Abstract
The medial preoptic nucleus (POM) is a brain region outside of the song control system of songbirds. It has been implicated in song production, sexual motivation, and the integration of both sensory and hormonal information with appropriate behavioral responses. The POM is well positioned neuroanatomically to interact with multiple regions involved in song, social behavior, and motivation. However, little is known about the brain regions with which the POM directly or indirectly communicates to influence song. To gain insight into the neuronal circuits normally activated in association with POM activity during male song, we compared activity within multiple brain regions using immunolabeling for protein products of immediate early genes (IEGs) zenk (aka egr-1) and c-fos (indirect markers of neuronal activity) in sham and POM-lesioned male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Compared to sham lesions, POM lesions disrupted song and interest in a nest box and females responded less to POM-lesioned males. POM lesions reduced numbers of IEG-labeled cells and disrupted correlations between numbers of IEG-labeled cells and song within several song control, limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain regions. These results are consistent with the possibility that the POM integrates activity among nuclei involved in song control, social behavior, and motivational state that work in concert to promote sexually motivated communication.
Keywords: song control system, preoptic area, vocal communication, sexually motivated song, electrolytic lesion
INTRODUCTION
Converging data indicate that vocal communication in songbirds involves coordination among brain regions involved in vocal output, social behavior and motivation (e.g. (Alger and Riters, 2006; Goodson, 1998; Goodson et al., 1999; Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2006, 2007; Riters and Ball, 1999; Riters et al., 2005; Riters et al., 2004). The medial preoptic area is a brain region implicated in communication (Alger and Riters, 2006; Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2007; Riters and Ball, 1999; Riters et al., 2000; Riters et al., 2005; Riters et al., 2004), sexual motivation (reviewed in (Hull et al., 1999; Panzica et al., 1996), and the integration of sensory and hormonal information with appropriate behavioral responses to environmental and social stimuli (reviewed in (Ball and Balthazart, 2004; Wood, 1998). Increasing evidence suggests a critical and possibly integrative role for the POM in song. However, the brain regions with which the POM communicates to regulate song have not been identified.
Many songbird species, including European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris), breed seasonally. Breeding condition male starlings (i.e. males in spring-like photoperiod conditions with high testosterone) produce high levels of song, referred to here as sexually motivated song, when presented with female conspecifics (reviewed in (Eens, 1997). Evidence supports a stimulatory role for the POM in male song produced within this context. Lesions to the POM inhibit song production in male starlings exclusively within a breeding context and reduce song bout length (Alger and Riters, 2006; Riters and Ball, 1999), a song attribute used by females to assess male attractiveness (e.g. (Eens et al., 1991; Gentner and Hulse, 2000; Mountjoy and Lemon, 1996). Relationships between song and immediate early gene (IEG; indirect markers of neuronal activity) immunolabeling further implicate the POM in sexually motivated male song in songbirds (Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2007; Riters et al., 2004). These data suggest a key role for the POM in the production and modulation of sexually motivated male song.
The POM is anatomically well situated to influence brain areas involved in both song control and social behavior (Fig. 1). It projects both directly and indirectly to nuclei of the song control system (Riters and Alger, 2004), a network of nuclei that collectively regulate song (for review, see (Bottjer and Johnson, 1997; Brainard, 2004; Margoliash, 1997; Wild, 1997). Additionally, the POM shares reciprocal connections with a steroid-sensitive network of limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain regions involved in song, various other forms of social behavior, motivation, and reward (Goodson, 2005; Riters and Alger, 2004).
Figure 1.
Sagittal schematics showing functionally distinct pathways by which the POM possibly influences sexually motivated song production via A) a direct connection with the song control motor pathway, B) indirect connections with the song control anterior forebrain and motor pathways and C) connections with nuclei involved in social behavior.
Together, several lines of research implicate the POM in song. Neuroanatomical connections of the POM suggest this region may play a central role in the coordination of activity within multiple brain regions involved in motivation, social behavior, and song. However, the circuits influencing or influenced by the POM during song production have not yet been identified. The goal of the present study was to begin to identify neural circuits interacting with the POM to regulate song.
METHODS
We compared immunolabeling for two IEGs in POM- and sham-lesioned male starlings to highlight functional neural circuits normally influenced by POM activity during song production. IEG activation is closely associated with neuronal activity that is correlated with behaviorally relevant stimuli. However, because not every brain region expresses every IEG, the absence of labeled cells within an area cannot be interpreted as reflecting an absence of neural activity. Thus, some regions important for song may not be identified with this technique. Despite these limitations, studies involving immunolabeling for IEGs have proven to be useful tools and have provided important insight into multiple brain regions active in association with song (reviewed in (Ball et al., 2006). To strengthen our use of this technique, we examined the protein products of two IEGs, zenk (aka egr-1) and c-fos, which are expressed at high levels in only partially overlapping brain areas involved in song, motivation, and social behavior (e.g. (Charlier et al., 2005; Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2006, 2007; Riters et al., 2004; Velho et al., 2005). The use of two indirect indicators of neural activity can potentially provide more information on brain regions regulating song than either alone.
Animals
Thirty-one male and 78 female adult starlings were captured during fall and winter of 2004 on a farm outside of Madison, Wisconsin. They were housed indoors in stainless steel cages in single-sex groups on a light cycle reflective of the natural outside photoperiod (ranging from 9:30L to 12L) until the experiment. All experiments were in accordance with the Guidelines of the University of Wisconsin Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee and the National Institutes of Health Guidelines. All procedures were approved by the University of Wisconsin Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
Hormone Manipulations
All subject males were gonadectomized. Briefly, a small incision was made just anterior to the last rib and gonads were removed using forceps in males anesthetized with isoflurane, as in Alger and Riters (2006). Males were then placed on a photoperiod of 11L:13D for at least 12 weeks, a photoperiod under which birds will become fully reproductively active in response to testosterone (T), but will not enter a state of photorefractoriness (Falk and Gwinner, 1988).
At the time of lesion surgery, subject males received two subcutaneous 14 mm silastic implants (1.47 mm i.d., 1.96 mm o.d.; Dow Corning, Midland, MI) filled for 10 mm with crystalline T (Sigma, St. Louis, MO). Males received T as in Alger and Riters (2006) to maximize sexual behavior, promote similar T concentrations across treatments, and allow us to attribute observed effects to POM lesions independent of relationships between the POM and gonadal T. Past work shows this treatment in castrated male starlings results in T concentrations within the physiological range of breeding males for at least 8 weeks (Duffy et al., 2000).
Females to be used as behavioral stimuli were housed on 11L:13D for at least 12 weeks. These females received subcutaneous estrogen (E2) implants to facilitate sexual interest and associated male song production. Each female received two 18 mm silastic implants (1.47 mm i.d., 1.96 mm o.d., Dow Corning) filled for 13 mm with 17-beta-estradiol (Sigma). The hormone implantation procedure was identical to that used for male T implants.
Lesion Surgery
Males were randomly assigned to the bilateral POM lesion group (n = 21) or the bilateral POM sham lesion (n = 10) group. We assigned more birds to be lesioned than to receive a sham operation because we expected that, due to natural variation in brain size among wild-caught starlings, some of the lesions would miss the POM.
Lesion methods were as previously described (Alger and Riters, 2006). Briefly, before each surgery, the anterior-posterior and ventral-dorsal zero coordinates were taken directly from the ear bar of a small animal stereotaxic apparatus (Kopf, Tujunga, CA) fitted with a beak cone for gas anesthesia. Males anesthetized with isoflurane were placed into the stereotaxic apparatus with the beak angled at a 45° angle below the horizontal plane. The skull was opened above the POM and the lateral-medial zero coordinate was taken from the midline at the surface of the brain. An electrode was lowered to the coordinates (A = −0.5 mm, L = ±0.15 mm, V = 7.0 mm from the zero coordinate). For each lesion 0.25 mA of anodal direct current, produced by an S88 stimulator (Grass, West Warwick, RI) and held constant by a constant current unit (Grass), was passed for 20 s through the exposed tip of the electrode. For a sham procedure, the electrode remained in the brain for 20 s without the passage of current. The electrode was rapidly removed after each lesion, the skull was closed with dental cement and the skin was sutured. In order to allow for recovery and for the dissipation of any possible stimulatory effects of the electrode, one month was allowed for recovery prior to behavioral testing.
Behavioral Testing
Test Cage Set-up and Habituation
Subject males were introduced into a separate (76 × 76 × 81 cm3) cage alone in a sound-attenuated indoor observation room with an 11L:13D photoperiod. The room contained a nest box, two perches, and food and water ad libitum. A microphone was located in the room, but outside of the cage. Twenty-four hours after introduction to the cage, a stimulus female was introduced into the room (but outside of the male’s cage) for 10 minutes and then into the male’s cage for 10 minutes to habituate the male and encourage his future singing behavior.
Song Recording
An observer recorded songs from behind a one-way mirror using a laptop computer, M-Audio MoblePre pre-amplifier (Irwindale, CA), and Avisoft Recorder software (an automated sound-recording software; Berlin, Germany). Avisoft software was set to record and save all vocalizations longer than 10 seconds in duration. At least 24 hours after habituation of the male with a stimulus female, male song was recorded in the presence of a stimulus female outside of the male’s cage. This was continued, often in multiple recording sessions, until at least 20 songs had been recorded or until 10 hours of recording had been completed, whichever came first. After each recording session, the stimulus female was placed in the cage with the male for 10 minutes to encourage future singing behavior. Stimulus females were reused multiple times during song recording sessions. Between recording sessions and after completion of song recording, subject males were housed alone in the observation cage to which they were habituated.
Songs, defined as vocalizations at least 10 s in duration with a pause no longer than 2 s, were measured for song bout length using Avisoft SASLab Pro software. Mean song bout length was determined for each male from these recordings.
Behavioral Testing for IEG Analysis
At least 24 hours after the completion of song recording, an observer placed a handful of green nest material (which males use during courtship) into the male’s cage and released a novel stimulus female into the room and recorded the male’s behavior for 60 minutes from behind a one-way mirror. Sexually motivated behaviors recorded included: latency to sing from the time the female was released into the room, number of times each male sang, number of nest box directed behaviors (including the number of times the male brought green nest materials to the nest box, entered the nest box, and looked in the nest box), and number of times the female landed on the male’s cage (as an assay for the potency of the males’ courtship signals). Contextually nonspecific behaviors (eating, drinking, beak wiping, and preening) were also recorded. Although beak wiping is a courtship behavior in some passerine species, it does not appear to have a role in courtship in European starlings. The order of males observed was randomized.
Tissue Collection
All males were sacrificed 15 minutes following the 60-minute behavioral observation period. This time course was selected based on past research indicating this to be an effective time point at which to examine relationships between both immunolabeling for ZENK (the protein product of the zenk gene, the avian homologue of zif-268, egr-1, ngfi-a and krox-24) and FOS (the protein product of the c-fos gene) and song behavior (e.g. (Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2006, 2007). Subject males were rapidly decapitated and brains were removed and fixed overnight in a 5% acrolein solution. Males were checked at this time to confirm the presence of hormone implants and to check for testicular remnants. Following acrolein fixation, brains were cryoprotected in 30% sucrose for two days, frozen with crushed dry ice, and stored at −80°C. Frozen brains were cut in coronal sections at 40 µm using a cryostat. Every third section was collected for Nissl, labeling for FOS, or labeling for ZENK, except for the region containing POM, in which every section was collected exclusively for Nissl for the purpose of accurate lesion reconstruction. Tissue to be analyzed for lesion reconstruction was Nissl stained with Thionin.
Lesion Reconstruction
The percentage of lesion damage located within and outside the POM for each bird was quantified from the Nissl-stained tissue using a Spot camera (Diagnostic Instruments, Sterling Heights, MI) attached to a Nikon microscope (Melville, NY) and a computer. As in our previous work (Alger and Riters, 2006), the volumes of POM, lesions inside POM and lesions outside POM were traced with a mouse using MetaVue software (Universal Imaging Corp., Downingtown, PA). Lesions were reconstructed by tracing the areas that showed obvious lesion damage. No attempt was made to identify additional necrotic tissue, so our lesion measurements were likely conservative. We estimated POM and lesion volumes by multiplying the area for each section by 0.04 mm for the section thickness and summing these volumes for the length of the nucleus.
Immunocytochemistry
Immunocytochemistry for the protein products of the IEGs c-fos and zenk was performed in two of the series of sections collected anterior and posterior to POM [i.e. sections anterior to the septopalliomesencephalic tract (TSM) and posterior to the anterior commissure (AC)] using standard techniques (Heimovics and Riters, 2005, 2007). FOS and ZENK primary antibodies were c-fos (K-25 made in rabbit, SC-253, 1:18000; Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, CA) and egr-1 (C-19 made in rabbit, SC-189, 1:5000; Santa Cruz Biotechnology). The specificity of the egr-1 antibody has been validated in starlings using a preadsorption study (Sockman et al., 2002). To validate the specificity of the anti-FOS antibody in starling tissue, immunocytochemistry was performed using the brains of three photosensitive, T-implanted males. For each of these brains, half of the sections were incubated in K-25 antibody and the other half were incubated in K-25 antibody with the blocking peptide SC-253 P (1:50; Santa Cruz Biotechnology). We observed high levels of staining in the sections containing antibody alone and no staining in sections containing antibody plus peptide. For both FOS and ZENK, secondary was biotinylated goat anti-rabbit (1:250; Jackson ImmunoResearch Laboratories, West Grove, PA) and sections were incubated in AB solution (Vector ABC kit, Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA) followed by Vector SG as a chromagen (Vector Laboratories). Sections were mounted on gel-coated slides, dehydrated with alcohol, and coverslipped.
IEG-immunoreactive (IEG-ir) cells were counted using a Spot camera attached to a Nikon microscope and a computer using MetaVue software. Brain nuclei analyzed included song control regions [dorsomedial nucleus intercollicularis (DM), HVC (abbreviation as proper name), robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA), Area X (proper name), lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN), and nucleus uvaeformis (Uva)] and limbic, hypothalamic, and midbrain regions implicated in social behavior, motivation, and reward [midbrain central gray (GCt), ventral tegmental area (VTA), dorsal and ventral subdivisions of caudal lateral septum (SLcd and SLcv, respectively) nucleus taeniae of the arcopallium (TnA), lateral hypothalamus (LHy), and ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus (VMH); Fig. 1 and 2]. For each antibody, all tissue was run in a single batch and background labeling was fairly uniform across individuals. The MetaVue autoscale function was used to calculate the correct exposure of each image as a percentage of the total range of light, thus further reducing the variation in background among individuals. Therefore, it was possible to use a single threshold to determine which cells were to be counted as labeled for each specific antibody. In all cases the threshold selected material that a blind observer agreed was specific labeling. Cell counts were made within boxes or ovals centered within each region of interest (Fig. 2) and counts were generated by MetaVue in each of three serial sections in both hemispheres for each bird. The nuclei locations were based on Heimovics and Riters (2007) and Goodson et al. (2005). Counts were summed for each brain region for each male. In cases of extensive tissue damage, the individual was dropped from analysis for affected brain areas.
Figure 2.
POM lesion damage and immunocytochemistry regions. A, E–G) Boxes/ovals represent areas in which cFOS- and ZENK-labeled cells were counted. Region areas (mm2) are indicated in parentheses. B–D) Reconstruction of POM lesion damage in Nissl stained tissue, cumulative across all 12 POM-lesioned subjects. Light gray ovals represent lesion damage located in one bird. Darker gray ovals represent lesioned areas common to at least 2 males.
Statistics
Data were analyzed using the statistical software program Statistica (StatSoft 2001, Tulsa, OK) and R (R Foundation, http://www.r-project.org). Assumptions and appropriateness of parametric statistics were checked by establishing if sample sizes were larger than 10, establishing normal distributions using Q-Q plots and Lilliefors tests, and establishing homogeneity of variance using Brown-Forsythe tests. Behaviors and numbers of FOS- and ZENK-ir cells were compared between control males and lesioned males using Student’s t-tests if all parametric assumptions were met and using Mann-Whitney U tests if parametric assumptions were not met. We used Spearman Rank Correlations to assess the relationship between numbers of IEG-ir cells and measures of song.
To investigate the extent to which differences in IEG immunolabeling result from behavioral changes induced by POM lesions (i.e. secondary effects of changes in motor behavior caused by POM lesions) or represent a disruption of integrative inputs from the POM independent of motor behavior changes, we performed multiple linear regression analyses. For each IEG and each brain area studied here, the number of IEG-ir cells were entered as the response variable and lesion condition and all measured behaviors were entered as predictors into Forward, Backward and Stepwise selection procedures for variable reduction purposes. Predictor variables chosen by these procedures were plotted against the associated response variable to assess the need for appropriate variable transformations. From the resulting candidate models, models with high adjusted R2, randomly scattered residual plots centered at zero, and Q-Q plots nearest the 45° line were then chosen as the final models for each IEG and each brain area. These final models were examined using multiple linear regression.
Additionally, Principal Components Analyses (PCAs) were performed on correlation matrices of IEG-ir cell counts for each brain region. Separate PCAs were performed for control and for POM-lesioned males. PCA is a statistical technique applied to a large set of variables to determine which variable subsets form coherent groups that are relatively independent of one another. Variables that are correlated with one another and which are also largely independent of other variable subsets are combined into components. The first principal component is the component retaining characteristics of the data set that contribute most to its variance; the second principal component retains the second greatest; and so on. Thus, PCA can be thought of as identifying the structure in the relationships between multiple variables in a way which best explains the variance in the complete data set. PCA allows us to explore potential shifts in relationships between FOS- and ZENK-immunoreactivity in multiple brain areas that result from POM lesions, something that is impossible with traditional statistical hypothesis testing methods.
RESULTS
Lesion Reconstruction
Of the 21 males with attempted POM lesions, 12 had successful bilateral POM damage, 7 had unilateral POM damage, and 2 had damage completely outside of the POM. Ten males had successful sham operations. The POM-lesioned group discussed here consisted of only males with successful bilateral POM damage. Data from the two males with lesions completely outside of the POM fell within the range of the data from the sham group for all behaviors measured, so they were included in the control group. This resulted in 12 males in the control group and 12 males in the POM lesion group.
The estimate of POM damage ranged from 0.9% to 13.1% (mean ± SD = 5.2 ± 4.0, n = 12), and the portion of POM damage varied slightly across males (Fig. 2B–2D). Estimates of POM damage were likely conservative because no attempt was made to identify additional necrotic tissue likely surrounding the areas of obvious lesion damage. We divided the POM into three subregions: rostral (located ventral to the TSM and dorsal to the supraoptic decussation), intermediate (POM located caudal to the TSM and rostral to the AC) and caudal (just ventral to the AC). Five lesions were confined to caudal POM. Four were restricted to intermediate POM. The remaining 3 all included intermediate POM (1 included rostral + intermediate POM, 1 included intermediate + caudal POM, and 1 included all 3 subregions).
Data from individuals with lesions confined to caudal POM were compared to data from individuals with lesions including intermediate POM using Mann-Whitney U tests. The POM subregion lesioned did not have an effect on behavior or the numbers of IEG-labeled cells, with the exception of HVC (discussed below). Due to small sample sizes when males were divided by POM subregions and lack of evidence for distinct behavioral effects of intermediate versus caudal POM lesions, data from all POM-lesioned males were pooled for further analyses.
POM lesions and behavior
During song recording (which lasted up to 10 hours), POM-lesions did not appear to affect whether or not birds sang (92% of lesioned birds sang compared to 83% of control birds). However, only 50% of lesioned animals sang at least 20 songs within the 10 hours of recording compared to 75% of control animals. Recorded songs produced by males with POM lesions were shorter compared to songs produced by control males (Z = −1.972, p = 0.049; Fig. 3B).
Figure 3.
Histograms illustrating effects of POM lesions on measures of mean A) song latency, B) song bout length, C) numbers of songs produced, D) next box directed behaviors and E) number of female landings on the subject males’ cages. Asterix indicates p < 0.05.
During behavioral testing (which lasted 60 minutes), 67% of POM-lesioned birds sang compared to 83% of control birds. Compared to controls, males with POM lesions took longer to start singing (Z = 2.098, p = 0.036; Fig. 3A) and produced fewer songs (Z = −2.083, p = 0.037; Fig. 3C) after the introduction of a female. Males with POM lesions showed fewer nest box directed behaviors (Z = −2.709, p = 0.007; Fig. 3D) and females landed less frequently on the cages of POM-lesioned males compared to control males (Z = −2.083, p = 0.037; Fig. 3E). In contrast, males with POM lesions fed and drank more frequently than control males did (for feeding, control: 5.4 ± 4.0, n = 12; lesioned: 10.6 ± 6.2, n = 12; t = 2.412, df = 22, p = 0.025; for drinking, control: 5.3 ± 3.8, n = 12; lesioned: 10.9 ± 7.3, n = 12; Z = 2.057, p = 0.040). POM lesions did not affect preening or beak wiping.
POM lesions decreased numbers of IEG-ir cells in song, limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain nuclei
Compared to control males, POM-lesioned males had fewer FOS-ir cells in HVC (Z = −3.077, p = 0.002), Uva (Z = −2.890, p = 0.004), GCt (t = −2.838, df = 19, p = 0.011) and LHy (t = −2.305, df = 21, p = 0.031; Fig. 4A, C, E, G). POM-lesioned males also had more FOS-ir cells in LMAN compared to control males. However, this comparison included an influential outlier, which once removed, failed to show a significant difference. Additionally, POM-lesions significantly decreased the numbers of ZENK-ir cells in HVC (t = −3.729, df = 21, p = 0.001), Area X (Z = −2.078, p = 0.038), GCt (Z = −2.708, p = 0.007) and SLcd (Z = −2.521, p = 0.012; Fig. 4B, D, F, H; Fig. 5A). POM lesions did not significantly affect IEG-ir cell counts in any other brain region measured.
Figure 4.
Histograms illustrating mean numbers of IEG-ir cells in sham-lesioned and POM-lesioned males for A) FOS-ir in HVC, B) ZENK-ir in HVC, C) FOS-ir in Uva, D) ZENK-ir in Area X, E) FOS-ir in GCt, F) ZENK-ir in GCt, G) FOS-ir in LHy and H) ZENK-ir in SLcd. Asterix indicates p < 0.05.
Figure 5.
Photomicrographs at 100X of representative IEG-immunolabeling. Both FOS- and ZENK-ir were similar in appearance. This figure shows representative ZENK-immunoreactivity in A) Area X and B) VMH. For each brain area, top panels are from control birds and bottom panels are from birds with POM lesions. Also for each brain area, left panels are from birds producing no song and right panels are from birds with high song output (This control bird sang 52 songs and this lesioned bird sang 49 songs). The scale bar indicates 0.5 mm.
When tested for an effect of POM subregion lesioned, ZENK-labeling was significantly reduced in HVC in individuals with lesions to caudal POM compared to males with lesions to rostral/intermediate or intermediate POM lesions (Z = 2.402, p = 0.016). No other region was affected by the subregion of POM lesioned.
POM lesions affected correlations between measures of song and numbers of IEG-ir cells in song, limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain nuclei
Correlations between IEG-ir cell counts and measures of song differed between POM-lesioned males and controls in several brain regions. The number of songs produced had a positive correlation with numbers of ZENK-ir cells in Area X (ρ = 0.688, p < 0.05), LHy (ρ = 0.708, p < 0.05), and VMH (ρ = 0.626, p < 0.05) in control males, but not in males with POM lesions (Fig. 5; Fig 6. A, B, D). The number of FOS-ir cells in SLcv (ρ = −0.820, p < 0.05) was negatively correlated with the number of songs produced in control males, but not POM-lesioned males (Fig. 6C). The numbers of FOS-ir cells in HVC (ρ = 0.700, p < 0.05) and SLcd (ρ = 0.929, p < 0.05) were positively correlated with song bout length in control males, but not POM-lesioned males (Fig. 7A, C). The number of ZENK-ir cells in SLcd (and ρ = 0.786, p < 0.05) was positively correlated with song bout length in control males, but not POM-lesioned males (Fig. 7D). POM-lesioned males, but not control males, showed a positive correlation between the number of FOS-ir cells in GCt (ρ = 0.627, p < 0.05) and song bout length (Fig. 7B). IEG-ir cell counts did not correlate with measures of song in POM-lesioned or control males in any other brain area measured.
Figure 6.
Scatterplots illustrating relationships between numbers of IEG-labeled cells and numbers of song produced in sham-lesioned and POM-lesioned males for A) ZENK-ir in Area X, B) ZENK-ir in LHy, C) FOS-ir in SLcv and D) ZENK-ir in VMH. Each dot represents data for a single individual. Missing data points indicate tissue lost due to damage. Solid lines indicate lines of best fit for significant correlations.
Figure 7.
Scatterplots illustrating relationships between numbers of IEG-labeled cells and song bout length in sham-lesioned and POM-lesioned males for A) FOS-ir in HVC, B) FOS-ir in GCt, C) FOS-ir in SLcd and D) ZENK-ir in SLcd. Each dot represents data for a single individual. Missing data points indicate tissue lost due to damage. Solid lines indicate lines of best fit for significant correlations.
Multiple linear regression analysis revealed that POM lesion condition was a significant predictor of the number of FOS-ir cells in HVC, GCt and Uva and the number of ZENK-ir cells in HVC, SLcd and LHy. All of these best-fit regression models contained behavioral predictors as well, although these behavioral predictors were not always statistically significant. In the best-fit model relating to the number of FOS-ir cells in HVC, lesion condition was the only significant variable (F2,17=13.97, Adjusted R2=0.577, t=−3.682, p=0.002), although average song length improved model fit (t=1.729, p=0.102). Lesion condition was also the only significant variable (t=−2.707, p=0.017) in the best-fit model relating to the number of ZENK-ir cells in HVC (F5,14=5.383, Adjusted R2=0.536), although average song length (transformed with natural log, t=1.301, p=0.214), the number of songs produced (t=0.934, p=0.366), feeding (transformed as a power of e, t=−0.796, p=0.439), and the number of beakwipes (t=1.843, p=0.087) all improved model fit. The number of FOS-ir cells in GCt were significantly affected by lesion condition (F5,15=10.44, Adjusted R2=0.703, t=−6.027, p<0.001), the number of songs produced (t=−3.289, p=0.005), feeding (t=−2.223, p=0.042), drinking (t=3.128, p=0.007), and preening (t=−4.479, p<0.001). The number of FOS-ir cells in Uva were significantly affected by lesion condition (F2,17=8.924, Adjusted R2=0.455, t=−2.182, p=0.043) and average song bout length (t=2.248, p=0.038). The number of ZENK-ir cells in SLcd were significantly affected by lesion condition (F3,10=7.637, Adjusted R2=0.605, t=−2.693, p=0.023) and average song bout length (t=2.729, p=0.021) and the number of beakwipes improved model fit but was not significant (t=2.037, p=0.069). The number of ZENK-ir cells in LHy were significantly affected by lesion condition (F2,20=5.433, Adjusted R2=0.287, t=−2.505, p=0.021) and preening (t=−2.494, p=0.022). Lesion condition improved the fit of models for the number of FOS-ir cells in Area X and in LMAN, but it was not significant in either case (p=0.196 and p=0.413, respectively). Lesion condition was not present in any other linear model.
POM lesions affect interactions among song, limbic, hypothalamic, and midbrain nuclei
Only birds that had both FOS and ZENK data for all brain regions analyzed were included in the PCA. The following brain regions were included: HVC, Area X, Uva, GCt, VTA, VMH, and LHy. FOS- and ZENK-ir cells in RA, DM, LMAN, SL and TnA were not included due to the lack of sufficient birds with data for all regions. This resulted in one PCA for control males (n=8) and one PCA for POM-lesioned males (n=10), each with 14 variables. To verify that data associated with the decreased sample sizes used in PCA were consistent with the complete data set, scatterplots and correlations of FOS- and ZENK-labeling with number of songs produced were created for the reduced data set. Correlation patterns were consistent with the complete data set.
The PCA performed on IEG data for control males yielded seven principal components (PCs; Table 1, Fig. 8A). Of these seven, the first two described 57.0% of the variation. Variables contributing at least 10% to the first two PCs are reported here. PC1 was largely a function of ZENK- and FOS-labeling in LHy, FOS-labeling in VMH and Area X and ZENK-labeling in GCt. The second PC was primarily composed of ZENK- and FOS-labeling in HVC, ZENK-labeling in Area X, and FOS-labeling in Uva (Table 3).
Table 1.
The loadings and cumulative percentage of variation explained by each of the PCs obtained by PCA of FOS- and ZENK-labeled cells in seven brain regions in control males. Numbers in bold indicate variables contributing at least 10% to the first two PCs.
Principal Component Loadings | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | PC7 | |
Variables | |||||||
HVC FOS | −0.102 | −0.599 | −0.487 | −0.617 | 0.003 | 0.091 | −0.070 |
HVC ZENK | 0.309 | −0.807 | −0.467 | 0.099 | −0.063 | 0.136 | −0.050 |
Area X FOS | 0.749 | −0.215 | −0.465 | 0.301 | −0.005 | −0.294 | 0.018 |
Area X ZENK | −0.424 | −0.725 | 0.194 | −0.232 | −0.392 | −0.213 | −0.067 |
Uva FOS | 0.652 | −0.573 | −0.419 | −0.180 | −0.037 | −0.159 | 0.111 |
Uva ZENK | −0.135 | −0.416 | 0.765 | −0.321 | 0.062 | 0.311 | 0.140 |
GCt FOS | −0.439 | 0.472 | 0.084 | −0.208 | −0.715 | −0.151 | −0.004 |
GCt ZENK | −0.729 | −0.355 | −0.168 | 0.540 | −0.099 | 0.057 | 0.095 |
VTA FOS | −0.262 | 0.559 | −0.678 | −0.248 | −0.116 | −0.073 | 0.281 |
VTA ZENK | −0.660 | −0.251 | −0.426 | 0.349 | −0.307 | 0.321 | 0.034 |
VMH FOS | −0.789 | 0.241 | −0.402 | −0.081 | 0.188 | −0.076 | −0.331 |
VMH ZENK | −0.541 | −0.423 | 0.566 | 0.114 | 0.155 | −0.405 | 0.081 |
LHy FOS | −0.737 | 0.128 | −0.428 | −0.219 | 0.440 | −0.028 | 0.125 |
LHy ZENK | −0.889 | −0.387 | −0.035 | 0.044 | 0.195 | −0.125 | 0.054 |
Cumulative % of Variation |
34.047 | 56.979 | 77.079 | 86.058 | 93.790 | 98.084 | 100.00 |
Figure 8.
Illustration of PCA results: A) Projection of IEG-labeling on the factor plane for PC1 and PC2 for control animals. B) Projection of IEG-labeling on the factor plane for PC1 and PC2 for POM-lesioned animals.
Table 3.
Summary of correlated associations between IEG-labeling in POM-lesioned and control animals based on PCA. Brain regions are listed in the order of the strength of their influence on the associated PC.
Control Males | POM Lesion Males | |
---|---|---|
Factors influencing PC1 with ≥ 10% variable contribution |
LHy, VMH, Area X, GCt | VMH, VTA, Uva |
Factors influencing PC2 with ≥ 10% variable contribution |
HVC, Area X, Uva | HVC, Area X, VTA |
The PCA performed on IEG data for POM-lesioned males yielded nine PCs (Table 2, Fig. 8B). Of these nine, the first two described 50.4% of the variation. The first PC mostly reflected FOS- and ZENK-labeling in VMH, FOS-labeling in VTA, and ZENK-labeling in Uva. The second PC was primarily described by FOS- and ZENK-labeling in HVC, ZENK-labeling in Area X, and FOS-labeling in VTA (Table 3).
Table 2.
The loadings and cumulative percentage of variation explained by each of the PCs obtained by PCA of FOS- and ZENK-labeled cells in seven brain regions in POM-lesioned males. Numbers in bold indicate variables contributing at least 10% to the first two PCs.
Principal Component Loadings | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PC1 | PC2 | PC3 | PC4 | PC5 | PC6 | PC7 | PC8 | PC9 | |
Variables | |||||||||
HVC FOS | 0.254 | 0.870 | 0.389 | 0.016 | 0.044 | −0.111 | −0.082 | −0.084 | 0.006 |
HVC ZENK | −0.026 | 0.784 | −0.487 | −0.267 | 0.051 | 0.121 | 0.012 | 0.233 | 0.063 |
Area X FOS | 0.382 | −0.188 | 0.583 | −0.474 | 0.198 | −0.226 | 0.371 | 0.160 | −0.023 |
Area X ZENK | −0.167 | 0.763 | −0.179 | 0.124 | 0.048 | −0.560 | 0.007 | −0.163 | −0.017 |
Uva FOS | 0.395 | 0.211 | 0.808 | 0.070 | 0.296 | −0.071 | −0.144 | 0.164 | −0.035 |
Uva ZENK | −0.721 | 0.357 | 0.231 | −0.044 | 0.386 | 0.313 | −0.209 | −0.033 | −0.070 |
GCt FOS | 0.613 | 0.545 | −0.391 | 0.360 | −0.131 | 0.058 | 0.128 | 0.088 | 0.005 |
GCt ZENK | 0.068 | −0.246 | 0.361 | 0.834 | −0.302 | −0.039 | 0.058 | 0.108 | −0.031 |
VTA FOS | 0.723 | −0.093 | 0.501 | 0.195 | 0.331 | 0.144 | −0.024 | −0.176 | 0.129 |
VTA ZENK | −0.490 | 0.643 | 0.370 | 0.312 | 0.004 | 0.242 | 0.223 | 0.064 | 0.006 |
VMH FOS | −0.968 | −0.085 | 0.021 | 0.065 | 0.024 | −0.096 | 0.168 | 0.062 | 0.091 |
VMH ZENK | −0.885 | −0.155 | 0.270 | 0.137 | 0.218 | −0.093 | 0.194 | −0.078 | 0.009 |
LHy FOS | −0.416 | 0.016 | 0.619 | −0.180 | −0.489 | −0.161 | −0.360 | 0.116 | 0.058 |
LHy ZENK | 0.069 | 0.360 | 0.506 | −0.363 | −0.581 | 0.239 | 0.222 | −0.183 | −0.020 |
Cumulative % of Variation |
28.274 | 50.433 | 70.793 | 81.265 | 89.309 | 94.160 | 97.900 | 99.708 | 100.00 |
DISCUSSION
POM lesions suppressed song and other courtship behaviors and altered activity in and interactions among nuclei that work in concert to promote sexually motivated communication. These data highlight relationships between the POM and multiple brain regions involved in song production, motivation and social behavior that likely work together to regulate sexually motivated song.
POM lesions disrupt sexually motivated song and other breeding context appropriate behaviors
In contrast to control males who displayed high levels of sexually motivated song and interest in nest boxes, POM-lesioned males behaved in a manner more typical of males in a non-breeding context. Specifically, POM-lesioned males produced shorter songs than control males, a behavior characteristic of the non-breeding season when song is used for purposes other than mate attraction (Riters et al., 2000). Consistent with this finding, females landed less often on the cages of POM-lesioned males than on cages of controls, suggesting lesions to the POM reduced the attractiveness to females of male song or other male attributes. In addition to these novel findings, our data confirmed past studies showing that relative to control males, POM-lesioned males produce fewer songs, take longer to initiate singing, and interact less frequently with a nest box in response to a female (Alger and Riters, 2006; Riters and Ball, 1999). Behavioral deficits were specific to sexually motivated behaviors, indicating that reductions in song and interest in a nest box produced by lesions did not represent global behavioral deficits.
IEG data suggest that the POM communicates with multiple regions to influence song
POM lesions disrupt activity in the song control system
Despite extensive overlap in song production (Fig. 6), POM-lesioned males had fewer IEG-labeled cells in Area X compared to controls. This suggests that differences in POM-lesioned and control males are not simply due to differences in the amount of song produced by the two groups but may be attributed to fundamental differences in the neural circuitry underlying song. (This is the case for Area X and additionally LHy, VMH and SL, discussed below). Similar to the mammalian basal ganglia, Area X appears to be part of a generalized pathway for sensorimotor learning, including song learning, in songbirds (reviewed in (Doupe et al., 2005; Margoliash, 1997; Person et al., 2008). Area X also exhibits differential activity depending on the social context in which song is produced (Hessler and Doupe, 1999; Jarvis and Nottebohm, 1997; Jarvis et al., 1998; Riters et al., 2004; Sasaki et al., 2006). Previous evidence shows that Area X activity is negatively related to song directed towards a conspecific in adult male zebra finches (Hessler and Doupe, 1999; Jarvis et al., 1998; Sasaki et al., 2006) and presumed sexually motivated song in male house sparrows (Riters et al., 2004). In contrast to these findings, in the present study the numbers of ZENK-ir cells in Area X of control males positively correlated with song production. Given the role of Area X in song learning, differences between this study and previous ones could relate to species differences in sensorimotor aspects of song learning between closed-ended learners (e.g. zebra finches and house sparrows) and open-ended learners (e.g. starlings). Alternatively, these differences could result from the fact that starlings do not sing directed and undirected song in as obvious a manner as do zebra finches and thus cannot be quantified in the same manner. Future studies are required to explore these ideas.
HVC is a song control region involved in motor output (reviewed in (Fee et al., 2004; Margoliash, 1997) that can be influenced by indirect projections from POM to DM, GCt, and VTA (Fig. 1; (Appeltants et al., 2000; Bottjer et al., 1989; Riters and Alger, 2004). HVC is implicated in the regulation of song bout length in starlings and other songbirds (e.g. (Bernard et al., 1996; DeVoogd et al., 1993), and here lesions to the POM abolished the positive correlation between FOS-ir and song bout length that was observed in control animals. This deficit along with the reduction of IEG-ir cells in HVC induced by POM-lesions may reflect a reduction in motor-driven IEG-expression, given that POM-lesioned males sang fewer and shorter songs than controls. However, the linear regression analysis suggests that this is not the case. The presence of POM lesions was the only significant predictor of the numbers of both FOS-ir and ZENK-ir cells in HVC, indicating that the effect of POM lesions on IEG-labeling in HVC was independent of behavioral differences. Interestingly, the precise rostral-caudal location of POM lesions affected HVC labeling, with caudal lesions more substantially reducing IEG-labeling compared to more rostral lesions. These results are consistent with past studies indicating that the POM consists of functionally distinct subdivisions (reviewed in (Balthazart and Ball, 2007).
The POM can also influence the motor pathway of the song control system through indirect projections to Uva (Bottjer et al., 1989; Riters and Alger, 2004; Striedter and Vu, 1998; Williams and Vicario, 1993); Fig. 1). Similar to HVC, Uva exhibits singing-induced IEG expression (Jarvis et al., 1998), suggesting the reduction in FOS-ir in Uva caused by POM lesions could also be a motor-driven effect resulting from the decreased song production in lesioned males. In support of this interpretation, both the presence of POM lesions and average song bout length were significant predictors of the number of FOS-ir cells in Uva. Uva is involved in interhemispheric coordination of song (reviewed in (Margoliash, 1997; Schmidt et al., 2004) and the integration of visual and auditory information (reviewed in (Margoliash, 1997; Wild, 1997). Thus, the disruption of activity in Uva resulting from lesions to the POM may underlie the failure of POM-lesioned males to sing long song appropriate within a courtship context.
POM lesions disrupt activity in limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain regions
The POM, GCt, VMH, and SL are considered components of a “social behavior network”, a reciprocally connected set of steroid sensitive nuclei proposed to underlie multiple forms of social behavior (Goodson, 2005; Newman, 1999). Although not proposed as part of this network, LHy is another region implicated in social behavior that is reciprocally connected with the POM (Riters and Alger, 2004). In the present study, POM lesions affected IEG-labeling within each of these regions.
Although the precise role of the septum in song is not known, septal modulation of male song is strongly associated with social context (e.g. (Goodson, 1998; Goodson et al., 1999; Heimovics and Riters, 2006, 2007). Past work in starlings shows correlations between IEG-labeling in SLcv and song production to be negative in males singing sexually motivated song (Heimovics and Riters, 2006, 2007). Consistent with these results, we found a negative correlation between song and FOS-labeling in SLcv in control males. POM lesions thus appeared to abolish this relationship. Perhaps damage to the POM disrupted the normal involvement of the septum in context appropriate song, causing lesioned males to sing in a manner uncharacteristic of the breeding season. In the present study, IEG quantification was also taken within another subdivision of SL, SLcd. Both the presence of POM lesions and average song bout length were significant predictors of the number of ZENK-ir cells in SLcd. POM lesions significantly decreased IEG-labeling in SLcd and eliminated IEG correlations with song bout length in SLcd. Interestingly, in contrast to SLcv, IEG-labeling in SLcd correlated positively with song, consistent with several studies indicating that the septum consists of functionally distinct subdivisions (Goodson et al., 2004; Goodson et al., 2005; Heimovics and Riters, 2006, 2007). These results are consistent with data suggesting a complex, possibly subdivision specific role for the septum in song regulation that may differ depending upon social context. The precise role of the septum and its subregions in song must be clarified in future studies.
POM lesions additionally reduced FOS-ir cell counts in LHy and eliminated the positive correlations between ZENK-labeling in VMH and LHy and song production. Both the presence of POM lesions and the number of preening events were significant predictors of the number of ZENK-ir cells in LHy, suggesting that the effects of POM lesions on ZENK-immunolabeling is not completely independent of behavior. Both LHy and VMH are hypothalamic regions that respond to sexually salient cues (e.g. (Orsini et al., 1985). Furthermore, LHy activation may be reinforcing (reviewed in (Wise, 1996). Thus, by affecting activity in VMH and LHy, POM lesions may disrupt sexual salience or reward associated with seeing a female or the act of singing itself. Interestingly, previous work showed a positive correlation between the number of ZENK-labeled cells in VMH and song production in breeding condition male starlings but not non-breeding condition males (Heimovics and Riters, 2007). So in this way, POM-lesioned breeding condition male starlings again appear to be similar to non-breeding condition males.
POM-lesioned males had fewer IEG-ir cells in GCt compared to controls. GCt (the avian homolog of mammalian periaqueductal gray) is implicated in sexual behavior (e.g. (Charlier et al., 2005; Floody and DeBold, 2004; Heimovics and Riters, 2007; Marson, 2004) and vocal communication in multiple species (reviewed in (Jurgens, 1994). Interestingly, FOS-labeling in GCt was positively correlated with song bout length in POM-lesioned animals, but not in control animals. Although difficult to interpret, this result could reflect a compensatory response of GCt to POM lesions. Furthermore, the presence of POM lesions was found to contribute significantly to the number of FOS-ir cells in GCt; however, behavioral measures including the number of times the bird sang, fed, drank and preened, were also significant predictors. This suggests complex interrelationships between POM lesions, behavior and IEG-labeling in GCt.
POM lesions alter relationships between song and social behavior nuclei
POM lesions affected how song and social behavior nuclei work together as a circuit, as indicated by different first and second principal components in POM-lesioned males compared to controls (Table 3). Furthermore, males with and without POM lesions had different association patterns among IEG-immunoreactivity in various brain regions when plotted on these PCs. When IEG-ir cell counts for each brain area in controls were graphed by the first two PCs, all song control nuclei grouped together and nuclei involved in social behavior formed a separate group, suggesting separate physiological associations in activity (Fig. 8). This pattern was lost in POM-lesioned males, suggesting that interfering with normal POM function disrupts song production by disrupting relationships among multiple brain regions.
Conclusion
Our results indicate that the POM interacts with a number of song production, limbic, hypothalamic and midbrain circuits during the production of sexually motivated song and nest box interactions. Lesions to the POM may thus hinder sexually motivated song through impedance of interhemispheric coordination and motor aspects of song production, motivation and reward associated with song production, and integration of external social context information with the animal’s internal state.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This paper is based upon work supported by NIMH grants R01MH65645 and R01MH080225 to LVR and an NSF graduate research fellowship to SJA. We gratefully acknowledge Sara Dudgeon, Charity Juang and Benjamin Pawlisch for their help with tissue processing; Becky Skover for surgical assistance; Benjamin Pawlisch, Jenny Beissinger, Lakshmi Gudimella and Jenny Papenthien for song recording; Kate Skogen and Martin Lund for animal care; Bill Feeny for assistance with illustrations; and the editors and anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on the manuscript.
ABBREVIATIONS
- AC
anterior commissure
- Area X
proper name
- DLM
medial nucleus of the dorsolateral thalamus
- DM
dorsomedial nucleus intercollicularis
- DSV
ventral supraoptic decussation
- GCt
midbrain central gray
- HVC
abbreviation as proper name
- IEG
immediate early gene
- LHy
lateral hypothalamus
- LMAN
lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium
- nCPa
nucleus commissurae pallii
- Nif
nucleus interface of the nidopallium
- NIII
oculomotor nerve
- nXIIts
tracheosyringeal portion of the hypoglossal nucleus
- POM
medial preoptic nucleus
- PVN
paraventricular nucleus
- RA
robust nucleus of the arcopallium
- Ram/rVRG
nucleus retroambigualis/rostral ventral respiratory group
- Rt
nucleus rotundus
- SL
lateral septal nucleus
- SLcd
dorsal subdivision of caudal lateral septum
- SLcv
ventral subdivision of caudal lateral septum
- TnA
nucleus taeniae of the arcopallium
- TSM
septopallio-mesencephalic tract
- Uva
nucleus uvaeformis
- vIII
third ventricle
- VMH
ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus
- VTA
ventral tegmental area
REFERENCES
- Alger SJ, Riters LV. Lesions to the medial preoptic nucleus differentially affect singing and nest box-directed behaviors within and outside of the breeding season in European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behav Neurosci. 2006;120:1326–1336. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.120.6.1326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appeltants D, Absil P, Balthazart J, Ball GF. Identification of the origin of catecholaminergic inputs to HVc in canaries by retrograde tract tracing combined with tyrosine hydroxylase immunocytochemistry. J Chem Neuroanat. 2000;18:117–133. doi: 10.1016/s0891-0618(99)00054-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ball GF, Balthazart J. Hormonal regulation of brain circuits mediating male sexual behavior in birds. Physiology & Behavior. 2004;83:329–346. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ball GF, Sockman KW, Duffy DL, Gentner TQ. A neuroethological approach to song behavior and perception in European starlings: Interrelationships among testosterone, neuroanatomy, immediate early gene expression, and immune function. Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol 36. 2006;36:59–121. [Google Scholar]
- Balthazart J, Ball GF. Topography in the preoptic region: differential regulation of appetitive and consummatory male sexual behaviors. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2007;28:161–178. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2007.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard DJ, Eens M, Ball GF. Age- and behavior-related variation in volumes of song control nuclei in male European starlings. J Neurobiol. 1996;30:329–339. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4695(199607)30:3<329::AID-NEU2>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer SW, Johnson F. Circuits, hormones, and learning: vocal behavior in songbirds. Journal of Neurobiology. 1997;33:602–618. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<602::aid-neu8>3.0.co;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottjer SW, Halsema KA, Brown SA, Miesner EA. Axonal connections of a forebrain nucleus involved with vocal learning in zebra finches. J Comp Neurol. 1989;279:312–326. doi: 10.1002/cne.902790211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard MS. Contributions of the anterior forebrain pathway to vocal plasticity. Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong. 2004;1016:377–394. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlier TD, Ball GF, Balthazart J. Sexual behavior activates the expression of the immediate early genes c-fos and Zenk (EGR-1) in catecholaminergic neurons of male Japanese quail. Neuroscience. 2005;131:13–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.09.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeVoogd TJ, Krebs JR, Healy SD, Purvis A. Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analyses amongst oscine birds. Proc Biol Sci. 1993;254:75–82. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1993.0129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doupe AJ, Perkel DJ, Reiner A, Stern EA. Birdbrains could teach basal ganglia research a new song. Trends in Neurosciences. 2005;28:353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duffy DL, Bentley GE, Drazen DL, Ball GF. Effects of testosterone on cell-mediated and humoral immunity in non-breeding adult European starlings. Behavioral Ecology. 2000;11:654–662. [Google Scholar]
- Eens M. Understanding the complex song of the European starling: and integrated ethological approach. Advances in the Study of Behavior. 1997;26:255–435. [Google Scholar]
- Eens M, Pinxten R, Verheyen RF. Male Song as a Cue for Mate Choice in the European Starling. Behaviour. 1991;116:210–238. [Google Scholar]
- Falk H, Gwinner E. Timing of photorefractoriness in the European starling - significance of photoperiod early and late in the reproductive-cycle. Biology of Reproduction. 1988;39:1004–1008. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod39.5.1004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fee MS, Kozhevnikov AA, Hahnloser RHR. Neural mechanisms of vocal sequence generation in the songbird. Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong. 2004;1016:153–170. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Floody OR, DeBold JF. Effects of midbrain lesions on lordosis and ultrasound production. Physiology & Behavior. 2004;82:791–804. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gentner TQ, Hulse SH. Female European starling preference and choice for variation in conspecific male song. Animal Behaviour. 2000;59:443–458. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL. Territorial aggression and dawn song are modulated by septal vasotocin and vasoactive intestinal polypeptide in male field sparrows (Spizella pusilla) Horm Behav. 1998;34:67–77. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL. The vertebrate social behavior network: evolutionary themes and variations. Hormones and Behavior. 2005;48:11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL, Evans AK, Lindberg L. Chemoarchitectonic subdivisions of the songbird septum and a comparative overview of septum chemical anatomy in jawed vertebrates. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2004;473:293–314. doi: 10.1002/cne.20061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL, Eibach R, Sakata J, Adkins-Regan E. Effect of septal lesions on male song and aggression in the colonial zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) and the territorial field sparrow (Spizella pusilla) Behav Brain Res. 1999;101:167–180. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson JL, Evans AK, Lindberg L, Allen CD. Neuro-evolutionary patterning of sociality. Proc Biol Sci. 2005;272:227–235. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heimovics SA, Riters LV. Immediate early gene activity in song control nuclei and brain areas regulating motivation relates positively to singing behavior during, but not outside of, a breeding context. Journal of Neurobiology. 2005;65:207–224. doi: 10.1002/neu.20181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heimovics SA, Riters LV. Breeding-context-dependent relationships between song and cFOS labeling within social behavior brain regions in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Horm Behav. 2006;50:726–735. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heimovics SA, Riters LV. ZENK labeling within social behavior brain regions reveals breeding context-dependent patterns of neural activity associated with song in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behav Brain Res. 2007;176:333–343. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.10.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hessler NA, Doupe AJ. Social context modulates singing-related neural activity in the songbird forebrain. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:209–211. doi: 10.1038/6306. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hull EM, Lorrain DS, Du J, Matuszewich L, Lumley LA, Putnam SK, Moses J. Hormone-neurotransmitter interactions in the control of sexual behavior. Behav Brain Res. 1999;105:105–116. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(99)00086-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis ED, Nottebohm F. Motor-driven gene expression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA. 1997;94:4097–4102. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.8.4097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis ED, Scharff C, Grossman MR, Ramos JA, Nottebohm F. For whom the bird sings: context-dependent gene expression. Neuron. 1998;21:775–788. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80594-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jurgens U. The role of the periaqueductal grey in vocal behaviour. Behav Brain Res. 1994;62:107–117. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)90017-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margoliash D. Functional organization of forebrain pathways for song production and perception. Journal of Neurobiology. 1997;33:671–693. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<671::aid-neu12>3.0.co;2-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marson L. Lesions of the periaqueductal gray block the medial preoptic area-induced activation of the urethrogenital reflex in male rats. Neuroscience Letters. 2004;367:278–282. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountjoy DJ, Lemon RE. Female choice for complex song in the European starling: A field experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1996;38:65–71. [Google Scholar]
- Newman SW. The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior. A node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1999;877:242–257. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orsini JC, Jourdan F, Cooper HM, Monmaur P. Influence of female odors on lateral hypothalamus in the male rat. Semiquantitative deoxyglucose analysis. Physiol Behav. 1985;35:509–516. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90132-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panzica GC, Viglietti-Panzica C, Balthazart J. The sexually dimorphic medial preoptic nucleus of quail: a key brain area mediating steroid action on male sexual behavior. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1996;17:51–125. doi: 10.1006/frne.1996.0002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Person AL, Gale SD, Farries MA, Perkel DJ. Organization of the songbird basal ganglia, including Area X. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2008;508:840–866. doi: 10.1002/cne.21699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riters LV, Ball GF. Lesions to the medial preoptic area affect singing in the male European starling (Sturnus vulgaris) Hormones and Behavior. 1999;36:276–286. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1999.1549. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riters LV, Alger SJ. Neuroanatomical evidence for indirect connections between the medial preoptic nucleus and the song control system: possible neural substrates for sexually motivated song. Cell and Tissue Research. 2004;316:35–44. doi: 10.1007/s00441-003-0838-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riters LV, Teague DP, Schroeder MB, Cummings SE. Vocal production in different social contexts relates to variation in immediate early gene immunoreactivity within and outside of the song control system. Behavioural Brain Research. 2004;155:307–318. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riters LV, Eens M, Pinxten R, Duffy DL, Balthazart J, Ball GF. Seasonal changes in courtship song and the medial preoptic area in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Hormones and Behavior. 2000;38:250–261. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riters LV, Schroeder MB, Auger CJ, Eens M, Pinxten R, Ball GF. Evidence for opioid involvement in the regulation of song production in male European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) Behavioral Neuroscience. 2005;119:245–255. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.1.245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki A, Sotnikova TD, Gainetdinov RR, Jarvis ED. Social context-dependent singing-regulated dopamine. J Neurosci. 2006;26:9010–9014. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1335-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt MF, Ashmore RC, Vu ET. Bilateral control and interhemispheric coordination in the avian song motor system. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2004;1016:171–186. doi: 10.1196/annals.1298.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sockman KW, Gentner TQ, Ball GF. Recent experience modulates forebrain gene-expression in response to mate-choice cues in European starlings. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences. 2002;269:2479–2485. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Striedter GF, Vu ET. Bilateral feedback projections to the forebrain in the premotor network for singing in zebra finches. Journal of Neurobiology. 1998;34:27–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Velho TAF, Pinaud R, Rodrigues PV, Mello CV. Co-induction of activity-dependent genes in songbirds. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;22:1667–1678. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04369.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wild JM. Neural pathways for the control of birdsong production. Journal of Neurobiology. 1997;33:653–670. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<653::aid-neu11>3.0.co;2-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams H, Vicario DS. Temporal Patterning of Song Production - Participation of Nucleus-Uvaeformis of the Thalamus. Journal of Neurobiology. 1993;24:903–912. doi: 10.1002/neu.480240704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wise RA. Addictive drugs and brain stimulation reward. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1996;19:319–340. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.19.030196.001535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood RI. Integration of chemosensory and hormonal input in the male Syrian hamster brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1998;855:362–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb10594.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]