Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2009 Jan 21;29(3):705–715. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3822-08.2009

Mood Influences Supraspinal Pain Processing Separately from Attention

Chantal Villemure 1,, M Catherine Bushnell 1
PMCID: PMC2768393  NIHMSID: NIHMS92861  PMID: 19158297

Abstract

Studies show that inducing a positive mood or diverting attention from pain decreases pain perception. Nevertheless, induction manipulations, such as viewing interesting movies or performing mathematical tasks, often influence both emotional and attentional states. Imaging studies have examined the neural basis of psychological pain modulation, but none has explicitly separated the effects of emotion and attention. Using odors to modulate mood and shift attention from pain, we previously showed that the perceptual consequences of changing mood differed from those of altering attention, with mood primarily altering pain unpleasantness and attention preferentially altering pain intensity. These findings suggest that brain circuits involved in pain modulation provoked by mood or attention are partially separable. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging to directly compare the neurocircuitry involved in mood- and attention-related pain modulation. We manipulated independently mood state and attention direction, using tasks involving heat pain and pleasant and unpleasant odors. Pleasant odors, independent of attentional focus, induced positive mood changes and decreased pain unpleasantness and pain-related activity within the anterior cingulate (ACC), medial thalamus, and primary and secondary somatosensory cortices. The effects of attentional state were less robust, with only the activity in anterior insular cortex (aIC) showing possible attentional modulation. Lateral inferior frontal cortex [LinfF; Brodmann's area (BA) 45/47] activity correlated with mood-related modulation, whereas superior posterior parietal (SPP; BA7) and entorhinal activity correlated with attention-related modulation. ACC activity covaried with LinfF and periacqueductal gray activity, whereas aIC activity covaried with SPP activity. These findings suggest that separate neuromodulatory circuits underlie emotional and attentional modulation of pain.

Keywords: pain, fMRI, attention, emotion, odor, heat

Introduction

Attention and emotion impart a value system to sensory processing, allowing the CNS to mold sensory experience into a subjective landscape (Mesulam, 1998). Experimental and clinical studies show that diverting attention from pain and inducing a positive mood usually decrease pain perception, whereas focusing on pain or inducing a negative mood increases it (Villemure and Bushnell, 2002).

Imaging studies of attentional pain modulation commonly show that decreased pain during distraction is paralleled by decreased activity in thalamocortical ascending pain pathways (Apkarian et al., 2005). Such areas include thalamus, primary and secondary somatosensory cortices (S1, S2), insular cortex (IC), and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Bushnell et al., 1999; Frankenstein et al., 2001; Longe et al., 2001; Bantick et al., 2002; Brooks et al., 2002; Hoffman et al., 2004; Seminowicz et al., 2004; Buffington et al., 2005; Wiech et al., 2005; Dunckley et al., 2007). Brain areas actively involved in modulating pain transmission during distraction from pain have been less studied. Existing reports suggest that periacqueductal gray (PAG), ACC, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and posterior parietal cortex (PPC) are involved (Peyron et al., 1999; Petrovic et al., 2000; Tracey et al., 2002; Valet et al., 2004).

Nevertheless, most of these studies have not measured or controlled for simultaneous changes in the subjects' emotional state or arousal evoked by the stimuli and/or tasks creating the distraction. In some cases, scans were compared during a pain-alone condition and a condition in which pain was presented along with another sensory stimulus (Longe et al., 2001; Brooks et al., 2002; Hoffman et al., 2004). Other studies presented pain with and without a demanding cognitive task (Petrovic et al., 2000; Frankenstein et al., 2001; Bantick et al., 2002; Seminowicz et al., 2004; Valet et al., 2004; Buffington et al., 2005). Either way, the level of arousal, anxiety, and/or mood could differ between conditions, contributing to the findings.

Imaging studies directly addressing emotional pain modulation are fewer. One study evaluated the influence of anxiety on pain perception (Ploghaus et al., 2001). High anxiety increased pain and evoked more activity in entorhinal cortex (ERC), mid insular cortex (midIC), and perigenual cingulate. Another study showed that nonpainful esophageal stimulation evoked more ACC activity in a fearful than in a neutral emotional context (Phillips et al., 2003). Furthermore, high fear increased discomfort and anxiety ratings as well as brain activation within ACC, anterior insular cortex (aIC), and Brodmann's area (BA) 45 more than low fear. Neither study measured or controlled for possible concurrent changes in the subjects' attentional state.

We previously showed differential pain modulation by mood and attention, with mood modifying preferentially pain unpleasantness and attention affecting mainly pain intensity (Villemure et al., 2003). Consistently, other studies show that mood selectively alters pain affect (Zelman et al., 1991) and pain unpleasantness is more strongly affected by emotions than pain intensity (Rainville et al., 2005). Together, these studies suggest that emotions and attention alter pain by invoking partially separable neuromodulatory circuits. To test this hypothesis, we directly compared changes in neural activity associated with emotional and attentional pain modulation, using the paradigm that permitted us to dissociate the perceptual effects of these manipulations (Villemure et al., 2003).

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Fourteen subjects (five males) between 18 and 28 years of age (mean, 23; SD, 3) completed the study and were paid for their participation. The study was approved by the McGill University Institutional Review Board, and written informed consent was obtained from each subject. Potential subjects were included if they demonstrated attentional or emotional modulations in a screening session using our experimental task. Exclusion criteria were the same as in our previous study (Villemure et al., 2003), with the addition of significant claustrophobia and other magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-related contraindications. Subjects were instructed not to wear scented products.

General procedure

Written consent was obtained, olfactory function was evaluated (for details, see Villemure et al., 2003), and olfactory and thermal stimuli were chosen for each subject. The specific temperatures were determined individually to produce moderate pain in the absence of odors, and odorants were chosen individually to insure highly pleasant and unpleasant valences. Subjects then practiced the intensity discrimination task involving either the olfactory or thermal stimuli, to direct attention to the thermal or olfactory modality, while simultaneously manipulating odor hedonics. Subjects demonstrating attentional or emotional modulation of pain perception in the discrimination task were scheduled for their scanning session on a separate day. On the scanning day, in-scanner ventilation was stopped to prevent odor dispersion. An anatomical scan was first acquired, followed by five functional runs. In the first run, subjects received alternating warm and painful heat stimuli. The other runs were devoted to the four conditions of the intensity discrimination task described below.

Painful heat stimuli

A temperature that evoked moderate pain in the absence of the experimental odors was chosen for each subject, by presenting two ascending series of discrete temperatures ranging from 36 to 50°C to three areas of the inner left calf using a 9 cm2 contact thermode (TSA II NeuroSensory analyzer; Medoc Advanced Medical System). Each stimulus had a plateau time of 3 s and a rise/fall time of 10°C/s leading to an approximate stimulus length of 5 s. After each heat pulse, subjects rated stimulus intensity and pleasantness/unpleasantness, and a temperature was identified that the subject rated ∼5 on the intensity scale (0–10). A second heat stimulus was identified that subjects could distinguish from the first with ∼80% accuracy to be used in the discrimination task. This stimulus was typically 1° more. In the discrimination task described below, the lower painful temperature was always used as the first stimulus of the pair, whereas the higher painful temperature appeared pseudo-randomly in 50% of cases as the second stimulus of the pair. Subjects were never notified that only two painful temperatures would be used in the experimental tasks.

Odor stimuli

Subjects evaluated six odors diluted to 0.1–5% v/v in an appropriate inodorous solvent (distilled water or propylene glycol). Different types of odors were presented including pyridine, which has a smell reminiscent of rotten fish at the concentration used (Sigma-Aldrich Canada), and cosmetic-grade fragrance oils with food and floral scents: China rain, creamsicle, lemon meringue, mint, and violet (K & W Specialties). For the odor selection, the diluted fragrances (10 ml) were presented in 60 ml amber bottles identified with numbers. Using the pleasantness/unpleasantness ratings, the most preferred and disliked odors were chosen.

During the discrimination task (described below), 5 s odor pulses were delivered by a computer-controlled odor generator (Knosys Olfactometers). A transistor–transistor logic pulse generated by the thermode at the beginning of the heat pulse triggered the opening of the appropriate valve of the olfactometer, resulting in synchronized presentation of the thermal and olfactory stimuli. An air flow of 0.4 l/min was used. The odorized air from each independent channel reached the subjects through a 10-m-long Teflon-lined tube. Each tube was connected to a separate leg of a glass manifold. A separate opening of the manifold connected to a vacuum pump that operated between presentations to prevent lingering odors. A Y-shape glass piece was secured with tape just below the subject's nostrils for birhinal stimulation. A 10 × 21 cm Teflon sheet was loosely taped over the subject's nose and mouth to prevent odor dispersion. Subjects were instructed to close their mouth and to breathe normally through their nose. The concentrations used in the discrimination task were 0.1 and 1% v/v for pyridine, 0.5 and 5% v/v for mint, and 0.3 and 3% v/v for the other odors. The stronger concentration was always used as the second stimulus of the pair, whereas the weaker concentration appeared pseudo-randomly in 50% of cases as the first stimulus of the pair. None of the odors used were judged as pungent or irritating.

Measures

Numerical rating scales (NRSs) of 0–10 were used to evaluate the intensity and hedonic quality (pleasantness/unpleasantness) of the odor and painful stimuli. We stressed the differences between stimulus intensity and pleasantness/unpleasantness using explanations adapted from Price et al. (1983). Mood and anxiety were also assessed with similar scales. The odor intensity scale was anchored with 0 (no odor) and 10 (extremely intense). The pain intensity scale was anchored with 0 (no pain) and 10 (most intense pain tolerable). The pleasantness scale was used for odor evaluation and anchored with 0 (neutral) and 10 (extremely pleasant). The unpleasantness scale was used for both the odor and painful stimuli and anchored with 0 (neutral) and 10 (extremely unpleasant). The good and bad mood scales were anchored with 0 (neutral) and 10 (extremely good or bad). The calmness and anxiety scales were anchored with 0 (neutral) and 10 (extremely calm or anxious). Subjects gave their ratings verbally.

Experimental tasks

Alternating warm/pain task.

In this task, depicted in Figure 1, subjects were asked to attend 20 warm (40°C) and 20 painfully hot (the lowest temperature used in the discrimination task) stimuli presented in an alternating manner with a 4 s interstimulus interval (ISI). Puffs of unscented air (5 s) were delivered with each thermal stimulus. After the termination of the scan, subjects were asked to verbally give their overall ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness, odor intensity and hedonics, mood, and anxiety/calmness using the scales described above.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Alternating warm/pain task. In this task, subjects received 20 warm (40°C) and 20 painfully hot (3 s plateau) stimuli in an alternating manner separated by a 4 s ISI in which the temperature was 38°C. Three areas of the inner calf were used for the presentation of the heat stimuli. The thermode was moved from one area to the next after each warm and painful heat pair. Puffs of unscented air were delivered with each thermal stimulus. Subjects had to attend to the heat and were asked for their ratings at the end of the trial (see Materials and Methods for details). Because the baseline temperature and the rise/fall time were constants (38 and 10°C/s, respectively), the total duration of the task varied according to the temperatures used (higher painful temperatures resulted in longer duration). The shadowed boxes represent the time periods analyzed for brain activations. Periods of baseline heat when the thermode was not moved (pale gray boxes) were subtracted from painful heat periods (dark gray boxes).

Intensity discrimination/attention task.

For this task, both painful heat and an odorant were presented simultaneously, but on some trials subjects performed a heat-intensity discrimination task, whereas on other trials they performed an odor-intensity discrimination task, thus ensuring that they attended to one or the other sensory modality. In addition to manipulating direction of attention, in this task the hedonic value of the odorant was manipulated so that a pleasant odor was presented in half the trials and an unpleasant odor was presented in the other half. Figure 2 provides a depiction of the paradigm. Stimuli of a pair were separated by 4 s, and the interpair interval was set at 12 s. Subjects received four separate conditions, each including 12 discriminations lasting ∼5 min. In one condition, subjects attended to the intensity of the noxious heat in the presence of the good odor (AHGO), and in another, in the presence of the bad odor (AHBO). A third and fourth condition involved attending to the intensity of the good (AOGO) or bad (AOBO) odor in the presence of noxious thermal stimuli. The order of the conditions was counterbalanced across subjects. In 50% of cases (pseudo-random), both stimuli of the pair were the same intensity. The subjects' task was to decide whether the second stimulus of the pair was stronger or the same intensity as the first stimulus. They used a nonverbal code to give their answer (closed their left fist for “same” and a thumb up for “stronger”). They received nonverbal feedback about their performance after each discrimination (the experimenter tapped the subject's ankle once to indicate that the answer was correct and three times to indicate a wrong answer). These nonverbal signals were given during time periods excluded from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) analysis. To maximize the attentional demand of the discrimination tasks, we chose stimulus intensity differences that led to a submaximal performance (we aimed for 80% correct). Immediately after the 12th discrimination of each condition, once the scan acquisition was over, subjects provided the experimenter with verbal ratings of overall pain intensity and unpleasantness, odor intensity and hedonics, mood, and anxiety/calmness using the scales described above.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Intensity discrimination task. For this task, both painful heat and odorants (pleasant or unpleasant) were delivered simultaneously. The thermode was moved after each pair of stimuli to another of the three regions of the inner calf. In the example shown here, the subjects attended the painful heat and performed the heat intensity discrimination task, evaluating whether the second stimulus of the pair was stronger or the same intensity as the first after each pair. Twelve discriminations were performed in each of the following four conditions: (1) attending heat in the presence of a good odor (AHGO); (2) attending heat in the presence of a bad odor (AHBO); (3) attending to a good odor in the presence of painful heat (AOGO); and (4) attending to a bad odor in the presence of painful heat (AOBO). The order was counterbalanced between subjects. At the end of the trial, subjects gave their ratings of pain intensity and unpleasantness, odor intensity and hedonics, mood, and anxiety (see Materials and Methods for details). Because the baseline temperature and the rise/fall time were constants (38 and 10°C/s, respectively), the total duration of the task varied according to the temperatures used (higher temperatures resulted in longer duration). The shadowed boxes represent the time periods analyzed for brain activations reported in this study. Periods of baseline with a touch component (pale gray boxes) were subtracted from periods of painful heat (dark gray boxes).

Statistical analysis of behavioral data

All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS 15.0 for Windows (SPSS). A significance level of p < 0.05 was adopted for all analyses. We used the general linear model with two repeated measures with two levels each (ATTENTION: pain or odor; ODOR VALENCE: good or bad) for the separate analyses of the dependent variables performance, pain intensity, pain unpleasantness, mood, and anxiety. The Tukey's honest significance difference (HSD) was used for post hoc analyses whenever appropriate. In addition, we used stepwise linear regression analysis to examine the model that best predicted changes in pain unpleasantness ratings. The independent variables used in the regression model were changes in odor hedonics, mood, and anxiety. Pain unpleasantness ratings in the GO condition were subtracted from the pain unpleasantness ratings obtained in the BO condition. Odor hedonics, mood, and anxiety ratings obtained in the BO condition were subtracted from those obtained in the GO conditions. A positive number reflected an improvement in pain, odor hedonics, mood, or anxiety associated with the pleasant odorant.

MRI acquisition and general procedures

A 1.5 tesla MRI scanner (Siemens) with a standard head coil was used. Anatomical scans were collected using a high-resolution, T1-weighted anatomical protocol [repetition time (TR), 22 ms; echo time (TE), 9.2 ms; flip angle, 30°; field of view, 256 mm2]. Functional scans were collected using a blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) protocol with a T2*-weighted gradient echoplanar imaging sequence (TR, 4 s; TE, 50 ms; flip angle, 90°). The scanning planes were oriented parallel to the line between the anterior and posterior commissures. Thirty-six 4 mm slices were acquired covering the brain from the top of the cortex down to the base of the cerebellum.

Global searches for pain-evoked activation

Functional data were first corrected for motion by registering all volume acquisitions to the third volume in the scan and then spatially smoothed with a 6 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. The first three volumes were excluded to ensure steady-state magnetization. Both the anatomical and functional volumes were resampled into standard stereotaxic space based on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 305 template (Collins et al., 1994). Therefore, all activations are reported using MNI coordinates. Functional images were processed, and t-statistic images representing changes in BOLD contrast were generated with fMRISTAT-MULTISTAT software developed at the MNI (Montreal, Canada; available at http://www.math.mcgill.ca/keith/BICstat). The statistical analysis of the fMRI data was based on a linear model with correlated errors (Worsley et al., 2002). Temporal drift was removed by adding polynomial covariates in the frame times, up to degree 3, to the design matrix. All t-statistic images were thresholded (p = 0.05) using a Bonferroni correction based on the number of voxels in the search region or the random field theory, whichever gave the minimum threshold (Worsley et al., 1996; Cao, 1999).

The volume of the whole-brain gray matter was estimated to be 1,200,000 mm3 (18,750 voxels), yielding a threshold t value of 4.8 for the global search.

Directed search analysis

Based on a review of previous studies (Apkarian et al., 2005), directed searches of pain processing-related areas were performed in S1 leg area, S2, aIC, entire IC, dorsal ACC, perigenual cingulate, and thalamus. For pain modulatory areas, directed searches were performed in regions previously shown to be activated during psychological modulation of pain or otherwise implicated in attentional or emotional processes. These areas were the PAG (Tracey et al., 2002; Valet et al., 2004), the inferior frontal cortex including the orbital parts (Esch and Stefano, 2004; Kringelbach, 2005; Petrovic et al., 2005), the ERC (Ploghaus et al., 2001), and the superior parietal cortex (Petersen et al., 1994; Hsieh et al., 1995; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Duncan and Albanese, 2003; Behrmann et al., 2004; Shomstein and Yantis, 2004, 2006; Fan et al., 2005; Hahn et al., 2006; Lepsien and Nobre, 2006). The volume of the different regions of interest (ROIs) were obtained by drawing each region on the average brain of the 14 subjects, except for frontal and S1 regions where sulcal anatomy was not clear enough in the average brain. For these two regions, individual brains were used. These volumes were used to calculate the threshold t values for directed searches and extent thresholds for cluster analysis. Refer to Table 1 for the volumes, peak thresholds, and cluster size thresholds of the different ROIs.

Table 1.

Thresholds for the different ROIs

Brain area Total volume (mm3) Threshold t for peak Threshold volume (mm3) for cluster with t > 2.5
Thresholds for pain processing-related areas
    S1 leg area 4229 3.2622 177
    S2 9177 3.4989 324
    aIC 6968 3.4171 268
    Entire IC 14,509 3.6369 424
    ACC 6647 3.4030 259
    Perigenual cingulate 4840 3.3061 201
    Thalamus 6892 3.3144 266
Thresholds for pain modulator areas
    ERC 2439 3.0855 93
    PAG 385 2.4350 n.a.
    Superior parietal 12,906 3.6017 397
    Inferior frontal cortex including lateral OFC 9000 3.4947 320

n.a., Not applicable.

fMRI analysis for alternating warm/pain task

In this task, we contrasted the brain activation evoked by pain (defined as the beginning of each painful temperature plateau plus 4 s) with the brain activation evoked by the baseline temperature (defined as the 3.5 s period preceding the rise of pain) (Fig. 1). This represented the brain activity associated with heat pain for each subject. We then combined the scans of all subjects to obtain group statistical maps. Global and directed searches within the group statistical map permitted the identification of the thalamic and cortical areas representing the pain network susceptible to be modified by the attentional and emotional modulations in this study.

fMRI analysis of discrimination task

Identification of pain-processing areas.

To determine brain regions activated by the painful stimuli, we compared the BOLD signal during pain with that during a contrast period of nonpainful temperature. Analyses were performed contrasting the first painful stimulus of each pair, the second painful stimulus of each pair, or all painful stimuli of the pairs with the 3.5 s period preceding the rise of either the first or second painful stimulus of the pair. Independent of whether we used the first, second, or both painful stimuli of the pairs, we found the most robust pain-evoked activation using the contrast period preceding the second stimulus of each pair. Furthermore, overall, using all painful stimuli generated higher t values, so data presented below were determined by contrasting all painful stimuli to the period preceding the second stimuli of each pair (Fig. 2).

The contrast was obtained for all four experimental conditions (AHGO, AHBO, AOGO, and AOBO) for each subject. For each subject, we combined specific contrasts to obtain the brain activation associated with (1) attending heat regardless of odor type (AH = AHGO + AHBO), (2) attending odors with pain present regardless of odor type (AO = AOGO + AOBO), (3) good odor regardless of attention direction (GO = AOGO + AHGO), and (4) bad odor regardless of attention direction (BO = AOBO + AHBO) and performed the following subtractions (1) AH − AO and (2) BO − GO. The scans of all subjects for each of these contrasts were combined to obtain group statistical maps. Examination of the group statistical map of AH, AO, and AH − AO identified the pain processing-related areas modulated by attention direction, whereas examination of BO, GO, and BO − GO identified the pain processing-related areas modulated by emotional context. For the subtraction maps, because we were testing unidirectional a priori hypotheses about the activation decrease caused by distraction or mood improvement related to the pleasant odors, we applied a threshold t value of 1.771 (df = 13; p < 0.05, single sided). This threshold t value was adjusted for the number of subtractions being assessed in a given condition. For example, when evaluating four subtractions in a given condition, a threshold t value of 2.533 was used (df = 13; p < 0.0125, single sided).

Identification of possible pain modulators.

