Skip to main content
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2009 Nov 12;364(1533):3209–3216. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0108

Adaptation and the genetics of social behaviour

Laurent Keller 1,*
PMCID: PMC2781871  PMID: 19805428

Abstract

In recent years much progress has been made towards understanding the selective forces involved in the evolution of social behaviour including conflicts over reproduction among group members. Here, I argue that an important additional step necessary for advancing our understanding of the resolution of potential conflicts within insect societies is to consider the genetics of the behaviours involved. First, I discuss how epigenetic modifications of behaviour may affect conflict resolution within groups. Second, I review known natural polymorphisms of social organization to demonstrate that a lack of consideration of the genetic mechanisms involved may lead to erroneous explanations of the adaptive significance of behaviour. Third, I suggest that, on the basis of recent genetic studies of sexual conflict in Drosophila, it is necessary to reconsider the possibility of within-group manipulation by means of chemical substances (i.e. pheromones). Fourth, I address the issue of direct versus indirect genetic effects, which is of particular importance for the study of behaviour in social groups. Fifth, I discuss the issue of how a genetic influence on dominance hierarchies and reproductive division of labour can have secondary effects, for example in the evolution of promiscuity. Finally, because the same sets of genes (e.g. those implicated in chemical signalling and the responses that are triggered) may be used even in species as divergent as ants, cooperative breeding birds and primates, an integration of genetic mechanisms into the field of social evolution may also provide unifying ideas.

Keywords: genetics of behaviour, adaptation, conflict, altruism, indirect genetic effects, pleiotropy

1. Introduction

The success and increased complexity of organisms in the course of evolution is thought to have depended on a small number of major transitions in how information is transmitted from one generation to the next (Maynard Smith & Szathmáry 1995). One such transition was the shift from solitary organisms to societies with a marked reproductive division of labour (eusociality). This transition has led to the tremendous ecological success of social insects, which are now dominant in many terrestrial ecosystems. This success stems from the benefits conferred by sociality, which allows individuals in a group to more efficiently modify their environment and conduct tasks that could not be performed by single individuals (Hölldobler & Wilson 1990).

Over recent years much progress has been made in understanding the selective forces involved in the evolution of social behaviour. There is currently no doubt that kin selection has been the all important selective force for the evolution of reproductive altruism (Bourke & Franks 1995; Queller & Strassmann 1998; Foster et al. 2006; Lehmann & Keller 2006). Numerous genetic studies in insects, other invertebrates and vertebrates have shown that eusociality with reproductive division of labour evolved in groups of highly related individuals, such as those formed by a mother and her offspring (Reeve & Keller 1996; Hughes et al. 2008). However, kin selection theory also predicts that groups of cooperating individuals should be the scene of potential conflicts, because, in contrast to cells of an organism, group mates are not genetically identical. Over the last decade much attention has focussed on within-group conflicts, in particular in social Hymenoptera. While these studies have revealed striking cases of conflicts being modulated by variation in kin structure (e.g. Trivers & Hare 1976; Bourke & Franks 1995; Sundström et al. 1996; Ratnieks & Helanterä in press), there are also many situations where variation in relatedness does not, or only to a very small extent, influence the dynamics of conflicts (Hammond & Keller 2004; Langer et al. 2004; Ratnieks et al. 2006). This has lead to the realization that the proximate mechanisms affecting the relative power of parties need to be considered if one is to understand the resolution of potential conflicts (e.g. Beekman & Ratnieks 2003; Helms et al. 2005). In this essay, I argue that an important need for further progress is the inclusion of genetic mechanisms, in particular those underlying intraspecific variation in behaviour and social organization. I also suggest that a lack of understanding of the genetic basis of traits under investigation may lead to erroneous conclusions about their adaptive significance.

2. The genetic pathways to social evolution

The first basic question, which is starting to attract attention, is to what extant changes in social behaviour are due to changes in gene regulation rather than to sequence differences at genes influencing behaviour (Robinson & Ben-Shahar 2002). That differences in gene expression can lead to major behavioural differences and even marked morphological differences is well illustrated by the differences between queens and workers. In many social insects, queens and workers have extremely divergent morphologies and behaviour. However, usually these differences do not stem from genetic differences but rather from environmental factors triggering differential gene expression during development. At the adult stage, queens and workers typically have thousands of differently expressed genes (Miura et al. 1999; Evans & Wheeler 2001; Whitfield et al. 2002; Gräff et al. 2007; Goodisman et al. 2008) just as different tissues or cell types in a multicellular organism.

A major challenge will be to identify the mechanisms involved in the developmental switch, in particular the causal environmental and social factors and how they mediate changes in gene regulation. A first step in this direction has come from a pioneering study in the honeybee Apis mellifera where the experimental decrease in the methylation level of worker-destined larvae has been shown to result in the production of many larvae with queen-like phenotypic characteristics (Kucharski et al. 2008). This suggests that the specific food (i.e. the addition of royal jelly, pollen and worker glandular secretions in addition to honey) provided to queen-destined larvae may affect the level of methylation and thereby alter the processes of gene expression and caste determination. Interestingly, a recent study in humans also suggested a role of nutrition on patterns of methylation. Individuals who were prenatally exposed to famine during the Dutch Hunger Winter in 1944–1945 had, six decades later, less DNA methylation of the imprinted IGF2 gene than their unexposed siblings (Heijmans et al. 2008).

The suggestion that early-life environmental conditions can cause epigenetic changes that persist throughout life has important implications for our understanding of the dynamics of within-group conflicts. It has been proposed that, in addition to kin selection, parental manipulation could be another mechanism favouring altruism in social insects (Alexander 1974; Michener & Brothers 1974). For example, the reproduction of workers may be prevented if they are physically dominated by the queen or if they are underfed (making them poor potential reproductive individuals). However, it was pointed out that the parental manipulation hypothesis contained a flaw because a gene causing an adult to act against the interests of an offspring will be counter-selected when it is present in juveniles, through these juveniles having a parent bearing the gene (Dawkins 1976; Parker & Macnair 1978; Bourke & Franks 1995; see also Smiseth et al. 2008 for a review of models of parent–offspring conflict). Importantly, however, these arguments are based on simple genetic or game-theoretical models and the conclusion might be altered if the expression of the trait is conditional and/or if imprinting mechanisms are involved in parental manipulation. While some theoretical work has been conducted on the potential role of differential expression of genes inherited from the mother and father (Haig 2000; Queller 2003; Kronauer 2008), it remains to be investigated how imprinting and epigenetic trans-generational effects on behaviour may affect conflict resolution.

3. Genetic architecture, heterozygote advantage, pleiotropy and adaptation

Adaptation, including in social life, can never be perfect because of constraints in the genetic system, including mutation, drift, inbreeding, selection, pleiotropy, linkage disequilibrium, heterozygote advantage and gene flow (Crespi 2000). The fire ant Solenopsis invicta provides a good example to illustrate how these effects can lead to surprising and unexpected behaviours. This species exhibits a fundamental social polymorphism with a monogyne form in which colonies have a single queen and a polygyne form where colonies contain several queens. As in many other ants, this difference in queen number is associated with differences in a wide range of reproductive and social traits, including queen phenotype and breeding strategy and the mode of colony reproduction (Ross & Keller 1995). In the polygyne form, the probability that a queen will be accepted in an established colony is strongly associated with their genotypes at the locus Gp-9 (General protein-9). All homozygous Gp-9BB queens are killed by workers when they initiate reproduction (Keller & Ross 1993, 1998). Intriguingly, Gp-9BB queens are heavier and more fecund than queens with alternate genotypes (Gp-9Bb and Gp-9bb), raising the question of why workers selectively eliminate queens with apparently the ‘best’ phenotype. To resolve this paradox, it was suggested (Keller & Ross 1998) that the execution of reproductively superior queens may represent a mechanism selected to maintain multiple queens within a colony if, as has been demonstrated in some ants, polygyny is advantageous under some ecological conditions. However, further genetic and behavioural studies revealed unexpected twists to the story and a completely different explanation for the workers' behaviour. This is because the Gp-9b allele was found to be a kind of green beard gene inducing workers carrying one copy of that allele to selectively kill queens lacking the same allele (Keller & Ross 1998).

In addition to these effects, the locus Gp-9 is also strongly associated with queen behaviour. After their mating flight, Gp-9BB queens typically attempt to start a new colony independently by feeding their progeny from their body reserves. By contrast, queens of the two other genotypes do not fly far and try to enter established colonies rather than starting a new one on their own. Interestingly, these behavioural differences are tightly correlated with an important physiological difference as only Gp-9BB queens accumulate sufficient fat reserves before their mating flight to raise a first cohort of workers alone (Ross & Keller 1995; DeHeer et al. 1999; Keller & Ross 1999).

The strong association between the genotype at Gp-9 and all these behavioural, morphological and physiological differences most likely results from the combined effect of several linked genes in the genomic region marked by Gp-9. This region is expected to have many of the unusual properties of regions containing the sex-determining genes in species with sex chromosomes because the b haplotype is found only in the polygyne social form, just as the Y chromosome is found only in males in species with male heterogamety. As a result, the Gp-9b region is predicted to (i) accumulate genes beneficial in the polygyne social environment (as the Y chromosome accumulates genes beneficial to male function; Rice 1987); (ii) evolve reduced recombination to preserve associations of genes advantageous for polygyny (as occurs for genes advantageous to males on the Y chromosome; Charlesworth et al. 2005); and (iii) accumulate deleterious alleles and transposable elements (because of reduced recombination; Charlesworth et al. 2005). Consistent with these expectations, the Gp-9 genomic region is characterized by low recombination (Ross 1997; Krieger & Ross 2005; Wang et al. 2008), the b allele behaves as a homozygous lethal allele (Ross 1997; Keller & Ross 1999; Hallar et al. 2007), and two transposons are preferentially expressed on the b haplotype (evidence suggests that at least one of them likely reflects a single insertion in the b haplotype; Wang et al. 2008).

This example illustrates the danger of searching for adaptive explanations without a clear understanding of the genetic basis of the behaviour. The consideration of selection at the individual or colony levels only would have led to an erroneous explanation of why workers eliminate the heavier and more fecund queens in polygyne colonies. This is an important point, in particular because of the current confusion by many scientists in how selection works at the different levels of biological organization.

Similarly, it is likely that the interpretation of the social behaviours of many other organisms would change if one had information on their genetic bases. In this respect, it should be mentioned that pleiotropy is probably the rule rather than the exception for many traits, particularly for behaviours which are the product of many sensory, integrative, motivational and motor processes. For example, a recent study (Ducrest et al. 2008) predicted that, and provided evidence for, the widespread association between the degree of melanin-based coloration and many physiological and behavioural traits in vertebrates stems from the melanocortins binding to the melanocortin-1-receptor (regulating the eumelanin synthesis), also binding to five other melanocortin receptors with very different functions. Similarly, it has been suggested that the pleiotropic linkage of a gene in stalk and spore formation might be an important component stabilizing cooperation in the social amoeba Dyctiostelium discoideum (Foster et al. 2004). A big challenge will be to determine how commonly suites of behavioural differences in social organisms are similar due to pleiotropic effects and also determine to what extent pleiotropy affects the adaptive values of traits.

4. Genes, cooperation and manipulation

In insect societies with strong queen–worker dimorphism, it was thought that queens could manipulate workers into pursuing actions that are contrary to their inclusive fitness through ‘pheromonal queen control’, whereby chemicals exuded by the queen(s) replace physical intimidation in forcing workers to behave in ways that increase queen fitness (e.g. Wilson 1971; Fletcher & Ross 1985; Hölldobler & Wilson 1990). However, this view was challenged on the basis that pheromonal queen control had never conclusively been demonstrated and was evolutionarily difficult to justify (Keller & Nonacs 1993). The main arguments proposed against the pheromonal queen control hypothesis was that, if workers have their fitness significantly reduced, there would be strong selection to escape control by building up tolerance or immunity to the queen pheromone. To retain strong control, queens would therefore be required to continually produce new compounds to stay one step ahead of the workers, and/or to invest more and more resources in producing larger quantities of the pheromone as effective dosage levels increase. As both of these solutions would eventually lose cost-effectiveness, queens would probably gain more overall fitness by allowing workers to win in some respects rather than indulge in an escalating arms race that would eventually decrease overall colony productivity. Later experimental studies have indeed concluded that queen pheromones are more likely to be honest signals to which workers respond in a way that generally increases their own inclusive fitness (e.g. Cuvillier-Hot et al. 2004; Endler et al. 2006; Bhadra & Gadagkar 2008; Smith et al. 2009).

Interestingly, a similar type of argument has also been made with regard to male–female conflict between mating partners. However, given that recent studies of sexual conflicts in Drosophila are revealing unexpected sophistication in the males' ability to manipulate females, the theoretical and empirical basis of queen control in social insects needs to be re-evaluated. During mating, Drosophila males have been shown to transfer more than 100 proteins (e.g. Findlay et al. 2008), causing a wide variety of fitness-related effects in females, including decreased sexual receptivity, increased egg production, altered morphology of the reproductive tract, increased production of immune-related peptides and the liberation of juvenile hormone (JH) (reviewed in Wolfner 2002; Ravi Ram & Wolfner 2007). While some of these changes are beneficial to both sexes, others are costly to females.

Information on the role of seminal proteins in sexual conflict, including whether they mediate physiological and behavioural changes against the females' interest, can be gained by the analyses of the tissues that are targeted by the protein (McGraw et al. 2004; Ravi Ram & Wolfner 2007). For example, proteins binding to receptors in the reproductive tract are unlikely to permit males to force females to behave contrary to their interest. By contrast, proteins acting through neuroendocrine pathways after having entered the female circulatory system (haemolymph) may allow chemical manipulation. Interestingly, at least 10 of the proteins transferred during mating have been shown to pass from the reproductive tract into the circulation system of Drosophila females (Ravi Ram & Wolfner 2005), some probably even reaching the brain where they could directly affect the female's behaviour. Further evidence for seminal proteins being implicated in sexual conflict (Rice 2000) comes from their very rapid evolution, the expected pattern if there is antagonistic coevolution between molecules in males and females (Swanson et al. 2001; Begun & Lindfors 2005; Findlay et al. 2008).

The apparent ability of males to chemically manipulate females during the process of mating re-opens the question of whether pheromonal queen control really does not occur in social insects. In contrast to the conflict between males and females, an additional issue that needs to be considered in the case of social insects is that chemicals are generally distributed among all colony members. Thus, a queen producing a chemical aimed at preventing workers from reproducing may suffer herself from the effect of the chemical (Keller & Nonacs 1993). Chemical manipulation would thus require the queen to be much less sensitive than workers to pheromone produced. It is necessary, therefore, to precisely identify the pheromones involved and their target in workers. Most current studies consist of searching for an association between the fertility of queens and their chemical signature. Ultimately, it will be important to determine whether queen-produced pheromones exclusively bind to antennal receptors (which would support the view that they are honest signals) or whether they also enter the worker circulatory system and mediate hormonal changes directly affecting reproduction (which would be consistent with pheromonal queen control). If pheromones entering the circulatory system of workers are to be identified, it will also be interesting to determine if they also affect queen physiology or whether queens evolved immunity to their own chemicals.

5. Direct and indirect effects

All behaviours are modulated by interactions between genes and the environment. In social organisms, social interactions are a key component of the environment. To understand the link between genotypes and phenotypes, therefore, requires determining how an individual's phenotype is influenced by its own genes (direct genetic effects) and those expressed in social partners (indirect effects) (e.g. Moore et al. 1997; Linksvayer & Wade 2005). While indirect effects are increasingly being recognized as an important component of the genetic architecture of species (e.g. Moore et al. 1997; Linksvayer & Wade 2005), there are still almost no empirical data on such effects. One of the social systems where indirect effects have been studied is the fire ant Solenopsis invicta, where the behaviour of Gp-9BB workers was shown to depend on the ratio of Gp-9Bb workers in their colony (Ross & Keller 1998, 2002). When this ratio is lower than 5–10 per cent, Gp-9BB workers accept only a single queen per colony that must also bear the genotype Gp-9BB (i.e. they exhibit a typical monogyne behaviour). However, when there are more than 5–10 per cent of Gp-9Bb workers, Gp-9BB workers will accept many additional queens (up to hundreds), but only Gp-9Bb queens. Thus Gp-9 exerts indirect genetic effects, in that a threshold ratio of Gp-9Bb workers induce changes in the social behaviour of all colony members (even those lacking the b allele) and determines a fundamental aspect of social organization (monogyne versus polygyne social organization).

Microarray experiments (Wang et al. 2008) revealed that differences at the genomic region marked by Gp-9 have direct effects on the level of expression of 39 genes in workers (i.e. these genes are differentially expressed between Gp-9Bb and Gp-9BB workers, irrespective of their social environment) and indirect effects on the level of expression of 91 genes (i.e. the level of expression of these genes in Gp-9BB workers depends on the presence or absence of GP-9Bb workers, possibly as a result of changes in the processes of within-colony chemical communication). Remarkably, there is almost no overlap between the genes whose level of expression is influenced by the focal workers' Gp-9 genotypes and genes whose expression is influenced by the social environment, with only one of the 129 differentially expressed genes appearing in both categories.

There have been very few other studies of indirect genetic effects within social groups. In Drosophila melanogaster, the genotypic composition of social groups (single versus mixed genotypes) was shown to affect behaviours and gene expression (i.e. the transcription of the clock gene, pheromonal profile on the cuticle and mating frequency) (Kent et al. 2008). Similarly, the mixing within colonies of individuals coming from honeybee strains selected for high and low pollen hoarding revealed that the ovariole number and dry mass of workers produced was influenced by interactions between their genotypes and those of other colony members (Linkvayer et al. 2009). In another study with European honey bees, the defensive behaviour of workers was increased when they were in colonies containing Africanized honeybees (Guzmán-Novoa & Page 1994). It will be of great interest to investigate the consequences of such indirect genetic effects on social evolution. For example, it remains to be studied whether variation in within-group genetic diversity may mediate changes in worker behaviour via indirect effects and, if so, whether it might have favoured multiple mating in some species.

6. Genes, division of labour and phenotypic plasticity

While it used to be thought that the morphological and physiological differences between castes in social insects stem only from environmental effects influencing developmental processes, several exceptional cases of genetic caste determination have recently been discovered (Helms Cahan et al. 2002, 2004; Julian et al. 2002; Volny & Gordon 2002; Helms Cahan & Keller 2003; Helms Cahan & Vinson 2003; Fournier et al. 2005; Ohkawara et al. 2006). In many of these examples, populations contain two distinct genetic lineages and the developmental fate of female brood depends on the genetic origin of the parents; the inter-lineage eggs develop into workers while intra-lineage eggs develop into queens (Helms Cahan & Keller 2003). This system of caste determination has important implications on mating behaviour and social organization. In monogyne species with such a mode of reproduction (e.g. some Pogonomyrmex ants), queens have to mate multiply (Gadau et al. 2003) to ensure matings with males of both lineages and the production of both worker-destined and queen-destined eggs. In another similar system in the genus Solenopsis, some queens mate with males of the same species and others with males of another species (Helms Cahan & Vinson 2003). The presence of many queens in the same nest ensures the production of both queens (conspecific matings) and workers (interspecific matings). Finally, in two other ants (Wasmannia, Fournier et al. 2005; Vollenhovia, Ohkawara et al. 2006), the problem of producing both queen- and worker-destined eggs has been resolved by the conditional use of sexual and asexual reproduction. As males and females are from different lineages, queens produce workers by laying fertilized eggs and queens by reproducing clonally.

Although these systems of reproduction are highly unusual, I believe that they are much more common than realized. My prediction is that maybe as many as 10 per cent of social insects have such unusual modes of reproduction. This is based on three lines of evidence. First, there is an increasing number of examples of new ant reproductive systems (see Heinze 2008). Second, I know of several unpublished population genetic studies having data inconsistent with conventional modes of reproduction; yet these studies make no reference to the unusual mode of reproduction that must be involved. Finally, controlled mating experiments revealed genetic compatibility effects on caste differentiation in Pogonomyrmex rugosus, an ant thought to have an environmental system of caste determination. In this species, the viability of queens and workers depends on genetic interactions between the parental genomes with some parental combinations being mostly compatible with queen development and others with worker development (Schwander & Keller 2008). Because similar controlled mating experiments have never been conducted in other social insects, it is impossible to determine how common such genetic compatibility effects are. Intriguingly, however, many observations are suggestive of incompatibility effects in other social insects. In several social insects where queens mate multiply, patrilines are differently represented in queens and workers (Osborne & Oldroyd 1999; Chaline et al. 2003; Moritz et al. 2005). The explanations proposed so far include nepotism (the preference of closest relatives over less-related individuals) and royalty genes (i.e. alleles increasing the likelihood of their bearers to develop into queens) (Osborne & Oldroyd 1999; Moritz et al. 2005; Hughes et al. 2008). However, nepotism between patrilines or matrilines is rare or absent in social insects (Keller 1997) and royalty genes are unlikely because they would rapidly go to fixation. Thus, it is likely that, as in Pogonomyrmex rugosus, the different representation of patrilines in queens and workers of many social insects reflects incompatibility effects.

Unequal distribution of patrilines has also been reported between morphologically divergent worker castes in the leaf-cutting ants (Acromyrmex echinatior; Hughes & Boomsma 2008) and three ant species (Camponotus consobrinus, Fraser et al. 2000; Pogonomyrmex badius, Rheindt et al. 2005; Eciton burchellii, Jaffe et al. 2007). Given that discrete worker morphologies also stem from different developmental pathways (Wilson 1971), the unequal distribution of patrilines among worker castes and between queens and workers might also be simply due to larval development being affected by genetic compatibility effects. Finally, incompatibility effects may also explain the frequently observed association between patrilines and division of labour, whereby particular tasks are preferentially performed by workers of a given patriline (see Smith et al. 2008). In the same line, it would be interesting to determine whether incompatibility effects play a role in the establishment of dominance hierarchies in vertebrate societies and small insect colonies.

7. Conclusion

In this review I have selected a few examples to illustrate why knowledge of the genetics of behaviour should be of interest not only to those with a proximal or mechanistic interest in behaviour but also to those interested in ultimate questions. In fact, I would go as far as to say that, in many cases, a compelling demonstration of behaviour being adaptive requires one to have a clear understanding of its genetic basis. This is of course a challenging task because the identification of genes involved in social behaviour is still in its early phases (e.g. Robinson et al. 2008), and functional analyses are still impossible in most social insects. However, there is no doubt that many new genes will soon be identified setting up the possibility of studies of social behaviour and adaptation from both an evolutionary and mechanistic perspective.

Most of the known genetic polymorphisms influencing social behaviour have been discovered serendipitously during the course of population genetic studies of social insects. In the future it would be helpful to also use a candidate gene approach, for example by investigating genes known to be involved in orchestrating the perception and processing of sensory information or genes known to affect behaviour as for example the vasopressin receptor 1a (avpr1a) gene, which is involved in interspecific differences in social and mating behaviour in voles (Young et al. 1999; Lim et al. 2004) and possibly individual behavioural difference in humans (e.g. Knafo et al. 2008; Walum et al. 2008). Because these genes are likely to figure prominently in social evolution it would be very interesting to conduct population genetic studies to identify natural polymorphisms affecting behaviour and social organization.

Finally, an integration of genetics may also contribute to bridging of the gap between studies conducted in invertebrate and vertebrate societies. Until recently, studies in these two taxa have typically proceeded independently and, in fact, there has been too little interaction between students of social vertebrates and of social insects. As the same set of genes (e.g. those implicated in chemical signalling and the responses that are triggered) are likely to be implicated in species as divergent as ants, cooperative breeding birds and primates, an integration of genetics in social evolution should provide a general framework helping in bridging the different communities.

Acknowledgements

My work has been continuously supported by grants from the Swiss NSF. I thank Andrew Bourke, Philippe Christe, Heikki Helanterä, Rob Page and Francis Ratnieks for useful comments on the manuscript.

Footnotes

One contribution of 16 to a Discussion Meeting Issue ‘The evolution of society’.

References

  1. Alexander R. D.1974The evolution of social behavior. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5, 325–383 (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.05.110174.001545) [Google Scholar]
  2. Begun D. J., Lindfors H. A.2005Rapid evolution of genomic Acp complement in the melanogaster subgroup of Drosophila. Mol. Biol. Evol. 22, 2010–2202 (doi:10.1093/molbev/msi201) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bhadra A., Gadagkar R.2008We know that the wasps ‘know’: cryptic successors to the queen in Ropalidia marginata. Biol. Lett. 4, 634–637 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0455) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Beekman M., Ratnieks F. L. W.2003Power over reproduction in social Hymenoptera. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 358, 1741–1753 (doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1262) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bourke A. F. G., Franks N. R.1995Social evolution in ants Princeton, USA: Princeton University Press [Google Scholar]
  6. Chaline N., Arnold G., Papin C., Ratnieks F. L. W.2003Patriline differences in emergency queen rearing in the honey bee Apis mellifera. Insect. Soc. 50, 234–236 (doi:10.1007/s00040-003-0664-6) [Google Scholar]
  7. Charlesworth D., Charlesworth B., Marais G.2005Steps in the evolution of heteromorphic sex chromosomes. Heredity 95, 118–128 (doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800697) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Crespi B. J.2000The evolution of maladaptation. Heredity 84, 623–629 (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2540.2000.00746.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cuvillier-Hot V., Lenoir A., Peeters C.2004Reproductive monopoly enforced by sterile police workers in a queenless ant. Behav. Ecol. 15, 970–975 (doi:10.1093/beheco/arh072) [Google Scholar]
  10. Dawkins R.1976The selfish gene Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  11. DeHeer C. J., Goodisman M. A. D., Ross K. G.1999Queen dispersal strategies in the multiple-queen form of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. Am. Nat. 153, 660–675 (doi:10.1086/303205) [Google Scholar]
  12. Ducrest A.-L., Keller L., Roulin A.2008Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration and behavioural syndromes. Trends Ecol. Evol. 23, 502–510 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.06.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Endler A., Liebig J., Hölldobler B.2006Queen fertility, egg marking and colony size in the ant Camponotus floridanus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 490–499 (doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0073-0) [Google Scholar]
  14. Evans J. D., Wheeler D. E.2001Expression profiles during honeybee caste determination. Genome Biol. 2, 0001.1–0001.6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Findlay G. D., Yi X., MacCoss M. J., Swanson W. J.2008Proteomics reveals novel Drosophila seminal fluid proteins transferred at mating. PLoS Biol. 6, e178 (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060178) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fletcher D. J. C., Ross K. G.1985Regulation of reproduction in social Hymenoptera. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 30, 319–343 (doi:10.1146/annurev.en.30.010185.001535) [Google Scholar]
  17. Foster K. R., Shaulsky G., Strassmann J. E., Queller D. C., Thompson C. R. L.2004Pleiotropy as a mechanism to stabilise cooperation. Nature 431, 693–969 (doi:10.1038/nature02894) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Foster K. R., Wenseleers T., Ratnieks F. L. W.2006Kin selection is the key to altruism. Trends Ecol. Evol. 21, 57–60 (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2005.11.020) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Fournier D., Estoup A., Orivel J., Foucaud J., Jourdan H., Le Breton J., Keller L.2005Clonal reproduction by males and females in the little fire ant. Nature 435, 1230–1234 (doi:10.1038/nature03705) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Fraser V. S., Kaufmann B., Oldroyd B. P., Crozier R. H.2000Genetic influence on caste in the ant Camponotus consobrinus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 47, 188–194 (doi:10.1007/s002650050010) [Google Scholar]
  21. Gadau J., Strehl C.-P., Oettler J., Hölldobler B.2003Determinants of intracolonial relatedness in Pogonomyrmex rugosus (Hymenoptera; Formicidae): mating frequency and brood raids. Mol. Ecol. 12, 1931–1938 (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2003.01853.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goodisman M. A. D., Kovacs J. L., Hunt B. G.2008Functional genetics and genomics in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): The interplay of genes and social life. Myrmecol. News 11, 107–117 [Google Scholar]
  23. Gräff J., Jemielity S., Parker J. D., Parker K. M., Keller L.2007Differential gene expression between adult queens and workers in the ant Lasius niger. Mol. Ecol. 16, 675–683 (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2007.03162.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Guzmán-Novoa E., Page R. E., Jr1994Genetic dominance and worker interactions affect honeybee colony defense. Behav. Ecol. 5, 91–97 (doi:10.1093/beheco/5.1.91) [Google Scholar]
  25. Haig D.2000The kinship theory of genomic imprinting. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 31, 9–32 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.31.1.9) [Google Scholar]
  26. Hallar B. L., Krieger M. J. B., Ross K. G.2007Potential cause of lethality of an allele implicated in social evolution in fire ants. Genetica 131, 69–79 (doi:10.1007/s10709-006-9114-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hammond R. L., Keller L.2004Conflict over male parentage in social insects. PLoS Biol. 2, e248 1472–1482 (doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020248) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Heijmans B. T., Tobia E. W., Steinb A. D., Putterc H., Blauwd G. J., Sussere E. S., Slagbooma P. E., Lumeye L. H.2008Persistent epigenetic differences associated with prenatal exposure to famine in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 17 046–17 049 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0806560105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heinze J.2008The demise of the standard ant (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol. News 11, 9–20 [Google Scholar]
  30. Helms Cahan S., Keller L.2003Complex hybrid origin of genetic caste determination in harvester ants. Nature 424, 306–309 (doi:10.1038/nature01744) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Helms Cahan S., Vinson B. S.2003Reproductive division of labor between hybrid and nonhybrid offspring in a fire ant hybrid zone. Evolution 57, 1562–1570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Helms Cahan S., Parker J. D., Rissing S. W., Johnson R. A., Polony T. S., Weiser M. D., Smith D. R.2002Extreme genetic differences between queens and workers in hybridizing Pogonomyrmex harvester ants. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 1871–1877 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2061) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Helms Cahan S., Julian G. E., Rissing S. W., Schwander T., Parker J. D., Keller L.2004Loss of phenotypic plasticity generates genotype–caste association in harvester ants. Curr. Biol. 14, 2277–2282 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Helms K. R., Reuter M., Keller L.2005Sex-ratio conflict between queens and workers in eusocial Hymenoptera: mechanisms, costs, and the evolution of split colony sex ratios. Evolution 59, 2626–2638 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hölldobler B., Wilson E. O.1990The ants Cambridge, UK: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press [Google Scholar]
  36. Hughes W. O. H., Boomsma J. J.2008Genetic royal cheats in leaf-cutting ant societies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 5150–5153 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0710262105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hughes W. O. H., Oldroyd B. P., Beekman M., Ratnieks F. L. W.2008Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is key to the evolution of eusociality. Science 320, 1213–1216 (doi:10.1126/science.1156108) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jaffe R., Kronauer D. J. C., Kraus F. B., Boomsma J. J., Moritz R. F. A.2007Worker caste determination in the army ant Eciton burchellii. Biol. Lett. 3, 513–516 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2007.0257) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Julian G. E., Fewell J. H., Gadau J., Johnson R. A., Larrabee D.2002Genetic determination of the queen caste in an ant hybrid zone. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 8157–8160 (doi:10.1073/pnas.112222099) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Keller L.1997Indiscriminate altruism: unduly nice parents and siblings. Trends Ecol. Evol. 12, 99–103 (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(96)10065-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Keller L., Nonacs P.1993The role of queen pheromones in colonies of social insects: queen control or queen signal? Anim. Behav. 45, 787–794 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1993.1092) [Google Scholar]
  42. Keller L., Ross K. G.1993Phenotypic basis of reproductive success in a social insect: genetic and social determinants. Science 260, 1107–1110 (doi:10.1126/science.260.5111.1107) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Keller L., Ross K. G.1998Selfish genes: a green beard in the red fire ant. Nature 394, 573–575 (doi:10.1038/29064) [Google Scholar]
  44. Keller L., Ross K. G.1999Major gene effects on phenotype and fitness: the relative roles of Pgm-3 and Gp-9 in introduced populations of the fire ant Solenopsis invicta. J. Evol. Biol. 12, 672–680 (doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.1999.00064.x) [Google Scholar]
  45. Kent C., Azanchi R., Smith B., Formosa A., Levine J. D.2008Social context influences chemical communication in Drosophila melanogaster males. Curr. Biol. 18, 1384–1389 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.088) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Knafo A., et al. 2008Genes Brain Behav. 7, 266–275 (doi:10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00341.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Krieger M. J. B., Ross K. G.2005Molecular evolutionary analyses of the odorant-binding protein gene Gp-9 in fire ants and other Solenopsis species. Mol. Biol. Evol. 22, 2090–2103 (doi:10.1093/molbev/msi203) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kronauer D. C.2008Genomic imprinting and kinship in the social Hymenoptera: what are the predictions? J. Theor. Biol 254, 737–740 (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.06.019) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kucharski R. J., Maleszka J., Foret S., Maleszka R.2008Nutritional control of reproductive status in honeybees via DNA methylation. Science 319, 1827–1830 (doi:10.1126/science.1153069) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Langer P., Hogendoorn K., Keller L.2004Tug-of-war over reproduction in a social bee. Nature 428, 844–847 (doi:10.1038/nature02431) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lehmann L., Keller L.2006The evolution of cooperation and altruism—a general framework and a classification of models. J. Evol. Biol. 19, 1365–1376 (doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01119.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lim M. M., Wang Z., Olazábal D. E., Xianghui R., Terwilliger E. F., Young L. J.2004Enhanced partner preference in a promiscuous species by manipulating the expression of a single gene. Nature 429, 754–757 (doi:10.1038/nature02539) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Linksvayer T. A., Wade M. J.2005The evolutionary origin and elaboration of sociality in the aculeate Hymenoptera: maternal effects, sib-social effects, and heterochrony. Q. Rev. Biol. 80, 317–336 (doi:10.1086/432266) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Linksvayer T. A., Fondrk M. K., Page R. E., Jr2009Honeybee social regulatory networks are shaped by colony level selection. Am. Nat. 173, E99–E107 (doi:10.1086/596527) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Maynard Smith J., Szathmáry E.1995The major transitions in evolution Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press [Google Scholar]
  56. McGraw L. A., Gibson G., Clark A. G., Wolfner M. F.2004Genes regulated by mating, sperm or seminal proteins in mated female Drosophila melanogaster. Curr. Biol. 14, 1509–1514 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.08.028) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Michener C. D., Brothers D. J.1974Were workers of eusocial Hymenoptera initially altruistic or oppressed? Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 68, 1242–1245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Miura T., Kamikouchi A., Sawata M., Takeuchi H., Natori S., Kubo T., Matsumoto T.1999Soldier caste-specific gene expression in the mandibular glands of Hodotermopsis japonica (Isoptera: Termopsidae). Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 96, 13874–13879 (doi:10.1073/pnas.96.24.13874) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Moore A. J., Brodie E. D., III, Wolf J. B.1997Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process I. Direct and indirect genetic effects of social interactions. Evolution 51, 1352–1362 (doi:10.2307/2411187) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Moritz R. F. A., Lattorff H. M., Neumann P., Kraus F. B., Radloff S. E., Hepburn H. R.2005Rare royal families in honeybees, Apis mellifera. Naturwissenschaften 92, 488–491 (doi:10.1007/s00114-005-0025-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Ohkawara K., Nakayama M., Sato A., Trindl A., Heinze J.2006Clonal reproduction and genetic caste differences in a queen-polymorphic ant, Vollenhovia emeryi. Biol. Lett. 2, 359–363 (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0491) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Osborne K. E., Oldroyd B. P.1999Possible causes of reproductive dominance during emergency queen rearing by honeybees. Anim. Behav. 58, 267–272 (doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1139) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Parker G. A., MacNair M. R.1978Models of parent–offspring conflict. I. Monogamy. Anim. Behav. 26, 97–110 (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(78)90009-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Queller D. C.2003Theory of genomic imprinting conflict in social insects. BMC Evol. Biol. 3, 15 (doi:10.1186/1471-2148-3-15) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Queller D. C., Strassmann J. E.1998Kin selection and social insects. Bioscience 48, 165–175 (doi:10.2307/1313262) [Google Scholar]
  66. Ratnieks F. L. W., Helanterä H.In press The evolution of extreme altruism and inequality in insect colonies. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B (doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0129) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ratnieks F. L. W., Foster K. R., Wenseleers T.2006Conflict resolution in insect societies. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 51, 581–608 (doi:10.1146/annurev.ento.51.110104.151003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Ravi Ram K. R., Wolfner M. F.2005Fates and targets of male accessory gland proteins in mated female Drosophila melanogaster. Insect Biochem. Mol. Biol. 35, 1059–1071 (doi:10.1016/j.ibmb.2005.05.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Ravi Ram K. R., Wolfner M. F.2007Seminal influences: Drosophila Acps and the molecular interplay between males and females during reproduction. Int. Comp. Biol. 47, 427–445 (doi:10.1093/icb/icm046) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Reeve H. K., Keller L.1996Relatedness asymmetry and reproductive sharing in animal societies. Am. Nat. 148, 764–769 (doi:10.1086/285954) [Google Scholar]
  71. Rheindt F. E., Strehl C. P., Gadau J.2005A genetic component in the determination of worker polymorphism in the Florida harvester ant Pogonomyrmex badius. Insectes Soc. 52, 163–168 (doi:10.1007/s00040-004-0787-4) [Google Scholar]
  72. Rice W. R.1987Genetic hitchhiking and the evolution of reduced genetic activity of the Y sex chromosome. Genetics 116, 161–167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Rice W. R.2000Dangerous liaisons. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 12953–12955 (doi:10.1073/pnas.97.24.12953) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Robinson G. E., Ben-Shahar Y.2002Social behavior and comparative genomics: new genes or new gene regulation? Genes Brain Behav. 1, 197–203 (doi:10.1034/j.1601-183X.2002.10401.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Robinson G. E., Fernald R. D., Clayton D. F.2008Genes and social behavior. Science 322, 896–900 (doi:10.1126/science.1159277) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ross K. G.1997Multilocus evolution in fire ants: effects of selection, gene flow and recombination. Genetics 145, 961–974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ross K. G., Keller L.1995Ecology and evolution of social organization: insights from fire ants and other highly eusocial insects. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 26, 631–656 (doi:10.1146/annurev.es.26.110195.003215) [Google Scholar]
  78. Ross K. G., Keller L.1998Genetic control of social organization in an ant. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 14232–14237 (doi:10.1073/pnas.95.24.14232) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Ross K. G., Keller L.2002Experimental conversion of colony social organization by manipulation of worker genotype composition in fire ants (Solenopsis invicta). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 287–295 [Google Scholar]
  80. Schwander T., Keller L.2008Genetic compatibility affects queen and worker caste determination. Science 322, 552 (doi:10.1126/science.1162590) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Smiseth P. T., Wright J., Kölliker M.2008Parent–offspring conflict and co-adaptation: behavioural ecology meets quantitative genetics. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 1823–1830 (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0199) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Smith C. R., Toth A. L., Suarez A. V., Robinson G. E.2008Genetic and genomic analyses of the division of labour in insect societies. Nat. Rev. Genet. 9, 735–748 (doi:10.1038/nrg2429) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Smith A. A., Hölldobler B., Liebig J.2009Cuticular hydrocarbons reliably identify cheaters and allow enforcement of altruism in a social insect. Curr. Biol. 19, 78–81 (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.11.059) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sundström L., Chapuisat M., Keller L.1996Conditional manipulation of sex ratios by ant workers—a test of kin selection theory. Science 274, 993–995 (doi:10.1126/science.274.5289.993) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Swanson W. J., Clark A. G., Waldrip-Dail H. M., Wolfner M. F., Aquadro C. F.2001Evolutionary EST analysis identifies rapidly evolving male reproductive proteins in Drosophila. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 7375–7379 (doi:10.1073/pnas.131568198) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Trivers R. L., Hare H.1976Haplodiploidy and the evolution of the social insects. Science 191, 249–263 (doi:10.1126/science.1108197) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Volny V. P., Gordon D. M.2002Genetic basis for queen–worker dimorphism in a social insect. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99, 6108–6111 (doi:10.1073/pnas.092066699) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Walum H., et al. 2008Genetic variation in the vasopressin receptor 1a gene (AVPR1A) associates with pair-bonding behavior in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 14 153–14 156 (doi:10.1073/pnas.0803081105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Wang J., Ross K. G., Keller L.2008Genome-wide expression patterns and the genetic architecture of a fundamental social trait. PLoS Genet. 7, e1000127 (doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000127) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Whitfield C. W., Band M. R., Bonaldo M. F., Kumar C. G., Liu L., Pardinas J. R., Robertson H. M., Soares M. B., Robinson G. E.2002Annotated expressed sequence tags and cDNA microarrays for studies of brain and behavior in the honey bee. Genome Res. 12, 555–566 (doi:10.1101/gr.5302) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Wilson E. O.1971The insect societies Cambridge, UK: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press [Google Scholar]
  92. Wolfner M. F.2002The gifts that keep on giving: physiological functions and evolutionary dynamics of male seminal proteins in Drosophila. Heredity 88, 85–93 (doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Young L. J., Nilsen R., Waymire K. G., MacGregor G. R., Insel T. R.1999Increased affiliative response to vasopressin in mice expressing the V1a receptor from a monogamous vole. Nature 400, 766–768 (doi:10.1038/23475) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES