement. In: Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, editors. Consequences of Growing up Poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation Press; 1997. pp. 132–189. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Farrell AD, Bruce SE. Impact of exposure to community violence on violent behavior and emotional distress among urban adolescents. J Clin Child Psychol. 1997;26:2–14. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2601_1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Blanton RE, Levitt JG, Peterson JR, et al. Gender differences in the left inferior frontal gyrus in normal children. Neuroimage. 2004;22:626–636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Matsui M, et al. Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: Correlations with cognitive performance. J Neurosci. 1999;19:4065–4072. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-10-04065.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Reiss AL, Abrams MT, Singer HS, et al. Brain development, gender, and IQin children: A volumetric imaging study. Brain. 1996;199:1763–1774. doi: 10.1093/brain/119.5.1763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shaywitz BA, Shaywitz SE, Pugh KR, et al. Sex differences in the functional organization of the brain for language. Nature. 1995;373:607–609. doi: 10.1038/373607a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Schuler ME, Nair P, Black MM. Ongoing maternal drug use, parenting attitudes, and a home intervention: Effects on mother-child interaction at 18 months. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2002;23:87–94. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200204000-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Infant Health and Development Program. Enhancing the outcomes of low-birth weight, premature infants. JAMA. 1990;263:3035–3042. doi: 10.1001/jama.1990.03440220059030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stanford- Binet Intelligence Test. 4. Chicago, IL: The Riverside Publishing Company; 1987. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wilkinson GS. Wide Range Achievement Test-revision 3. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Achenbach TM. Manuel for child behavior checklist and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mattison R, Spitznagel E. Long-term stability of Child Behavior Checklist profile types in a child psychiatric clinic population. J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiat. 1999;38:700–707. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199906000-00017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments; 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psych Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 33.McLellan AT, Luborsky L, O’Brien CP, et al. An improved diagnostic instrument for substance abuse patients: The Addiction Severity Index. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1980;168:26–33. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198001000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. 1992;112:155–159. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nair P, Black M, Schuler M, Keane V, Snow L, Rigney BA, Magder L. Risk factors for disruption in primary caregiving among infants of substance abusing women. Child Abuse Negl. 1997;21:1039–1051. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(97)00064-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Suchman NE, McMahon TJ, Slade A, Luthar SS. How early bonding, depression, illicit drug use, and perceived support work together to influence drug-dependent mothers’ caregiving. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 2005;75:431–45. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.75.3.431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Mayes LC. A behavioral teratogenic model of the impact of prenatal cocaine exposure on arousal regulatory systems. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 2002;24:385–395. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(02)00200-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bendersky M, Gambini G, Lastella A, et al. Inhibitory motor control at five years as a function of prenatal cocaine exposure. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2003;24:345–351. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200310000-00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Nair P, Schuler ME, Black MM, et al. Cumulative environmental risk in substance abusing women: Early intervention, parenting stress, child abuse potential, and child development. Child Abuse Negl. 2003;27:997–1017. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(03)00169-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Dennis T, Bendersky M, Ramsay D, et al. Reactivity and regulation in children prenatally exposed to cocaine. Dev Psych. 2006;42:688–697. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Vathy I. Effects of prenatal morphine and cocaine on postnatal behaviors and brain neurotransmitters. NIDA Res Mono Ser. 1995;158:88–114. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Gur RC, Alsop D, Glahn D, et al. An fMRI study of sex differences in regional activation to a verbal and a spatial task. Brain Lang. 2000;74:157–70. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bradley RH, Corwyn RF. Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:371–99. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Petterson SM, Albers AB. Effects of poverty and maternal depression on early child development. Child Dev. 2001;72:1794–813. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.NICHD. Duration and developmental timing of poverty and children’s cognitive and social development from birth through third grade. Child Dev. 2005;76:795–810. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00878.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]