Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009 Jan;15(1):42–52. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708090012

Timing Is Everything: Antiretroviral Non-adherence is Associated with Impairment in Time-based Prospective Memory

Steven Paul Woods 1, Matthew S Dawson 1, Erica Weber 1, Sarah Gibson 2, Igor Grant 1, J Hampton Atkinson 1,3; The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group
PMCID: PMC2776623  NIHMSID: NIHMS156117  PMID: 19128527

Abstract

Non-adherence to combination antiretroviral therapies (cART) is highly prevalent and significantly increases the risk of adverse HIV disease outcomes. The current study evaluated the hypothesis that prospective memory – a dissociable aspect of episodic memory describing the ability to execute a future intention – plays an important role in successful cART adherence. Seventy-nine individuals with HIV infection who were prescribed at least one antiretroviral medication underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological and neuromedical evaluation prior to completing a one-month observation of their cART adherence as measured by electronic medication monitoring. Non-adherent individuals (n = 31) demonstrated significantly poorer prospective memory functioning as compared to adherent persons (n = 48), particularly on an index of time-based ProM (i.e., elevated loss of time errors). Deficits in time-based prospective memory were independently predictive of cART non-adherence, even after considering the possible influence of established predictors of adherence, such as general cognitive impairment (e.g., retrospective learning and memory) and psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., depression). These findings extend a nascent literature showing that impairment in time-based prospective memory significantly increases the risk of medication non-adherence and therefore may guide the development of novel strategies for intervention.

Keywords: Human immunodeficiency virus, AIDS dementia complex, Neuropsychological tests, Cognitive science, Patient compliance, Time perception

INTRODUCTION

Medication non-adherence is a significant public health problem, particularly among persons living with chronic medical conditions. In fact, it has been estimated that as many as 50% of participants in clinical trials for chronic disorders may not adhere to their prescribed medication regimens (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). Medication non-adherence also presents a significant healthcare cost, including an estimated $100 billion in annual expenditures and 33 to 69% of medication-related hospital admissions (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005). Adequate adherence to prescribed medication regimens is particularly important for persons infected with the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), for which optimal disease outcomes are intricately tied to the efficacy of combination antiretroviral (ARV) therapies (cART). Over the past decade, cART has altered the landscape of HIV disease management in developed countries by dramatically reducing HIV-associated morbidity and mortality (Centers for Disease Control, 2006) and also improving health-related quality of life (Liu et al., 2006). However, approximately 40 to 50% of patients are not adherent to their cART regimens (e.g., Nieuwkerk et al., 2001), which has led to adherence being branded as the “Achilles’ heel” of HIV treatment (Simoni et al., 2003). Although there is variability among different classes of ARVs, the literature generally shows that adherence levels of 95–100% are needed to ensure optimal treatment effectiveness (Bangsburg, 2008). Non-adherence to cART (most commonly classified as less than 90% compliance) is associated with poorer HIV disease outcomes, including higher rates of virologic failure (Perno et al., 2002), the development of drug-resistant viral mutations (Harrigan et al., 2005), and an increased risk of mortality (Lima et al., 2007).

Considering the numerous adverse clinical outcomes associated with cART non-adherence, the importance of identifying salient risk factors to target for screening and remediation is readily apparent. Prior studies have identified a variety of factors that influence non-adherence, including demographics (e.g., age), psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., depression, substance abuse), psychosocial variables (e.g., attitudes and beliefs related to medications, familial support), and systemic factors (e.g., limited access to healthcare). HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment is also associated with increased risk of cART non-adherence, which is pertinent in that approximately 30 to 50% of HIV-infected persons demonstrate neuropsychological deficits (Heaton et al., 1995; Robertson et al., 2007). Although progress has been made toward simplifying cART regimens, successful medication management nevertheless remains a complex cognitive challenge that typically requires tracking multiple drugs (oftentimes including non-ARVs) with varying dosages, administration times, and special instructions. Supporting the hypothesized role of cognitive deficits in cART non-adherence, Chesney et al. (2000) found that nearly 70% of individuals who were non-adherent to cART reported that they “simply forgot” to take their medication. Subsequent research demonstrated that HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment is associated with poorer performance on laboratory medication management tasks (e.g., Albert et al., 1999; 2003; Heaton et al., 2004), higher rates of self-reported problems with medication management (e.g., Avants et al., 2001; Benedict et al., 2000; Waldrop-Valverde et al., 2006; Woods et al., 2008b), and non-adherence as measured by electronic medication monitors (e.g., Barclay et al., 2007; Hinkin et al., 2002; 2004). For example, Hinkin et al. (2002) reported that individuals with neuropsychological impairment experienced a twofold risk of non-adherence, even when the potentially confounding effects of demographic factors and psychiatric comorbidities were considered. Across this literature, the domains of episodic learning and memory, executive functions, and psychomotor speed have emerged as the most robust and reliable cognitive predictors of cART non-adherence.

It has been argued that HIV-associated impairment in the domain of prospective memory (ProM) may be a particularly strong risk factor for cART non-adherence (Carey et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2007; Woods et al., 2008b). ProM is a dissociable aspect of episodic memory that refers to the execution of a future intention in the face of ongoing distractions (i.e., “remembering to remember”). The cognitive aspects of medication adherence can be readily mapped on a conceptual framework of ProM. Specifically, successful independent medication adherence requires one to: 1) encode an intention to take a specific medication at a future occasion (e.g., take medication X with food before going to bed); 2) retain the paired intention (i.e., take medication X) and cue (i.e., at bedtime) vis-à-vis the usual barrage of normal daily events (e.g., work, chores, and recreation); 3) accurately identify the retrieval cue and effectively disengage from an ongoing activity (e.g., preparing for bed); 4) recall the specific intention (i.e., take medication X with food); and 5) execute the intention (i.e., take the correct medication as instructed). In fact, the most commonly cited example of ProM in daily life is remembering to take a medication on schedule, for example after a meal (i.e., event-based ProM) or at a specific time during the day (i.e., time-based ProM). Despite these conceptual similarities, only three prior studies have directly examined the relationship between ProM and medication adherence (Hertzog et al., 2000; Vedhara et al., 2004; Woods et al., 2008b).

Beyond its conceptual appeal, ProM may be of particular relevance to cART adherence because HIV infection is associated with an increased risk of ProM impairment. Individuals living with HIV disease report an elevated level of ProM complaints (Woods et al., 2007a) and demonstrate mild-to-moderate deficits on performance-based laboratory (Carey et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2007) and semi-naturalistic (Carey et al., 2006) measures of ProM. The profile of HIV-associated ProM impairment is hypothesized to reflect deficits in the strategic aspects of intention encoding and retrieval (Carey et al., 2006), including increased errors of omission (i.e., not responding to a cue), commission (e.g., incorrectly responding to a cue), and loss of time (i.e., responding to a cue at the incorrect time) in the setting of normal recognition. HIV-associated ProM impairment correlates with deficits in executive functions, working memory, retrospective episodic memory, and information processing speed (Carey et al., 2006; Martin et al., 2007), as well as biological markers of neuroaxonal injury and macrophage activation (Woods et al., 2006b).

Only one prior study has examined the role of HIV-associated ProM impairment in medication management. Woods et al. (2008b) reported that higher frequency of ProM complaints and objective deficits on laboratory and semi-naturalistic ProM measures were associated with poorer self-reported medication management in HIV. Of particular note, HIV-associated ProM impairment demonstrated incremental validity as a predictor of medication management, above-and-beyond established risk factors for non-adherence, including psychiatric distress, psychosocial variables, environmental structure, and deficits in retrospective memory and executive functions. Although this study provided promising evidence that ProM may play a unique role in medication management, it nevertheless possessed several methodological limitations. For example, the study design was exclusively cross-sectional and therefore only provided evidence of concurrent, rather than predictive validity. Another significant limitation of this study was its use of a self-report measure of general medication management (i.e., Beliefs Related to Medications Survey [BERMA]; McDonald-Miszczak et al., 2004). Although there is no gold standard for adherence (Osterberg & Blaschke, 2005), self-report measures tend to overestimate actual adherence (Levine et al., 2006). Moreover, this particular self-report measure was not specific to ARVs, but rather assessed all currently prescribed medications. Use of this measure also limited the prior study’s ecological validity by not allowing for an objective cutpoint to identify individuals who were non-adherent to cART, which is the classification of greatest clinical relevance.

Accordingly, the current study was undertaken to determine whether baseline indicators of ProM functioning accurately predict cART adherence as measured in a subsequent one-month observation period using electronic medication monitors. It was hypothesized that HIV-associated ProM impairment would be associated with an increased risk of cART non-adherence, independent of demographics, HIV disease severity, psychiatric comorbidity, and psychosocial factors.

METHOD

Participants

The study sample included 79 participants with HIV infection who were recruited from the San Diego community (e.g., via newspaper advertisements) and local HIV treatment clinics. All participants provided written, informed consent prior to enrolling in this study, which was approved by the institution’s human research protections program. To be considered for inclusion, participants must have been prescribed at least one ARV medication. Study exclusions at enrollment included severe psychiatric illness (e.g., schizophrenia), neurological disease (e.g., seizure disorders, stroke, closed head injuries with loss of consciousness greater than 15 minutes, and central nervous system neoplasms or opportunistic infections), estimated verbal IQ scores < 70 (based on the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading [WTAR]; Psychological Corporation, 2001), a recent diagnosis of substance dependence (i.e., within six months of baseline evaluation), and a urine toxicology screen positive for illicit drugs on the day of testing. A positive toxicology screen for marijuana (n = 17) was not a basis for exclusion since its metabolites remain detectable in urine for as long as one month and several drugs commonly used in the management of HIV (e.g., efavirenz, marinol) are known to produce positive toxicology results.

Participants were classified as either Adherent or Non-adherent based on the outcome of a 4-week continuous observation period using the (non-alarmed) Medication Event Monitoring System (MEMS; Aprex Corporation, Union City, CA). The MEMS observation period began on the day following participants’ neuropsychological evaluation (described below). The MEMS cap system uses a medication bottle cap (Trackcap®) microchip device that recorded the time, date, and frequency with which the participant’s opened their medication bottle over the 4-week period. Participants were instructed to use only the MEMS bottle to dispense the target ARV and to remove only one dose at a time. Non-adherence was determined by a blind clinical review of the MEMS protocols and was defined as < 90% adherence to their target ARV on any of the following variables: 1) percent days correct number of doses taken; 2) percent prescribed number of doses taken; and 3) percent prescribed doses taken on schedule. Table 1 displays the demographic, HIV disease, and MEMS ARV adherence characteristics of the Adherent (n = 48) and Non-adherent (n = 31) groups.

Table 1.

Participants’ demographic, disease and medication characteristics

Dependent Variable Adherent (n = 48) Non-adherent (n = 31) p
Demographic Characteristics
 Age (years) 47.1 (9.4) 45.2 (9.7) 0.392
 Education (years) 13.5 (2.8) 12.6 (2.7) 0.186
 Estimated Verbal IQa 106.0 (11.0) 101.0 (12.5) 0.075
 Sex (% men) 79.2 93.6 0.082
 Ethnicity (% Caucasian) 66.7 58.1 0.439
Disease Characteristics
 Estimated duration of infection (years) 15.0 (7.1) 17.2 (6.6) 0.178
 Current CD4 countb 568.0 (449.5, 770.5) 520.0 (267.8, 693.3) 0.153
 Nadir CD4 countb 152.0 (57.0, 269.0) 80.0 (25.0, 180.0) 0.022
 HIV RNA log10b 1.7 (1.7, 1.7) 1.7 (1.7, 1.7) 0.571
 AIDS status 64.6% 87.1% 0.027
 HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders 23.0% 29.0% 0.113
Medication Characteristics
 Total pill burden (no. of pills/day) 9.0 (5.7) 9.8 (5.2) 0.527
  No. of ARVs currently prescribed 3.8 (0.9) 3.7 (1.0) 0.971
  Duration ARV regimen (months) 27.1 (29.4) 29.0 (43.3) 0.834
 Length of MEMS observation (days) 38.3 (10.3) 34.3 (13.4) 0.168
  % Prescribed no. of doses taken 99.2 (2.8) 80.5 (16.0) <0.001
  % Doses taken on schedule 94.0 (7.8) 52.7 (26.1) <0.001
  % Days correct no. of doses taken 95.4 (7.4) 70.8 (17.6) <0.001
a

Note. Verbal IQ (M = 100; SD = 15) was derived from the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR);

b

Median (interquartile range). MEMS = Medication Event Monitoring System.

Materials and Procedure

The day prior to the beginning of their MEMS observation period, all participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological, psychiatric, and medical research evaluation.

Prospective Memory Assessment

The primary measure of interest was the Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST; Raskin, 2004), which is a 30-min, 8-trial test during which participants engage in a word search puzzle as the distractor task. Consistent with prior studies (e.g., Carey et al., 2006), we examined the following MIST variables: 1) summary score; 2) time-based scale; 3) event-based scale; 4) distractor total; 5) recognition total; 6) a retrieval index; and 7) a 24hr delay trial for which examinees were instructed to leave a voicemail message for the examiner the day after the exam indicating the number of hours the participant slept the night after the evaluation (see Carey et al., 2006). In addition, the following error types were coded: 1) no response (i.e., response omission errors); 2) task substitutions (e.g., replacement of a verbal response with an action or vice-versa); 3) loss of content (e.g., acknowledgment that a response is required to a cue, but failure to recall the content); and (4) loss of time (i.e., performance of an intention greater than ± 15% of the target time). Participants also completed the 8-item Prospective Memory Scale from the Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire (PRMQ; Smith et al., 2000), which was used to assess self-reported ProM complaints.

Basic Neuropsychological Assessment

A standardized battery of clinical tests of neuropsychological functioning was also administered to each participant. This battery was designed to be consistent with NIH guidelines for assessing the cognitive domains that are most sensitive to HIV (Antinori et al., 2007; Butters et al., 1990), including retrospective learning and memory, executive functions, information processing speed, attention/working memory, verbal fluency, and motor coordination. Raw scores were converted to population-based z-scores derived from the entire sample, then averaged across the tests in that domain to create a mean domain z-score. The specific tests that comprised each of these domains (and their associated descriptive data) are displayed in Table 3 (Benton et al., 1994; Culbertson & Zillmer, 2001; Delis et al., 2000; Klove, 1963; Morgan et al., in press; Psychological Corporation, 1997; Reitan & Wolfson, 1985; Shimamura et al., 1994; Stern et al., 1999; Woods et al., 2005).

Table 3.

Descriptive data on the standard neuropsychological tests in the Adherent and Non-adherent groups.

Adherent (n = 48) Non-Adherent (n = 31) p Cohen’s d
Retrospective learning z score 0.15 (0.81) −0.24 (0.65) 0.039 −0.52
 CVLT-II Total Trials 1–5 50.42 (12.47) 45.27 (12.23) 0.130 −0.42
 WMS-III Logical Memory I 43.15 (12.70) 35.90 (12.35) 0.011 −0.58
 Rey BQSS Immediate 11.98 (3.75) 11.29 (2.81) 0.240 −0.20
Retrospective memory z score 0.14 (0.85) −0.23 (0.66) 0.035 −0.47
 CVLT-II long delay 10.48 (4.40) 9.20 (4.33) 0.172 −0.29
 WMS-III Logical Memory II 27.17 (9.90) 20.35 (9.11) 0.002 −0.71
 Rey BQSS long delay 11.9 (3.67) 11.42 (2.88) 0.458 −0.14
Executive functions z score 0.00 (0.79) 0.01 (0.93) 0.849 0.01
 Trail Making Test part B 66.35 (32.35) 71.35 (49.30) 0.725 0.13
 ToL total moves 25.72 (20.37) 23.61 (18.42) 0.732 −0.11
Information processing speed z score 0.06 (0.92) −0.08 (0.82) 0.428 −0.16
 Trail Making Test part A 27.50 (9.16) 24.68 (7.31) 0.181 −0.33
 ToL total execution time (sec) 209.51 (93.78) 205.10 (94.19) 0.803 −0.05
Attention z score −0.07 (0.49) 0.10 (0.63) 0.361 0.31
 WMS-III Digit Span 17.83 (3.81) 17.13 (3.97) 0.549 −0.18
 SOPT total errors 3.50 (1.69) 4.55 (2.45) 0.056 0.52
Verbal fluency z score −0.21 (2.30) 0.29 (2.50) 0.407 0.21
 Letter fluency (C) 15.06 (4.64) 15.03 (5.00) 0.944 −0.01
 Animal fluency 20.19 (5.01) 23.07 (5.98) 0.043 0.53
 Action fluency 17.23 (5.52) 16.71 (5.21) 0.744 −0.10
Motor coordination z score 0.04 (0.87) −0.05 (1.00) 0.610 −0.10
 Grooved pegboard dominant 72.65 (20.02) 68.58 (16.42) 0.303 −0.22
 Grooved pegboard non-dominant 77.87 (19.54) 79.39 (31.79) 0.992 0.06

Note. Values reflect raw scores unless indicated. CVLT-II = California Verbal Learning Test (2nd ed.); WMS-III = Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd ed.); BQSS = Boston Qualitative Scoring System; ToL = Tower of London, Drexel version; SOPT = self-ordered pointing test.

Psychiatric Assessment

Structured psychiatric interviews were conducted using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI version 2.1; World Health Organization, 1998), from which lifetime and current (i.e., within 1 month of evaluation) diagnoses of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD), and Substance-Related Disorders were generated per Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria. Participants also completed the Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair et al., 1981) to assess current affective distress across four areas (i.e., Depression/Dejection, Fatigue/Inertia, Vigor/Activity, and Tension/Anxiety) and a Total Mood Disturbance score, for which higher scores indicate greater distress.

Psychosocial and Environmental Factors

Participants were administered the BERMA (McDonald-Miszczak et al., 2004) questionnaire, from which three scales were derived. The 23-item “Dealing with Health Professionals” scale is intended to assess the strength of participants’ relationship with their medical providers (e.g., “I have difficulty talking openly with my physician”). The 20-item “Medication Management Efficacy” Scale was designed to assess general medication management abilities (e.g., “I am less efficient at adhering to my medication regimen than I used to be”). Finally, the 10-item “Attitudes About Medications” scale is comprised of items that measure participants’ general health beliefs (e.g., “I am taking too much medication for my medical conditions”). All subscale items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).

In addition, participants completed the Prospective Memory for Medications Questionnaire (PMMQ; Gould et al., 1997). The PMMQ is a 33-item questionnaire that assesses the frequency with which an individual uses different internal (e.g., “Do you regularly repeat to yourself the instructions for taking a prescription…?”) and external (e.g., “Do you use a clock or watch alarm to remind you when it is time to take your medications?”) medication taking strategies. Participants are asked to rate how often they use each strategy on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“always”), such that higher scores indicate more frequent strategy use.

Data Analyses

The MIST variables were non-normally distributed (i.e., negatively skewed) and therefore a series of Wilcoxon Rank-Sum tests were conducted and complemented by Cohen’s d effect size estimates. Group differences on a priori selected measures of neuropsychological functioning, psychiatric variables, psychosocial and environmental factors, and disease and treatment status were conducted using either Wilcoxon Rank-Sum tests or a chi-square test. We then conducted a planned follow-up binary logistic regression analysis to evaluate the relative independence of ProM as a predictor of non-adherence as compared to the other cognitive (e.g., retrospective memory) factors. As noted above, there are numerous non-cognitive factors that also increase the risk of non-adherence (e.g., demographics, psychiatric disease, substance-related disorders, and psychosocial variables). As such, we also conducted a follow-up binary logistic regression to examine the uniqueness of ProM as a predictor of non-adherence relative to salient non-cognitive variables that differentiated the groups. A critical alpha level of .05 was used for all analyses.

RESULTS

Non-adherent participants performed significantly below the Adherent group on the MIST Summary Score (p < .05). As shown in Table 2, accounting for this overall effect were the Non-adherent group’s lower scores on the time-based scale (p < .05), which were primarily driven by an elevation in Loss of Time (LoT) errors (p < .01). Although significantly correlated with all three indicators of adherence (ps < .05), LoT errors were most strongly associated with the proportion of ARV doses taken on schedule (Spearman’s ρ= −0.29, p = .011). The Adherent and Non-adherent groups did not differ in their performance on the MIST event-based scale, other error types, the distractor test, the recognition post-test, or 24hr delay trial (all ps > .05). Similarly, the Adherent and Non-adherent groups reported similar levels of ProM complaints on the PRMQ (ps > .10). As such, we conducted a series of focused, nonparametric (i.e., descriptive) classification accuracy statistics on the MIST LoT error score. As shown in Figure 1, a normative cutoff score of ≥ 1 LoT error (see Woods et al., in press) afforded adequate overall predictive power, characterized by excellent specificity, but rather poor sensitivity. Of greater clinical value, positive and negative predictive power for LoT were each approximately 70%. In fact, the nonparametric odds ratio (see Bieliauskas et al., 1997) associated with elevated LoT errors was 5.8 (95% confidence interval = 1.9, 17.5).

Table 2.

Prospective memory performance in the Adherent and Non-adherent groups

ProM Variable Adherent (n = 48) Non-adherent (n = 31) p Cohen’s d
Memory for Intentions Screening Test
 Summary score 39.9 (6.7) 35.9 (8.5) 0.047 −0.54
  Time based 6.4 (1.3) 5.6 (1.4) 0.022 −0.60
  Event based 6.9 (1.3) 6.5 (1.6) 0.359 −0.28
 Error Types
  Loss of time 0.1 (0.3) 0.6 (1.1) 0.001 0.69
  No response 0.5 (0.7) 0.8 (1.0) 0.229 0.36
  Task substitutions 0.9 (1.1) 1.0 (1.1) 0.582 0.09
  Loss of content 0.7 (0.9) 0.7 (0.8) 0.704 0.00
 Recognition post test 7.7 (0.6) 7.5 (1.5) 0.817 −0.16
 Retrieval index 1.9 (1.6) 2.8 (1.7) 0.022 0.55
 Word search 16.5 (6.6) 16.9 (6.3) 0.402 0.06
 24 hour delay 1.0 (0.9) 0.7 (0.8) 0.106 −0.35
PRMQ ProM total score 18.9 (5.3) 20.1 (6.1) 0.286 0.21

Note. PRMQ = Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire; ProM = prospective memory.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Descriptive classification accuracy statistics for Loss of Time (LoT) errors on the Memory for Intentions Screening Test as an indicator of Non-adherence. A normative cutpoint of ≥ 1 LoT error was used (see Woods et al., in press) to classify Non-adherent (n = 31) versus Adherent (n = 48) participants

OPP = overall predictive power; NPP = negative predictive power; PPP = positive predictive power; Sens = sensitivity; Spec = specificity

Post-hoc analyses were undertaken to examine the possible correspondence between MIST LoT errors and a measure of time estimation (Mimura et al., 2000). Participants were asked to estimate how much time had elapsed during four brief intervals (i.e., 15, 30, 45, and 90 sec in a randomized order), without the aid of a clock. The discrepancy between the actual time elapsed and the participants’ response did not differ between the Adherent (M = 49.3, SD = 37.2) and Non-adherent (M = 42.3, SD = 23.2) groups (p > .10). LoT errors did not correlate with time estimation in either study group (ps > .10).

Table 3 displays the descriptive data for the two study samples on the basic battery of neuropsychological tests. The Adherent group performed significantly better than the Non-adherent sample on RetM Learning and RetM Memory domain z-scores (ps < .05), which were primarily attributable to group differences on the WMS-III Logical Memory I and II subtests (ps < .05). In a follow-up binary logistic regression that included the RetM Learning and Memory z-scores and MIST LoT errors, (χ2 [3, N = 79] = 15.0, p = .002), only LoT errors emerged as an independent predictor of Non-adherence (p = .002). Findings did not differ if only the significant RetM variables (i.e., WMS-III Logical Memory) or clinical ratings (Woods et al., 2004) were included in the regression instead of the domain z-scores.

Descriptive data regarding the various non-cognitive variables associated with non-adherence are displayed in Table 1 (i.e., demographics and HIV disease and treatment characteristics) and Table 4 (i.e., psychiatric, substance-related, psychosocial, and environmental factors). The Non-adherent sample had significantly lower nadir CD4 counts anda larger proportion of individuals with diagnoses of AIDS (ps < .05), but there was no association between ARV pill burden and adherence (see Table 1). With regard to psychiatric predictors of adherence, Table 4 shows that the Non-adherent participants were significantly more likely to have lifetime diagnoses of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), endorsed higher levels of acute distress on the POMS Tension/Anxiety scale, and reported more difficulties in their general ability to manage their medications (BERMA Medication Management scale (ps .05). A planned follow-up logistic regression analysis that included all of these non-cognitive factors (χ2 [6, N = 77] = 33.9, p < .0001) showed that MIST LoT errors (p < .0001) and the POMS Tension/Anxiety scale (p = .03) were the sole independent predictors of Non-adherence. Additionally, the independence of the MIST LoT errors did not waiver if the various trend-level findings (e.g., sex, WTAR VIQ, BERMA Dealing with health professionals) were included in the statistical model.

Table 4.

Psychiatric and psychosocial characteristics of the study groups.

Adherent (n = 48) Non-Adherent (n = 31) p Cohen’s d
Major Depressive Disorder (%)
 Current 8.33 6.45 0.758 -
 Lifetime 39.58 64.52 0.030 -
Current Generalized Anxiety Disorder (%)
 Current 0.00 0.00 - -
 Lifetime 8.33 6.45 0.758 -
Lifetime substance dependencea(%) 56.25 51.61 0.686 -
Profile of Mood States total 46.96 (32.08) 57.07 (33.51) 0.192 0.31
 Tension/Anxiety 6.90 (4.94) 10.39 (6.90) 0.029 0.60
 Depression/Dejection 8.04 (10.25) 8.35 (9.52) 0.766 0.03
 Vigor/Activity 17.50 (7.08) 15.68 (7.33) 0.198 −0.25
 Fatigue/Inertia 7.46 (6.89) 8.90 (7.44) 0.318 0.20
Beliefs Related to Medication Adherence
 Medication management 82.90 (10.70) 75.67 (14.96) 0.053 −0.58
 Dealing with health professionals 99.90 (12.83) 94.29 (15.94) 0.080 −0.40
 Attitudes about medications 38.03 (6.03) 35.81 (6.76) 0.131 −0.35
PMMQ Total strategy use 26.90 (13.77) 29.65 (17.10) 0.456 0.18

Note. PMMQ = Prospective Memory for Medications Questionnaire.

a

Reflects any prior substance dependence diagnosis.

DISCUSSION

Non-adherence to combination antiretroviral therapies (cART) is highly prevalent and significantly increases the risk of poor HIV disease outcomes, thus underscoring the value of identifying salient cognitive predictors of non-adherence that may inform the development of effective interventions. Results from the current study indicate that HIV-infected individuals with deficits in prospective memory (ProM) are at elevated risk of cART non-adherence as measured by electronic pill monitoring. At a group level, Non-adherent individuals demonstrated significantly poorer ProM functioning as compared to Adherent sample, particularly on an index of time-based ProM. These findings were associated with a medium-to-large effect size and were primarily driven by an elevated rate of Loss of Time (LoT) errors in the Non-adherent participants, meaning that although Non-adherent individuals remembered to perform the prescribed intention, they did so at an incorrect time (i.e., > 15% away from the target execution time). Although slightly less than 50% of Non-adherent persons made one or more LoT errors (sensitivity = 45.2%), the corresponding – and arguably more clinically relevant (Ivnik et al., 2000) – positive (70%) and negative (71%) predictive values of such errors was considerably better. In fact, individuals who committed one or more LoT error were almost six times more likely to be classified as Non-adherent at one-month follow-up (odds ratio = 5.8).

Notably, ProM LoT errors were a unique and independent predictor of non-adherence when considered alongside well-established predictors of adherence. Consistent with prior research, impairment in retrospective learning and memory (e.g., Hinkin et al., 2002), psychiatric comorbidity (e.g., DiIorio et al., in press), HIV disease severity (Nieuwkerk et al., 2001), and psychosocial factors (e.g., Wagner, 2002) were also associated with non-adherence. Nevertheless, ProM LoT errors remained a significant predictor of non-adherence, even when these other factors were included in the statistical model. The independence of ProM as a predictor of non-adherence suggests that this construct may play a unique role in successful medication management, as has also been demonstrated with general instrumental activities of daily living (Woods et al., 2008a) and in other clinical populations (e.g., schizophrenia; Twamley et al., in press). In this way, assessment of ProM may augment the ecological relevance of neuropsychological evaluations of persons infected with HIV.

Time-based ProM, and particularly LoT errors, demonstrated the strongest association with medication non-adherence in this cohort. LoT errors are rare in healthy adults (Carey et al., 2006; Woods et al., in press), but are mildly elevated in individuals with HIV infection (Carey et al., 2006), as well as in schizophrenia (Woods et al., 2007b). Post-hoc analyses revealed that the occurrence of Loss of Time errors was not a function of deficient basic time perception, as Loss of Time errors did not correlate with time estimation (and moreover, the Adherent and Non-adherent groups did not differ in time estimation). An alternate hypothesis is that Loss of Time errors reflect difficulties monitoring time concurrently with an ongoing task. Numerous studies, including those in healthy older adults and individuals with central nervous system disease (for a review, see Mäntylä & Carelli, 2006), show that better performance on time-based ProM measures is associated with more frequent time monitoring. For example, Shum and colleagues (2004) demonstrated that healthy adults engaged in clock monitoring more frequently during a time-based ProM task than individuals with schizophrenia, particularly as the time for execution neared. Moreover, time monitoring was positively correlated with performance on the time-based ProM task (Shum et al., 2004). In this way, the current findings converge with the profile of HIV-associated ProM impairment, which is thought to reflect difficulties in the strategic allocation of cognitive resources to properly manage the simultaneous burden of the cue monitoring (i.e., time) and ongoing foreground activities (e.g., Carey et al., 2006). In more applied terms, HIV-infected individuals with impaired ProM might not notice important time-based cues to take their medications during the course of day-to-day activities, thereby delaying (or missing) scheduled doses, which decreases the likelihood of maintaining adequate virologic control and favorable disease outcomes.

Results from this study extend a surprisingly small, but growing literature supporting the relationship between ProM impairment and medication non-adherence (Hertzog et al., 2000; Vedhara et al., 2004), which includes only one prior investigation in HIV (Woods et al., 2008b). Building on the limitations of the existing ProM and adherence literature, the present study employed a prospective, longitudinal design and utilized an electronic monitoring device to assess medication adherence. Although not without its limitations (see Bova et al., 2005), this methodology provides an objective, behavioral indication of adherence patterns (Paterson et al., 2002) that is more sensitive to non-adherence than self-report (Levine et al., 2006). Indeed, in contrast to our prior study using a generic self-report measure of medication management (Woods et al., 2008b), the current data did not show an association between cART non-adherence and either ProM complaints or the 24hr semi-naturalistic ProM task. Importantly, however, both studies demonstrated that a laboratory-based measure of time-based ProM functioning (i.e., the MIST) was independently predictive of medication management in HIV.

A few methodological limitations should be considered when interpreting findings from this study. Most importantly, little is known about the psychometric properties of LoT errors, which tend to be infrequent, raising concerns about their reliability and possible floor and ceiling effects. Another limitation of this study is the absence of a measure of time production (Barkley et al., 2001) or time monitoring during ProM (e.g., clock checking), both of which would allow for a better characterization of the relationship between time-based ProM and adherence. Considering the hypothesized relationship between time perception and frontal systems (see Meck, 2005 for a review), future studies may wish to examine brief and extended time estimation and production intervals in HIV more generally, as well as in the specific context of cART adherence. The external validity of the study is restricted because the sample was predominately male (85%) and had generally mild HIV disease (median current CD4 count = 559). Although these sample characteristics are fairly representative of the HIV epidemic in the United States, whether they generalize to specific subpopulations (e.g., older women with advanced HIV disease) remains to be determined. In addition, we excluded individuals with active substance dependence, which prior research shows is a strong predictor of non-adherence (e.g., Hinkin et al., 2007). Finally, other important predictors of adherence were not available in this cohort, including such psychosocial factors such as access to healthcare, socioeconomic status, and health literacy.

In summary, findings from this study indicate that HIV-associated impairment in time-based ProM increases the risk of cART non-adherence independent of psychiatric comorbidity, HIV disease severity, general cognitive impairment, demographics, and select psychosocial factors. Together with prior literature on ProM and adherence (Hertzog et al., 2000; Vedhara et al., 2004; Woods et al., 2008b), such findings suggest that interventions that target ProM may be effective in improving adherence. For example, cognitive techniques such as Goal Management Training (Levine et al., 2000), which uses structured exercises designed to teach individuals to engage in an “executive review” of their plans and intentions for the day (e.g., “What am I doing right now?”, “What else do I have to do today and when?”) may be effective in improving ProM, as was recently shown in patients with traumatic brain injury (Fish et al., 2007). Other intervention approaches might focus on reducing the need for strategic monitoring, perhaps by reducing cognitive load (i.e., reducing the number and complexity of intentions held “online”; Woods et al., 2006a) and/or minimizing ongoing distraction (e.g., McDaniel & Einstein, 2007). Relatedly, a non-invasive and relatively inexpensive (cf. caregivers) intervention option might involve a programmable electronic device (e.g., a watch) that prominently notifies the patient when it is time to take a medication with a detailed text message that includes the medication name, dosage, and particular conditions under which it should be taken (e.g., Andrade et al., 2005; Leirer et al., 1991; van den Broek et al., 2000). Prospective, theory-driven controlled trials of the effectiveness of these various strategies (perhaps as well as combined, individualized therapeutic approaches) as treatments for HIV-associated ProM impairment and non-adherence are needed.

Acknowledgments

The HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center (HNRC) Group is affiliated with the University of California, San Diego, the Naval Hospital, San Diego, and the Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System, and includes: Director: Igor Grant, M.D.; Co-Directors: J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D., Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D., and J. Allen McCutchan, M.D.; Center Manager: Thomas D. Marcotte, Ph.D.; Naval Hospital San Diego: Braden R. Hale, M.D., M.P.H. (P.I.); Neuromedical Component: Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D. (P.I.), J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., Scott Letendre, M.D., Edmund Capparelli, Pharm.D., Rachel Schrier, Ph.D.; Neurobehavioral Component: Robert K. Heaton, Ph.D. (P.I.), Mariana Cherner, Ph.D., David J. Moore, Ph.D., Steven Paul Woods, Psy.D.; Neuroimaging Component: Terry Jernigan, Ph.D. (P.I.), Christine Fennema-Notestine, Ph.D., Sarah L., Archibald, M.A., John Hesselink, M.D., Jacopo Annese, Ph.D., Michael J. Taylor, Ph.D.; Neurobiology Component: Eliezer Masliah, M.D. (P.I.), Ian Everall, FRCPsych., FRCPath., Ph.D., T. Dianne Langford, Ph.D.; Neurovirology Component: Douglas Richman, M.D., (P.I.), David M. Smith, M.D.; International Component: J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., (P.I.); Developmental Component: Ian Everall, FRCPsych., FRCPath., Ph.D. (P.I.), Stuart Lipton, M.D., Ph.D.; Clinical Trials Component: J. Allen McCutchan, M.D., J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D., Ronald J. Ellis, M.D., Ph.D., Scott Letendre, M.D.; Participant Accrual and Retention Unit: J. Hampton Atkinson, M.D. (P.I.), Rodney von Jaeger, M.P.H.; Data Management Unit: Anthony C. Gamst, Ph.D. (P.I.), Clint Cushman, B.A., (Data Systems Manager), Daniel R. Masys, M.D. (Senior Consultant); Statistics Unit: Ian Abramson, Ph.D. (P.I.), Christopher Ake, Ph.D., Florin Vaida, Ph.D.

This research was supported by grants MH073419 and MH62512 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of the Navy, Department of Defense, nor the United States Government. No relationships exist, financial or otherwise, that could be interpreted as a conflict of interest affecting this manuscript. No information from this manuscript nor the manuscript itself has ever been published either electronically or in print. The authors thank Dr. Catherine L. Carey and Lisa M. Moran for their substantial contributions to the parent grant, Ofilio Vigil for managing the psychiatric aspects of this study, Nancy Anderson for her assistance with data entry, and Dr. Sarah Raskin for providing us with the MIST.

References

  1. Albert SM, Weber CM, Todak G, Polanco C, Clouse R, McElhiney M, Rabkin J, Stern Y, Marder K. An observed performance test of medication management ability in HIV: Relation to neuropsychological status and medication adherence outcomes. AIDS and Behavior. 1999;3:121–128. [Google Scholar]
  2. Albert SM, Flater SR, Clouse R, Todak G, Stern Y, Marder K. Medication management skill in HIV: I. Evidence for adaptation of medication management strategies in people with cognitive impairment. II. Evidence for a pervasive lay model of medication efficacy. AIDS and Behavior. 2003;7:329–338. doi: 10.1023/a:1025404105378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrade AS, McGruder HF, Wu AW, Celano SA, Skolasky RL, Jr, Selnes OA, Huang IC, McArthur JC. A programmable prompting device improves adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy in HIV-infected subjects with memory impairment. Clinical Infectious Diseases. 2005;41:875–882. doi: 10.1086/432877. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, Brew BJ, Byrd DA, Cherner M, Clifford DB, Cinque P, Epstein LG, Goodkin K, Gisslen M, Grant I, Heaton RK, Joseph J, Marder K, Marra CM, McArthur JC, Nunn M, Price RW, Pulliam L, Robertson KR, Sacktor N, Valcour V, Wojna VE. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007;69:1789–1799. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Avants SK, Margolin A, Warburton LA, Hawkins KA, Shi J. Predictors of nonadherence to HIV-related medication regimens during methadone stabilization. The American Journal on Addictions. 2001;10:69–78. doi: 10.1080/105504901750160501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bangsburg DR. Preventing HIV antiretroviral resistance through better monitoring of treatment adherence. Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2008;197(Suppl 3):272–278. doi: 10.1086/533415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barclay TR, Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Mason KI, Reinhard MJ, Marion SD, Levine AJ, Durvasula RS. Age-associated predictors of medication adherence in HIV-positive adults: health beliefs, self-efficacy, and neurocognitive status. Health Psychology. 2007;26:40–49. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.1.40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barkley RA, Murphy KR, Bush T. Time perception and reproduction in young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2001;15(3):351–360. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.15.3.351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benedict RHB, Mexhir JJ, Walsh K, Hewitt RG. Impact of human immunodeficiency virus Type-1-associated cognitive dysfunction on activities of daily living and quality of life. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2000;15:529–534. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City: AJA Associates; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bieliauskas LA, Fastenau PS, Lacy MA, Roper BL. Use of the odds ratio to translate neuropsychological test scores into real-world outcomes: From statistical significance to clinical significance. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1997;19:889–896. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bova CA, Fennie KP, Knafl GJ, Dieckhaus KD, Watrous E, Willimas AB. Use of electronic monitoring devices to measure antiretroviral adherence: Practical considerations. AIDS and Behavior. 2005;9:103–110. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-1685-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Butters N, Grant I, Haxby J, Judd LL, Martin A, McClelland J, Pequegnat W, Schacter D, Stover E. Assessment of AIDS-related cognitive changes: Recommendations of the NIMH workshop on neuropsychological assessment approaches. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1990;12:963–978. doi: 10.1080/01688639008401035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Carey CL, Woods SP, Rippeth JD, Heaton RK, Grant I The HNRC Group. Prospective memory in HIV-1 infection. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2006;28:536–548. doi: 10.1080/13803390590949494. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV/AIDS Surveillance Report, 2005. Vol. 17. Atlanta: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Center for Disease Control and Prevention; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  17. Chesney MA, Ickovics JR, Chambers DB, Gifford AL, Neidig J, Zwickl B, Wu AW. Self-reported adherence to antiretroviral medications among participants in HIV clinical trials: the AACTG adherence instruments. Patient Care Patient Care Committee & Adherence Working Group of the Outcomes Committee of the Adult AIDS Clinical Trials Group (AACTG) AIDS Care. 2000;12:255–266. doi: 10.1080/09540120050042891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. The Tower of London DX (TOL-DX) manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  19. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. The California Verbal Learning Test. 2. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  20. DiIorio C, McCarty F, Depadilla L, Resnicow K, Holstad MM, Yeager K, Sharma SM, Morisky DE, Lundberg B. Adherence to Antiretroviral Medication Regimens: A Test of a Psychosocial Model. AIDS and Behavior. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9318-4. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fish J, Evans JJ, Nimmo M, Martin E, Kersel D, Bateman A, Wilson BA, Manly T. Rehabilitation of executive dysfunction following brain injury: “Content-free” cueing improves everyday prospective memory performance. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(6):1318–1330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gould ON, McDonald-Miszczak L, King B. Metacognition and medication adherence: How do older adults remember? Experimental Aging Research. 1997;23:315–342. doi: 10.1080/03610739708254034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Harrigan PR, Hogg RS, Dong WW, Yup B, Wynhoven B, Woodward J, Brumme CJ, Brumme ZL, Mo T, Alexander CS, Montaner JS. Predictors of HIV drug-resistance mutations in a large antiretroviral-naive cohort initiating triple antiretroviral therapy. Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2005;191:339–347. doi: 10.1086/427192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, White DA, Kirson D, Atkinson JH, McCutchan JA, Taylor MJ, Kelly MD, Ellis RJ. The HNRC 500: Neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 1995;1:231–251. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700000230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Heaton RK, Marcotte TD, Mindt MR, Sadek J, Moore DJ, Bentley H, McCutchan JA, Reicks C, Grant I The HNRC Group. The impact of HIV-associated neuropsychological impairment on everyday functioning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2004;10:317–331. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hertzog C, Park D, Morell RW, Martin M. Ask and ye shall receive: Behavioural specificity in the accuracy of subjective memory complaints. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2000;14:257–275. [Google Scholar]
  27. Hinkin CH, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, Hardy DJ, Lam MN, Mason KI, Thrasher D, Goetz MB, Stefaniak M. Medication adherence among HIV+ adults: Effects of cognitive dysfunction and regimen complexity. Neurology. 2002;59:1944–1950. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000038347.48137.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hinkin CH, Hardy DJ, Mason KI, Castellon SA, Durvasula RS, Lam MN, Stefaniak M. Medication adherence in HIV-infected adults: Effect of patient age, cognitive status, and substance abuse. AIDS. 2004;18:S19–S25. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200418001-00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hinkin CH, Barclay TR, Castellon SA, Levine AJ, Durvasula RS, Marion SD, Myers HF, Longshore D. Drug use and medication adherence among HIV-1 infected individuals. AIDS and Behavior. 2007;11:185–194. doi: 10.1007/s10461-006-9152-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Ivnik RJ, Smith GE, Petersen RC, Boeve BF, Kokmen E, Tangalos EG. Diagnostic accuracy of four approaches to interpreting neuropsychological test data. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:163–177. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kløve H. Clinical neuropsychology. In: Forster FM, editor. The Medical Clinics of North America. New York: Saunders; 1963. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Leirer VO, Morrow DG, Tanke ED, Pariante GM. Elders’ nonadherence: Its assessment and medication reminding by voice mail. The Gerontologist. 1991;31:514–520. doi: 10.1093/geront/31.4.514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Levine B, Robertson IH, Clare L, Carter G, Hong J, Wilson BA, Duncan J, Stuss DT. Rehabilitation of executive functioning: an experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;6:299–312. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700633052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Levine AJ, Hinkin CH, Marion S, Keuning A, Castellon SA, Lam MM, Robinet M, Longshore D, Newton T, Myers H, Durvasula RS. Adherence to antiretroviral medications in HIV: differences in data collected via self-report and electronic monitoring. Health Psychology. 2006;25:329–335. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lima VD, Geller J, Bangsberg DR, Patterson TL, Daniel M, Kerr T. The effect of adherence on the association between depressive symptoms and mortality among HIV-infected individuals first initiating HAART. AIDS. 2007;21:1175–1183. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32811ebf57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Liu C, Ostrow D, Detels R, Hu Z, Johnson L, Kingsley L, Jacobson LP. Impacts of HIV infection and HAART use on quality of life. Quality of Life Research. 2006;15:941–949. doi: 10.1007/s11136-005-5913-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Mäntylä T, Carelli MG. Time monitoring and executive functioning: Individual and developmental differences. In: Glicksohn J, Myslobodsky MS, editors. Timing the Future: The Case for a Time-Based Prospective memory. River Edge, NJ, US: World Scientific Publishing Co; 2006. pp. 191–211. [Google Scholar]
  38. Martin EM, Nixon H, Pitrak DL, Weddington W, Rains NA, Nunnally G, Grbesic S, Gonzalez R, Jacobus J, Bechara A. Characteristics of prospective memory deficits in HIV-seropositive substance-dependent individuals: Preliminary observations. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2007;29:496–504. doi: 10.1080/13803390600800970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. McDaniel MA, Einstein GO. Prospective memory: An overview and synthesis of an emerging field. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  40. McDonald-Miszczak L, Maris P, Fitzgibbon T, Ritchie G. A pilot study examining older adults’ beliefs related to medication adherence: The BERMA Survey. Journal of Aging and Health. 2004;16:591–614. doi: 10.1177/0898264304265772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Manual for the Profile of Mood States. San Diego: Educational and Industrial Testing Service; 1981. [Google Scholar]
  42. Meck WH. Neuropsychology of timing and time perception. Brain and Cognition. 2005;58:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mimura M, Kinsbourne M, O’Connor M. Time estimation by patients with frontal lesions and by Korsakoff amnesics. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2000;6:517–528. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700655017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Morgan EE, Woods SP, Weber E, Dawson MS, Carey CL, Moran LM, Grant I The HNRC Group. HIV-associated episodic memory impairment: Evidence of a possible differential deficit in source memory for complex visual stimuli. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21.2.189. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nieuwkerk PT, Sprangers MA, Burger DM, Hoetelmans RM, Hugen PW, Danner SA, van Der Ende ME, Schneider MM, Schrey G, Meenhorst PL, Sprenger HG, Kaufmann RH, Jambroes M, Chesney MA, de Wolf F, Lange JM The ATHENA Project. Limited patient adherence to highly active antiretroviral therapy for HIV-1 infection in an observational cohort study. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2001;161:1962–1968. doi: 10.1001/archinte.161.16.1962. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Osterberg L, Blaschke T. Adherence to medication. New England Journal of Medicine. 2005;353:487–497. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra050100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Paterson DL, Potoski B, Capitano B. Measurement of adherence to antiretroviral medications. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome. 2002;15:S103–106. doi: 10.1097/00126334-200212153-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Perno CF, Ceccherini-Silberstein F, De Luca A, Cozzi-Lepri A, Gori C, Cingolani A, Bellocchi MC, Trotta MP, Piano P, Forbici F, Scasso A, Vullo V, d’Arminio Monforte A, Antinori A The AdICoNA Study Group. Virologic correlates of adherence to antiretroviral medications and therapeutic failure. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndrome. 2002;31:S118–S122. doi: 10.1097/00126334-200212153-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Psychological Corporation. WAIS-III and WMS-III technical manual. San Antonio, TX: Author; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  50. Psychological Corporation. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Author; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  51. Raskin S. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. Suppl 1. Vol. 10. 2004. Memory for intentions screening test [abstract] p. 110. [Google Scholar]
  52. Reitan RM, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  53. Robertson KR, Smurzynski M, Parsons TD, Wu K, Bosch RJ, Wu J, McCartur JC, Collier AC, Evans SR, Ellis RJ. The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. AIDS. 2007;21:915–921. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32828e4e27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Shimamura AP, Jurica PJ. Memory interference effects and aging: Findings from a test of frontal lobe function. Neuropsychology. 1994;8:408–412. [Google Scholar]
  55. Shum D, Ungvari GS, Tang WK, Leung JP. Performance of schizophrenia patients on time-, event-, and activity-based prospective memory tasks. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2004;30:693–701. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Simoni JM, Frick PA, Pantalone DW, Turner BJ. Antiretroviral adherence interventions: A review of current literature and ongoing studies. Topics in HIV Medicine. 2003;11:185–198. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Smith G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Maylor EA. Prospective and retrospective memory in normal aging and dementia: a questionnaire study. Memory. 2000;8:311–321. doi: 10.1080/09658210050117735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Stern RA, Javorsky DJ, Singer EA, Singer Harris NG, Somerville JA, Duke LM, Thompson JA, Kaplan E. The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (BQSS): Manual. Lutz, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  59. Twamley EW, Woods SP, Zurhellen CH, Vertinski M, Narvaez JM, Mausbach BT, Patterson TL, Jests DV. Neuropsychological substrates and everyday functioning implications of prospective memory impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.030. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. van den Broek MD, Downes J, Johnson Z, Dayus B, Hilton N. Evaluation of an electronic memory aid in the neuropsychological rehabilitation of prospective memory deficits. Brain Injury. 2000;14:455–462. doi: 10.1080/026990500120556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Vedhara K, Wadsworth E, Norman P, Searle A, Mitchel J, Macrae N, O’Mahony M, Kemple T, Memel D. Habitual prospective memory in elderly patients with Type 2 diabetes: Implications for medication adherence. Psychology, Health & Medicine. 2004;9:17–27. [Google Scholar]
  62. Wagner GJ. Predictors of antiretroviral adherence as measured by self-report, electronic monitoring, and medication diaries. AIDS Patient Care STDs. 2002;16:599–608. doi: 10.1089/108729102761882134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Waldrop-Valverde D, Ownby RL, Wilkie FL, Mack A, Kumar M, Metsch L. Neurocognitive aspects of medication adherence in HIV-positive injecting drug users. AIDS and Behavior. 2006;10:287–297. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-9062-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. World Health Organization. Composite international diagnostic interview. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1998. (CIDI, version 2.1) [Google Scholar]
  65. Woods SP, Carey CL, Moran LM, Dawson MS, Letendre SL, Grant I The HNRC Group. Frequency and predictors of self-reported memory complaints in individuals infected with HIV. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2007a;22:187–195. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Woods SP, Dawson MS, Carey CL, Morgan EE, Scott JC, Grant I The HNRC Group. Increased cognitive load exacerbates HIV-1-associated time-based prospective memory impairment [abstract] Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2006a;12(Suppl 1):148. [Google Scholar]
  67. Woods SP, Iudicello JE, Moran LM, Carey CL, Dawson MS, Grant I The HNRC Group. HIV-associated prospective memory impairment increases risk of dependence in everyday functioning. Neuropsychology. 2008a;22:110–117. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.1.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Woods SP, Moran LM, Carey CL, Dawson MS, Iudicello JE, Gibson S, Grant I, Atkinson JH The HNRC Group. Prospective memory in HIV infection: Is “remembering to remember” a unique predictor of self-reported medication management? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2008b;23:257–270. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Woods SP, Moran LM, Dawson MS, Carey CL, Grant I The HNRC Group. Psychometric characteristics of the Memory for Intentions Screening Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. doi: 10.1080/13854040701595999. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Woods SP, Morgan EE, Marquie-Beck J, Carey CL, Grant I, Letendre SL The HNRC Group. Markers of macrophage activation and axonal injury are associated with prospective memory in HIV-1 disease. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology. 2006b;19:217–221. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000213916.10514.57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Woods SP, Rippeth JD, Frol AB, Levy JK, Ryan E, Soukup VM, Hinkin CH, Lazzaretto D, Cherner M, Marcotte TD, Gelman BB, Morgello S, Singer EJ, Grant I, Heaton RK. Interrater reliability of clinical ratings and neurocognitive diagnoses in HIV. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2004;26:759–778. doi: 10.1080/13803390490509565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Woods SP, Scott JC, Sires DA, Grant I, Heaton RK, Troster AI The HNRC Group. Action (verb) fluency: Test-retest reliability, normative standards, and construct validity. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2005;11:408–415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Woods SP, Twamley EW, Dawson MS, Narvaez JM, Jeste DV. Deficits in cue detection and intention retrieval underlie prospective memory impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2007b;90:344–350. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES