Abstract
Most research on the links between alcohol use and sexual behavior has used cross-sectional and between-subjects designs. However, a pivotal question is whether sexual behavior is more likely when the same persons drink more heavily than when they do not. A within-person approach was used in the current study to model the links between alcohol use and sex. Participants (51.4% male) were traditionally-aged first-year college students. Multilevel models were conducted for up to 14 days of sexual behavior data for each person with occasions (Level 1, N=2879 days) nested within people (Level 2, N=218 people). Between-persons (Level 2) effects were gender, relationship status, person means of alcohol use (aggregated across days), and alcohol-sex expectancies for sexual affect and sexual drive. Within-person (Level 1) effects were weekend days, the number of drinks of alcohol consumed on a given day, and the interaction between drinks consumed and alcohol-sex expectancies. Independent of average alcohol use, consuming more drinks on a given day was associated with a greater likelihood of oral sex and with experiencing more short-term positive (but not negative) consequences of sex on that day. Significant Alcohol Use × Alcohol-Sex Expectancies interactions were found for oral sex and total sex behaviors, indicating that individuals with more positive expectancies were more likely to have sex after drinking. The negative association between drinking and condom use was at a trend level of significance. Results support the potential for promoting sexual health by focusing on cross-behavior expectancies among late adolescents.
Keywords: alcohol use, sexual behavior, expectancies, college, daily data
Alcohol use is widely understood to be a common part of the collegiate experience (Schulenberg & Maggs, 2002; Straus & Bacon, 1953), based on a strong cultural expectation that drinking is central to the experience of the mythically carefree college years (Maggs, 1997). In addition, despite alcohol’s pharmacological impairment on sexual performance and inconsistent empirical evidence of alcohol’s effect on sexual behaviors (Leigh, 1993), there is a strong cultural belief that drinking may lead to pleasurable sexual experiences (Goldman & Roehrich, 1991; Leigh & Stall, 1993). It could be argued that public health warnings that drinking leads to sex may actually strengthen alcohol-sex expectancies and thus promote both drinking and sex by creating a self-fulfilling prophecy, or an “excuse in a bottle” (p. 75, Coleman & Cater, 2005; Dermen, Cooper, & Agocha, 1998). As the result of an expectancy bias, individuals may mistakenly attribute their sexual behavior to drunkenness, even in the absence of any causal disinhibitory effect of alcohol use (Cooper, 2002). The current work examines associations of alcohol use with sexual behaviors and consequences, and tests whether within-person links are larger for students with stronger expectancies that alcohol enhances their sexual affect and sexual drive (Abbey, McAuslan, Ross, & Zawacki, 1999).
Alcohol Use and Sexual Behavior
In the past two decades, over 600 studies have investigated the association between using alcohol and having sex (Cooper, 2006). The majority of studies have found a positive association (Cooper, 2002, 2006; Leigh & Stall, 1993), although most have used purely cross-sectional, between-subjects, and correlational designs (Cooper, 2006; Cooper & Orcutt, 2000; Leigh & Stall, 1993). Emerging adults commonly consume alcohol before having sex (Cooper, Peirce, & Huselid, 1994): estimates range from ¼ to more than ½ of people at the most recent sexual occasion (Desiderato & Crawford, 1995; MacNair-Semands & Simono, 1996). Hingson et al. (2005) estimated that 8% of U.S. college students aged 18 to 24 (474,000 people) have unprotected sexual intercourse resulting from alcohol use annually. In addition, students tend to overestimate the number of drinks they can consume without experiencing negative consequences such as unwanted sex (Mallett, Lee, Neighbors, Larimer, & Turrisi, 2006). However, a more complete picture of the associations between alcohol use and sexual behaviors within-persons and across occasions is required to understand whether a link between alcohol and sex is prevalent and whether it is perceived as rewarding (i.e., experienced positive sex consequences) or not (i.e., negative sex consequences). The perceived consequences of sex after drinking have particular relevance for the potential reinforcement of alcohol-sex expectancies and the perpetuation of both alcohol use and sexual behaviors.
Alcohol use may lead to sex directly through disinhibition (Kaly, Heesacker, & Frost, 2002; MacDonald, MacDonald, Zanna, & Fong, 2000; Steele & Josephs, 1990). Alcohol myopia theory, for example, states that the acute disinhibitory effects of alcohol reduce ability to process complex information (such as long-term goals), thus allowing immediate and salient goals (such as sexual arousal) to influence behavior more strongly (Cooper, 2002; Dermen & Cooper, 2000; Steele & Josephs, 1990). Conversely, the desire to engage in sexual behavior may lead to heavier drinking because of the belief that alcohol favorably influences sexuality (Abbey et al., 1999; Goldman & Roehrich, 1991). Based on alcohol expectancy theory (Cooper, 2002; Dermen & Cooper, 2000), one of the reasons some college students use alcohol is their expectation that alcohol facilitates sexual drive and sexual affect (Abbey et al., 1999) and decreases sexual inhibitions (Kotchick, Shaffer, Forehand, & Miller, 2001). An analysis of interviews with sexually active women, for example, revealed that about half had used alcohol or drugs instrumentally to make sex more likely to occur (Taylor, Fulop, & Green, 1999).
The Importance of Positive Expectancies
Alcohol expectancies are beliefs about the probability of experiencing positive and negative effects of alcohol (Baer, 2002; Cooper, 1994; Kuntsche, Knibbe, Gmel, & Engels, 2005; Leigh, 1989) and they include anticipated effects of alcohol on sex (Goldman & Roehrich, 1991). Alcohol use and sexual experiences are likely related by some sort of reciprocal or feedback association between expectancies about links between drinking and sex, sexual behaviors, and perceived positive and negative consequences of these events (Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar, 1995). The large body of research regarding alcohol expectancies and the smaller body of research focusing on alcohol-sex expectancies both suggest the importance of positive anticipated consequences as predictors of behavior. Having positive alcohol expectancies, or anticipating rewarding effects as a result of drinking, is consistently associated with higher rates of alcohol use (Baer, 2002; Leigh & Stacy, 2004) and with experiencing positive alcohol consequences among college students (Park & Grant, 2005). Consistent with a positive feedback loop, Patrick and Maggs (2008) found that experiencing more positive consequences from alcohol use in the prior week was associated with increases in plans to drink and in motivations to experience positive alcohol consequences the following week. Although less research has addressed alcohol-sex expectancies and experienced consequences of sexual behavior, it is similarly likely that individual differences in expectancies for the effects of alcohol use on sex (e.g., enhancing sexual feelings and arousal) predict both sexual behaviors and the likelihood of reporting positive and negative consequences as a result of sexual behaviors. Some data suggest that it is important both to have positive expectancies that alcohol leads to having sex and to consume alcohol in order to exhibit riskier sexual behavior on a given occasion (Dermen et al., 1998; Dermen & Cooper, 2000). In other words, alcohol-sex expectancies may be activated by drinking alcohol.
Within-Person Findings
As noted previously, the majority of studies regarding alcohol use and sexual behavior have employed correlational, cross-sectional designs (Cooper, 2006; Cooper & Orcutt, 2000; Leigh & Stall, 1993). However, global predictors measured cross-sectionally provide little information about whether individuals engage in alcohol use and sexual behaviors on the same days or what factors determine whether they use condoms on one occasion and not on another. To elucidate the within-person associations requires models that track behaviors in the same people across multiple occasions, thus providing natural controls for between-persons differences (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).
Cooper’s (2006) comprehensive review of studies linking alcohol use with sexual behavior and related consequences determined that within-person associations were inconclusive. Some within-person studies comparing behavior on drinking versus non-drinking occasions suggested that alcohol use was unrelated to condom use (Leigh, Ames, & Stacy, 2008). Among adults (aged 18 to 60 years old), alcohol did not predict engagement in sexual behaviors that posed risks for transmission of STDs (Leigh, 1993). In a diary study with individuals 15 to 21 years old with substance use disorders, event-level alcohol use did not predict condom use (Bailey, Gao, & Clark, 2006). Finally, in a sample of gay men 18 to 35 years old, daily diary reports showed no association between alcohol use and condom use with anal sex (Gillmore et al., 2002).
However, other repeated measures studies provide evidence that alcohol use may be associated with sexual behavior. For example, among individuals living with HIV/AIDS, alcohol use was associated with increased unprotected sex acts for men and decreased unprotected sex acts for women (Barta et al., 2008). A daily diary study of HIV-negative men who have sex with men found that alcohol use was associated both with having sex and a composite variable indicating sexual risk (Mustanski, 2008), although this study did not control for average alcohol use. Among college students, intoxication on a given day was not predictive of any sex, but did predict unsafe sex that day (Neal & Fromme, 2007). Similarly, among sexually active male college students, alcohol consumption was associated with decreased condom use with casual partners (but not with new partners or regular partners) with some evidence that decreased condom use after drinking was especially likely for individuals with stronger alcohol-sex expectancies (LaBrie et al., 2005).
It is likely that alcohol can have various effects, promoting or inhibiting sexual behavior, depending on characteristics of the people (e.g., expectancies), relationships, and situations (Cooper, 2006). Both drinking and condom use are more common with casual partners, but after controlling for partner type, greater alcohol use (within-person) has been shown to be associated with less condom use (Cooper & Orcutt, 2000). In a study using retrospective reports of the most recent sexual event, drinking was not related to condom use in the full sample (Brown & Vanable, 2007). However, for sexual events involving a non-steady partner, alcohol use was associated with a greater likelihood of unprotected intercourse. Other evidence suggests that individuals may be more likely to have sex with a new or casual partner after drinking (Cooper, 2002; Testa & Collins, 1997).
The Current Study
The current paper focuses on both objective behavioral co-occurrence of alcohol use with sexual behavior and the subjective consequences associated with sex in an effort to describe the covariation. Specifically, analyses address whether the number of drinks consumed on a given day is associated with sexual behaviors (i.e., oral sex, penetrative sex, any sex, number of sexual behaviors, and condom use) and short-term subjective consequences of sex (positive and negative), as well as the ways in which these associations may differ as a function of alcohol-sex expectancies regarding sexual affect and sexual drive. Gender and relationship type were included as between-persons controls. In addition, weekend days were included to control for the greater use of alcohol on weekends (Del Boca et al., 2004). Two specific research questions were addressed.
Are first-year college students more likely to engage in sexual behaviors and to experience short-term consequences of sexual behaviors on days they drink more alcohol?
Are within-person daily associations between alcohol use and sexual behaviors stronger for those with more positive alcohol-sex expectancies?
It was anticipated that all measures of sexual behavior (i.e., oral sex, penetrative sex, any sex, sum of sexual behaviors) would be positively predicted by the number of drinks consumed. It was also expected that the subjective experience of sex would differ, such that sexual occasions occurring after a greater number of drinks may be associated with more positive and negative consequences, compared to sexual occasions occurring after drinking fewer drinks. Based on research previously described, it was hypothesized that within-person associations of drinking with sexual behaviors and consequences would be stronger among individuals who expected alcohol to enhance their sexual affect and sexual drive (Abbey et al., 1999).
Method
Participants
Participants in the present analyses (N=218, 51.4% male) were recruited as part of a study to examine daily alcohol use and sexual behavior among traditionally-aged college students. Eligible participants were first-year, first-time, full-time students at a large state university in the Northeastern U.S., were 18 to 20 years of age, and were U.S. citizens or permanent residents. Individuals from racial and ethnic minorities were over-sampled for participant diversity. Based on self-reports, 27.9% of participants identified as Hispanic/Latino. Among non-Hispanic/Latino students, 27.0% identified as European American, 15.5% as African American, 19.3% as Asian American/Hawaiian/Pacific Islander, and 10.3% as more than one race. In the Spring semester, recruitment letters were sent to 330 students with a pen and $5 in cash enclosed. Email invitations followed, with secure links to the surveys administered via the world wide web. On campus residence was reported by 96.8% of the students. Mean age was 18.85 years (SD=0.38, range 18.12 – 20.74).
Students were invited to complete a baseline web-based survey and then 14 consecutive daily web-based surveys. After the baseline was completed, an invitation with a secure link was sent to participants to sign into the daily online survey. Once a participant started the daily surveys, an email reminder was sent each day. Incentives for participation were the $5 pre-incentive, a $25 baseline survey incentive, and $3 per daily survey with an $8 completion bonus (maximum $80 total for all surveys). Participants provided an electronic signature on an online consent form. The study was approved by The Institutional Review Board and protected by a federal Certificate of Confidentiality. Data collection was confidential, and tracking information was stored in separate and secured files.
Recruitment rate (i.e., percent of invited individuals who provided data) was 70.6% (N=233) for the baseline survey and 68.8% (N=227) for at least one daily survey. Of those who provided any data, the retention rate (i.e., percent who provided enough data to be included in the analyses presented here) was 93.6% (N=218). All 14 days were completed by 74.7% (n=174) and 12 or more days were completed by 88.4% (n=206) of the total sample. Of the possible 3262 days (233 people × 14 days), data on 3004 days (92.1%) were collected and data on 2879 (88.3% of possible) days were used in the present analyses.
Measures
Baseline Survey
Relationship Status was reported by the question, “Which of the following best describes you right now?” coded as 0=not dating anyone right now, 1=casually dating someone, and 2=in a serious and committed relationship/living with my partner. The majority of participants (52.8%) were not dating, 16.5% were casually dating, and 30.7% were in a serious relationship.
Alcohol Expectancies Regarding Sex were assessed by two subscales from Abbey et al.’s (1999) 25-item AESASVQ (Alcohol Expectancies Regarding Sex, Aggression, and Sexual Vulnerability Questionnaire). Scales measured beliefs about Sexual Affect Alcohol Expectancies (6 items; e.g., when drinking alcohol, I say and do romantic things, α=.89) and Sexual Drive Alcohol Expectancies (6 items; e.g., when drinking alcohol, I am likely to initiate sex, α=.96). Responses ranged from 0=not at all to 4=very much. The AESASVQ has demonstrated convergent validity with other measures of alcohol expectancies; discriminant validity by being uncorrelated with measures of social desirability, aggression, and sexuality; and predictive validity with actual alcohol use (Abbey et al., 1999). Sexual Affect and Sexual Drive Alcohol Expectancies were highly correlated in the present sample, r=.73, and therefore entered separately as predictors in analytical models.
Daily Survey (14 Days)
Weekend Days were coded as 1=Thursday, Friday, or Saturday and 0=Sunday, Monday, Tuesday, or Wednesday (similar to Lee, Maggs, & Rankin, 2006).
Alcohol Use was measured by reports of the number of standard drinks consumed in the prior day. The definition of “a drink” provided to respondents was “half an ounce of absolute alcohol, for example: 12 ounce can or glass of beer or cooler, 5 ounce glass of wine, drink containing 1 shot of liquor” (NIAAA, 2003). Respondents were asked, “How many drinks of alcohol did you drink yesterday?” A drop down menu allowed responses from 0 drinks to 25 or more drinks.
Sexual Behavior
Each day, participants were asked whether they had had oral sex (2 questions: “Did you perform oral sex on a partner yesterday?” and “Did a partner perform oral sex on you yesterday?”), vaginal sex (“Did you have vaginal sex yesterday?”), and anal sex (“Did you have anal sex yesterday?”). For each day, sexual behavior was coded in four ways to explain different aspects of behavior: (1) whether oral sex (received or performed) was reported (0=no, 1=yes); (2) whether penetrative sex (vaginal or anal) was reported (0=no, 1=yes); (3) whether any sex was reported (0=none, 1=oral and/or penetrative); and (4) a sum of the four sex behaviors (i.e., performing oral, receiving oral, vaginal, and anal).
Sexual behaviors were assessed with a variety of indicators to reflect a range of physical intimacy and health risk associated with multiple forms of sexual contact. For example, after drinking, individuals may be more likely to engage in behaviors with no risk for pregnancy and lower risk for STDs (i.e., oral sex rather than penetrative sex), or to engage in a wider variety of behaviors due to real or perceived disinhibition (i.e., sum of behaviors). Specifically, differentiating oral sex behaviors is especially important in this young college sample, 38% of whom had never engaged in penetrative sex, because individuals often engage in oral sex before they initiate penetrative sex (Prinstein et al., 2004; Schwartz, 1999). Participating in oral sex, vaginal sex, or anal sex each involve specific cumulative health risks of oral and genital STD contraction (CDC, 2000; Chambers, 2007).
On days participants reported engaging in any of the sexual behaviors, a series of follow-up questions were asked. First, participants reported if they used contraception or disease protection. If yes, they were asked to check all forms of protection that applied. In the present analyses, Condom Use was coded only for days with penetrative sex as 0=no and 1=yes. Second, experienced short-term Positive and Negative Consequences of Sex associated with any sex were assessed. The stem question read, “As a result of your sexual experiences yesterday, did you…” with a 0=no and 1=yes response for 19 consequences. Positive consequences (7 items, α=.67; e.g., feel attractive, feel closer to your partner) and negative consequences (12 items, α=.73; e.g., feel like things moved too fast, worried about pregnancy) were summed separately. Finally, on days with any sexual behavior, participants were asked about Drinking Prior to Sex with the question, “Did you or your partner consume alcohol before or during this sexual experience?” Responses were coded based on the participants’ drinking, 0=neither of us did or just my partner did and 1=we both did or just I did. This measure was used to ensure that on days when participants reported both drinking and sex, the alcohol use occurred before or during the sexual encounter. Therefore, the six days (out of 43 days on which both drinking and sex occurred) on which a participant reported both consuming alcohol and having sex but did not report drinking prior to or during sex were excluded from the analyses (yielding drinking before sex days, n=37).
Plan of Analysis
The extent to which there were daily associations between alcohol use and sexual behaviors and the extent to which these varied as a function of between-persons differences in alcohol-sex expectancies were the focus of the current work. Multilevel models (MLMs) (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Singer & Willett, 2003; Snijders & Bosker, 1999) were used to model up to 14 days of data for each person using HLM 6.04 software (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). These MLMs modeled days (Level 1) nested within people (Level 2) to test for both within-person and between-persons associations (example equations shown below). Seven daily dependent variables were modeled: oral sex, penetrative sex, any sex, the sum of sexual behaviors, condom use with penetrative sex, and short-term positive and negative consequences of sexual behavior. Between-persons (Level 2) effects were gender, relationship status, person means of alcohol (aggregated across days), and alcohol-sex expectancies (sexual affect and sexual drive, tested separately). Person means of alcohol use across days controlled for individual differences in the average levels of individuals’ drinking to isolate the effects of a given day’s drinking within individuals. Within-person (Level 1) effects were whether it was a weekend day, the (person-centered) number of drinks of alcohol consumed that day, and the interaction between number of drinks and alcohol-sex expectancies. The following equations describe the within-person (Level 1) and between-persons (Level 2) portions of the MLM:
Level 1 |
Level 2 |
As described in the Level 1 equation, sexual behavior for a given person on a given day was modeled as a function of an intercept (β0i), a slope indicating the incremental change in sexual behavior on weekend days (β1i), a slope indicating the incremental change in sexual behaviors with every additional drink consumed (β2i), and a within-person error term (rit). At Level 2, the model includes the sexual behavior intercept (γ00); the effect of being male (γ01); the effect of being in a more committed relationship (γ02); the effect of between-persons differences in average alcohol use across the measured days (γ03); the effect of between-persons differences in alcohol-sex expectancies (γ04); and between-persons residuals (u0i). The slope indicating the incremental change in sexual behavior on weekend days (γ10) is also included. Finally, the within-person alcohol-sexual behavior slopes (β2i) were modeled as the average association of daily alcohol use and sexual behavior across all individuals in the sample (γ20) plus the effect of between-persons differences in alcohol-sex expectancies on this association (γ21; i.e., the Alcohol Use × Alcohol-Sex Expectancies interaction). In addition, the potential for gender to moderate the association of daily drinking with the seven outcomes was tested for all models, but there was no evidence for gender moderation so these effects were not included in the final models shown.
The equations described above are linear. However, the varied distributional properties of the seven daily dependent variables necessitated the use of three types of MLMs. A logistic Bernoulli distribution was used for the dichotomous outcomes of oral sex, penetrative sex, any sex, and condom use (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), a Poisson distribution was used for the sum of sexual behaviors which was skewed with a large number of zeroes (Snijders & Bosker, 1999), and a linear model was used for the positive and negative sexual consequences outcomes which approximated a normal distribution. Results with trend level significance (p < .10) were reported and interpreted with caution, due to modest sample size and the relatively rare occurrence of the behaviors of interest (e.g., penetrative sex was reported on only 4.6% of days) and resulting low power to detect effects.
Results
Daily Behaviors
Data were available from 218 people on 2879 days. Table 1 shows the number of people who reported at least one occasion of each behavior, and the number of days that each behavior was reported in this sample. The majority of days (81.2%) were both non-drinking and non-sex days. Participants reported consuming five or more drinks on 236 days (59.0% of drinking days). In total, 87.6% of drinking days and 53.3% of sexual behavior days were on a weekend. Of days when participants reported both drinking and having sex, 94.9% were weekend days. Consistent with previous research (Desiderato & Crawford, 1995; MacNair-Semands & Simono, 1996), 40% of the students who had sex in the 14 days used alcohol prior to sex at least once. Alcohol use preceded sex on a total of 21% of days on which sex occurred. Table 2 reports descriptive statistics for all measured variables.
Table 1.
Drinking | Oral Sex | Penetrative Sex | Any Sex | No Condom† | Drinking and Sex‡ | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Number of People (%) | 132 (60.6) | 58 (26.6) | 60 (27.5) | 72 (33.0) | 33 (55.0) | 29 (40.3) |
Number of Days (%) | 400 (13.9) | 120 (4.2) | 133 (4.6) | 177 (6.1) | 65 (48.9) | 37 (20.9) |
Note. Total people N = 218, Total days N = 2879.
Reported for penetrative sex days only; denominators for percentages are 60 people and 133 days.
Reported for any sex days only; denominators for percentages are 72 people and 177 days.
Table 2.
M | SD | Actual Range | |
---|---|---|---|
Within-person Constructs (Level 1), Measured Daily | |||
Number of Drinks (All Days) | 0.85 | 2.59 | 0–20 |
Number of Drinks (Drink Days) | 6.15 | 4.00 | 1–20 |
Oral Sex | 0.04 | 0.20 | 0–1 |
Penetrative Sex | 0.05 | 0.21 | 0–1 |
Any Sex | 0.06 | 0.24 | 0–1 |
Number of Sex Behaviors | 0.10 | 0.43 | 0–3 |
No Condom Use† | 0.49 | 0.50 | 0–1 |
Positive Sex Consequences‡ | 4.14 | 1.48 | 0–7 |
Negative Sex Consequences‡ | 0.67 | 1.21 | 0–6 |
Between-persons Constructs (Level 2) | |||
Male Gender | 0.51 | 0.50 | 0–1 |
Mean Drinks Per Day | 0.89 | 1.23 | 0–6 |
Relationship Status | 0.78 | 0.89 | 0–2 |
Sexual Affect Expectancies | 1.68 | 1.01 | 0–4 |
Sexual Drive Expectancies | 1.47 | 1.20 | 0–4 |
Alcohol Use Predicting Sexual Behaviors Across Days
Oral sex and penetrative sex
Logistic MLMs were computed predicting the likelihood of engaging in oral sex and penetrative sex on a given day. Results with sexual affect alcohol expectancies are shown in Table 3. Similar models were tested substituting sexual drive alcohol expectancies for sexual affect alcohol expectancies and the results were similar, except as noted in the text.
Table 3.
Oral Sex OR [CI] |
Penetrative Sex OR [CI] |
Condom Use with Penetrative Sex‡ OR [CI] |
|
---|---|---|---|
Average Outcome over 14 Days, β0 | |||
Intercept, γ00 | 0.002 [0.001, 0.003]*** | 0.002 [0.001, 0.004]*** | 12.433 [0.465, 332.746] |
Male Gender, γ01 | 1.484 [0.734, 3.004] | 1.240 [0.615, 2.499] | 3.756 [0.432, 32.634] |
Relationship Status, γ02 | 3.744 [2.554, 5.489]*** | 3.590 [2.472, 5.215]** | 0.136 [0.029, 0.628]* |
Person Mean Drinks, γ03 | 0.867 [0.621, 1.211] | 1.059 [0.728, 1.542] | 0.918 [0.338, 2.492] |
Sexual Affect Expectancies, γ04 | 1.273 [0.899, 1.802] | 1.435 [0.983, 2.093]† | 0.442 [0.153, 1.275] |
Average Effect of Weekend, β1 | |||
Intercept, γ10 | 1.952 [1.173, 3.249]* | 2.573 [1.555, 4.258]*** | 2.300 [0.279, 18.925] |
Average Fluctuations in Daily Alcohol Use, β2 | |||
Intercept, γ20 | 1.117 [1.012, 1.233]* | 0.979 [0.888, 1.080] | 0.636 [0.371, 1.089]† |
Sexual Affect Expectancies, γ21 | 1.106 [1.007, 1.214]* | 1.078 [0.953, 1.219] | 1.150 [0.799, 1.651] |
Note.
p < .10,
p < .05,
p <. 01,
Between-persons, the likelihood of engaging in oral and penetrative sex on average across the 14 days did not differ between men and women (γ01) or as a function of person mean drinks (γ03). On average, participants in more committed relationships (γ02) were more likely to report oral sex and penetrative sex. Stronger sexual affect alcohol expectancies (γ04) were marginally significantly associated with a greater likelihood of penetrative sex only. Results with sexual drive expectancies (not tabled) showed that, on average, stronger sexual drive alcohol expectancies were associated with a greater likelihood of reporting oral sex (Odds Ratio [OR]=1.43, Confidence Interval=[1.09, 1.87], p < .01) and penetrative sex (OR=1.74 [1.32, 2.30], p < .001).
Examining the within-person associations, participants had a higher likelihood of having oral and penetrative sex on weekend days (γ10). Addressing Research Question 1, the number of drinks consumed (γ20) predicted a greater likelihood of oral sex. Specifically, for every additional drink, the odds of having oral sex on that day were 12% greater. Number of drinks consumed did not significantly predict the likelihood of penetrative sex. Examining Research Question 2, the between-persons differences in the association of alcohol use with oral and penetrative sex (γ21), there was a significant Alcohol Use × Sexual Affect Alcohol Expectancies interaction and a trend level Alcohol Use × Sexual Drive Alcohol Expectancies interaction (OR=1.07 [0.99, 1.16], p=.094). The shape of these interactions suggested that for people with stronger alcohol-sex expectancies, consuming more drinks was associated with a greater likelihood of engaging in oral sex. For people with less positive alcohol-sex expectancies, there was a weaker positive association between daily alcohol use and oral sex. Neither the Alcohol Use × Sexual Affect nor the Alcohol Use × Sexual Drive Alcohol Expectancies interactions were significant for penetrative sex.
Any sex
Models with the binomial outcome of any sex were also conducted (not tabled). Between-persons, the likelihood of engaging in any sex on average across days did not differ by gender (γ01) or by person mean drinks (γ03). On average, participants in more committed relationships (γ02) were more likely to report any sex (OR=3.07 [2.29, 4.11], p<.001). Stronger sexual affect alcohol expectancies (γ04) were marginally significantly associated with a greater likelihood of any sex (OR=1.30 [0.98, 1.74], p=.073). In a separate model, stronger sexual drive alcohol expectancies were associated with a greater likelihood of reporting any sex (OR=1.49 [1.22, 1.82], p<.001).
Examining the within-person associations, participants had a greater likelihood of having any sex on weekend days (γ10; OR=1.61 [1.08, 2.40], p=.021). Addressing Research Question 1, the number of drinks consumed (γ20) did not significantly predict a greater likelihood of any sex in models with sexual affect (OR=1.07 [0.98, 1.16], p=.129) or sexual drive (OR=1.07 [0.96, 1.17], p=.104). Examining Research Question 2 (γ21), there were no significant Alcohol Use × Alcohol Expectancies interactions for any sex.
Condom use
On average, participants in more committed relationships were less likely to report using condoms with penetrative sex (γ02). No other between-persons variables (i.e., gender [γ01], person mean drinks [γ03], alcohol-sex expectancies [γ04]) uniquely predicted average levels of condom use across the 14 days, nor did the likelihood of condom use with penetrative sex differ within-person by whether the sex occurred on a weekday or weekend (γ10). However, a greater number of drinks consumed on a given day (γ20) predicted lower odds of condom use at a trend level of significance (Research Question 1). The interactions of Alcohol Use × Sexual Affect (or Sexual Drive) Alcohol Expectancies (γ21) were not significantly associated with condom use (Research Question 2).
Daily sum of sexual behaviors
Results for the sum of sexual behaviors outcome variable using MLM with a Poisson distribution are shown in Table 4. Between-persons, gender (γ01) and person mean drinks across the 14 days (γ03) were not predictive of the number of sexual behaviors reported. On average, individuals in more committed relationships (γ02) reported a greater number of sexual behaviors. In addition, people with more positive sexual affect alcohol expectancies (γ04) and sexual drive alcohol expectancies (not tabled, Coefficient=1.54, Confidence Interval=[1.23, 1.93], p<.001) reported more sexual behaviors.
Table 4.
Sexual Behavior Sum | ||
---|---|---|
Coefficient (SE) | Event Rate Ratio | |
Average Outcome over 14 Days, β0 | ||
Intercept, γ00 | −5.472 (0.323)*** | 0.004 |
Male Gender, γ01 | 0.402 (0.303) | 1.494 |
Relationship Status, γ02 | 1.310(0.167)*** | 3.706 |
Person Mean Drinks, γ03 | −0.002 (0.155) | 0.998 |
Sexual Affect Expectancies, γ04 | 0.330 (0.157)* | 1.391 |
Average Effect of Weekend, β1 | ||
Intercept, γ10 | 0.680 (0.188)** | 1.974 |
Average Fluctuations in Daily Alcohol Use, β2 | ||
Intercept, γ20 | 0.035 (0.037) | 1.036 |
Sexual Affect Expectancies, γ21 | 0.101 (0.043)* | 1.107 |
Within-person, on weekend days (γ10) participants reported engaging in a greater number of sexual behaviors. On days individuals consumed a greater number of drinks (γ20), they reported no difference in the number of sexual behaviors, providing a null result for Research Question 1. However, there was an Alcohol Use × Sexual Affect Alcohol Expectancies interaction (γ21) supporting hypotheses regarding Research Question 2. In other words, the effect of alcohol use on the sexual behaviors sum was contingent on sexual affect alcohol expectancies. That is, number of drinks only predicted a greater number of sexual behaviors among individuals who reported stronger alcohol-sex expectancies. The same interaction with sexual drive alcohol expectancies was not significant.
Positive and negative sex consequences
Variations in short-term positive and negative consequences of sex across days with oral or penetrative sex were modeled using linear MLM equations. Results with sexual drive alcohol expectancies are shown in Table 5; results with sexual affect expectancies are not tabled but differences obtained in analyses are noted in the text. Between-persons variables including gender (γ01), relationship status (γ02), and person mean drinks (γ03) did not uniquely predict consequences. On average, sexual drive alcohol expectancies (γ04) were associated with more positive (but not more negative) sexual consequences across days. Sexual affect alcohol expectancies (not tabled) were not associated with consequences of sex across days.
Table 5.
Positive Consequences | Negative Consequences | |
---|---|---|
Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) | |
Average Outcome over 14 Days, β0 | ||
Intercept, γ00 | 4.247 (0.468)*** | 1.554 (0.544)** |
Male Gender, γ01 | 0.107 (0.306) | −0.120 (0.319) |
Relationship Status, γ02 | −0.110 (0.077) | −0.382 (0.253) |
Person Mean Drinks, γ03 | 0.077 (0.139) | −0.110 (0.107) |
Sexual Drive Expectancies, γ04 | 0.273 (0.126)* | −0.134 (0.253) |
Average Effect of Weekend, β1 | ||
Intercept, γ10 | −0.089 (0.254) | −0.143 (.168) |
Average Fluctuations in Daily Alcohol Use, β2 | ||
Intercept, γ20 | 0.171 (0.077)* | −0.105 (0.075) |
Sexual Drive Expectancies, γ21 | −0.125 (0.079) | 0.134 (0.077)† |
Within-person, reported sex consequences did not differ between weekdays and weekends (γ10). Number of drinks (γ20) was significantly associated with number of positive (but not negative) consequences of sex in the models with sexual drive alcohol expectancies (Research Question 1). There was an interaction of Alcohol Use × Sexual Drive Alcohol Expectancies (γ21) at a trend level of significance predicting negative sex consequences (Research Question 2). The shape of the interaction suggested that for people with stronger sexual drive alcohol expectancies, consuming more drinks was associated with experiencing more negative consequences. For people with weaker sexual drive alcohol expectancies, consuming more drinks was associated with experiencing fewer negative consequences. In models with sexual affect expectancies, number of drinks and the Alcohol Use × Sexual Affect Alcohol Expectancies interactions were both non-significant.
Gender interactions
The potential moderating role of gender was tested in a series of additional models by adding a Gender × Alcohol Use (γ22) coefficient to all models shown. However, there was no evidence that the association between alcohol use and sexual behavior or consequences on a given day differed for men and women in these analyses.
Discussion
After controlling for individual differences in alcohol use and for whether the day was on a weekend, within-person variation in the number of drinks consumed was positively associated with variation in oral sex. This provided only partial support for the hypothesis regarding the first research question, that drinking and sex would be associated on a day-to-day basis. No significant main effect of within-person variation in drinking was found for penetrative sex, any, sex, or the sum of sexual behaviors; a trend level of significance was found indicating a lower likelihood of condom use after drinking. The second research question pertained to the potential moderating role of alcohol-sex expectancies. Hypotheses were partially supported, in that individuals with stronger alcohol-sex expectancies were more likely to engage in oral sex and to report a greater total number of sexual behaviors after drinking, compared to individuals with weaker alcohol-sex expectancies. This moderation effect was not significant for penetrative sex, any sex, or condom use.
Perhaps oral sex is a behavior that is more likely to result from the disinhibitory effects of alcohol, while penetrative sex is more often determined by other factors including the strength of the interpersonal relationship. Developmentally, oral sex behaviors are often engaged in prior to individuals’ first penetrative sexual encounter (Prinstein et al., 2004; Schwartz, 1999), and suggesting that it may require less commitment in a relationship. Although we cannot address these issues with the present data, we also speculate that the meanings and norms surrounding oral sex versus penetrative sex are likely to differ, due to some combination of the differences in risk for pregnancy and for STDs, perceptions that oral sex is less intimate (Chambers, 2007), and ambiguity regarding whether or not it is “sex” (Saunders & Reinisch, 1999).
Findings related to consequences of sexual behavior were also mixed. Alcohol-sex expectancies had a direct association with perceived positive (but not negative) consequences of sex. In addition, on heavier drinking days, participants reported experiencing more short-term positive (but not negative) consequences of sex. When students perceive more positive sex consequences on days they consume more drinks, compared to days they consume fewer drinks, expectancies about alcohol’s facilitative effects on sex may be reinforced. There is likely a reciprocal association between alcohol use, positive sex consequences, and alcohol-sex expectancies that perpetuates both alcohol use and sexual behaviors (Cooper, Frone, Russell, & Mudar, 1995). For negative consequences, an interaction at a trend level of significance suggested that individuals with stronger sexual drive expectancies were more likely to experience negative consequences of sex after drinking than those individuals with weaker expectancies. This was consistent with the hypothesized link between drinking and negative consequences for persons with stronger expectancies, but awaits replication in an independent and larger sample.
Gender was not a significant main effect predictor of sexual behaviors or a moderator of the effect of consuming a greater number of drinks on a given day in this study of first-year college students. Prior research is not consistent in predicting a gender difference in the prevalence of sexual behaviors in this age range, and findings of higher prevalence among men have been questioned (e.g., Lefkowitz & Gillen, 2005). The assessment of recent (i.e., daily) rather than long-term retrospective (e.g., lifetime) of sexual behaviors is less impacted by cognitive aggregation biases in reported sexual behaviors that result in gender differences across longer but not shorter reporting periods (Brown & Sinclair, 1999). In addition, this null result may be the result of insufficient variation to test for significant gender differences in within-person associations, given the relatively low frequency of sexual behaviors. However, future research should utilize research designs with greater power to investigate the whether alcohol use may differentially influence sexual behaviors of male and female college students, given evidence of gender differences in the association of alcohol use on sexual behaviors among people living with HIV/AIDS (e.g., Barta et al., 2008).
Use of alcohol and other substances is commonly understood as a contributor to risky sexual behavior, defined as behavior that increases the probability of negative outcomes such as unwanted pregnancy or STDs (e.g., Hingson et al. 2005). It is important to note, however, that although drinking did predict oral sex on a given day, on the majority of days on which sex was reported there was no alcohol use reported, and vice versa. In fact, on four-fifths of all days, participants reported neither drinking nor sexual behaviors. On the other hand, although students abstained from drinking and from sex on the great majority of days, behavioral risk across days was arguably high. That is, on days that college students reported drinking, they consumed more than 5 drinks over half of the time, and on days they had penetrative sex they did not use condoms half of the time.
Implications
This study illustrated that alcohol use and sexual behavior were related particularly among people who expected that alcohol facilitated sexual affect and sexual drive. Independent of individual differences in relationship status and average drinking level, holding stronger beliefs that alcohol enhances sexual affect or sexual drive predicted a greater likelihood of sexual behaviors (except condom use) and positive consequences of sex. This supports alcohol expectancy theory, which states that drinking may lead to sexual risk-taking because of the self-fulfilling prophecy that alcohol is believed to lead to sexual behavior (Cooper, 2002; Dermen & Cooper, 2000; Goldman & Roehrich, 1991). The central role of expectancies suggests that expectancy challenge approaches (Darkes & Goldman, 1993) focusing on alcohol-sex expectancies may be particularly appropriate. Furthermore, researchers and practitioners should be cautious about public health messages implying that alcohol use has a causal effect on sexual behavior, because strong statements that lead people to believe that alcohol is universally associated with sexual behavior may have an iatrogenic effect by reinforcing alcohol-sex expectancies and thereby increasing health risk behaviors.
Strengths and Limitations
The current study addressed an important gap in the research literature. Despite the hundreds of research studies focusing on the behaviors of alcohol use and sexual behavior (Cooper, 2006), less empirical data has linked the behaviors on a day-to-day basis. Strengths of the present study include the within-person analysis of daily data on alcohol use and sexual behavior across 14 consecutive days and controlling for weekend fluctuations and individual differences in behavior. The use of daily person-centered number of drinks as the predictor provides an appropriately conservative test that isolates the within-person association between alcohol use and sexual behavior, because it requires that individuals drink more or less than usual (i.e., differ from their own mean number of drinks) to find a statistical relationship. Level of relationship commitment was also considered and was a consistent predictor of sexual behavior.
Limitations include a sample of first-year college students in a single academic institution and data from a two-week period that may or may not generalize to other times in the academic and calendar years (e.g., Del Boca et al., 2004). The time period contains limited variation, particularly for sexual behaviors, and therefore reduced power to demonstrate associations among variables. Therefore, null results are less conclusive than would be the case in a larger sample or with a longer time interval. Another limitation is that the perceived desirability of alcohol’s effects on sexual affect and sexual drive was not measured. Some individuals may view an enhancing effect of alcohol on sexual drive as highly rewarding, whereas others may see this as a reason to avoid drinking. The extent to which sex after drinking was a desired goal (e.g., drank alcohol expressly to affiliate with romantic partners) or a mistake (e.g., judgment was impaired) was not assessed.
Future Directions
According to alcohol myopia theory, if the strongest and most salient cues encourage sex (e.g., arousal) then an individual may be more likely to have risky sex. On the other hand, when the environmental cues highlight behavioral risks (e.g., “AIDS Kills” stamped on bar patrons hands), intoxicated individuals have been found to be more likely to have safe sex intentions than sober individuals (MacDonald, Fong, Zanna, & Martineau, 2000; Steele & Josephs, 1990). As a result, future studies with innovative measures of environmental cues supporting or inhibiting sex will be especially helpful in elucidating how drinking affects sexual behaviors. This includes a better understanding of where and with whom individuals are drinking. In particular, drinking venues (e.g., bars, clubs) also tend to be places to interact with potential partners (Abbey, Saenz, & Buck, 2005; Cooper, 2002; Dermen & Cooper, 2000; Leigh, 1993) and may be a third variable explanation for associations between drinking and sex.
Future research should incorporate additional behaviors (e.g., kissing), distinguish alcohol’s effects on oral sex versus penetrative sex, and explore potential similarities and differences between homosexual and heterosexual behaviors in more diverse samples to more fully describe how alcohol use is associated with behaviors that have important personal, relational, developmental, and public health significance (Gulledge, Stahmann, & Wilson, 2004; Welsh, Haugen, Widman, Darling, & Grello, 2005). In addition, a wider variety of expectancies should be investigated, including expectancies for one’s ability to use condoms when intoxicated (Gebhardt, Kuyper, & Gruensven, 2003). Finally, the ways in which behavioral links between drinking and sex may change over time developmentally and within and across specific relationships are areas for future investigation.
Understanding the processes linking alcohol use and sexual behavior is necessary to inform more differentiated and effective interventions to reduce health risks and promote relational and sexual health. For instance, the expanding market for brief, motivation-based approaches to intervention demands specific knowledge of the role of expectancies in producing behavior and behavioral consequences (Burke, Arkowitz, & Menchola, 2003; Tevyaw & Monti, 2004). As expectancies for drinking and sex are better understood, it will become clearer whether these behaviors could be targeted together in motivation-based interventions designed to promote public health by reducing the harm associated with alcohol use (e.g., alcohol poisoning) and sexual behavior (e.g., STDs).
Acknowledgments
This work was conducted by Megan E. Patrick and Jennifer L. Maggs, Department of Human Development and Family Studies, The Pennsylvania State University. Megan E. Patrick’s current affiliation is the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan.
This research was supported by grants from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism to J. Maggs (R01 AA016016) and to M. Patrick (F31 AA017014).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/journals/adb.
References
- Abbey A, McAuslan P, Ross LT, Zawacki T. Alcohol expectancies regarding sex, aggression, and sexual vulnerability: Reliability and validity assessment. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1999;13:174–182. [Google Scholar]
- Abbey A, Saenz C, Buck P. The cumulative effects of acute alcohol consumption, individual differences and situational perceptions on sexual decision making. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2005;66:82–90. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armeli S, Mohr C, Todd M, Maltby N, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G. Daily evaluation of anticipated outcomes from alcohol use among college students. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2005;24:767–792. [Google Scholar]
- Baer JS. Student factors: Understanding individual variation in college drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:40–53. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bailey SL, Gao W, Clark DB. Diary study of substance use and unsafe sex among adolescents with substance use disorders. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;38(297):e213–e297. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.12.001. e220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barta WD, Portnoy DB, Kiene SM, Tennen H, Abu-Hasaballah KS, Ferrer R. A daily process investigation of alcohol-involved sexual risk behavior among economically disadvanataged problem drinkers living with HIV/AIDS. AIDS and Behavior. 2008;12:729–740. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9342-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolger N, Davis A, Rafaeli E. Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology. 2003;54:579–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown JL, Vanable PA. Alcohol use, partner type, and risky sexual behavior among college students: Findings from an event-level study. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32:2940–2952. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke AL, Arkowitz J, Menchola M. The efficacy of motivational interviewing: A meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2003;71:843–861. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.5.843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coleman LM, Cater SM. A qualitative study of the relatinoship between alcohol consumption and risky sex in adolescents. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2005;34:649–661. doi: 10.1007/s10508-005-7917-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychological Assessment. 1994;6:117–128. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML. Alcohol use and risky sexual behavior among college students and youth: Evaluating the evidence. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. (Supplement No. 14) 2002:101–117. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML. Does drinking promote risky sexual behavior?: A complex answer to a simple question. Association for Psychological Science. 2006;15:19–23. [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1995;69:990–1005. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Orcutt HK. Alcohol use, condom use and partner type among heterosexual adolescents and young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2000;61:413–419. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2000.61.413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Peirce RS, Huselid RF. Substance use and sexual risk taking among black adolescents and white adolescents. Health Psychology. 1994;3:251–262. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.13.3.251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cooper ML, Shapiro CM, Powers AM. Motivations for sex and risky sexual behavior among adolescents and young adults: A functional perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;75:1528–1558. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.6.1528. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darkes J, Goldman MS. Expectancy challenge and drinking reduction: Experimental evidence for a mediational process. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:344–353. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.2.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Boca FK, Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Up close and personal: Temporal variability in the drinking of individual college students during their first year. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2004;72:155–164. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dermen KH, Cooper ML. Inhibition conflict and alcohol expectancy as moderators of alcohol's relationship to condom use. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 2000;8:198–206. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.8.2.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dermen KH, Cooper ML, Agocha VB. Sex-related alcohol expectancies as moderators of the relationship between alcohol use and risky sex in adolescents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1998;59:71–77. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1998.59.71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desiderato LL, Crawford HJ. Risky sexual behavior in college students: Relationships between number of sexual partners, disclosure of previous risky behavior, and alcohol use. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 1995;24:55–68. [Google Scholar]
- Gebhardt WA, Kuyper L, Gruensven G. Need for intimacy in relationships and motives for sex as determinants of adolescent condom use. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;33:154–164. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(03)00137-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gillmore MR, Morrison DM, Leigh BC, Hopee MJ, Gaylord J, Rainey DT. Does "high = high risk"? An event-based analysis of the relationship between substance use and unprotected anal sex among gay and bisexual men. AIDS and Behavior. 2002;6:361–369. [Google Scholar]
- Goldman MS, Roehrich L. Alcohol expectancies and sexuality. Alcohol, Health, & Research World. 1991;15:126–132. [Google Scholar]
- Gulledge AK, Stahmann RF, Wilson CM. Seven types of nonsexual romatic physical affection among Brigham Young University students. Psychological Reports. 2004;95:609–614. doi: 10.2466/pr0.95.2.609-614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higgins ET. Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist. 1997;52:1280–1300. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.52.12.1280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hingson R, Heeren T, Winter M, Wechsler H. Magnitude of alcohol-related mortality and morbidity among U.S. college students ages 18–24: Changes from 1998 to 2001. Annual Review of Public Health. 2005;26:259–279. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaly PW, Heesacker M, Frost HM. Collegiate alcohol use and high-risk sexual behavior: A literature review. Journal of College Student Development. 2002;43:838–850. [Google Scholar]
- Kotchick BA, Shaffer A, Forehand R, Miller KS. Adolescent sexual risk behavior: A multi-system perspective. Clinical Psychology Review. 2001;21:493–519. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00070-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R. Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clinical Psychology Review. 2005;25:841–861. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBrie J, Earleywine M, Schiffman J, Pedersen E, Marriot C. Effects of alcohol, expectancies, and partner type on condom use in college males: Event-level analyses. The Journal of Sex Research. 2005;42:259–266. doi: 10.1080/00224490509552280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee CM, Maggs JL, Rankin LA. Spring break trips as a risk factor for heavy alcohol use among first-year college students. Journal of Stuides on Alcohol. 2006;67:911–916. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.911. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lefkowitz ES, Gillen MM. "Sex is just a normal part of life": Sexuality in emerging adulthood. In: Arnett JJ, Tanner JL, editors. Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2005. pp. 235–255. [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC. In search of the seven dwarves: Issues of measurement and meaning in alcohol expectancy research. Psychological Bulletin. 1989;105:361–373. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC. Alcohol consumption and sexual activity as reported with a diary technique. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1993;102:490–493. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC, Ames SL, Stacy AW. Alcohol, drugs, and condom use among drug offenders: An event-based analysis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2008;93:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC, Stacy AW. Alcohol expectancies and drinking in different age groups. Addiction. 2004;99:215–227. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00641.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leigh BC, Stall R. Substance use and risky sexual behavior for exposure to HIV: Issues in methodology, interpretation, and prevention. American Psychologist. 1993;48:1035–1045. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.48.10.1035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald TK, Fong GT, Zanna MP, Martineau AM. Alcohol myopia and condom use: Can alcohol intoxication be associated with more prudent behavior? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:605–619. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.4.605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald TK, MacDonald G, Zanna MP, Fong GT. Alcohol, sexual arousal, and intentions to use condoms in young men: Applying alcohol myopia theory to risky sexual behavior. Health Psychology. 2000;19:290–298. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacNair-Semands RR, Simono RB. College student risk behaviors: Implications for the HIV-AIDS pandemic. Journal of College Student Development. 1996;37:574–583. [Google Scholar]
- Maggs JL. Alcohol use and binge drinking as goal-directed action during the transition to postsecondary education. In: Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL, Hurrelmann K, editors. Health risks and developmental transitions during adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1997. pp. 289–304. [Google Scholar]
- Mallett KA, Lee CM, Neighbors C, Larimer ME, Turrisi R. Do we learn from our mistakes? an examination of the impact of negative alcohol-related consequences on college students' drinking patterns and perceptions. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:269–276. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morrison DM, Gillmore MR, Hoppe MJ, Leigh BC, Rainey D. Adolescent drinking and sex: Findings from a daily diary study. Perspectives on Sexual and Reproductive Health. 2003;35:162–168. doi: 10.1363/psrh.35.162.03. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustanski B. Moderating effects of age on the alcohol and sexual risk taking association: An online daily diary study of men who have sex with men. AIDS and Behavior. 2008;12:118–126. doi: 10.1007/s10461-007-9335-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neal DJ, Fromme K. Event-level covariation of alcohol intoxication and behavioral risks during the first year of college. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2007;75:294–306. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.75.2.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NIAAA. Task force on recommended alcohol questions: National Council on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism recommended sets of alcohol consumption questions. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Park CL, Grant C. Determinants of positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students: Alcohol use, gender, and psychological characteristics. Addictive Behaviors. 2005;30:755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2004.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patrick ME, Maggs JL. Short-term changes in plans to drink and importance of positive and negative alcohol consequences: Between- and within-person predictors. Journal of Adolescence. 2008;31:307–321. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2007.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2 ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. Would you say you "had sex" if …? JAMA. 1999;281:275–277. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.3.275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulenberg JE, Maggs JL. A developmental perspective on alcohol use and heavy drinking during adolescence and the transition to young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2002 Supplement No. 14:54–70. doi: 10.15288/jsas.2002.s14.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Snijders T, Bosker R. Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM, Josephs RA. Alcohol myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. American Psychologist. 1990;45:921–933. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.45.8.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus R, Bacon SD. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1953. Drinking in college: A survey of the customs and attitudes toward alcohol of men and women in 27 American colleges. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor J, Fulop N, Green J. Drink, illicit drugs and unsafe sex in women. Addiction. 1999;94:1209–1218. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1999.948120911.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Testa M, Collins RL. Alcohol and risky sexual behavior: Event-based analyses among a sample of high-risk women. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1997;11:190–201. [Google Scholar]
- Tevyaw OT, Monti PM. Motivational enhancement and other brief interventions for adolescent substance abuse: Foundations, applications, evaluations. Addiction. 2004;99 Supplement 2:63–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00855.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welsh DP, Haugen PT, Widman L, Darling N, Grello CM. Kissing is good: A developmental investigation of sexuality in adolescent romantic couples. Sexuality Research & Social Policy: A Journal of NSRC. 2005;2:32–41. [Google Scholar]