Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Nov 16.
Published in final edited form as: Psychon Bull Rev. 2009 Jun;16(3):491–496. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.3.491

Rapid eye-fixation training without eye tracking

Emmanuel Guzman-Martinez 1,1, Parkson Leung 2,1, Steve Franconeri 3, Marcia Grabowecky 3, Satoru Suzuki 3
PMCID: PMC2777709  NIHMSID: NIHMS143159  PMID: 19451374

Abstract

Maintenance of stable central eye fixation is crucial for a variety of behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging experiments. Naïve observers in these experiments are not typically accustomed to fixating, requiring the use of cumbersome and costly eye-tracking or producing confounds in results. We devised a flicker display that produced an easily detectable visual phenomenon whenever the eyes moved. A few minutes of training using this display dramatically improved the accuracy of eye fixation while observers performed a demanding spatial attention cueing task. The same amount of training using control displays did not produce significant fixation improvements and some observers consistently made eye movements to the peripheral attention cue, contaminating the cueing effect. Our results indicate that (1) eye fixation can be rapidly improved in naïve observers by providing real-time feedback about eye movements, and (2) our simple flicker technique provides an easy and effective method for providing this feedback.

Introduction

In many visual experiments, maintenance of central eye fixation is crucial. Some experiments require that stimuli are presented at specific locations on the retina (e.g., to investigate hemispheric or eccentricity-based differences in visual processing). Other experiments aim to solely manipulate covert visual attention, and need to minimize overt eye movements. Eye movements also cause artifacts in EEG recordings and lead to loss of trials. Eye fixation can be improved by monitoring eye movements with an eye tracker and providing feedback when eye movements occur (e.g., Steinman et al., 1973). Our goal was to devise an easy and effective method that does not require the use of an eye tracker and that rapidly trains naïve observers to maintain central eye fixation, even under challenging conditions.

We took advantage of an eye-movement contingent visual effect. Individual flashes of a rapidly flickering dot, which are too fast to resolve when the eyes are stationary, are seen as a spatially displaced array of flashes during the fast flight of a saccadic eye movement, as each flash is ‘painted’ at a different location on the retina (e.g., Hershberger, 1987). We adapted this phenomenon into a display that generated a clear visual effect whenever observers made even a small eye movement (e.g., saccade, mechanically induced [e.g., by tapping on head or eye] movement of the eye, blink).

Our display consisted of a fine-grained random-dot pattern (with 50% black and 50% white pixels) that rapidly flickered (37.5 Hz, the fastest rate using a typical 75 Hz monitor) in counter-phase (i.e., all black pixels became white and all white pixels became black in alternate frames) (Fig. 1). When the eyes were stationary the display appeared uniformly gray as the black and white pixels were perceptually averaged at each location (except for some graininess perceived due to the visual system's sensitivity to borders defined by regions flickering in opposite phase; e.g., Forte et al., 1999).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

An illustration of the flicker pattern used for fixation training. A random-dot pattern (with 50% black pixels and 50% white pixels, each pixel subtending 0.047° visual angle) was rapidly (37.5 Hz, 13.3 ms per frame) alternated with its contrast-reversed complement (i.e., the black pixels became white and the white pixels black; see the magnified illustrations on the right). When the eyes were stationary, the display appeared uniformly gray. However, a random-dot pattern momentarily popped out whenever the observer made an eye movement, providing real-time visual feedback on involuntary eye movements (see text for details).

As soon as the observer made even a small eye movement, the precise temporal averaging of the black and white pixels was disrupted as individual frames fell at shifted locations on the retina, and the observer saw a clear black-&-white random-dot pattern momentarily popping out from the gray field. Because naïve observers are often unaware of their eye movements and blinks, we thought that this eye-movement-contingent effect might provide a useful form of feedback for training naïve observers to maintain steady central eye fixation, by making them aware of their unintended eye movements in real time.

We determined whether a few minutes of training with this flicker display substantially improved central eye fixation in naïve observers. We measured the effect of this training using a typical spatial attention cueing task (see Funes et al., 2005, for a review), in which a peripheral onset cue was flashed to the left or right of a central fixation marker, and the subsequent target stimulus was presented on the cued or opposite side (Fig. 2). Observers responded as to whether the target (an arrow shape) was pointing up or down, and were instructed to maintain central eye fixation during a fairly long (3200 ms) period from the start of the trial (initiated by the observer's button press) to the offset of the target stimulus. The abrupt onset of the peripheral attention cue, the temptation to look at the cued location due to the high (80%) cue validity, and the long (1500 ms) stimulus-onset-asynchrony (SOA) between the attention cue and the target stimulus, all made it challenging for naïve observers to maintain central eye fixation.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

An illustration of a trial sequence for the attention task. The attention cue was flashed to the left or right of the fixation marker with equal probability. The target (an up or down arrow) was presented at the cued location on 80% of the trials; on the remaining 20% of the trials, the target was presented on the opposite side of the fixation marker. Observers were instructed to maintain central eye fixation from the beginning of the trial through the offset of the target stimulus; eye movements were monitored throughout this 3200 ms period.

During the fixation-training period (lasting a few minutes), observers in the experimental group were informed that whenever they saw a jagged random-dot pattern briefly popping out of the gray field they had made an eye movement, and they were asked to try to reduce eye movements as much as possible — informed-flicker training condition. We also ran two separate control groups of observers. Observers in one control group were asked to practice fixating the central fixation marker while looking at the flicker display (as in the informed-flicker training condition), but were not informed of the fact that the popping out of random-dot patterns was a consequence of their eye movements; they were instead told that the display would occasionally be disrupted due to a computer problem — misinformed-flicker control condition. Observers in the second control group saw only the central fixation marker during training (in the absence of the flicker display), and were asked to practice fixating the fixation marker — no-flicker control condition.

Our goal was to demonstrate that naïve observers could rapidly and substantially improve central eye fixation when they were given eye-movement-contingent visual feedback during the informed-flicker training. The misinformed-flicker condition controlled for the potential effect of being exposed to the flicker stimulus while trying to improve fixation; the no-flicker condition controlled for the effect of simply trying to improve fixation during the training period.

Methods

Observers

Thirty-six Northwestern University students gave informed consent to participate for partial course credit or monetary compensation. Observers were randomly assigned to the informed-flicker training or one of the two control conditions (misinformed-flicker or no-flicker control), with the following constraint. Observers were assigned to these conditions on the basis of their fixation performance in the initial attention task, so that the average level of fixation performance before training was similar across the three conditions (see Fig. 3).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Measures of eye movements during the attention task. (A) The average number of saccades per trial before (white bars) and after (gray bars) undergoing the informed-flicker training (left), the misinformed-flicker control (middle), or the no-flicker control (right). (B) The average amplitude of saccades (in degrees of visual angle) before (white bars) and after (gray bars) undergoing the informed-flicker training (left), the misinformed-flicker control (middle), or the no-flicker control (right). Note that only the informed-flicker training improved fixation performance. The error bars represent ±1 SEM.

Stimuli & Procedure

All stimuli were presented on a 19” CRT monitor driven at 75 Hz, at a viewing distance of 42 cm. The bull's-eye fixation marker was 1.27° in diameter (the diameters of the inner circles were 0.69° and 0.21°).

The Attention task

The stimuli were drawn in black against a white background. The attention cue was a 2.87° by 2.87° square presented 9.76° to the left or right of the fixation marker. The target arrow (1.27° in length and 0.55° in width, pointing up or down) was presented either at the cued location (80% of the time) or at the opposite location (20% of the time). The two cue locations (left and right) and two directions of the target arrow (up and down) were equally probable and randomly intermixed across the 40 trials. Details of the timing of trial events are shown in Figure 2. Observers were instructed to report whether the target arrow was pointing up or down as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the corresponding keys on the computer keyboard. They were also instructed to maintain central eye fixation from the beginning of the trial through the offset of the target stimulus. Eye movements were recorded throughout this 3200 ms trial period with an EyeLink 1000 eye tracker (SR Research, 0.15° resolution).

For each observer, we computed the attention cueing effect by subtracting the mean response time (RT) for the cue-valid trials from the mean RT for the cue-invalid trials (a higher positive value indicating a stronger cueing effect). The errors were very small and slightly higher in the cue-invalid condition (0.018 in the cue-valid condition and 0.028 in the cue-invalid condition), providing no evidence of a speed-accuracy trade-off. The RT outliers were eliminated based on the 3SD criterion for each condition (cue-valid and cue-invalid) for each observer, but the trimming did not affect the overall statistical results.

The fixation training and control tasks

The entire fixation training lasted 150 sec and it was given in 30 intervals (5 sec per interval), so that observers had ample opportunity to take breaks. Each training interval was initiated by the observer's button press. In the informed-flicker training condition, observers viewed a flickered random-dot pattern, with 50% of the pixels black and 50% white; each pixel subtended 0.047°. The random-dot pattern was presented in a circle (17.27° in diameter) with a black bull's-eye fixation marker in the center, and was rapidly flickered at 37.5 Hz in counter-phase (i.e., all black pixels became white and all white pixels became black in alternate frames) (Fig. 1). The flickered random-dot pattern appeared gray when the eyes were stationary due to temporal summation of black and white pixels, but this summation was disrupted when observers moved their eyes, resulting in momentary perception of a jagged random-dot pattern. Observers were informed that every time they saw a random-dot pattern pop-out from the gray field this indicated that they had made an eye movement, and they were instructed to use this feedback to minimize eye movements.

In the misinformed-flicker control condition, observers viewed the identical display. However, these observers were told that the random-dot patterns that popped-out were generated by an error in the computer program, and were instructed to ignore them and to practice maintaining eye fixation on the central bull's-eye marker. In the no-flicker control condition, observers viewed a white central bull's-eye fixation marker presented against a black background, and were instructed to practice maintaining eye fixation on the bull's-eye marker. Data for the attention experiment were collected both before and after the fixation training or one of the control conditions.

Results

Central eye fixation substantially improved following the informed-flicker training. The average number of saccades (made during the 3200 ms period from the beginning of the trial through the offset of the target stimulus) decreased from 2.63 to 1.35, t(11) = 5.13, d = 1.48, p < 0.0005 (the left graph in Fig. 3A). The average amplitude of saccades also substantially reduced from 1.66° to 0.55°, t(11) = 3.65, d = 1.05, p < 0.005 (the left graph in Fig. 3B). No improvement occurred following the misinformed-flicker control (the average number of saccades changed from 2.32 to 2.36, t(11) = 0.23, d = 0.06, n.s., and the average amplitude of saccades changed from 1.78° to 1.58°, t(11) = 1.01, d = 0.29, n.s.), or following the no-flicker control (the average number of saccades changed from 3.21 to 3.08, t(11) = 0.41, d = 0.12, n.s., and the average amplitude of saccades changed from 2.29° to 1.85°, t(11) = 1.38, d = 0.40, n.s.) (the middle and right graphs in Figs. 3A and 3B). Thus, the informed-flicker training substantially improved central eye fixation by significantly reducing both the number and amplitude of saccades. In contrast, simply asking observers to practice central fixation with or without the flicker display had little effect.

Histograms of horizontal eye position (sampled every 8 ms on each trial, and averaged across observers and the left and right cue locations) show that eye movements prior to fixation training were systematically directed towards the attention cue (the upper row in Fig. 4). Note that the proportions (Y-axis) are shown in log scale to highlight eye movements even though observers successfully fixated during large portions of each trial. The patterns of eye movements showing a secondary peak around the cued location remained essentially the same before and after the misinformed-flicker control (Fig. 4B) and the no-flicker control (Fig. 4C), indicating that the control conditions did not reduce eye movements towards the attention cue. In contrast, the informed-flicker training virtually eliminated eye movements towards the cue (Fig. 4A).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Histograms of horizontal eye position before (upper panels) and after (lower panels) the informed-flicker training (A), the misinformed-flicker control (B), or the no-flicker control (C). The proportions (the Y-axis) are shown in log scale to highlight eye movements toward the attention cue, which were relatively infrequent. The horizontal bar along the X-axis represents the attention cue. The data shown have been averaged across observers and the cued location (left and right). Note that observers made eye movements toward the attention cue during intended fixation (upper panels), and these eye movements toward the cue were eliminated after the informed-flicker training (the lower panel in part A).

Eye movements directed towards the attention cue during intended fixation may confound behavioral measurements of spatial attention, for example, by increasing visual acuity for processing the target when it is presented at the cued location. Our behavioral results corroborate this concern (at least for paradigms similar to our spatial attention cueing experiment with a high cue validity and long cue-target SOA). Consistent with the fact that larger eye movements were primarily directed towards the attention cue prior to fixation training (the upper row in Fig. 4), observers who made larger saccades produced stronger cueing effects, as indicated by the significant positive correlation between the average amplitude of saccades and the cueing effect (cue-invalid RT minus cue-valid RT), r = 0.49, t(34) = 3.27, p < 0.003 (Fig. 5A). Importantly, the informed-flicker training substantially reduced the average amplitude of saccades during intended fixation for all observers to within ~1° (the filled circles in Fig. 5B), and eliminated systematic eye movements towards the attention cue (the lower panel in Fig. 4A).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Scatter plots showing the relationship between the average amplitude of saccades (in degrees of visual angle) and the cueing effect (cue-invalid RT minus cue-valid RT), before (A) and after (B) the fixation training or control. Different symbols represent the data from observers who underwent different conditions; the filled circles represent the informed-flicker training, the open triangles represent the misinformed-flicker control, and the open circles represent the no-flicker control. Note that the average saccade amplitudes for all observers who underwent the informed-flicker training (filled circles) reduced to ~1° after the training (see part B).

In summary, a few minutes of training using the informed-flicker condition dramatically improved central eye fixation, substantially reducing both the number (the left graph in Fig. 3A) and amplitude (reducing to only ~0.5° on average; the left graph in Fig. 3B) of undesirable eye movements throughout a relatively long (3200 ms) period of intended central fixation. This training also eliminated systematic eye movements toward a peripheral attention cue (the lower panel in Fig. 4A).

Discussion

People constantly make saccadic eye movements (e.g., Yarbus, 1967) and are often unaware of making them. Our results demonstrate that providing real-time feedback about involuntary eye movements using a flicker display rapidly and dramatically improves people's ability to maintain central eye fixation, presumably through making them aware of sensations related to eye movements. The improvement is impressive because the informed-flicker training produced accurate central eye fixation in each observer (reducing his or her average saccade amplitudes to less than ~1° and eliminating systematic saccades toward the attention cue) in an attention task that was specially designed to make it difficult to maintain central eye fixation — we used an abrupt-onset peripheral cue with a high cue validity and a long SOA between the cue and the target. We recommend that a few minutes of this flicker-based fixation training be added prior to experiments that are sensitive to eye-movement artifacts.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a National Institutes of Health grant R01 EY018197 and National Science Foundation grant BCS 0643191.

Contributor Information

Emmanuel Guzman-Martinez, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois and Universidad Nacional Autonóma de México, Mexico City, Mexico.

Parkson Leung, Northwestern University, Evanston, Illinois.

References

  1. Forte J, Hogben JH, Ross J. Spatial limitations of temporal segmentation. Vision Research. 1999;39:4052–4061. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(99)00124-8. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00124-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Funes MJ, Lupiáñez J, Milliken B. The role of spatial attention and other processes on the magnitude and time course of cueing effects. Cognitive Processing. 2005;6(2):98–116. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0038-7. doi:10.1007/S10339-004-0038-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Hershberger W. Saccadic eye movements and the perception of visual direction. Perception & Psychophysics. 1987;41(1):35–44. doi: 10.3758/bf03208211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Steinman RM, Haddad GM, Skavenski AA, Wyman D. Miniature eye movement. Science. 1973;181:810–819. doi: 10.1126/science.181.4102.810. doi:10.1126/science.181.4102.810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Yarbus AL. Eye Movements and Vision. Plenum; New York: 1967. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES