Abstract
Anthropomorphic virtual agents can serve as powerful technological mediators to impact motivational outcomes such as self-efficacy and attitude change. Such anthropomorphic agents can be designed as simulated social models in the Bandurian sense, providing social influence as virtual ‘role models’. Of particular value is the capacity for designing such agents as optimized social models for a target audience and context. Importantly, the visual presence and appearance of such agents can have a major impact on motivation and affect regardless of the underlying technical sophistication. Empirical results of different instantiations of agent presence and appearance are reviewed for both autonomous virtual agents and avatars that represent a user.
Keywords: virtual agents, avatars, motivation, persuasion, visual presence, appearance
1. Anthropomorphic virtual agents implemented as social models
Research indicates the effectiveness of human social models in influencing another to change behaviours, beliefs or attitudes, as well as social and cognitive functioning (e.g. Schunk 1981; Bandura 1986). Through processes such as observation, vicarious experience (experience gained by observing another) and social interaction, one can acquire the behaviours or expertise mediated through a human social model. The most effective social model is similar to the observer while representing someone whom the observer aspires to be like. One of the key attributes for a social model is appearance; how he/she looks influences the observer in making important assumptions regarding factors such as age, status, attractiveness and credibility.
The focus in this paper is on the motivational (e.g. self-efficacy beliefs, attitude, interest) and affective (e.g. feelings of connection, relief of frustration) changes that result from observing or socially interacting with anthropomorphic agents that are instantiated in the role of social models. Building upon Nass's paradigm of computers as social actors, there is significant evidence that humans can be socially influenced by such agents just as they would be by human social models (Nass & Steuer 1993; Reeves & Nass 1996; Nass & Moon 2000; Baylor 2007; Ebbers 2007; Kim & Baylor 2007a; Lee et al. 2007).
Further, there are several advantages to implementing anthropomorphic agents as social models. First, an agent implemented as a social model can be available when it is most convenient or timely for a user, online or through a computer-based application. Given the difficulties in arranging for a human mentor to coach another to lose weight, or for a teenage idol to persuade youth against joining gangs, the availability of an always-live agent as a social model is advantageous. Second, perhaps more valuable than availability, is the capability of customizing a computer-based social model to represent an ideal social model for a particular user or group of users. For example, an agent social model designed to positively influence inner-city youth to stay in school could be designed to appear as an older ‘cool’ peer, with respect to dress, age, race, gender and socio-economic status, with a persona that represents someone with whom they can identify as part of their in-group. This first step of designing how the agent will appear is critical (and is a focus of this paper). Other design features include providing an appropriate voice (Nass & Brave 2005), engaging non-verbal communication (e.g. deictic gestures and emotional expressions) (Baylor & Kim 2009) and establishing and maintaining rapport between the agent and the participant (Gratch et al. 2007). For additional functionality, the agent could also respond and interact adaptively through cognitive, emotional and motivational models of the user (e.g. de Rosis et al. 2003; Prendinger & Ishizuka 2005; de Melo & Paiva 2006; Swartout et al. 2006; Gratch et al. 2007; Arellano et al. 2008; McQuiggan et al. 2008), or interact with natural language, all of which can add to its believability.
The advantages of accessibility in real time and customization are significant, but not if considerable time is required to construct these personalized agent social models. Interestingly, while an agent with greater computational functionality may be perceived as more believable, research indicates that learners perceive, interact socially with and are influenced by anthropomorphic agents even when their functionality and adaptability are limited (e.g. Baylor et al. 2004, 2005a, 2006; Baylor 2005; Baylor & Plant 2005; Baylor & Kim 2005a, 2009; Ryu & Baylor 2005; Kim & Baylor 2006, 2007b; Guadagno et al. 2007; Kim et al. 2007; Rosenberg-Kima et al. 2007, 2008). Consequently, designing agents as social models is within the reach of many educators, health promoters and others who wish to promote new attitudes, change behaviours or positively influence others.
The focus of this paper is on two of the most influential features of an agent social model—its visual presence within the computational system and its appearance. These features are critical for designing an effective agent as a social model regardless of its underlying level of computational functionality.
2. Visual presence of the agent
(a). It must be seen, not only heard
Before delving into implementing agents as social models, it is important to first consider the role of the agent's visual presence. The visual presence of an agent can enhance one's perception that ‘someone’ is socially present and collaborating in the same space (e.g. Heeter 1995; Nam et al. 2008). Research confirms that for motivational and affective outcomes in particular, the visual presence of an agent is critical; in other words, a voice alone (human or machine generated) with the same persuasive message is not sufficient. Baylor & Ryu (2003) found that providing an anthropomorphic agent (either static or animated) together with a human voice led to greater learner perceptions of agent credibility in the context of a learning environment. More recently, in a two-way between-subjects design of agent gender (male, female) and agent presence (visible, invisible), controlling for agent attractiveness and voice (prosody, tone), it was found that college students who interacted with visible agents reported significantly greater positive motivational outcomes. Specifically, effect sizes indicated a large effect on students' beliefs in utility of the topic, a large effect on student self-efficacy in success at the topic and a moderate effect on their interest in the topic (Rosenberg-Kima et al. 2007).
In other research, undergraduate students were purposefully frustrated through an obstacle—an error message pop-up window that interfered with their answering of web-based survey items (Baylor et al. 2005b; Baylor & Rosenberg-Kima 2006). During their attempts to close and/or move the window, an interface agent ‘Survey Sam’ was either visible (figure 1) or invisible. Following the frustrating event, an apologetic or empathetic message was delivered (by the agent, or within a text box). Results indicated that the agent-delivered message was significantly more effective at mitigating participants' frustration than the message delivered in a text box, with a large effect size (d > 1.0). Specifically, when the agent delivered the message, participants tended to attribute the cause of their frustration to the computer system instead of to themselves.
Further, this simple five-sentence agent intervention not only reassured users that they were not at fault but also led to positive attitudes towards the experience (e.g. they were more likely to suggest the study to a friend). This suggests that delivery of a frustration-mitigating message via an anthropomorphic agent may be more effective than simple text-based feedback.
(b). When more than one agent is visually present
A unique affordance of multiple anthropomorphic agents is the split-persona effect, which indicates that splitting agent roles/functionality into two distinct agent personas is preferable to combining those roles/functionality into one agent persona (Baylor & Ebbers 2003). In the context of learning, three anthropomorphic agents were designed to represent the distinct roles of motivator (e.g. providing confidence-boosting messages), expert (e.g. providing informational support) and mentor (combining both the motivator and expert roles) (see Baylor & Kim 2005b).
In multiple studies with agents of different gender and race, this effect indicates that students learned significantly more and had significantly greater motivation when working with the two motivator and expert agents as compared to working with the one mentor agent (figure 2). This can be explained by the fact that it is easier for students to figuratively ‘compartmentalize’ the agent information when it was delivered by two distinct sources. With respect to motivational design, this effect suggests that having an affective or motivational coach implemented as a distinct agent may be preferable to having its functionality figuratively ‘rolled up’ into a more inclusive help or training agent.
Alternatively, instead of interacting with multiple anthropomorphic agents, could affective support be enhanced when a student's avatar (i.e. his/her self-representation within the program) co-learns with a computer-driven agent? Lee et al. (2007) conducted an experimental study with 76 Japanese college students in three conditions: a caring co-learner agent (expressed empathic emotion and provided supportive and encouraging verbal feedback—figure 3), a non-caring co-learner agent (did not manifest caring orientations towards the participants, although all the other behavioural features were identical) and a control (no co-learner agent). As compared to students in the other two conditions, results indicated that students who worked with the caring co-learner agent had significantly greater feelings of social support (an index comprising five items: ‘not alone’, ‘praised’, ‘attended’, ‘appreciated’ and ‘supported’), trust and enhanced recall.
In a different type of agent instantiation, Ebbers (2007) explored the roles of virtual agents serving as ‘mastery models’ (demonstrating positive attitudes towards the task and/or desired levels of performance so that a learner can learn vicariously) or as ‘coping models’ (modelling for the learners on how to cope with a situation as a novice) (e.g. Schunk et al. 1987). In an experimental study with 103 undergraduate students, she manipulated type of agent model (coping versus mastery) and interaction type (direct versus vicarious) and found a main effect where the agent as a coping model positively impacted learner motivation, self-efficacy and attitude. Similarly, Kim & Baylor (2006) experimentally found that agents that had similar competencies to learners were more influential in enhancing student self-efficacy beliefs than highly competent agents. These results are aligned with Bandura's (1997) concept of attribute similarity, that it is desirable to have similar personal characteristics of the learner and social model.
Overall, these studies suggest that having a visually present agent is desirable for enhancing motivation and positive affect (e.g. frustration mitigation, feelings of support). Based on the split-persona effect, it may be preferable for a motivational agent (e.g. as a coach or persuader) to be employed as a separate and distinct agent instead of adding motivational functionality to existing agents within a system. In a learning environment, if the agent is caring and/or reflects a ‘coping model’ role, motivational and affective results can be enhanced.
3. Impact of agent appearance
Once the visual and social presence of an anthropomorphic interface agent is established, one of the most important factors in its design as a social model is appearance. As supported by research with humans, a social model's appearance is key to influencing another's motivation, attitude and future behaviours through directly impacting message acceptance and self-efficacy (confidence) beliefs (Bandura 1997).
(a). Agent gender and race/ethnicity
Building on social psychology research that indicates that humans are more persuaded by a person from a member of their in-group, work with anthropomorphic agents has also shown that observers tend to be more influenced by an agent of the same gender (e.g. Baylor & Kim 2004; Baylor 2005; Guadagno et al. 2007; Kim et al. 2007) and same ethnicity/race (Baylor & Kim 2004; Gulz et al. 2007; Pratt et al. 2007; Plant et al. in press). However, these generalizations are context dependent. For example, if Caucasian students are randomly assigned a Black agent as an ‘expert’, it may surprise them with respect to their expectations, leading them to have greater self-efficacy (confidence) and interest towards the topic (Baylor & Kim 2004). In contrast, it may be particularly valuable for Black students to work with agents of the same race as they tend to have strong affiliations with same-race agents, both when they are given a choice and also when it is randomly assigned (Baylor & Kim 2003; Baylor et al. 2003). However, with respect to learning, this affiliation may be detrimental as Black students may actually perform better with different-race agents (Moreno & Flowerday 2006). When interacting with an agent who attempted to convince them of the merits of engineering as a career, middle-school students (male and female) were significantly more persuaded and reported greater positive attitudes towards engineering from a female (as opposed to male) agent (Plant et al. 2009). This suggests that for younger students, female agents may be more powerful role/social models overall, perhaps because of both parental influences and the fact that most schoolteachers are female.
(b). Other appearance features: attractiveness, ‘coolness’, age
Moving beyond the effects of agent gender and race/ethnicity, Baylor and colleagues (Baylor & Plant 2005; Rosenberg-Kima et al. 2008; Plant et al. 2009) manipulated other appearance features such as agent attractiveness, ‘coolness’ (operationalized by clothing and hairstyle) and age to investigate their influence on students' motivation towards engineering as a possible career. When given a choice of 16 validated agents varying with respect to attractiveness, ‘coolness’, age and gender (figure 4), undergraduate women were significantly more likely to choose the female, attractive, young, cool agent as ‘most like themselves’ and also as the agent they ‘most wanted to be like’. However, they tended to select ‘male, older, uncool’ agents as most like engineers (confirming the stereotype of an engineer), and tended to choose to ‘learn about engineering’ from agents who were male and attractive, but uncool.
After receiving a 15 minute persuasive message from the chosen agent (figure 5), students' attitudes and motivation towards engineering were positively impacted as compared to a control group.
Next, given that the attractive agents were most influential as social models in this choice study, a large-scale experimental study was conducted with the eight attractive agents for undergraduate women (Rosenberg-Kima et al. 2008). Results revealed a main effect for gender where participants reported more positive stereotypes of engineering after interacting with a female agent, perhaps because it challenged their existing beliefs of a typical engineer. In contrast, participants interacting with a male agent reported that engineering was more useful and engaging. An interaction of ‘coolness’ and age indicated that agents who were young and ‘cool’ (i.e. peer-like; similar to participants) and agents who were old and ‘uncool’ (stereotypical engineers) were both most effective in enhancing student self-efficacy for engineering; thus, for self-efficacy, it appears that either the perception of similarity or expertise increased the effectiveness of the agent. Overall the most effective agent social model was the young, attractive, cool, female agent; this agent enhanced students' self-efficacy towards being successful as an engineer and positively influenced their negative stereotypes towards engineering.
In general, these studies suggest that individuals are more influenced by agents who are similar to themselves with respect to appearance-related characteristics (e.g. Bailenson et al. 2008). However, individuals' prior stereotypes and expectations can sometimes be used to a positive advantage depending on the desired outcome. For example, in the case of influencing women's motivation towards engineering, it was found that male agents were more influential than female agents in promoting the usefulness of engineering and potential engagement with it as a career. This is probably due to participants' existing stereotypes where they tended to believe a message from a male engineer is more credible than the same message from a female engineer. Consequently, the context of the persuasion (e.g. participants' prior knowledge, topic area) as well as the desired outcomes (e.g. interest, attitude change) must always be front and central when designing an agent's appearance.
In the next section, the focus on agent appearance will come from a different perspective—that of an avatar that physically represents a user in an online environment. While an avatar's appearance may or may not authentically reflect the user's actual appearance, it can still have a powerful impact in influencing his/her beliefs and behaviour in the real world.
4. Avatars that represent (and potentially impact) one's possible self
Virtual environments (such as web-based chat rooms) or gaming environments (such as the Wii) allow one to easily create a personalized avatar to represent his/her virtual ‘self’. Here, the user can choose to create an appearance and physical representation that reflects reality or not. Of interest is the general question—is it better for one's avatar to match the reality of who she sees herself to be, or rather as who she aspires herself to be? In other words, as we change our self-representation, do our self-representations change our real behaviour and beliefs in turn (Yee & Bailenson 2007)? These questions have been systematically investigated by Jeremy Bailenson and colleagues, and the answer is strongly affirmative. His research has shown that the way in which people are represented in virtual environments can substantially change their verbal, non-verbal and task-related behaviour.
In one line of research, the potential effect of an avatar representing one's ‘possible’ self was investigated. For example, if users watch an avatar that looks like themselves exercising and losing weight in a virtual environment, they will subsequently exercise more and eat more healthy in the real world as compared to a control group (Fox & Bailenson 2009). In a different study, participants were exposed to an avatar representing themselves running on a treadmill, another avatar running or an avatar representing themselves loitering. Within the 24 h after the experiment, participants who were exposed to the avatar running that represented themselves exercised significantly more than those in other conditions. Similarly, if college-aged students observe their avatar ageing in a virtual mirror, they form a psychological connection to their ‘future selves’ and decide to invest more money in a retirement account as compared to a control group (Ersner-Hershfield et al. 2008). These studies extend prior work on ‘self-perception theory’ (Bem 1967), showing that changes in aspects of one's virtually represented self can lead to changes in one's real physical self.
In a different line of research, they investigated how people tend to conform to how their avatar appears, regardless of how it is perceived by others—a process referred to as the Proteus effect (Yee & Bailenson 2007). In one study, they manipulated the attractiveness of the avatars and observed participants' behaviours. Participants with more attractive avatars demonstrated increased self-disclosure, friendliness and extroversion and were more willing to approach opposite-gendered strangers after less than 1 min of acting through their ‘altered’ avatar. In contrast, participants with an unattractive avatar kept a significantly farther distance away from strangers (5 feet versus 3.5 feet) and shared significantly less personal details. In a second study, participants with taller avatars behaved more confidently in a negotiation task than participants with shorter avatars; specifically, they were more willing to make unfair splits in negotiation tasks. In contrast, participants with shorter avatars were more willing to accept unfair offers than those who had taller avatars. As indicated by the authors, ‘These two studies show the dramatic and almost instantaneous effect that avatars have on behaviour in digital environments’ (p. 285). More recently, the authors have extended this work beyond the more controlled laboratory setting to an actual online community (Yee et al. 2009). In this more ecologically valid context, they found that both the height and attractiveness of an avatar in an online game were significant predictors of the player's performance. Further, behavioural changes originating within the virtual environment tended to transfer to subsequent face-to-face interactions. Participants were placed in an immersive virtual environment and were given either shorter or taller avatars. They then interacted with a human confederate for about 15 min. In addition to causing a behavioural difference within the virtual environment, the authors found that participants given taller avatars negotiated more aggressively in the subsequent face-to-face interaction with the confederate than participants given shorter avatars.
In both cases—an autonomous interface agent or an avatar that represents one's self—the significance of the agent's appearance is clear. However, there is limited research to suggest what role appearance plays, over time, in a longer-term human–agent relationship. For example, is it beneficial for one's avatar to gain weight when the human does? Future research needs to examine more carefully the role of appearance when the human–agent/avatar social relationship persists over time (see Bickmore & Picard 2004).
5. Conclusion
Placing an anthropomorphic agent as the front-end for a motivational system requires careful attention to design. In general, while having such an agent visually present to deliver persuasive or soothing messages can be beneficial, there are several considerations regarding how to physically instantiate it and how to design its appearance. With respect to the agent's visual presence, multiple agents may be preferable (e.g. in a learning system, where motivational support is best kept separate from instructional information) or the implementation of a ‘companion’ peer agent (e.g. instantiated as a co-learner or as a coping model) may be beneficial. The appearance of the agent(s) as a social model is a key factor for its success in promoting motivation and should be carefully considered. While providing a social model from the same in-group as the user is generally advantageous, there are certain contexts where the opposite may be better (e.g. Black students may learn more when the agent is White). Along this line, providing users with a choice of agents is generally unwise as users tend to choose agents who are not the most beneficial for them (Baylor et al. 2003; Baylor & Plant 2005; Moreno & Flowerday 2006). Overall, though, there are great possibilities to customize an agent so that it can serve as an effective social model.
When we design avatars to represent ourselves within virtual worlds and gaming environments, their appearance has considerable impact on our virtual and real-world behaviour. This blending of our real and virtual selves is powerful and exciting, and future research should address the motivational and affective impacts.
Acknowledgements
This manuscript was supported as independent research while the author was serving at the National Science Foundation in Arlington, VA, USA. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.
Footnotes
One contribution of 17 to a Discussion Meeting Issue ‘Computation of emotions in man and machines’.
References
- Arellano D., Varona J., Perales F. J.2008Generation and visualization of emotional states in virtual characters. Comput. Anim. Virt. Worlds 19, 259–270 (doi:10.1002/cav.234) [Google Scholar]
- Bailenson J. N., Blascovich J., Guadagno R. E.2008Self-representations in immersive virtual environments. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 38, 2673–2690 [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A.1986Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA: Prentice-Hall [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A.1997Self-efficacy: the exercise of control New York, NY: W. H. Freeman [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L.2005The impact of pedagogical agent image on affective outcomes. Proc. Workshop ‘Affective Interactions: The Computer in the affective Loop’ at the 10th Int. Conf. on Intelligent User Interfaces, pp. 1–6 New York, NY: ACM Press [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L.2007Pedagogical agents as a social interface. Educ. Technol. 47, 11–14 [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Ebbers S. J.2003Evidence that multiple agents facilitate greater learning In AI-ED Sydney, Australia [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Kim S.2009Designing nonverbal communication for pedagogical agents: when less is more. Comput. Hum. Behav. 25, 450–457 (doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.008) [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Kim Y.2003The role of gender and ethnicity in pedagogical agent perception. In Proc. World Conf. on E-learning in Corporate, Government, Healthcare, & Higher Education 2003 (ed. Richards G.), pp. 1503–1506 Chesapeake, VA: AACE [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Kim Y.2004Pedagogical agent design: the impact of agent realism, gender, ethnicity, and instructional role. In Lecture notes in computer science: intelligent tutoring systems, vol. 3320 (eds Lester J. C., Vicari R. M., Paraguacu F.), pp. 592–603 Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Kim Y.2005aSimulating instructional roles through pedagogical agents. Int. J. Artif. Intell. Educ. 15, 95–115 (doi:10.1145/1067860.1067867) [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Kim Y.2005bSimulating instructional roles through pedagogical agents. Int. J. Artif. Intell. Educ. 15, 95–115 [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Plant E. A.2005Pedagogical agents as social models for engineering: the influence of appearance on female choice. In Artificial intelligence in education: supporting learning through intelligent and socially informed technology, vol. 125 (eds Looi C.-K., McCalla G., Bredeweg B., Breuker J.), pp. 65–72 Bristol, UK: IOS Press [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Rosenberg-Kima R. B.2006Interface agents to alleviate online frustration. Proc. Seventh Int. Conf. on Learning Sciences, pp. 30–36 Bloomington, IN: ISLS [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Ryu J.2003The effects of image and animation in enhancing pedagogical agent persona. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 28, 373–395 [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Rosenberg-Kima R. B., Plant E. A.2006Interface agents as social models: the impact of appearance on females' attitude toward engineering. CHI 2006, Conf. on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Montreal, Canada [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Shen E., Huang X.2003Which pedagogical agent do learners choose? The effects of gender and ethnicity. In Proc. World Conf. on E-Learning in Corporate, Government, Healthcare, & Higher Education 2003 (ed. Richards G.), pp. 1507–1510 Chesapeake, VA: AACE [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Shen E., Warren D.2004Supporting learners with math anxiety: the impact of pedagogical agent emotional and motivational support. Proc. Workshop on Social and Emotional Intelligence in Learning Environments at the Int. Conf. on Intelligent Tutoring Systems (ITS), Maceio, Brazil, pp. 6–12 Berlin, Germany: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Warren D., Park C. H., Shen E., Perez R.2005aThe impact of frustration-mitigating messages delivered by an interface agent. In Artificial intelligence in education: supporting learning through intelligent and socially informed technology, vol. 125 (eds Looi C.-K., McCalla G., Bredeweg B., Breuker J.), pp. 73–79 Bristol, UK: IOS Press [Google Scholar]
- Baylor A. L., Warren D., Park S., Shen E., Perez R.2005bThe impact of frustration-mitigating messages delivered by an interface agent. Proc. AI-ED (Artificial Intelligence in Education)
- Bem D. J.1967Self-perception: an alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychol. Rev. 74, 183–200 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickmore T., Picard R.2004Establishing and maintaining long-term human–computer relationships. Trans. Comput.—Hum. Interact. 12, 293–327 [Google Scholar]
- de Melo C., Paiva A.2006Multimodal expression in virtual humans. Comput. Anim. Virtual Worlds 17, 239–248 (doi:10.1002/cav.127) [Google Scholar]
- de Rosis F., Pelachaud C., Poggi I., Carofiglio V., De Carolis B.2003From Greta's mind to her face: modelling the dynamics of affective states in a conversational embodied agent. Int. J. Hum.—Comput. Stud. 59, 81–118 (doi:10.1016/s1071-5819(03)00020-x) [Google Scholar]
- Ebbers S. J.2007The impact of social model agent type (coping, mastery) and social interaction type (vicarious, direct) on learner motivation, attitudes, social comparisons, affect and learning performance. Doctoral dissertation, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, USA. (http://etd.lib.fsu.edu/theses/available/etd-07092007-151016/)
- Ersner-Hershfield H., Bailenson J., Carstensen L. L.2008A vivid future self: immersive virtual reality enhances retirement saving Chicago, IL: Association for Psychological Science [Google Scholar]
- Fox J., Bailenson J. N.2009Virtual self-modeling: the effects of vicarious reinforcement and identification on exercise behaviors. Media Psychol. 12, 1–25 (doi:10.1080/15213260802669474) [Google Scholar]
- Gratch J., Wang N., Gerten J., Fast E., Duffy R.2007Creating rapport with virtual agents. Lecture notes in artificial intelligence: Proc. Int. Conf. on Intelligent Virtual Agents Paris, France: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Guadagno R. E., Blascovich J., Bailenson J. N., McCall C.2007Virtual humans and persuasion: the effects of agency and behavioral realism. Media Psychol. 10, 1–22 [Google Scholar]
- Gulz A., Haake M., Tärning B.2007Visual gender and its motivational and cognitive effects—a user study. Lund Univ. Cogn. Stud. 137, 1–22 [Google Scholar]
- Heeter C.1995Communication research on consumer VR. In Communication in the age of virtual reality (eds Biocca F., Levy M.). Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y., Baylor A. L.2006Pedagogical agents as learning companions: the role of agent competency and type of interaction. Etr&D-Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 54, 223–243 [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y., Baylor A. L.2007aPedagogical agents as social models to influence learner attitudes. Educ. Technol. 47, 23–28 [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y., Baylor A. L.2007bPedagogical agents as social models to influence learner attitudes. Educ. Technol. 47, 23–28 [Google Scholar]
- Kim Y., Baylor A. L., Shen E.2007Pedagogical agents as learning companions: the impact of agent emotion and gender. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 23, 220–234 (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2006.00210.x) [Google Scholar]
- Lee J.-E. R., Nass C., Brave S., Morishima Y., Nakajima H., Yamada R.2007The case for caring co-learners: the effects of a computer-mediated co-learner agent on trust and learning. J. Commun. 57, 183–204 [Google Scholar]
- McQuiggan S. W., Rowe J. P., Lester J. C.2008The effects of empathetic virtual characters on presence in narrative-centered learning environments. CHI, Florence, Italy [Google Scholar]
- Moreno R., Flowerday T.2006Students' choice of animated pedagogical agents in science learning: a test of the similarity-attraction hypothesis on gender and ethnicity. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 31, 186–207 (doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.05.002) [Google Scholar]
- Nam C., Shu J., Chung D.2008The roles of sensory modalities in collaborative virtual environment (CVEs). Comput. Hum. Behav. 24, 1404–1417 [Google Scholar]
- Nass C., Brave S.2005Wired for speech: how voice activates and advances the human–computer relationship Cambridge, MA: MIT Press [Google Scholar]
- Nass C., Moon Y.2000Machines and mindlessness: social responses to computers. J. Soc. Issues 56, 81–103 [Google Scholar]
- Nass C., Steuer J.1993Anthropomorphism, agency, and thopoeia: computers as social actors. Hum. Commun. Res. 19, 504–527 [Google Scholar]
- Plant E. A., Doerr C., Rosenberg-Kima R., Baylor A. L.In press The influence of computer-based model's race and gender on female students' attitudes and beliefs towards engineering. J. Engin. Educ. [Google Scholar]
- Plant E. A., Baylor A. L., Doerr C., Rosenberg-Kima R.2009Changing middle-school students' attitudes and performance regarding engineering with computer-based social models. Comput. Educ. 53, 209–215 [Google Scholar]
- Pratt J. A., Hauser K., Ugray Z., Patterson O.2007Looking at human–computer interface design: effects of ethnicity in computer agents. Interact. Comput. 19, 512–523 (doi:10.1016/j.intcom.2007.02.003) [Google Scholar]
- Prendinger H., Ishizuka M.2005The empathic companion: a character-based interface that addresses users' affective states. Appl. Artif. Intell. 19, 267–285 (doi:10.1080/08839510590910174) [Google Scholar]
- Reeves B., Nass C.1996The media equation Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg-Kima R., Baylor A. L., Plant E. A., Doerr C.2007The importance of interface agent visual presence: voice alone is less effective in impacting young women's attitudes toward engineering. Persuasive 2007, vol. 4744, pp 214–222 Stanford, CA: Springer [Google Scholar]
- Rosenberg-Kima R. B., Baylor A. L., Plant E. A., Doerr C. E.2008Interface agents as social models for female students: the effects of agent visual presence and appearance on female students' attitudes and beliefs. Comput. Hum. Behav. 24, 2741–2756 (doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.03.017) [Google Scholar]
- Ryu J., Baylor A. L.2005The psychometric structure of pedagogical agent persona. Technol. Instruct. Cogn. Learn. (TICL) 2, 291–315 [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H.1981Modeling and attributional effects on children's achievement: a self-efficacy analysis. J. Educ. Psychol. 73, 93–105 (doi:10.1037/0022-0663.73.1.93) [Google Scholar]
- Schunk D. H., Hanson A. R., Cox P. D.1987Peer model attributes and children's achievement behaviors. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 54–61 (doi:10.1037/0022-0663.79.1.54) [Google Scholar]
- Swartout W., Gratch J., Hill R. W., Hovy E., Marsella S., Rickel J., Traum D.2006Toward virtual humans. Ai Mag. 27, 96–108 [Google Scholar]
- Yee N., Bailenson J. N.2007The proteus effect: the effect of transformed self-representation on behavior. Hum. Commun. Res. 33, 271–290 (doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00299.x) [Google Scholar]
- Yee N., Bailenson J. N., Ducheneaut N.2009The Proteus effect implications of transformed digital self-representation on online and offline behavior. Commun. Res. 36, 285–312 (doi:10.1177/0093650208330254) [Google Scholar]