To identify brain areas possibly implicated in attentional and emotional pain modulation, we linearly weighted the BOLD signal with the subject's NRS ratings (for AH, AO, BO, GO) and changes in NRS ratings between conditions (for subtractions AH − AO and BO − GO). Pain intensity scores were used as the weighting factor for the attentional condition involving the comparison of AH and AO, because direction of attention primarily influences perceived pain intensity (Villemure et al., 2003). Pain unpleasantness scores were used as the weighting factor for the emotional modulation involving the comparison of BO and GO, because odor-evoked changes in mood primarily influence pain unpleasantness (Villemure et al., 2003). To consider a region modulatory for pain, we required that the activation in that region meet the following criteria: (1) the area was significantly activated in the individual conditions (AH and BO) when weighted with pain perception scores; and, most importantly, (2) the area was significantly activated in the subtractions (AH − AO and BO − GO) weighted with the change in pain perception. We refer to these regions as possible pain modulators.

Covariation (functional connectivity) analyses

Functional connectivity analyses can identify brain regions in which the BOLD response covaries with the activity of a selected reference voxel during a particular task. We were interested to see whether the activity in the key pain-processing regions modulated by the attentional and emotional manipulations covaried with the activity of regions identified as possible pain modulators. Covariation of the signal of the pain processing-related regions and the potential pain modulators would further support a functional link between the two. However, no causal relationship can be identified with this method. The key cortical area of the pain matrix modulated by mood was identified by comparing signals in BO and GO conditions. Individual peak activation levels within this area during the BO condition were measured and used as regressors (weighting factors) in a new model to estimate covariation. The BO condition was chosen over the GO condition or BO − GO subtraction map because the pain processing-related signal was the strongest in BO. A similar approach was performed for the attention modulation using the AH condition.

Results

Psychophysical results

Selection of stimuli

The temperatures that were determined in the preliminary session to be moderately painful ranged from 44 to 50° (mean ± SD, 46.8 ± 1.5°C). The second temperature of the pair that was discriminated from the first ∼80% of the time was most commonly 1°C greater than this (47.8 ± 1.5°C). The average performance scores on the discrimination task were as follows (mean ± SE): AHGO, 83 ± 3%; AHBO, 85 ± 3%; AOGO, 72 ± 4%; AOBO, 57 ± 5%. There was no significant effect of the factor ODOR (F(1,13) = 3.78; p > 0.07). There was a significant effect of the factor ATTENTION (F(1,13) = 17.55; p < 0.01) reflecting a better performance on the heat discrimination task (84 ± 2%) than on the odor discrimination task (65 ± 4%), but there was also a significant interaction between the factors ATTENTION and ODOR (F(1,13) = 5.46; p < 0.04). The post hoc test (Tukey's HSD) shows that this was attributable to the poorer performance in AOBO compared with all other conditions (all p < 0.05), whereas the performances in AHGO, AHBO, and AOGO were not significantly different from each other (all p > 0.09). So, although the matching of task performance was not as successful in the scanner as during pretesting, the performances for all tasks were above chance and submaximal, indicating that the tasks were difficult enough to maximize attentional demand but not so difficult as to discourage the subject from performing the tasks.

Influence of direction of attention

Direction of attention (to pain or odor) significantly influenced pain intensity ratings. Subjects rated pain as more intense when they attended to it (main effect of factor ATTENTION: AH (mean ± SE), 7.8 ± 0.2; AO, 6.5 ± 0.4; F(1,13) = 13.46; p < 0.01). Direction of attention did not significantly affect pain unpleasantness (main effect of factor ATTENTION: AH, 6.1 ± 0.4; AO, 5.3 ± 0.5; F(1,13) = 3.13; p > 0.1; or interaction: F(1,13) = 0.40; p > 0.5), mood (main effect of factor ATTENTION: AH, 1.9 ± 0.7; AO, 1.4 ± 0.7; F(1,13) = 2.76; p > 0.12; or interaction between ATTENTION and ODOR: F(1,13) = 3.25; p > 0.09), or anxiety/calmness (main effect of factor ATTENTION: AH, −0.3 ± 0.8; AO, −0.5 ± 0.8; F(1,13) = 0.16; p > 0.7; or interaction: F(1,13) = 3.31; p > 0.09). This replicates our previous findings (Villemure et al., 2003).

Influence of odor valence

Odor valence (good vs bad odor) significantly influenced mood, anxiety/calmness, and pain unpleasantness but not pain intensity ratings. The thermal stimuli were rated as more unpleasant in the presence of the bad odor than the good odor (main effect of factor ODOR: BO, 6.3 ± 0.5; GO, 5.1 ± 0.5; F(1,13) = 6.95; p < 0.05) but were not rated as more intense (main effect of factor ODOR: BO, 7.3 ± 0.3; GO, 6.9 ± 0.4; F(1,13) = 2.59; p > 0.1; or interaction: F(1,13) = 1.43; p > 0.2). Mood was significantly better in the good odor condition (main effect of factor ODOR: BO, −0.4 ± 1.1; GO, 3.7 ± 0.6; F(1,13) = 13.76; p < 0.0027). Subjects were also calmer when the good odor was present (main effect of factor ODOR: BO, −1.7 ± 0.9; GO, 0.9 ± 0.8; F(1,13) = 13.84; p < 0.01). This replicates our previous findings (Villemure et al., 2003).

Stepwise linear regression analysis

Because odor hedonics was found to affect pain unpleasantness, mood, and anxiety, odor hedonics, mood, and anxiety changes were included as independent variables in a stepwise linear regression analysis to determine which best predicted changes in pain unpleasantness ratings. The only significant predictor of pain unpleasantness changes in the stepwise regression was mood changes (R2 = 0.29; F(1,13) = 4.898; p < 0.05). This suggests that effects of odors on pain unpleasantness were indeed mediated through mood changes.

fMRI results

Activation evoked by painful stimuli compared with warm stimuli

In the absence of attentional or emotional modulation, the following thalamic and cortical brain areas were activated by the painful heat stimuli: contralateral (right) medial thalamus (medialTH) and S1, bilateral S2, a large extent of midIC and aIC bilaterally, and bilateral ACC (Fig. 3, Table 2).

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Thalamic and cortical activity evoked by heat pain in the alternating warm/pain task. The pain network activated by the painful heat stimuli when no attentional or emotional modulations are used is shown. Ipsi, Ipsilateral; Contra, contralateral. All figures depict neurological orientation (left is left).

Table 2.

Pain processing-related areas in the absence of attentional or emotional modulations

Region (BA) MNI coordinates t score Cluster size
Contralateral ACC 8, 14, 34 3.466 368
Ipsilateral ACC −8, 12, 34 3.568 216
Contralateral insula 32, 16, 8 3.737 13,128
42, 6, −10 4.773
42, −4, −2 4.246
Contralateral S2 56, −14, 14 5.138
62, −18, 22 4.111
Ipsilateral insula −44, 2, 2 3.958 7112
−34, 2, 6 3.438
−34, −2, 10 3.446
−42, −8, 0 4.806
−42, −10, 2 4.820
−42, −14, 8 4.394
−38, −16, 18 3.648
Ipsilateral S2 −50, −24, 22 3.541 1632
Contralateral medialTH 6, −24, 0 5.532 1376
2, −18, 0 4.839
Contralateral S1 10, −42, 64 4.102 896
14, −44, 62 3.916

Thalamic and cortical regions show increased BOLD response during the alternating warm/pain condition for the painful stimuli compared with warm nonpainful baseline. Cluster size refers to the size of the cluster in cubed millimeters with t values superior to 2.5. t scores significant in global search are bolded and underlined. t scores and cluster sizes significant in the directed search are indicated in bold.

Pain processing areas modulated by direction of attention

Within the pain network identified previously, direction of attention altered pain-evoked activity only in the ipsilateral aIC (Fig. 4, Table 3). Activity in this area was significant both in terms of peak t value and spatial extent when attending to pain (AH condition) but not when attending to odors (AO condition). In the subtraction map (AH − AO), aIC activation did not reach the statistical criterion for a new discovery in this region (criterion t = 3.4171) (Table 1) but met the one-tailed t test criterion established to test the a priori hypothesis that the activation was stronger in AH than AO.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Attentional modulation network. Attentional effects within the previously identified pain network are observed in the aIC. All figures depict neurological orientation (left is left).

Table 3.

Pain processing-related areas modulated by the attentional and emotional tasks

Region (BA) Condition MNI coordinates t score Cluster size
Attentional modulation
    Anterior insula AH −28, 16, 6 4.310 1160*
AO −28, 16, 6 1.105 0
AH − AO −28, 14, 6 1.898 0
AH −34, 20, 16 3.579 1160*
AO −34, 20, 16 0.792 0
AH − AO −34, 18, 16 1.898 0
Emotional modulation
    Contralateral ACC (BA24) BO 6, 4, 34 3.536 1800*
GO 6, 4, 34 −0.493 0
BO − GO 6, 6, 30 3.195 760*
BO 4, −4, 32 4.042 1800*
GO 4, −4, 32 0.090 0
BO − GO 6, 0, 32 3.434 760*
    medialTH BO 2, −8, 0 3.871 208
GO 2, –8, 0 −0.881 0
BO − GO −2, −8, 0 3.501 456
    Contralateral S2 BO 52, −38, 24 3.754 432
GO 52, −38, 24 −0.260 0
BO − GO 52, −40, 22 3.242 96
    Contralateral S1 BO 22, −38, 62 3.934 816
GO 20, −32, 62 2.677 32
BO − GO 24, −38, 58 2.978 536

Cluster size refers to the size of the cluster in cubed millimeters with t values superior to 2.5. t scores and cluster sizes significant in the directed search are indicated in bold.

*Same cluster with more than one peak.

Pain processing areas modulated by mood

Pain-evoked activity was greater during the bad odor/worse mood (BO condition) than during the good odor/better mood (GO condition) in the contralateral ACC, medialTH, and contralateral S1 and S2 (Fig. 5, Table 3). In the subtraction map (BO − GO), all these activations reached the one-tailed t test criterion established to test the a priori hypothesis that the activation was stronger in BO than GO and the statistical criterion for a new discovery in these regions (see Table 1 for the different thresholds) except for S2, which met only the former criterion.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Emotional modulation network. Emotional effects within the previously identified pain network are observed in ACC, thalamus, S2, and S1. All figures depict neurological orientation (left is left).

Possible pain modulators in attentional context

There was a significant superior posterior parietal (SPP; BA7) activation in the AH condition weighted with the pain intensity score and in the subtraction AH − AO weighted with the change in the pain intensity score (Fig. 6A, Table 4). Additionally, there was a significant ERC (BA28) activation in AH condition weighted with the pain intensity score and in the AH − AO condition weighted with the change in pain intensity perception. Furthermore, in the AH condition, the activity of aIC positively covaried with the activity of SPP (Fig. 6A, Table 5). There was a positive correlation between activity within aIC and ERC, but it did not reach statistical significance (Fig. 6A, Table 5).

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Possible pain modulating regions. A, Attentional condition weighted (w) with pain intensity scores for AHw and AOw conditions and with the difference in pain intensity ratings between attentional conditions (NRS score in AH minus NRS score in AO) for the subtraction AH − AOw. Activity within the SPP (BA7) and the ERC (BA28) correlated with both the pain intensity ratings and the change in pain intensity ratings between attentional conditions. Activity within aIC (the key perceptual region modulated by attention direction) covaried with superior parietal activity and, to a lesser degree (not statistically significant), with entorhinal activity in the AH condition. B, Emotional condition weighted with pain unpleasantness scores for BOw and GOw conditions and with the difference in pain unpleasantness ratings between emotional conditions (NRS score in BO minus NRS score in GO) for the subtraction BO − GOw. Activity within the LinfF, triangular and orbital parts (BA45/47), correlated with both the pain unpleasantness ratings and the change in pain unpleasantness ratings between emotional conditions. Activity within ACC (the key perceptual region modulated by mood) covaried with both the inferior frontal cortex and PAG activity in the BO condition. All figures depict neurological orientation (left is left).

Table 4.

Potential pain modulators associated with the attentional and emotional conditions

Region (BA) Condition MNI coordinates t score Cluster size
Attentional modulation
    SPP (BA7) AHw −18, −50, 70 4.362 208
AOw −18, −50, 70 −1.375 0
AH − AOw −18, −50, 70 3.400 184
    ERC (BA28) AHw 18, −4, −38 3.335 80
AOw 18, −4, −38 0.70 0
AH − AOw 14, −4, −34 3.171 104
Emotional modulation
    Inferior frontal gyrus—triangular part (BA45/47) BOw 46, 32, 10 3.464** 192
GOw 46, 32, 10 −0.507 0
BO − GOw 48, 32, 12 3.633 376
    Inferior frontal gyrus—orbital part (BA47) BOw 50, 32, −4 5.364 1336
GOw 50, 32, −4 −1.364 0
BO − GOw 48, 32, −6 3.653 1344

The AH and AO conditions were weighted (w) with pain intensity scores, whereas the BO and GO conditions were weighted with pain unpleasantness scores. The subtractions were weighted with the change in pain intensity or unpleasantness ratings for the attention and emotion conditions, respectively. Cluster size refers to the size of the cluster in cubed millimeters with t values superior to 2.5. t scores significant in the directed search are indicated in bold. t scores significant in global search are bolded and underlined.

**Approaching significance.

Table 5.

Covariation between the main pain processing-related areas modulated by attention (aIC) or emotion (ACC) and potential pain modulators

Region (BA) Condition MNI coordinates (x, y, z) t score Cluster size
Attentional condition (areas that covary with aIC)
    aIC AH −28, 16, 6 5.623 5336
    SPP (BA7) AH −18, −50, 70 4.333 224
    ERC (BA28) AH 16, −4, −38 2.761 40
Emotional condition (areas that covary with ACC)
    ACC (BA24) BO −2, 10, 32 3.774 4672*
BO 4, −2, 32 5.981 4672*
Inferior frontal—triangular part (BA45/47) BO 46, 32, 10 4.158 752*
Inferior frontal—orbital part (BA47) BO 48, 30, −4 3.706 752*
PAG BO 6, −32, −16 3.184 480

Cluster size refers to the size of the cluster in cubed millimeters with t values superior to 2.5. t scores and cluster sizes significant in the directed search are indicated in bold. t scores significant in global search are bolded and underlined.

*Same cluster with more than one peak.

Possible pain modulators in emotional context

The right LinfF gyrus orbital part (BA47) was significantly activated in the BO condition weighted with pain unpleasantness scores and in the BO − GO condition weighted with the change in the pain unpleasantness score (Fig. 6B, Table 4). Additionally, there was a close to significant activation (tscore = 3.464; tthreshold = 3.495) of the right LinfF gyrus triangular part (BA45/47) in the BO condition weighted with the pain unpleasantness score and a significant activation of this region in the subtraction BO − GO weighted with the change in the pain unpleasantness score. Furthermore, in the BO condition, the activity of ACC positively covaried with the activity in LinfF cortex triangular and orbital parts (BA45/47) and PAG (Fig. 6B, Table 5).

Discussion

We found evidence of separate neural circuits underlying emotional and attentional pain modulation. Pleasant odors, independent of attentional focus, induced positive mood changes and decreased pain unpleasantness perception and pain-evoked activity in ACC, medialTH, S1, and S2. LinfF (BA45/47) activity correlated with the amount of pain modulation produced by emotional context and, ACC activity covaried with LinfF and PAG activity. Attending odors during pain decreased pain intensity perception and pain-evoked activity in aIC. SPP (BA7) and entorhinal activity correlated with the amount of pain modulation produced by attention direction, but aIC activity covaried only with SPP activity.

Emotional modulation

The ACC showed the largest mood modulation, but modulation also occurred in medialTH, S2, and S1. ACC activity was previously implicated in pain affect (Rainville et al., 1997; Tolle et al., 1999), consistent with our finding that odor-evoked mood changes preferentially modulate pain unpleasantness. The mood-related modulation in medialTH suggests that mood-related changes in nociceptive processing involve descending modulatory circuits affecting subcortical regions. S1 and S2 activity is most often attributed to sensory-discriminative aspects of pain perception. However, there are neural pathways connecting these regions to brain areas implicated in pain affect such as ACC (Price, 2000). Thus, S1 and S2 could indirectly participate in the affective dimension of pain through serial mechanisms by which sensory aspects of pain contribute to unpleasantness (Price, 2000).

We identified LinfF (BA47/BA45) as potential emotional pain modulators, because their activity correlated with changes in pain unpleasantness ratings between emotional conditions and covaried with ACC and PAG activity. Phillips et al. (2003) implicated BA45 in emotional modulation of nonpainful esophageal stimulation. The OFC, including BA47 in humans, receives input from all sensory modalities (including visceral) and has direct reciprocal connections with brain regions important for pain perception, such as ACC and PAG (Cavada et al., 2000; Kringelbach, 2005). Consequently, OFC is well placed to integrate sensory and visceral information, suggesting its involvement in emotional processing (Kringelbach, 2005). LateralOFC activity generally relates to negative emotions (Murphy et al., 2003) and evaluation of punishers, whereas medialOFC is implicated in reward (Kringelbach, 2005). Consistent with this idea, more lateralOFC activity was observed when pain was associated with unpleasant odors. Similarly, lateralOFC was activated when pain and thirst were experienced simultaneously, but not separately (Farrell et al., 2006).

Animal studies implicate the ventrolateralOFC in antinociception/analgesia, suggesting the effects are mediated through descending connections with PAG. This is consistent with our finding that PAG activity covaried with ACC and BA45/47 activity during emotional modulation. In humans, lateralOFC was also implicated in a variety of cognitively driven pain modulations. OFC, with its rostral ACC and brainstem connections, belongs to a network using cognitive cues to activate the endogenous opioid system (Petrovic et al., 2002). The relative activity increase in lateralOFC during pain could represent a source of cognitive modulation of emotional aspects of pain processing (Petrovic and Ingvar, 2002). Alternatively, this covariation between LinfF, ACC, and PAG could reflect descending pro-nociceptive facilitatory mechanisms (Lovick, 2008).

Attentional modulation

When subjects attended odors, painful stimuli evoked less aIC activation than when they attended pain. This finding is arguably weaker than the emotional context findings because aIC activation reached significance in an a priori hypothesis but not using a new discovery criterion. Nevertheless, the modulation of aIC is consistent with the literature on attentional pain modulation, in which insula modulation is often described (Longe et al., 2001; Bantick et al., 2002; Brooks et al., 2002; Hoffman et al., 2004; Seminowicz et al., 2004; Dunckley et al., 2007). Furthermore, our psychophysical data showing that attention preferentially altered pain intensity ratings are consistent with other data implicating aIC in thermal intensity perception (Coghill et al., 1999; Craig et al., 2000; Olausson et al., 2005).

SPP (BA7) was identified as a potential attentional pain modulator because its activity was related to the change in pain intensity between attentional conditions and covaried with aIC activity. Increased SPP activity corresponded to increased aIC activity.

A role of the PPC, including SPP (BA7), in pain attention has been suggested (Duncan and Albanese, 2003) but not demonstrated (Apkarian et al., 2005). Studies involving other sensory modalities implicate BA7 in attentional processes as part of a bilateral dorsal frontoparietal system involved in intentional attention orienting (Petersen et al., 1994; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002; Shomstein and Yantis, 2004, 2006; Hahn et al., 2006; Lepsien and Nobre, 2006) and focused attention (Pardo et al., 1991; Burton and Sinclair, 2000; Behrmann et al., 2004; Nebel et al., 2005). Attentional orienting can bias information processing, optimizing cortical representation and thus perception of relevant information (Yantis and Serences, 2003; Lepsien and Nobre, 2006). Superior parietal activity would enhance activity within sensory cortices (Wu et al., 2007), possibly synchronizing the spiking activity in targeted neurons representing the attended object, thus driving more effective representations (Yantis and Serences, 2003). Attending painful stimuli could increase parietal activity leading to better processing of those stimuli, reflected by increased aIC activity and pain intensity perception.

A positron emission tomography study showing increased regional cerebral blood flow in PPC in chronic pain patients provides additional evidence for a role of PPC in attentional aspects of pain processing (Hsieh et al., 1995). The authors suggested that PPC activation during chronic pain may reflect increased attention and vigilance that patients devote to their condition.

Our data suggest an involvement of ERC in attentional pain modulation, because ERC activity correlated with the amount of pain modulation produced by attention direction. However, this must be interpreted with caution, because the correlation between the activities of aIC and ERC failed to reach significance. When weighing the data by magnitude of attention-related perceptual changes, we found that ERC was preferentially activated when subjects attended pain. Other studies have implicated ERC in attentional processes, particularly in relation to its connections with PPC (Burwell, 2000). This contrasts with the results of Ploghaus et al. (2001), who reported ERC's involvement in emotional pain modulation. They compared brain activity evoked by mildly painful stimuli during a condition in which subjects were certain that they would receive mild pain (low anxiety) and a condition in which they knew they might equally well receive an intense pain (high anxiety). However, because no direct measures of anxiety or attention were taken, it is equally feasible that subjects were more attentive to the stimulus in the uncertain than in the certain condition. We cannot exclude that some ERC areas are associated with emotional pain modulation and others with attentional modulation because our peak activity was more anterior than theirs.

Some neuroimaging studies involving pain and different types of distraction implicated PAG in attentional pain modulation. Tracey et al. (2002) observed that PAG activation increased significantly when subjects used thoughts to distract themselves. Valet et al. (2004), using the incongruent color-word-Stroop as the distractor, observed that PAG activation correlated with prefrontal activity during the distraction task. Despite the apparently consistent observations, these distraction tasks could easily have changed arousal, mood, anxiety, or other emotional factors. In our study, we found PAG activity related to mood but not attention. Our results suggest that the previous reports of PAG involvement in distraction-related pain modulation could involve emotional differences between conditions. This interpretation is supported by Valet et al. (2004) finding that pain unpleasantness was more profoundly modulated than pain intensity, as observed during emotional pain modulation (Zelman et al., 1991; Villemure et al., 2003; Rainville et al., 2005). PAG activity correlates with anxiety during painful visceral stimulation (Dunckley et al., 2005) and, in other sensory modalities, is related to emotions such as “chills” associated with intensely pleasurable music (Blood and Zatorre, 2001). Finally, studies of directed attention involving vision, hearing, or touch fail to observe attention-related PAG activation but report activation in PPC similar to the attention-related activations in our study (Petersen et al., 1994; Burton and Sinclair, 2000; Behrmann et al., 2004; Shomstein and Yantis, 2004, 2006; Fan et al., 2005; Hahn et al., 2006).

Limitations

Choosing an appropriate contrast period for fMRI analysis presented a difficulty. We chose the ISI in favor of the expectation period before the stimulus pair, because it revealed more robust activation patterns. Nevertheless, possible expectancy and/or memory traces during ISI (Albanese et al., 2007) probably limited sensitivity, so that activations could have been present but not detected. This likely explains why we did not detect significant S1 activation or attention modulation in S1, contrasting with our previous studies (Bushnell et al., 1999).

Some studies showed that activity in parts of ACC correlates with odor-subjective pleasantness and unpleasantness (Rolls et al., 2003; Grabenhorst et al., 2007). This raises the possibility that the change in ACC activation we observed when pain is experienced with either pleasant or unpleasant odors could also reflect a change in the response to odors. However, ACC activity correlating with the subjective hedonic quality of odors was found considerably more rostrally (y = 28, 42) than our observed ACC modulation and, as such, is unlikely to contribute to mood-related modulation of nociceptive responses in ACC.

Finally, all pain reports were taken immediately after each scanning run and not after each individual stimulus. Because each run lasted for ∼5 min, these summary ratings could be influenced by memory and differ from those taken after each stimulus. Nevertheless, a short delay between a pain stimulus and its rating is likely not problematic (Seminowicz and Davis, 2007). Furthermore, we have observed attentional modulation of almost the exact same magnitude with pain ratings acquired after each stimulus (Miron et al., 1989).

In conclusion, our findings suggest that emotional and attentional modulations invoked mostly separable brain networks. Emotional context preferentially modulated pain unpleasantness and modified activity in ACC, medialTH, S1, and S2, with LinfF and PAG identified as possible emotion-related pain modulators. Attentional context preferentially modulated pain intensity and activity within aIC. SPP and ERC were identified as possible sources of attention-related modulation.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grant 1-RO1-NS44036-01. We thank Nazma Mohammed for skilled assistance at all stages of data acquisition; André Cormier, Nicole Fauteux, and Louise Marcotte for expert MRI data acquisition; and Mike Ferreira for expert support with data analysis. We also thank Petra Schweinhardt for valuable comments on a previous version of this manuscript.

References

  • Albanese et al., 2007.Albanese MC, Duerden EG, Rainville P, Duncan GH. Memory traces of pain in human cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27:4612–4620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0695-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Apkarian et al., 2005.Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede RD, Zubieta JK. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain. 2005;9:463–484. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bantick et al., 2002.Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I. Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain. 2002;125:310–319. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Behrmann et al., 2004.Behrmann M, Geng JJ, Shomstein S. Parietal cortex and attention. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2004;14:212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Blood and Zatorre, 2001.Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98:11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brooks et al., 2002.Brooks JC, Nurmikko TJ, Bimson WE, Singh KD, Roberts N. fMRI of thermal pain: effects of stimulus laterality and attention. Neuroimage. 2002;15:293–301. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Buffington et al., 2005.Buffington AL, Hanlon CA, McKeown MJ. Acute and persistent pain modulation of attention-related anterior cingulate fMRI activations. Pain. 2005;113:172–184. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burton and Sinclair, 2000.Burton H, Sinclair RJ. Attending to and remembering tactile stimuli: a review of brain imaging data and single-neuron responses. J Clin Neurophysiol. 2000;17:575–591. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200011000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burwell, 2000.Burwell RD. The parahippocampal region: corticocortical connectivity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2000;911:25–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2000.tb06717.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bushnell et al., 1999.Bushnell MC, Duncan GH, Hofbauer RK, Ha B, Chen JI, Carrier B. Pain perception: is there a role for primary somatosensory cortex? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1999;96:7705–7709. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.14.7705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cao, 1999.Cao J. The size of the connected components of excursion sets of chi(2), t and F fields. Adv Appl Prob. 1999;31:579–595. [Google Scholar]
  • Cavada et al., 2000.Cavada C, Company T, Tejedor J, Cruz-Rizzolo RJ, Reinoso-Suarez F. The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:220–242. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Coghill et al., 1999.Coghill RC, Sang CN, Maisog JM, Iadarola MJ. Pain intensity processing within the human brain: a bilateral, distributed mechanism. J Neurophysiol. 1999;82:1934–1943. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.82.4.1934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Collins et al., 1994.Collins DL, Neelin P, Peters TM, Evans AC. Automatic 3D intersubject registration of MR volumetric data in standardized Talairach space. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1994;18:192–205. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Corbetta and Shulman, 2002.Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Craig et al., 2000.Craig AD, Chen K, Bandy D, Reiman EM. Thermosensory activation of insular cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:184–190. doi: 10.1038/72131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Duncan and Albanese, 2003.Duncan GH, Albanese MC. Is there a role for the parietal lobes in the perception of pain? Adv Neurol. 2003;93:69–86. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dunckley et al., 2005.Dunckley P, Wise RG, Fairhurst M, Hobden P, Aziz Q, Chang L, Tracey I. A comparison of visceral and somatic pain processing in the human brainstem using functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2005;25:7333–7341. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1100-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dunckley et al., 2007.Dunckley P, Aziz Q, Wise RG, Brooks J, Tracey I, Chang L. Attentional modulation of visceral and somatic pain. Neurogastroenterol Motil. 2007;19:569–577. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2007.00908.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Esch and Stefano, 2004.Esch T, Stefano GB. The neurobiology of pleasure, reward processes, addiction and their health implications. Neuro Endocrinol Lett. 2004;25:235–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fan et al., 2005.Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26:471–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Farrell et al., 2006.Farrell MJ, Egan GF, Zamarripa F, Shade R, Blair-West J, Fox P, Denton DA. Unique, common, and interacting cortical correlates of thirst and pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103:2416–2421. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511019103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Frankenstein et al., 2001.Frankenstein UN, Richter W, McIntyre MC, Remy F. Distraction modulates anterior cingulate gyrus activations during the cold pressor test. Neuroimage. 2001;14:827–836. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grabenhorst et al., 2007.Grabenhorst F, Rolls ET, Margot C, da Silva MA, Velazco MI. How pleasant and unpleasant stimuli combine in different brain regions: odor mixtures. J Neurosci. 2007;27:13532–13540. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3337-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hahn et al., 2006.Hahn B, Ross TJ, Stein EA. Neuroanatomical dissociation between bottom-up and top-down processes of visuospatial selective attention. Neuroimage. 2006;32:842–853. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hoffman et al., 2004.Hoffman HG, Richards TL, Coda B, Bills AR, Blough D, Richards AL, Sharar SR. Modulation of thermal pain-related brain activity with virtual reality: evidence from fMRI. Neuroreport. 2004;15:1245–1248. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000127826.73576.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hsieh et al., 1995.Hsieh JC, Belfrage M, Stone-Elander S, Hansson P, Ingvar M. Central representation of chronic ongoing neuropathic pain studied by positron emission tomography. Pain. 1995;63:225–236. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(95)00048-W. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kringelbach, 2005.Kringelbach ML. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:691–702. doi: 10.1038/nrn1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lepsien and Nobre, 2006.Lepsien J, Nobre AC. Cognitive control of attention in the human brain: insights from orienting attention to mental representations. Brain Res. 2006;1105:20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Longe et al., 2001.Longe SE, Wise R, Bantick S, Lloyd D, Johansen-Berg H, McGlone F, Tracey I. Counter-stimulatory effects on pain perception and processing are significantly altered by attention: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2021–2025. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lovick, 2008.Lovick TA. Pro-nociceptive action of cholecystokinin in the periacqueductal grey: a role in neuropathic and anxiety-induced hyperalgesic states. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:852–862. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Mesulam, 1998.Mesulam MM. From sensation to cognition. Brain. 1998;121:1013–1052. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Miron et al., 1989.Miron D, Duncan GH, Bushnell MC. Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain. Pain. 1989;39:345–352. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(89)90048-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy et al., 2003.Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003;3:207–233. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.3.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nebel et al., 2005.Nebel K, Wiese H, Stude P, de GA, Diener HC, Keidel M. On the neural basis of focused and divided attention. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005;25:760–776. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Olausson et al., 2005.Olausson H, Charron J, Marchand S, Villemure C, Strigo IA, Bushnell MC. Feelings of warmth correlate with neural activity in right anterior insular cortex. Neurosci Lett. 2005;389:1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pardo et al., 1991.Pardo JV, Fox PT, Raichle ME. Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography. Nature. 1991;349:61–64. doi: 10.1038/349061a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Petersen et al., 1994.Petersen SE, Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Shulman GL. PET studies of parietal involvement in spatial attention: comparison of different task types. Can J Exp Psychol. 1994;48:319–338. doi: 10.1037/1196-1961.48.2.319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrovic and Ingvar, 2002.Petrovic P, Ingvar M. Imaging cognitive modulation of pain processing. Pain. 2002;95:1–5. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(01)00467-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrovic et al., 2000.Petrovic P, Petersson KM, Ghatan PH, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task. Pain. 2000;85:19–30. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00232-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrovic et al., 2002.Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia– imaging a shared neuronal network. Science. 2002;295:1737–1740. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrovic et al., 2005.Petrovic P, Dietrich T, Fransson P, Andersson J, Carlsson K, Ingvar M. Placebo in emotional processing–induced expectations of anxiety relief activate a generalized modulatory network. Neuron. 2005;46:957–969. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Peyron et al., 1999.Peyron R, Garcia-Larrea L, Gregoire MC, Costes N, Convers P, Lavenne F, Mauguiere F, Michel D, Laurent B. Haemodynamic brain responses to acute pain in humans: sensory and attentional networks. Brain. 1999;122:1765–1780. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.9.1765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Phillips et al., 2003.Phillips ML, Gregory LJ, Cullen S, Coen S, Ng V, Andrew C, Giampietro V, Bullmore E, Zelaya F, Amaro E, Thompson DG, Hobson AR, Williams SC, Brammer M, Aziz Q. The effect of negative emotional context on neural and behavioural responses to oesophageal stimulation. Brain. 2003;126:669–684. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ploghaus et al., 2001.Ploghaus A, Narain C, Beckmann CF, Clare S, Bantick S, Wise R, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN, Tracey I. Exacerbation of pain by anxiety is associated with activity in a hippocampal network. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9896–9903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09896.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Price, 2000.Price DD. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science. 2000;288:1769–1772. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Price et al., 1983.Price DD, McGrath PA, Rafii A, Buckingham B. The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain. Pain. 1983;17:45–56. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90126-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rainville et al., 1997.Rainville P, Duncan GH, Price DD, Carrier B, Bushnell MC. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science. 1997;277:968–971. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5328.968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rainville et al., 2005.Rainville P, Bao QV, Chretien P. Pain-related emotions modulate experimental pain perception and autonomic responses. Pain. 2005;118:306–318. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.08.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rolls et al., 2003.Rolls ET, Kringelbach ML, de Araujo IE. Different representations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18:695–703. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02779.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Seminowicz and Davis, 2007.Seminowicz DA, Davis KD. A re-examination of pain-cognition interactions: implications for neuroimaging. Pain. 2007;130:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.03.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Seminowicz et al., 2004.Seminowicz DA, Mikulis DJ, Davis KD. Cognitive modulation of pain-related brain responses depends on behavioral strategy. Pain. 2004;112:48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shomstein and Yantis, 2004.Shomstein S, Yantis S. Control of attention shifts between vision and audition in human cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24:10702–10706. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2939-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shomstein and Yantis, 2006.Shomstein S, Yantis S. Parietal cortex mediates voluntary control of spatial and nonspatial auditory attention. J Neurosci. 2006;26:435–439. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4408-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tolle et al., 1999.Tolle TR, Kaufmann T, Siessmeier T, Lautenbacher S, Berthele A, Munz F, Zieglgansberger W, Willoch F, Schwaiger M, Conrad B, Bartenstein P. Region-specific encoding of sensory and affective components of pain in the human brain: a positron emission tomography correlation analysis. Ann Neurol. 1999;45:40–47. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199901)45:1<40::aid-art8>3.0.co;2-l. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tracey et al., 2002.Tracey I, Ploghaus A, Gati JS, Clare S, Smith S, Menon RS, Matthews PM. Imaging attentional modulation of pain in the periacqueductal gray in humans. J Neurosci. 2002;22:2748–2752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02748.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Valet et al., 2004.Valet M, Sprenger T, Boecker H, Willoch F, Rummeny E, Conrad B, Erhard P, Tolle TR. Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain–an fMRI analysis. Pain. 2004;109:399–408. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.02.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Villemure and Bushnell, 2002.Villemure C, Bushnell MC. Cognitive modulation of pain: how do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain. 2002;95:195–199. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00007-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Villemure et al., 2003.Villemure C, Slotnick BM, Bushnell MC. Effects of odors on pain perception: deciphering the roles of emotion and attention. Pain. 2003;106:101–108. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00297-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wiech et al., 2005.Wiech K, Seymour B, Kalisch R, Stephan KE, Koltzenburg M, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Modulation of pain processing in hyperalgesia by cognitive demand. Neuroimage. 2005;27:59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Worsley et al., 1996.Worsley KJ, Marrett S, Neelin P, Vandal AC, Friston KJ, Evans AC. A unified statistical approach for determining significant signals in images of cerebral activation. Human Brain Mapping. 1996;4:58–73. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1996)4:1<58::AID-HBM4>3.0.CO;2-O. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Worsley et al., 2002.Worsley KJ, Liao CH, Aston J, Petre V, Duncan GH, Morales F, Evans AC. A general statistical analysis for fMRI data. Neuroimage. 2002;15:1–15. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0933. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wu et al., 2007.Wu CT, Weissman DH, Roberts KC, Woldorff MG. The neural circuitry underlying the executive control of auditory spatial attention. Brain Res. 2007;1134:187–198. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.11.088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yantis and Serences, 2003.Yantis S, Serences JT. Cortical mechanisms of space-based and object-based attentional control. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2003;13:187–193. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00033-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zelman et al., 1991.Zelman DC, Howland EW, Nichols SN, Cleeland CS. The effects of induced mood on laboratory pain. Pain. 1991;46:105–111. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(91)90040-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES