Abstract
Synaptic development and elimination are normal neurodevelopmental processes which if altered could contribute to various neuropsychiatric disorders. 31P-1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging and structural MRI exams were conducted on 106 healthy children ages 6–18 years in order to identify neuromolecular indices of synaptic development and elimination. Over the age range studied, age-related changes in high-energy phosphate (phosphocreatine), membrane phospholipid metabolism (precursors and breakdown products), and gray matter were found. These neuromolecular and structural indices of synaptic development and elimination are associated with development of several cognitive domains and changes in gray matter volume. Monitoring of these molecular markers is essential for devising treatment strategies for neurodevelopmental disorders.
Keywords: MRS, Neuroimaging, Metabolism, Cognition, Multiple Regression
INTRODUCTION
The four major stages that characterize human brain development are: 1) neuronal proliferation; 2) migration of neurons to specific sites throughout the CNS; 3) organization of the neuronal circuitry; and 4) myelination of the neuronal circuitry (Volpe, 1995).
The third stage of human brain development, organization of the neural circuitry, is most active from the sixth month of gestation to young adulthood. The major events associated with neuronal circuitry organization include: 1) proper alignment, orientation, and layering of cortical neurons; 2) dendritic and axonal differentiation; 3) synaptic development; 4) synaptic elimination (cell death and/or selective elimination of neuronal processes); and 5) glial proliferation and differentiation. These processes overlap with the timing of normal development of cognitive function and the onset of neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders such as attention deficit disorder, autism and schizophrenia. Normal synaptic elimination occurs during early adolescence in non-human primates (Rakic et al., 1986; Bourgeois & Rakic, 1993) and humans (Huttenlocher, 1979; Huttenlocher et al., 1982; Huttenlocher, 1990; Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997). Synaptic elimination in non-human primates is generally observed to occur synchronously in all regions (i.e., homochronous) (Rakic et al., 1986) but is heterochronous in humans (Huttenlocher et al., 1997). Normal synaptic elimination is predominantly of presumptive excitatory asymmetric junctions on dendritic spines (Smiley & Goldman-Rakic, 1993) which probably utilize amino acids, such as l-glutamate, as the neurotransmitter (Storm-Mathisen & Otterson, 1990). Perinatal insults, intrauterine disturbances, and perhaps environmental influences in childhood and adolescence can potentially result in disordered neuronal circuitry (Birch & Gussow, 1970). This study focuses on molecular and structural indices related to synaptic development and elimination.
31P and 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopic imaging (31P-1H MRSI) are well suited to monitor the processes of synaptic development and elimination, and neuronal cell death by measuring energy dynamics [phosphocreatine (PCr)], a putative biomarker of neurons and neuronal processes [n-acetylaspartate (NAA)] and measures of membrane phospholipid metabolism, such as phospholipid building blocks [short NMR correlation time phosphomonoesters (sPME)] and phospholipid breakdown products [short NMR correlation time phosphodiesters (sPDE)]. The hypothesis tested in this study is that neuromolecular underpinnings of synaptic development and elimination will be observed by changes in 31P-1H MRSI observed brain metabolites of individuals ages 6–18, and will be associated with changes in percent of gray matter reflecting synaptic development and elimination. Specifically, we investigated in an axial brain slice cross-sectional age differences in brain levels of PCr, sPME, sPDE, and NAA which reflect changes in neuronal synaptic activity (PCr), neuronal numbers and integrity (NAA), turnover of membrane phospholipids (sPME and sPDE) and the percent gray matter. We hypothesized the above neurodevelopmental metabolic and structural changes would be associated with corresponding development of cognitive function in the domains of language, visual spatial constructional, executive function, and memory abilities. In an age difference study, one should expect to find meaningful correspondences among cognitive growth, the above brain metabolite levels, and percent gray matter.
METHODS
Participants
The study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board. All subjects (and parents when appropriate) gave informed written consent to participate in the study. Personal interviews including the subject and family were conducted by a psychologist and following the informed consent procedure and execution of the appropriate consent form, procedures were accomplished to assure that recruited participants were healthy, normally developing individuals who met all inclusion/exclusion criteria: These included a review of the study inclusion/exclusion criteria and completion and review of the Devereux Scale for Mental Disorders and a cognitive test battery as described below. This interview was followed by a separate interview where magnetic resonance (MR) exclusion criteria were reviewed by an MR Center nurse or technician and by subject participation in an MR simulator. A pediatrician conducted the physical examination in an examination room at the MR Center. In addition to the report by the pediatrician, the Devereux, FH-DRC interview, and other tests from the cognitive neurodevelopmental battery were scored and reviewed with the subject and family. If all entry criteria were met, the full battery was completed followed by the entry time-point MRSI/MRI examination within one month after completion of the cognitive neurodevelopmental battery.
The sample consisted of 106 individuals ranging in age from 6 to 17. Fifty four were females and 51 were males. The age distribution covered the neurodevelopmental stages of childhood (6–10 years), pubescence (10–12 years), and adolescence (12–18 years). Many of these subjects also participated in the longitudinal component of the study involving evaluations on four occasions over a four year period. The longitudinal data will be reported separately. The present study only deals with data obtained at the first time point.
Neurocognitive Testing Procedures
A portion of the cognitive battery consisting of standardized tests with published manuals was administered using standard procedures under the supervision of a licensed psychologist in order to evaluate basic intellectual and academic abilities, and to detect behavioral abnormalities. This preliminary battery included an age appropriate version of the Wechsler intelligence scale [abbreviated WISC-R (1974) for children; WASI (1999) for adults] and the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test Screener (2001) to identify individuals functioning within the average range of intellectual ability (IQ 85-119) with commensurate progress in the acquisition of basic academic skills. Testing was typically completed in less than four hours. In addition to satisfying criteria for non-significant variance in intellectual ability, individuals demonstrating significant VIQ-PIQ score discrepancies (greater than 15 points) were eliminated from the study owing to the potential for possible asymmetric cognitive development. Any potential participant manifesting a significant discrepancy between IQ and reading and/or math development also was eliminated owing to the potential of confounds associated with an underlying learning disability. The Devereux Scale for Mental Disorders (1994) was used to assess social and emotional development. A T-score on any of the Devereux Scales (conduct, attention, anxiety, depression, autism, acute problems) ≥ 60 eliminated a child from the study owing to potential confounds associated with psychopathology. The FH-RDC (Andreasen et al., 1977) interview was given to rule out DSM-IV psychiatric disorders in first degree relatives. A pediatrician performed a medical examination, including Tanner staging of physical development, for each child at entry into the study as well as the 1, 2, and 3 year longitudinal follow-up to correspond with the MR examination. All tests included in the battery are appropriate for use across the age span of the study, are developmentally sensitive and have established age norms. The full neurocognitive battery included measures of cognitive domains divided here into language, visual spatial constructional, executive function, and memory abilities. The specific tests assigned to each domain are listed in Table 1. A composite score was computed for each domain by adding the scores of the tests and dividing by the number of tests used.
Table 1.
DOMAIN | TEST | VARIABLES |
---|---|---|
Language | Abbreviated WISC for children | Total Verbal raw scores |
Wechsler Individual Scale of Intelligence (WASI) for Adults | Total Verbal raw scores | |
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT) | Reading, Spelling raw scores | |
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF) | Concepts and Directions raw scores | |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Tests | Raw scores | |
Executive Function |
Wechsler Similarities and Matrix Reasoning Subtests | Raw scores |
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test | Perseverative Errors | |
Visual Spatial | Wechsler Block Design | Raw score |
Visual Motor Integration Test | Raw score | |
Test of Visual Perception | Spatial Relations Subtest- raw score | |
Memory | Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning (WRAML) |
Picture Memory, Design Memory, Verbal Learning, Story Memory and Number/Letter Subtests |
A set of test variables from the entire battery were selected to assess the cognitive domains of language, visual spatial, executive function, and memory. The cognitive domain scores were used for determination of associations with selected MRSI variables and percent gray matter. The first step was determining which of these cognitive measures, if any, were associated with the three MRSI variables, PCr, sPME/sPDE, and NAA. This analysis was followed by determining if synaptic elimination represented by percent gray matter was associated with the values of PCr, sPME/sPDE, and NAA. It was predicted that changes in one or more of the molecular indices obtained through the use of curve fitting procedures would coincide with or precede evidence of synaptic development and elimination. Cognitive tests were used to evaluate the overall strength of association among cognitive function, the targeted metabolites, and gray matter volume.
MR Procedures
MRI and MRSI procedures were conducted using a doubly tuned transmit/receive volume head coil on a GE LX 1.5-T whole-body MRI system (GE Medical Systems, Milwaukee, Wisconsin). A three-dimensional volume of T1-weighted images covering the entire brain [spoiled gradient recalled acquisition (SPGR), repetition time (TR) = 25 ms, echo time (TE) = 5 ms, flip angle = 40°, field of view (FOV) = 240×180mm, slice thickness = 1.5mm, 124 coronal slices, number of excitations = 1, matrix = 256×192, and scan time = 7m 44 s] was then collected for tissue-segmentation analysis of the 31P spectroscopy voxels. In addition a set of T2-weighted/proton density images (two-dimensional fast spin-echo; TR = 3000 ms; echo times = 17 and 102ms; echo-train length = 8; FOV = 240×240 mm2; approximately 24 axial slices, 5-mm thick, and no gap; number of excitations = 1; matrix = 256×192; and scan time 5m 12 s) to screen for neuroradiological abnormalities.
31P MRSI Acquisition
In order to prescribe the MRSI slice location, a 3-plane MRI localizer image was first collected, followed by a set of sagittal and axial scout images using the two-dimensional fast spin-echo sequence. Using the mid-sagittal image the anterior commissure-posterior commissure (AC-PC) line was defined and a 30 mm axial slice was positioned parallel to and superior to the AC-PC line for the spectroscopy (Figure 1A). Prior to the spectroscopy, automatic and manual shimming was applied to the axial slice. A single-slice selective excitation radio frequency (RF) pulse followed by phase-encoding pulses to spatially encode the two dimensions of the slice (termed FIDCSI on a GE system) was used to acquire the multi-voxel in vivo 31P spectroscopy data. An example of quantified 31P spectroscopy data, which is collected as described above, is shown in Figure 2. The acquisition parameters were FOV = 240×360 mm2, slice thickness = 30 mm, 8×8 phase encoding steps (nominal voxel volume = 3.0×4.5×3.0 cm3), TR = 2000 ms, complex data points = 1024, spectral bandwidth = 5.0 kHz, pre-acquisition delay = 1.7ms, number of averages = 16, and acquisition time approximately 34 minutes.
1H MRSI Acquisition
This acquisition method combined the PRESS sequence (Bottomley, 1987) with the phase encoding steps of a chemical shift imaging (CSI) sequence, which is termed PRESSCSI and is part of the GE spectroscopy package. Briefly, the 90E RF pulse followed by two 180E RF pulses, which make-up this double-echo sequence, are all slice selective and the intersection of the three orthogonal planes defines a large region of interest (ROI). In this study the ROI is positioned in the axial plan and the left-right and anterior-posterior dimensions will vary accordingly to ensure the ROI covers the brain in the defined axial plane. Surrounding the ROI in the axial plan are four spatially localized saturation slices to suppress the strong lipid signal at the corners of the ROI. Very selective suppression pulses are used for the PRESS localization and the lipid saturation, which provide a much sharper excitation slice profile relative to conventional pulses (Roux, 1998). An example of quantified short TE 1H spectroscopy data, which is collected as described above, is shown in Figure 3. The experimental parameters for the water suppressed PRESSCSI measurement were: FOV=240 mm, thickness of the ROI slice= 20 mm, phase encoding steps = 16×16 (nominal voxel dimension = 1.5×1.5×2 cm3), TR = 1,500 ms, TE = 30 ms, complex data points = 2,048, spectral bandwidth = 2.5 kHz, and NEX = 2. Using identical experimental parameters, water unsuppressed PRESSCSI data were also collected for post-processing purposes, except there are 8×8 phase encoding steps.
MRSI post-processing and quantification
To minimize the partial volume effect for the sampled regions, six different voxel grid shift schemes (Figure 1A) were applied to both 1H and 31P MRSI prior to two-dimensional inverse Fourier transformation (2D IFT). These grid schemes provided voxels that include left and right: prefrontal cortex (LPFC, RPFC); superior temporal cortex (LSTC, RSTC); inferior parietal cortex (LIPC, RIPC); basal ganglia (LBG, RBG); and centrum semiovale (LCS, RCS).
Spectral fitting of 31P MRSI
For 31P MRSI, a mild spatial apodization was applied in k-space (Fermi window with 90% diameter and 5% transition width) resulting in an effective voxel size of approximately 46.4 cm3, whereas a 5 Hz Gaussian apodization is applied in the chemical shift domain and PME, Pi, PDE, PCr, ∀-, ∃-, and (-ATP, and Pi were modeled in the time domain with Gaussian-damped sinusoids and by omitting the first 2.75 ms of the free induction decay (FID) using the Marquardt-Levenberg algorithm (Figure 2). This approach ensured that PME and PDE resonances primarily reflected freely mobile short NMR correlation time components (Stanley & Pettegrew, 2001). 31P MRSI quantification was expressed in relative mole percent of the observable 31P resonances. The mole percent method of quantification highly correlates with absolute quantification methods (Klunk et al., 1994). Reliability results (relative mole percent of observable phosphorus resonances ∀ SD and the CV in parentheses] for the multi-voxel obtained 31P MRSI metabolites quantification are: sPME, 10.4 ∀ 2.0 (19%); Pi, 5.4 ∀ 1.3 (24%); sPDE, 28.5 ∀ 3.3 (12%); PCr, 11.3 ∀ 1.4 (13%); ∀ATP, 14.3 ∀ 2.0 (14%); ∃ATP, 16.3 ∀ 2.1 (13%); and ATP, 13.8 ∀ 2.6 (19%).
Spectral fitting of short TE 1H MRSI
The residual of the unsuppressed water signal between 7 ppm and 4.2 ppm was removed by using the (operator-independent) SVD-based fitting algorithm (de Beer & van Ormondt, 1992). The LC Model software package was used to quantify the 1H metabolites which include: NAA, glutamate, glutamine, myo-inositol, trimethylamines (TMA), total creatine (creatine + phosphocreatine; Crt), taurine, alanine, aspartate, GABA, glucose, scyllo-inositol, n-acetylaspartylglutamate (NAAG), and the macromolecule signals. The key in simplifying the fitting of the complex overlapping resonances was to incorporate a priori knowledge into the fitting algorithm (deGraaf & Bovee, 1990; Provencher, 1993; Stanley et al., 1995). This included a priori knowledge of the macromolecule signals as part of the basis of LC Model (Seeger et al., 2003). The LC Model software package, which is commercially available, provided this feature and has been demonstrated by others (Brockmann et al., 1996; Frahm & Hanefeld, 1996; Ebert et al., 1997) to be an accurate and reliable method to quantify short TE 1H MRS data. Our studies gave mean absolute levels, relative to the unsuppressed water signal and appropriate correction factors, and coefficient of variation (CV) of the prominent 1H metabolites as: NAA, 7.41 mM (9.4%); Crt, 5.36 mM (13.5%); GPC + PC, 1.44 mM (16.8%); myo-inositol, 4.18 mM (20.8%); glutamate, 6.53 mM (20.4%); glutamine 2.77 mM (51.8%); and combined macromolecule signal 5.94 (33.4%) (scaled to H2O resonance but uncorrected for relaxation times and total number of observable protons). Quantification of the NAA resonance was most reliable (CV = 9.4%); however, measures of glutamate and myo-inositol are more variable (CV = 20%) and glutamine is highly variable (CV = 52%). The mean absolute metabolite levels and the variance were consistent with prior short TE studies at 1.5T (Bartha et al., 2000; Brockmann et al., 1996; Ebert et al., 1997; Frahm et al., 1996; Kreis, 1997; Provencher, 1993; Stanley et al., 1995). These CV values demonstrate sufficient reliability enabling us to conduct short TE 1H MRSI studies for the hypotheses to be tested. The post-processing was fully automated, required no operator input, and quantification results were 100% reproducible. An example of processed in vivo 1H spectrum of a healthy individual using the proposed short TE 1H MRSI technique and the postprocessing protocol is shown in Figure 3. Proton MRSI quantification was expressed as relative mole percent of observable proton resonances.
Morphometry
A fully automated segmentation procedure was used to segment T1- weighted 3D SPGR images. The functional magnetic resonance imaging of the brain (FMRIB) software Library (FSL) (Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009) (FSL Analysis Group, FMRIB, Oxford, UK) was used to co-register, correct for any B1 field bias, brain extraction and segmentation. The axial slices from the 3D-SPGR images were co-registered to the axial scout images using FMRIB's linear image registration tool (FLIRT) of the FSL package. The brain extraction tool (BET) software was used to extract brain by removing non-brain matter from the image. The FMRIB's automated segmentation tool (FAST) was used to segment the brain into gray, white and CSF/extra cortical space while also correcting for spatial intensity variations (also known as bias field or B1 field inhomogenity correction). Tools from the software package FreeSurfer (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harward.edu) was used to convert the MRI images from ANALYZE format to neuroimaging informatics technology initiative (NIFTI) format, extract header information, and intensity normalization prior to segmentation.
The tissue fractions were then calculated by extracting from the segmented images the region of interest matching the coordinates and size of the 31P spectroscopy voxels using miscellaneous FSL utilities for converting and processing images (FSLUTILS) tools (FSLROI, FSLSTATS, FSLMATHS). A segmented image and the voxel placement are illustrated in Figure 1B.
Data Analysis
Our molecular neurodevelopmental program, a part of which is reported here, was a combined cross-sectional and longitudinal study with the goal of evaluating subjects across the age range (6–18 years) that covers the physical developmental stages of childhood (6–10 years) pubescence (10–12 years), and adolescence (12–18 years). Subjects were studied at entry and yearly for a total of four assessments. In this report only the cross sectional aspect of the study is considered. Molecular and neuromorphometric findings were obtained from 12 brain regions, 6 in each hemisphere. Values for the targeted metabolites were averaged across the axial slice containing the 12 brain regions thereby yielding a single axial slice value for each metabolite. Our study’s primary hypothesis is that neuromolecular underpinnings of synaptic development and elimination, major processes in neurodevelopment, will coincide with or precede changes in percent gray matter and will be observed by differences in the targeted brain metabolites among age groups accompanied by corresponding differences in cognitive function.
To analyze the effect of age and therefore neurodevelopment on the targeted metabolites, scatterplots of age versus average axial slice metabolite levels were obtained and fitted with a locally weighted scatterplot smoothing (LOESS) smoothing function (Cleveland, 1979; Cleveland & Grosse, 1988). The scatterplots (Figure 4) show transition points approximating ages of known neurodevelopment stages, i.e., childhood (6–10 years), pubescence (10–12 years), and adolescence (12–18 years). To more clearly show these transition points for targeted metabolites and percent gray matter, we calculate z values for each (Figure 5). Based on the findings given in Figure 4 and Figure 5, it was elected to compare the 6–9.5 and the 12–18 year old age groups. Comparisons of average metabolite levels between the 6–9.5 and 12–18 years old age groups were performed using two-sided t tests.
The same graphic approach was taken in which z-values for PCr, composite cognitive test scores, and percent gray matter were plotted by age (Figure 6). This step was taken so that cognitive function and metabolite levels could be compared with changes in percent cerebral cortical gray matter, which we hypothesized would show an initial growth reflecting synaptic development followed by a reduction because of synaptic elimination.
Next, strength of association between cognitive function and metabolites with age was examined. For this purpose linear multiple regression analyses were performed relating composite scores for the major cognitive domains including language, visual spatial, executive function and memory, to PCr, sPME/sPDE, and NAA levels, as well as percent gray matter. Tests and variables used within each domain are listed in Table 1. Because this is an age-difference study raw scores of cognitive functions were used rather than age corrected standard scores.
RESULTS
MRSI metabolite level changes with age
Average axial slice metabolite levels and percent gray matter were plotted by age using a LOESS smoothing function (Figure 4). These plots demonstrate age-related transition points for PCr, sPME/sPDE, and percent gray matter. The plots show transition points at 9.5 and 12 years. Figure 5 displays the change of PCr, sPME/sPDE ratio, and percent gray matter by converting the metabolite levels and percent gray matter to Z-scores. Guided by these plots and other neurodevelopmental data the metabolite and percent gray matter data were grouped into ages 6–9.5, 9.5–12, and 12–18 years old in order to analyze changes in the metabolite levels with age.
Metabolic Differences Among Age Groups
Descriptive statistics for evaluating differences in PCr, sPME/sPDE, gray matter and composite cognitive scores among the 6–9.5, 9.5–12, and 12–18 years old age groups are presented in Table2. Since there were clear inflections in metabolite levels and percent gray matter in the 6–9.5 and 12–18 year old age groups and a flattening in the 9.5–12 year old group, we performed the statistical analysis as two group comparisons using two-sided t tests. The results indicated that comparing 6–9.5 year olds to the 12–18 year olds PCr was lower in the younger age group (PCr, p = .0001) and percent gray matter (p = .0013) and sPME/sPDE ratio (p = .046) were higher. No significant difference was found for NAA comparing the 6–9.5 year olds with 12–18 year olds. The coefficient of variance for PCr (13%) also is much smaller than those for sPME (19%) but similar to those for sPDE (12%) and NAA (9.4%).
Table 2.
Age Group | 6 – 9.5 (years) | 9.5 – 12 (years) | 12 – 18 (years) | 6 – 9.5 vs. 12–18 | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
M ± SD | N | M ± SD | N | M ± SD | N | p-value | |
sPME/sPDE (ratio mole %) | 0.538 ± 0.059 | 33 | 0.528 ± 0.78 | 26 | 0.507 ± 0.063 | 40 | .046 |
PCr (mole %) | 10.95 ± 0.82 | 33 | 11.33 ± 0.74 | 26 | 11.66 ± 0.81 | 40 | .0001 |
NAA (mM) | 6.98 ± 1.52 | 26 | 6.98 ± 1.26 | 21 | 7.82 ± 1.82 | 35 | Not Significant |
Gray Matter(% volume) | 39.88 ± 3.45 | 26 | 38.65 ± 2.90 | 25 | 36.31 ± 2.94 | 40 | .0013 |
Relations Among, PCr Levels, Cognitive Function, and Gray Matter with Age
Of the metabolites measured, PCr is the one which most directly reflects synaptic activity (see Discussion). For purposes of these graphic analyses all scores were converted to z-scores allowing for their comparisons on the same scales. Figure 6 illustrates the profiles of various cognitive domains across the age groups placed on the same graphs as PCr and gray matter percentage. In general, PCr level tracked the cognitive changes quite closely. The rate of PCr increase in the 6–9.5 year olds is similar to the rate of increase in percent gray matter. During the 9.5 to 12 year age range the slope of PCr is approximately flat while percent gray matter sharply decreases. Percentage of gray matter is characterized by an increase in the 6–9.5 year old age group and then a marked decrease in the 9.5–12 year age group with virtually no change from 12 to 18 years. PCr continued to increase in the 12 to 18 year age range.
Correlations of Composite Cognitive scores with MRSI metabolite levels
Multiple linear regression analysis of composite cognitive scores with average axial slice metabolite levels and percent gray matter (Table 3) demonstrated significant correlations of PCr levels with age and language, visual spatial construction, and memory domains. NAA showed a significant correlation only with the visual spatial construction domain. These findings taken together strongly suggest that cognitive function correlates with synaptic activity and not simply the amount of gray matter.
Table 3.
Age | Language | Visual Spatial | Executive Function | Memory | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r | N | r | N | r | N | r | N | r | N | |
PCr | .474*** | 99 | .421*** | 96 | .234* | 94 | .076 | 79 | .291** | 98 |
sPME/sPDE | −.150 | 98 | −.064 | 95 | −.108 | 94 | .008 | 79 | .009 | 97 |
NAA | .208 | 85 | .173 | 83 | .237* | 81 | .135 | 69 | .159 | 85 |
Gray Matter | −.399*** | 81 | −.287* | 78 | −.368*** | 76 | −.223 | 69 | −.323** | 80 |
DISCUSSION
In this cross-sectional age difference study we investigated the relationship between a set of 31P-1H MRSI measured metabolites and cognitive development. We did morphological studies for the purpose of evaluating percent gray matter across age groups. The major hypothesis was that neuromolecular underpinnings of synaptic development and elimination would reflect age related cognitive development and coincide or precede change in the percent gray matter.
The hypothesis would be supported if there were significant correlations in the appropriate direction between the metabolites and cognitive tests of varying abilities. We studied tests of language, visual spatial constructional abilities, executive function, and memory. Multiple correlations were computed to evaluate the strength of association between tests within the domains and the metabolites. In general, there was a significant degree of association between changes in the levels of PCr and age-associated improvement in cognitive ability but not with the percent of gray matter.
NAA Levels
NAA (470–974 :moles/100 g) (McIlwain & Bachelard, 1985, pg. 155), which is second to glutamate (781–1250 :moles/100 g) (McIlwain et al., 1985, pg. 155) in terms of total brain concentration of free amino acids, accounts for approximately 85–90% of the proton signal of the N-acetyl methyl group and NAAG contributes to the remaining 10–15% (Koller et al., 1984; Frahm et al., 1991; Pouwels & Frahm, 1997). Both NAA and NAAG are localized exclusively in mature neurons and neuronal processes, and not in mature glia (Tallan et al., 1956; Koller et al., 1984; Birken & Oldendorf, 1989; Urenjak et al., 1993). NAA is formed in mitochondria from acetyl-CoA and aspartate by the membrane bound enzyme L-aspartate N-acetyltransferase, an enzyme found in brain but not in other tissue such as heart, liver and kidney (Goldstein, 1959; Knizley, 1967; Goldstein, 1969; Truckenmiller et al., 1985). Monoclonal antibody studies show NAA to be localized to neurons with intense staining of the perikarya and proximal dendrites axons (Simmons et al., 1991). The neuronal immunoreactivity does not correspond to primary neurotransmitter characteristics. In general, NAA is considered a putative neuronal marker (Arnold et al., 1990) and a recent report of whole brain NAA levels in young adults gave a value of 9.5 ∀ 1.0 mM in white matter an 14.3 ∀ 1.1 mM of gray matter (Inglese et al., 2008). However, in vivo 1H MRS brain studies have shown that decreased NAA is reversible (De Stefano et al., 1995). Therefore, the levels of NAA detected by in vivo 1H MRS also will be dependent on factors that alter the levels of substrates or the activities of the metabolic enzymes as well as tissue factors. Our preliminary studies reveal that the NAA resonance undergoes reversible changes after brief hypoxia (Pettegrew et al., unpublished observations). In addition, factors that change the composition of the tissue sampled such as changing the percent of neurons with long projections or the percent of glial processes will alter NAA levels.
n-Acetyl moieties with short correlation times such as found in n-acetyl-sugars (n-acetyl-galactosamine, n-acetyl-glucosamine, n-acetyl-neuraminic acid) and glycosphingolipids such as gangliosides also can contribute to the "NAA" resonance. Gangliosides are enriched in gray matter (Agranoff & Hajra, 1994). Evidence indicates that gangliosides are specifically localized in neurons (Lowden & Wolfe, 1964), where gangliosides are enriched in synaptic membranes (Whittaker, 1966; Wiegandt, 1967). The levels of gangliosides in human brain cerebral gray matter (1.45–1.68 mmoles/100 g dry weight) and cerebral white matter (0.28–0.37 mmoles/100 g dry weight) have been determined by Eeg-Olofsson et al. (1966). The levels of gangliosides are developmentally regulated. In the rat brain ganglioside levels start to increase by post-natal day 5 and peak at approximately post-natal day 20 (Suzuki, 1966). Hess et al. (1976) report that human neurons contain 6 times more sialic acid groups than rat brain. Sialoproteins also contain n-acetylneuraminic acid and could contribute up to 15% of the total brain protein (McIlwain et al., 1985, pg. 316).
sPME/sPDE Ratio
The sPME/sPDE ratio reflects the metabolite turnover of membrane phospholipids such as phosphatidylcholie, phosphatidylethanolamine, and phosphatidylserine. With net synthesis of these phospholipids sPME/sPDE is greater than 1, with net breakdown it is less than 1, and equals 1 when synthesis and breakdown are in equilibrium. Increased sPME are found at the site and time of neuritic sprouting in brain and sPDE are increased at the site and time of membrane breakdown (Geddes et al., 1997). sPME and sPDE are not involved in the synthesis or breakdown of glycosphingolipids such as gangliosides which are enriched in synaptic membranes (Whittaker, 1966; Wiegandt, 1967). sPME/sPDE increases from age 6 to 9.5 years reflecting active synthesis of membrane phospholipids followed by decreases until approximately 14 years of age with evidence for net phospholipid breakdown from age 11 to 14 years. The percent gray matter curve also is shown in Figure 5. As observed, the percent gray matter increases for ages 6 to 9.5 years followed by a steeper decline until approximately 13 years. Changes in percent gray matter were consistent with synaptic development followed by synaptic elimination and are in keeping with the neuromorphometric findings of Giedd et al. (1999) and Gogtay et al. (2007), the molecular findings of Geddes et al. (1997), and the histological data of Huttenlocher and Dabholkar (1997). Note that the rate of increase in percent gray matter from 6–9.5 years and decrease from 9.5 to 13 years is greater than for sPME/sPDE. The difference in these rates (percent gray matter versus sPME/sPDE) could be due to ganglioside contributions to the gray matter.
PCr
Brain contains approximately 3mmole g−1 ATP which could maximally provide 6 mmoles g−1 of high-energy phosphate. ATP utilization in rat brain is approximately 0.5 mmoles g−1s−1 and therefore available stores of ATP would last approximately 12s without new ATP production. However, PCr is a storage form of ATP which can be converted to ATP by the creatine kinase (CrK) enzyme according to the reaction:
This is an equilibrium reaction ()G = 0), i.e., no net flux in either direction. ATP synthesis is stimulated by a reduction in ATP or increases in ADP or [H+]. In a comprehensive review Andres et al. (2008) describe PCr as a spatial energy “shuttle” or “circuit” that bridges sites of ATP generation and consumption. Brain concentration of PCr is approximately 5 mmole g−1 and therefore the stores of high-energy phosphate (ATP+PCr) will be consumed after about 20s of continuous utilization (Siesjo, 1978, pg. 12). Several studies have shown that anesthesia causes an elevation of PCr and ATP in parallel to the decrease in neuronal activity (McCandless & Wiggins, 1981; Hein et al., 1975). Sokoloff (1991; 1993) also has shown that the synapse is the site of highest energy consumption during increased activity and that most of this consumption is in the recovery period rather than during the activity itself. This is consistent with the studies of Jansson et al. (1979) who showed that PCr and ATP levels are lower in isolated cerebral nerve endings than in whole brain. Unlike nerve tissue in general, synaptosomes preferentially utilize endogenous PCr and ATP stores. Jansson et al. (1979) conclude that synaptic transmission primarily depends on local stores of high-energy phosphates rather than on the availability of glucose per se. Results of human 1H fMRS monitoring of brain PCr levels before, during, and following visual cortical activation are in keeping with the above explanations (Rango et al., 1997; 2001). Other sources of ATP production such as oxidative phosphorylation and glycolysis come into play after the consumption of PCr pools.
PCr levels are low at age 6 and then sharply increase until age 10. From age 10 to 12 years the increase in PCr levels is much less, but from age 12 to 17 years PCr levels again sharply increase. The low levels of PCr at age 6 years is consistent with the active synthesis of membrane phospholipids which require ATP. For example, one mole of dipalmitoylphosphatidylcholine (DPPC) derived from de novo synthesis requires 295 moles of ATP. Each palmitate requires 145 moles of ATP, uptake of choline requires 1 mole of ATP, synthesis of phosphocholine requires 1 mole of ATP, and the synthesis of the intermediate phosphatidic acid requires 3 moles of ATP. These numbers take into consideration the direct utilization of ATP and the ATP equivalents lost from the utilization of NADH (3 ATP), FADH2 (2 ATP), and acetyl-CoA (12 ATP) in the synthesis of DDPC. The breakdown of DPPC recovers only 258 moles of ATP from the oxidation of both palmitates. Therefore, the complete synthesis and breakdown of 1 mole of DPPC results in an energetic debt of 37 moles of ATP. The acylation/deacylation of DPPC results in an energetic debt of 2 moles of ATP. Likewise, the de novo synthesis of 1 mole of cholesterol, a major membrane constituent requires 276 moles of ATP with no ATP recovered from the breakdown of cholesterol (Pettegrew et al., 2003).
The slow increase in PCr levels from ages 9.5–12 years coincides with a steep decrease in gray matter volume and less rapid decrease in the sPME/sPDE ratio. One might speculate that the membranes being lost are predominantly non-phosphorus containing glycosphingolipids such as gangliosides which are predominantly found in gray matter and are enriched in synaptic membranes. By this reasoning, the decrease in gray matter is most likely due to decrease in synaptic membranes contained in the neuropil.
The decline in gray matter volume from 12–18 years corresponds to continued increases in PCr. In the brain ATP is primarily consumed at synaptic membranes for repolarizing synaptic membranes which have been depolarized and PCr is the buffer for ATP (Buchli et al., 1994; Chugani et al., 1987; Frey, 1994; Hein et al., 1975; Hess, 1961; Jansson et al., 1979; Kadekaro et al., 1985; Kennedy & Sokoloff, 1957; McCandless et al., 1981; Sokoloff, 1966; 1991). The increase in PCr in the 12–18 year olds could be caused by synaptic elimination resulting in fewer synapses or decreased activity of remaining synapses in older subjects.
Correlations Among PCr Levels, Gray Matter Volume, and Cognitive Composite Scores
Both PCr and percent gray matter in the axial slice are highly correlated with age, but NAA, sPME, sPDE, and sPME/sPDE are not. In addition, both PCr and percent gray matter in the axial slice are correlated with language, visual spatial construction, and memory but not with executive function. Finally, PCr levels and percent gray matter in the axial slice correlate (r = − .304, p = .0026) with each other, but NAA and percent gray matter are not correlated (r = −.023; p = .835). These findings taken together and inspection of Figure 6 suggests that axial slice PCr levels in general follow a curve similar to curves for the various cognitive domains. However, the curve for percent gray matter has qualitative findings dissimilar to the curve for PCr which is consistent with synaptic elimination (percent gray matter curve) and the functional consequence of synaptic elimination or reduced synaptic activity (PCr curve). Finally, given the lack of correlation between NAA and percent gray matter, it may be there are other molecular contributions to the "NAA" resonance signal besides n-acetylaspartate as discussed above. Regional analyses of these data is in progress and will be extremely useful to address some of these issues, such as whether synaptic elimination in PFC is occurring later than regions such as STC or IPC.
Conclusions
These conclusions are based on cross-sectional analysis and there would be clear advantages to testing our subjects longitudinally. We have done so, and will report the findings in a future publication. We would note though that it is not likely that the cross-sectional data were markedly influenced by cohort effects since all subjects were recruited from the same community over a period of only four years. There is the possibility of gender differences which we could not evaluate here because of small sample sizes. A final consideration is that the study was limited to an axial slice of brain measures of PCr, sPME, sPDE, and NAA and different results may be found at different regional locations. Since we had the capability of obtaining MRS measures from various brain regions, we will present the findings in a future report.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported in part by an NIHCD/NIH HD-39799 grant (JWP). We thank Terry Bradbury for conducting neuropsychological testing. Indebtedness is also expressed to the Medical Research Service and the VISNIV Mental Illness, Research, Education and Clinical Center (MIRECC), Department of Veterans Affairs for support of this work. We thank Harriet Marshman, Deborah Wetzler and Dennis McKeag for help in conducting the study.
Footnotes
There is no conflict of interest on the part of any of the authors.
REFERENCES
- Agranoff BW, Hajra AK. Lipids. In: Siegel GJ, Agranoff BW, Albers RW, Molinoff PB, editors. Basic Neurochemistry, Molecular, Cellular, and Medical Aspects. 5th ed. New York: Raven Press; 1994. pp. 97–116. [Google Scholar]
- Andreasen NC, Endicott J, Spitzer RL, Winoker G. The reliability and validity of the family history method using family history research diagnostic criteria (FH-RDC) Archives of General Psychiatry. 1977;34:1229–1235. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770220111013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andres RH, Ducray AD, Schlattner U, Wallimann T, Widmer HR. Functions and effects of creatine in the central nervous system. Brain Research Bulletin. 2008;76:329–343. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2008.02.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold DL, Matthews PM, Francis G, Antel J. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of human brain in vivo in the evaluation of multiple sclerosis: Assessment of the load of disease. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1990;14:154–159. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910140115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartha R, Drost DJ, Menon RS, Williamson PC. Comparison of the quantification precision of human short echo time (1)H spectroscopy at 1.5 and 4.0 Tesla. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2000;44:185–192. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200008)44:2<185::aid-mrm4>3.0.co;2-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch HG, Gussow JD. Disadvantaged children, health, and nutrition and school failure. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World; 1970. [Google Scholar]
- Birken DL, Oldendorf WH. N-Acetyl-L-aspartic acid: A literature review of a compound prominent in 1H-NMR spectroscopic studies of brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1989;13:23–31. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(89)80048-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bottomley PA. Spatial localization in NMR spectroscopy in vivo. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1987;508:333–348. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1987.tb32915.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourgeois J-P, Rakic P. Changes of synaptic density in the primary visual cortex of the Macaque monkey from fetal to adult stage. Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:2801–2820. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-07-02801.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brockmann K, Pouwels PJ, Christen HJ, Frahm J, Hanefeld F. Localized proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of cerebral metabolic disturbances in children with neuronal ceroid lipofuscinosis. Neuropediatrics. 1996;27:242–248. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-973772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buchli R, Martin E, Boesiger P, Rumpel H. Developmental changes of phosphorus metabolite concentrations in the human brain: A 31P magnetic resonance spectroscopy study in vivo. Pediatric Research. 1994;35:431–435. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chugani HR, Phelps ME, Mazziotta JC. Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Annals of Neurology. 1987;322:487–497. doi: 10.1002/ana.410220408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland WS. Robust locally-weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. Journal of the American Statistical Association. 1979;74:829–836. [Google Scholar]
- Cleveland WS, Grosse E. Regression by local fitting. Journal of Econometrics. 1988;37:87–114. [Google Scholar]
- de Beer R, van Ormondt D. Analysis of NMR data using time domain fitting procedures. In: Diehl P, Fluck EG, editors. NMR Basics, Principles and Progress. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1992. pp. 201–258. [Google Scholar]
- De Stefano N, Matthews PM, Arnold DL. Reversible decreases in N-acetylaspartate after acute brain injury. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1995;34:721–727. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- deGraaf AA, Bovee WMMJ. Improved quantification of in vivo1H NMR spectra by optimization of signal acquisition and processing and by incorporation of prior knowledge into the spectral fitting. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1990;15:305–319. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910150212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebert D, Speck O, Konig A, Berger M, Hennig J, Hohagen F. 1H-magnetic resonance spectroscopy in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence for neuronal loss in the cingulate gyrus and the right striatum. Psychiatry Research. 1997;74:173–176. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(97)00016-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eeg-Olofsson O, Kristensson K, Sourander P, Svennerholm L. Tay-Sach's Disease. A generalized emtabolic disorder. Acta Paediatrica Scandinavia. 1966;55:546–562. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1966.tb15254.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frahm J, Hanefeld F. Localized proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy of cerebral metabolites. Neuropediatrics. 1996;27:64–69. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-973751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frahm J, Michaelis T, Merboldt KD, Hanicke W, Gyngell ML, Bruhn H. On the N-acetyl methyl resonance in localized 1H NMR spectra of human brain in vivo. Nuclear Magnetic Resonance in Biomedicine. 1991;4:201–204. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1940040408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey KA. Positron emission tomography. In: Siegel GJ, Agranoff BW, Albers RW, Molinoff PB, editors. Basic Neurochemistry: Molecular, Cellular and Medical Aspects. 5 ed. New York: Raven Press; 1994. pp. 935–955. [Google Scholar]
- Geddes JW, Panchalingam K, Keller JN, Pettegrew JW. Elevated phosphocholine and phosphatidyl choline following rat entorhinal cortex lesions. Neurobiology of Aging. 1997;18:305–308. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(97)80312-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, Paus T, Evans AC, Rapoport JL. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gogtay N, Ordonez A, Herman DH, Hayashi KM, Greenstein D, Vaituzis C, Lenane M, Clasen L, Sharp W, Giedd JN, Jung D, Nugent TF, III, Toga AW, Leibenluft E, Thompson PM, Rapoport JL. Dynamic mapping of cortical development before and after the onset of pediatric bipolar illness. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines. 2007;48:852–862. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01747.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein FB. Biosynthesis of N-acetyl-L-aspartic acid. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1959;234:2702–2706. [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein FB. The enzymatic synthesis of N-acetyl-L-aspartic acid by subcellular preparations. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1969;244:4257–4260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hein H, Krieglstein J, Stock R. The effects of increased glucose supply and thiopental anesthesia on energy metabolism of the isolated perfused rat brain. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Archives of Pharmacology. 1975;289:399–407. doi: 10.1007/BF00508413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess H. The rates of respiration of neurons and neuroglia in human cerebrum. In: Kety SS, Elkes J, editors. Regional Neurochemistry. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1961. pp. 200–202. [Google Scholar]
- Hess HH, Bass NH, Thalheimer C, Devarakonda R. Gangliosides and the architecture of human frontal and rat somatosensory isocortex. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1976;26:1115–1121. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1976.tb06994.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR. Synaptic density in human frontal cortex. Developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research. 1979;163:195–205. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90349-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR. Morphometric study of human cerebral cortex development. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:517–527. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(90)90031-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1997;387:167–178. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR, de Courtten C, Garey LJ, Van der Loos LH. Synaptogenesis in human visual cortex--evidence for synapse elimination during normal development. Neuroscience Letters. 1982;33:247–252. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(82)90379-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Inglese M, Rusinek H, George IC, Babb JS, Grossman RI, Gonen O. Global average gray and white matter N-acetylaspartate concentration in the human brain. Neuroimage. 2008;41:270–276. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansson SE, Harkonen MH, Helve H. Metabolic properties of nerve endings isolated from rat brain. Acta Physiologica Scandinavica. 1979;107:205–212. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.1979.tb06464.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kadekaro M, Crane AM, Sokoloff L. Differential effects of electrical stimulation of sciatic nerve on metabolic activity in spinal cord and dorsal root ganglion in the rat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1985;82:6010–6013. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.17.6010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kennedy C, Sokoloff L. An adaptation of the nitrous oxide method to the study of the cerebral circulation in children; normal values for cerebral blood flow and cerebral metabolic rate in childhood. Journal of Clinical Investigation. 1957;36:1130–1137. doi: 10.1172/JCI103509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klunk WE, Xu CJ, Panchalingam K, McClure RJ, Pettegrew JW. Analysis of magnetic resonance spectra by mole percent: Comparison to absolute units. Neurobiology of Aging. 1994;15:133–140. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(94)90153-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knizley H. The enzymatic synthesis of N-acetyl-L-aspartic acid by a water-insoluble preparation of a cat brain acetone powder. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1967;242:4619–4622. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koller KJ, Zaczek R, Coyle J. N-acetyl-aspartyl-glutamate: Regional levels in rat brain and the effects of brain lesions as determined by a new HPLC method. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1984;43:1136–1142. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1984.tb12854.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreis R. Quantitative localized 1H MR spectroscopy for clinical use. Journal Progress Nuclear Magnetic Resonance. 1997;31:155–195. [Google Scholar]
- Lowden JA, Wolfe LS. Studies on brain gangliosides. III Evidence for the location of gangliosides specifically in neurones. Canadian Journal of Biochemistry. 1964;42:1587–1594. doi: 10.1139/o64-170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCandless DW, Wiggins RC. Cerebral energy metabolism during the onset and recovery from halothane anesthesia. Neurochemical Research. 1981;6:1319–1326. doi: 10.1007/BF00964353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McIlwain H, Bachelard HS. Biochemistry and the Central Nervous System. 5 ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Pettegrew JW, Keshavan MS, Stanley JA, McClure RJ, Johnson CR, Panchalingam K. Magnetic resonance spectroscopy in the assessment of phospholipid metabolism in schizophrenia and other psychiatric disorders. In: Peet M, Glen I, Horrobin DF, editors. Phospholipid Spectrum Disorder in Psychiatry. 2nd ed. Carnforth, UK: Marius; 2003. pp. 239–255. [Google Scholar]
- Pouwels PJ, Frahm J. Differential distribution of NAA and NAAG in human brain as determined by quantitative localized proton MRS. Nuclear Magnetic Resonance in Biomedicine. 1997;10:73–78. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-1492(199704)10:2<73::aid-nbm448>3.0.co;2-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provencher SW. Estimation of metabolite concentrations from localized in vivo proton NMR spectra. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1993;30:672–679. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910300604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rakic P, Bourgeois J-P, Eckenhoff MF, Zecevic N, Goldman-Rakic PS. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science. 1986;232:232–235. doi: 10.1126/science.3952506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rango M, Bozzali M, Prelle A, Scarlato G, Bresolin N. Brain activation in normal subjects and in patients affected by mitochondrial disease without clinical central nervous system involvement: a phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2001;21:85–91. doi: 10.1097/00004647-200101000-00011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rango M, Castelli A, Scarlato G. Energetics of 3.5 s neural activation in humans: a 31P MR spectroscopy study. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1997;38:878–883. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910380605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roux L. Magnetic Resonance. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging. 1998;8:1022–1023. [Google Scholar]
- Seeger U, Klose U, Mader I, Grodd W, Nagele T. Parameterized evaluation of macromolecules and lipids in proton MR spectroscopy of brain diseases. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2003;49:19–28. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siesjo BK. Brain Energy Metabolism. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Simmons ML, Frondoza CG, Coyle JT. Immunocytochemical localization of N-acetyl-aspartate with monoclonal antibodies. Neuroscience. 1991;45:37–45. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(91)90101-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smiley JF, Goldman-Rakic PS. Heterogeneous targets of dopamine synapses in monkey prefrontal cortex demonstrated by serial section electron microscopy: A laminar analysis using the silver enhanced diaminobenzidine-sulfide (SEDS) immunolabeling technique. Cerebral Cortex. 1993;3:223–238. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.3.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy R, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(S1):208–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokoloff L. Cerebral circulatory and metabolic changes associated with aging. Research Publications - Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease. 1966;41:237–254. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokoloff L. Measurement of local cerebral glucose utilization and its relation to local functional activity in the brain. Advances In Experimental Medicine & Biology. 1991;291:21–42. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-5931-9_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokoloff L. Function-related changes in energy metabolism in the nervous system: localization and mechanisms. Keio Journal of Medicine. 1993;42:95–103. doi: 10.2302/kjm.42.95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley JA, Drost DJ, Williamson PC, Thompson RT. The use of a priori knowledge to quantify short echo in vivo1H MR spectra. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 1995;34:17–24. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stanley JA, Pettegrew JW. Post-processing method to segregate and quantify the broad components underlying the phosphodiester spectral region of in vivo 31-P brain spectra. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine. 2001;45:390–396. doi: 10.1002/1522-2594(200103)45:3<390::aid-mrm1051>3.0.co;2-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storm-Mathisen J, Otterson OP. Immunocytochemistry of glutamate at the synaptic level. Journal of Histochemistry and Cytochemistry. 1990;38:1733–1743. doi: 10.1177/38.12.1979340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki K. The pattern of mammalian brain gangliosides III. Regional and developmental differences. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1966;12:969–979. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1965.tb10256.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tallan HH, Moore S, Stein WH. N-acetyl-L-aspartic acid in brain. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1956;219:257–264. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Truckenmiller ME, Namboodiri MAA, Brownstein MJ, Neale JH. N-Acetylation of L-aspartate in the nervous system: Differential distribution of a specific enzyme. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1985;45:1658–1662. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1985.tb07240.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urenjak J, Williams SR, Gadian DG, Noble M. Proton nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy unambiguously identifies different neural cell types. Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13:981–989. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-03-00981.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whittaker VP. Some properties of synaptic membranes isolated from the central nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1966;137:982–998. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1966.tb50211.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiegandt H. The subcellular localization of gangliosides in the brain. Journal of Neurochemistry. 1967;14:671–674. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1967.tb09571.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woolrich MW, Jbabdi S, Patenaude B, Chappell M, Makni S, Behrens T, Beckmann C, Jenkinson M, Smith SM. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage. 2009;45:S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]