Skip to main content
Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN logoLink to Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN
. 2009 Nov;34(6):418–432.

Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies

Paolo Fusar-Poli 1,, Anna Placentino 1, Francesco Carletti 1, Paola Landi 1, Paul Allen 1, Simon Surguladze 1, Francesco Benedetti 1, Marta Abbamonte 1, Roberto Gasparotti 1, Francesco Barale 1, Jorge Perez 1, Philip McGuire 1, Pierluigi Politi 1
PMCID: PMC2783433  PMID: 19949718

Abstract

Background

Most of our social interactions involve perception of emotional information from the faces of other people. Furthermore, such emotional processes are thought to be aberrant in a range of clinical disorders, including psychosis and depression. However, the exact neurofunctional maps underlying emotional facial processing are not well defined.

Methods

Two independent researchers conducted separate comprehensive PubMed (1990 to May 2008) searches to find all functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using a variant of the emotional faces paradigm in healthy participants. The search terms were: “fMRI AND happy faces,” “fMRI AND sad faces,” “fMRI AND fearful faces,” “fMRI AND angry faces,” “fMRI AND disgusted faces” and “fMRI AND neutral faces.” We extracted spatial coordinates and inserted them in an electronic database. We performed activation likelihood estimation analysis for voxel-based meta-analyses.

Results

Of the originally identified studies, 105 met our inclusion criteria. The overall database consisted of 1785 brain coordinates that yielded an overall sample of 1600 healthy participants. Quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis of brain activation provided neurofunctional maps for 1) main effect of human faces; 2) main effect of emotional valence; and 3) modulatory effect of age, sex, explicit versus implicit processing and magnetic field strength. Processing of emotional faces was associated with increased activation in a number of visual, limbic, temporoparietal and prefrontal areas; the putamen; and the cerebellum. Happy, fearful and sad faces specifically activated the amygdala, whereas angry or disgusted faces had no effect on this brain region. Furthermore, amygdala sensitivity was greater for fearful than for happy or sad faces. Insular activation was selectively reported during processing of disgusted and angry faces. However, insular sensitivity was greater for disgusted than for angry faces. Conversely, neural response in the visual cortex and cerebellum was observable across all emotional conditions.

Limitations

Although the activation likelihood estimation approach is currently one of the most powerful and reliable meta-analytical methods in neuroimaging research, it is insensitive to effect sizes.

Conclusion

Our study has detailed neurofunctional maps to use as normative references in future fMRI studies of emotional facial processing in psychiatric populations. We found selective differences between neural networks underlying the basic emotions in limbic and insular brain regions.

Introduction

Humans, like other primates,1 are intensely social creatures and their lives are intertwined with those of other people. Most of our social interactions involve recognizing other people’s identities, actions, emotions and intentions. Much of this information is available from their facial expressions. Facial expressions are powerful nonverbal displays of emotion that signal valence information to others and contain information that is vital in the complex social world.2 Recognizing facial expressions permits us to detect another person’s emotional state and provides cues on how to respond in these social interactions.3,4 Some basic emotions can be most reliably recognized from facial expressions (i.e., fear, disgust, anger, happiness, sadness) and have been shown to be universal in their performance and perception.5 Facial perception is defined as “any higher-level visual processing of faces,”6 which involves both perceptual processing — identifying the geometric configuration of facial features to discriminate among different stimuli on the basis of their appearance — and recognition of the emotional meaning of a stimulus.5 Thus, facial emotion perception combines current visual sensory input with retrievable memory and is an important inherited ability evident since the neonatal stages.4

Given the crucial role played by human emotional faces processing in social function, over the past 2 decades affective neurosciences have shown an intense interest in understanding the neural mechanisms that support face perception.7 In particular, functional brain imaging techniques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), which allows the in vivo investigation of the human brain, have been employed to address the neurophysiological substrates of emotional processing. Despite the growing number of fMRI studies in the field, when taken separately these individual imaging studies indicate contrasting findings8 and are unable to definitively characterize what brain regions are associated with each specific emotional condition. Although methodological factors such as different task designs, imaging methods and analysis may be a source of heterogeneity across studies, the major limitations of current literature are the small sample sizes and the associated low statistical power of most fMRI studies. Furthermore, the modulatory effect of other confounding factors such as age,913 sex1417 and type of emotional processing (i.e., explicit or implicit)1822 is not completely clear.

The above variations have made it particularly difficult to interpret the differences in activation patterns found in clinical populations such as people with depression or psychosis. Thus, the analysis of the consistency and convergence of results across experiments is a crucial prerequisite for correct generalizations about human brain functions.8 Without normative maps, it is difficult to definitively ascertain which of the fMRI alterations observed in depressed or psychotic patients are due to neurobiological disruptions and which are due to other external methodological inconsistencies. Although previous reviews2330 of facial emotional processing and some meta-analyses31,32 of functional neuroanatomy of emotions have been published, to our knowledge no activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of fMRI studies during facial emotional processing is currently available. In coordinate-based meta-analyses such as the activation likelihood estimation, activation coordinates reported from independently performed imaging experiments are analyzed in search of functional cortical areas that are relevant for the investigated function.8 Therefore, activation foci do not represent single points but rather localization probability distributions centered on the particular coordinates. In activation likelihood estimation, the foci reported by each study are modelled as a probability distribution. For these reasons, the activation likelihood estimation approach is currently one of the most powerful and reliable meta-analytical methods in imaging research and offers consistent advantages over other approaches.

The aim of our study was to clarify the participation of the different neural regions and networks in the processing of human emotional faces. In line with this premise, the first purpose of this meta-analysis was to provide a normative fMRI atlas of human faces processing in a standard stereotactic space.33 The second was to clarify what brain structures participate in the processing of different types of basic emotional faces (i.e., sadness, fear, happiness, disgust). Specifically, we wanted to clarify whether basic emotional faces were 1) encoded in specific brain regions or 2) were a property of the whole brain neural network or 3) whether there were overlapping neural networks processing different facial emotions. Finally, we aimed at addressing the modulatory effect of age, sex and implicit or explicit processing on the neurophysiological response to emotional faces.

Methods

Emotional faces paradigms

Emotion research uses different types of stimuli (e.g., Ekman faces and Gur faces) to probe affective processing. Facial expressions portraying specific emotions (e.g., happiness, sadness, anger, fear) are universally recognized. Even though facial expressions are used frequently as probes of emotion recognition, some studies have shown that faces can be inducers of emotion.34 The Ekman 60 Faces Test uses a range of photographs from the Ekman and Friesen series of Pictures of Facial Affect.35 The Ekman faces have been the most widely used series of photographs. However, facial stimuli usually encountered are typically of a restricted race and age range and they are mostly 2-dimensional (2-D) photographs. Since 2-D stimuli are not amenable to manipulations of angle and orientation and raise methodological concerns when applied to examination of facial asymmetries that could be related to hemispheric specialization, Gur and colleagues36 developed a set of 3-dimensional (3-D) images to overcome these limitations.

Selection of studies and contrasts for analyses

Two independent researchers (P.F.-P., P.L.) conducted a 2-step literature search. First, they conducted 2 separate comprehensive MEDLINE (January 1990 to May 2008) searches in the English-language literature to identify putative functional neuroimaging (fMRI) studies employing a variant of the emotional faces paradigm that had reported data on healthy participants. The search terms entered in MEDLINE were “fMRI AND happy faces,” “fMRI AND sad faces,” “fMRI AND fearful faces,” “fMRI AND angry faces,” “fMRI AND disgusted faces” and “fMRI AND neutral faces.” To qualify for inclusion in the review, studies must have 1) been an original paper published in a peer-reviewed journal, 2) used a variant of the emotional faces paradigm in healthy participants, 3) studied participants using fMRI, 4) used the image subtraction methodology to determine activation/deactivation foci (e.g., happy v. fixation or happy v. neutral) and 5) reported data in standard stereotactic coordinates (either Talairach or Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] space).37,38 The 2 researchers assessed the inclusion criteria by consensus. In a second step, we checked the reference lists of the articles included in the review for relevant studies not identified by computerized literature searching. Finally, we contacted authors of studies where Talairach or MNI coordinates were not reported to reduce the possibility of a biased sample set.

We included studies reporting single group data (i.e., healthy volunteers only). Thus, we did not consider spatial coordinates reporting a main effect of emotional faces processing across the control and a clinical group, nor did we consider coordinates relative to functional, psychophysiological or psychopathological correlations. We did not include fMRI studies investigating processes other than emotional processing (i.e., working memory,39 attention) by using similar emotional faces stimuli. To avoid an unwanted systematic confounding effect, we excluded other types of emotional faces such as schematic faces. Finally, we excluded positron emission tomography studies to prevent the methodological heterogeneity underlying these different functional imaging techniques.

To assist the reader in forming an independent view of the results, we collected meta-analytical data such as mean age, sex, intensity of magnet, type of analysis (whole brain or region of interest [ROI]) and type of emotional facial paradigms employed across all studies (Table 1). Specifically, when a study included young/old subgroups, different ages were listed for each specific contrast. The emotional faces category included specific paradigms other than the Ekman/Gur faces or a mixture of the original Ekman/Gur stimuli and unstandardized sets of emotional human faces. We coded the procedure as ROI only when specific ROI procedures (i.e., MarsBar) were applied to extract the blood oxygenation–level dependent response in selected brain areas.

Table 1.

Studies included in the meta-analysis

Year; study Magnetic field, T Sample
Faces paradigm
No. Female Age, yr*
2008
Wolfensberger et al.40 1.5 32 18 30 Ekman
Deeley et al.10 1.5 40 0 24 Ekman
Payer et al.41 3 12 3 32 Ekman
De Martino et al.42 1.5 15 NR NR Emotional
Surguladze et al.43 1.5 29 12 38 Ekman
Bryant et al.44 1.5 15 8 36 Gur
Seitz et al.45 1.5 14 7 29 Ekman
Loughead et al.46 4 17 6 27 Gur
Miskowiak et al.47 1.5 24 8 27 Ekman
Hermans et al.48 1.5 12 12 23 Emotional
Andersson et al.49 1.5 16 9 15 Emotional
2007
Pavuluri et al.50 3 10 5 14 Gur
Coccaro et al.51 3 10 5 31 Ekman
Hessl et al.52 1.5 13 0 40 Emotional
Dannlowski et al.53 3 23 11 39 Ekman
Habel et al.18 3 14 14 24 Emotional
Deeley et al.54 1.5 9 0 27 Ekman
Salloum et al.55 1.5 11 0 36 Ekman
Anderson et al.56 1.5 12 0 25 Ekman
Rauch et al.57 3 20 10 28 Ekman
Ashwin et al.58 3 13 0 26 Emotional
Gur et al.59 4 17 5 25 Gur
Miskowiak et al.60 1.5 24 10 24 Ekman
Kleinhans et al.61 1.5 22 1 25 Emotional
Fusar-Poli et al.62 1.5 10 2 26 Ekman
Pelphrey et al.63 1.5 8 2 24 Ekman
Benuzzi et al.64 3 24 12 26 Ekman
2006
Lee et al.65 1.5 18 9 26 Emotional
Williams et al.66 1.5 15 8 36 Gur
Bird et al.67 1.5 16 2 35 Ekman
Fitzgerald et al.68 4 20 10 26 Gur
Grosbras and Paus69 1.5 20 10 29 Videoclip
Somerville et al.70 1.5 16 9 19 Emotional
Britton et al.71 3 12 6 22 Gur
Holt et al.72 1.5 16 0 48 Ekman
Taylor et al.73 3 30 18 (18–36) Emotional
Batut et al.74 2 15 9 34 Emotional
Wright et al.75 1.5 18 12 24 Emotional
Sambataro et al.76 3 24 13 27 Ekman
Pessoa et al.77 1.5 37 18 28 Ekman
Williams et al.78 1.5 13 6 35 Gur
Dapretto et al.79 3 9 0 12 Emotional
Wright and Liu80 3 12 6 29 Ekman
2005
Fischer et al.9 1.5 24 12 25 Ekman
Shin et al.81 1.5 13 0 50 Ekman
Noesselt et al.82 3 12 5 23 Emotional
Straube et al.83 1.5 9 5 23 Emotional
Williams et al.84 3 13 9 28 Ekman
Strauss et al.85 1.5 8 0 27 Ekman
Cools et al.86 3 12 0 24 Ekman
Habel et al.87 1.5 26 0 33 Emotional
Reinders et al.88 1.5 15 8 26 Ekman
Moriguchi et al.89 1.5 16 10 29 Emotional
2004
Cannistraro et al.90 1.5 10 6 25 Ekman
Lennox et al.91 3 12 6 33 Ekman
Singer et al.92 1.5 11 5 26 Emotional
Williams et al.93 1.5 22 8 27 Ekman
Phillips et al.94 1.5 8 0 32 Ekman
Vuilleumier et al.95 2 13 6 36 Emotional
Straube et al.96 1.5 10 6 23 Emotional
Glascher et al.97 1.5 13 8 26 Ekman
Nomura et al.98 3 9 5 24 Ekman
Schroeder et al.99 1.5 20 10 33 Ekman
Somerville et al.100 3 16 8 25 Ekman
Etkin et al.101 1.5 26 13 26 Ekman
McClure et al.15 3 34 16 26 Ekman
Bishop et al.102 3 27 20 28 Ekman
2003
Surguladze et al.103 1.5 9 4 40 Ekman
Killgore et al.104 1.5 12 12 24 Emotional
Bradley et al.105 3 18 0 19 Emotional
Yang et al.106 3 12 6 16 Emotional
Wright et al.107 1.5 10 6 29 Ekman
Wicker et al.108 3 14 0 24 Videoclip
Nelson et al.109 3 34 16 22 Ekman
Schwartz et al.110 1.5 8 4 22 Gur
Wright et al.111 1.5 10 5 27 Gur
Lange et al.112 1.5 9 0 29 Ekman
Phillips et al.113 1.5 8 1 32 Ekman
Wild et al.114 1.5 10 5 29 Ekman
2002
Canli et al.115 3 15 11 NR Emotional
Yang et al.116 3 17 11 23 Emotional
Iidaka et al.11 3 24 12 25 Emotional
Gur et al.117 4 14 4 28 Gur
Gur et al.118 4 14 7 27 Emotional
Kosaka et al.119 1.5 12 6 25 Emotional
Hariri et al.120 3 12 6 28 Ekman
2001
Thomas et al.121 1.5 18 6 18 Ekman
Thomas et al.122 1.5 12 5 12 Ekman
Williams et al.123 1.5 11 0 30 Ekman
Narumoto et al.124 3 12 3 27 Ekman
Pine et al.12 1.5 16 NR 22 Ekman
Vuilleumier et al.125 2 12 6 28 Ekman
Gorno-Tempini et al.19 1.5 10 5 27 Ekman
Iidaka et al.126 3 12 6 25 Emotional
Kesler-West et al.127 1.5 21 10 22 Emotional
Whalen et al.128 3 8 4 25 Ekman
2000
Critchley et al.22 1.5 9 0 27 Ekman
Rauch et al.129 1.5 8 0 54 Ekman
1999
Phillips et al.130 1.5 5 ? 30 Ekman
1998
Phillips et al.131 1.5 8 1 32 Ekman
Whalen et al.20 1.5 10 0 24 Ekman
Phillips et al.132 1.5 6 0 37 Ekman
Sprengelmeyer et al.133 2 6 4 24 Ekman
1997
Phillips et al.134 1.5 7 5 27 Ekman
1996
Breiter et al.135 1.5 10 0 27 Ekman

NR = not reported.

*

Age is reported as the mean or (range).

Quantitative meta-analytical voxel-based procedure

Because the Talairach system is defined such that left is negative, we transformed coordinates based on the radiological convention. In addition, we determined the spatial normalization template for each study and converted all foci reported in MNI space to Talairach space with the foci conversion option available in the software used for the meta-analytical procedure. In the past, the Brett transform was used to convert MNI coordinates to Talairach space;136 however, recent findings show that MNI/Talairach coordinate bias associated with reference frame (position and orientation) and scale (brain size) can be substantially reduced using the best-fit MNI-to-Talairach transform.137 This transform has been validated and shown to provide improved fit over the Brett transform.136 We used the Talairach atlas to identify the anatomic landmarks of the activation results.

The main meta-analysis (all faces v. baseline) comprised all coordinates of emotional faces task-related activation reported in the primary literature, regardless of task variation or stimuli type. To distinguish the neural networks underlying specific emotional condition, we subdivided coordinates for the meta-analysis into different sub-groups: 1) neutral > baseline, 2) happy > baseline, 3) sad > baseline, 4) angry > baseline, 5) fearful > baseline and 6) disgusted > baseline. We defined baseline condition as fixation of a crosshair on the screen. Furthermore, to clarify what areas were active in response to each emotion in particular, we computed the following contrasts: 1) happy versus neutral, 2) sad versus neutral, 3) angry versus neutral, 4) disgusted versus neutral and 5) fearful versus neutral.

We used the equally weighted coordinates to form estimates of the activation likelihood for each voxel in the brain, as described by Turkeltaub and colleagues.38 In brief, to allow for error in spatial localization related to intersubject variation in functional anatomy and interstudy differences in data smoothing and registration, we modelled the reported loci of maximal activation as the peaks of 3-D Gaussian probability density functions with a full-width at half-maximum of 10 mm. Then we combined the probabilities of each voxel in standard space representing each primary locus of activation to form a map of the activation likelihood estimation score at each voxel. We assessed statistical significance using a permutation test with 5000 permutations, corrected for multiple comparisons (the false discovery rate [FDR] was set at p = 0.01). We defined clusters of suprathreshold voxels exceeding 200 mm3 in volume as loci of brain activation in common across all studies included in the meta-analysis;37 the resultant activation likelihood estimation maps were thresholded at p = 0.05, in line with previous studies.138 To investigate the effect of explicit/implicit emotional processing, sex and magnet field, we carried out meta-analytic comparisons between subgroups of studies (explicit v. implicit, male v. female participantss, magnet ≥ 3 T v. magnet ≤ 1.5 T) using the permutation test described in more detail by Laird and colleagues37 after ensuring that there were no significant differences in the number of coordinates found in each group. Similarly, we selected coordinates relative to studies investigating young participants (< 20 yr) and contrasted them with that of studies involving older participants (> 40 yr) to address the influence of age on emotional processing. Finally, to better clarify the main effect of valence, we contrasted specific emotions: 1) happy versus sad, 2) angry versus disgusted, 3) sad versus fearful and 4) happy versus fearful. The overall fMRI meta-analytical approach33 has been widely used in a number of studies.138143 We imported whole-brain maps of the activation likelihood estimation values into the MRIcron software program (www.sph.sc.edu/comd/rorden/mricron) and overlaid them onto the brain template for presentation purposes.

Results

Study selection

Our combined 1990–May 2008 literature search uncovered 551 potential articles. The number of published articles relating to each emotional condition of interest were as follows: “fMRI sad faces” returned 42 published papers, “fMRI angry faces” returned 65, “fMRI disgust faces” returned 22, “fMRI fearful faces” returned 142, “fMRI happy faces” returned 106 and “fMRI neutral faces” found 174. Most of the excluded studies (94%) did not meet inclusion criteria 2 and 3, whereas 2% did not meet inclusion criterion 4 and 4% did not meet criterion 5.

Overall, 105 articles satisfied the inclusion criteria and we considered them for the meta-analysis. The final database of 105 studies corresponded to a whole sample of 1600 healthy participants (of whom 639 were female, 40%). The mean age of the sample was of 27 (standard deviation [SD] 6.8) years. Forty-seven of 105 studies (45%) reported coordinates in MNI space. Fifty-five of 105 studies (52%) adopted an ROI approach in the analysis of functional activation. Sixty percent of studies employed a classical Ekman faces paradigm, 10% of them used a Gur faces paradigm and the final 30% employed other emotional faces paradigms. We used a total of 1785 brain voxels for the meta-analytical procedure. Table 1 lists all of the studies included in the meta-analysis by first author and year of publication.

Main effect for faces processing independent of emotional valence

The processing of faces (emotional and neutral) compared with baseline (1785 coordinates) was associated with increased activation in a number of visual areas (fusiform gyrus, inferior and middle occipital gyri, lingual gyrus), limbic areas (amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex), temporoparietal areas (parietal lobule, middle temporal gyrus, insula), prefrontal areas (medial frontal gyrus), subcortical areas (putamen) and the cerebellum (declive; Fig. 1, Table 2).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Neurofunctional network subserving processing of human faces. The areas in yellow represent activation likelihood estimation neurofunctional maps of brain areas activated during processing of human faces (human faces > fixation, false discovery rate = 0.01, cluster p < 0.001).

Table 2.

Brain activation during emotional faces processing as compared to fixation*

Emotional face; brain region Side BA Site of maximum ALE
Volume, mm3 Maximum ALE value
x y z
All faces
Fusiform gyrus R 37 40 − 50 − 18 17632 0.09972
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 20 − 6 − 12 14072 0.23064
Cerebellum L Dcl − 40 − 74 − 14 12848 0.08300
Parahippocampal gyrus R Amy 18 − 6 − 14 12320 0.15909
Insula R 30 20 2 11944 0.06221
Medial frontal gyrus L 32 0 10 42 8696 0.05548
Inferior occipital gyrus L 17 − 20 − 92 − 8 6224 0.07987
Middle temporal gyrus R 22 56 − 44 4 1880 0.03966
Parietal lobule L 40 − 36 − 38 40 1160 0.03714
Middle temporal gyrus L 21 − 56 − 58 4 688 0.04138
Middle occipital gyrus R 19 32 − 80 14 616 0.03631
Posterior cingulate R 23 2 − 54 20 352 0.03480
Lingual gyrus R 18 10 − 84 − 8 336 0.03235
Middle occipital gyrus R 18 32 − 92 2 240 0.03141
Fearful
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 22 − 6 − 14 7088 0.05640
Parahippocampal gyrus R Amy 18 − 6 − 14 6648 0.05611
Fusiform gyrus R 37 42 − 50 − 8 4520 0.02025
Cerebellum R Dcl 30 − 80 − 16 2016 0.02853
Inferior parietal lobule L 40 − 36 − 38 40 768 0.01981
Inferior frontal gyrus L 47 − 46 33 4 664 0.01960
Medial frontal gyrus R 6 0 16 42 552 0.01954
Medial frontal gyrus R 10 6 52 0 440 0.01807
Fusiform gyrus L 19 − 40 − 74 − 12 344 0.01481
Insula L 13 − 38 6 − 2 312 0.01635
Subgyral R 21 44 − 12 − 8 240 0.01476
Happy
Middle occipital gyrus R 18 42 − 76 − 8 3168 0.02072
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 22 − 8 − 12 2344 0.02612
Insula L 13 − 38 16 12 1312 0.01715
Medial frontal gyrus L 6 − 6 − 4 56 848 0.01662
Lentiform nucleus L Ptm − 22 2 8 568 0.01330
Precuneus L 31 − 18 − 76 28 520 0.01311
Cerebellum L Dcl − 38 − 64 − 20 336 0.01010
Supramarginal gyrus R 40 38 − 44 34 328 0.01127
Supramarginal gyrus L 40 − 56 − 52 32 272 0.01058
Middle temporal gyrus L 21 − 58 − 60 2 240 0.01217
Sad
Superior occipital gyrus R 19 32 − 70 24 496 0.01023
Insula L 13 − 46 − 30 22 480 0.00989
Thalamus L − 4 − 21 1 264 0.00743
Neutral
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 18 − 6 − 12 2656 0.03128
Cingulate L 32 − 2 10 42 1392 0.01820
Cerebellum L Dcl − 40 − 72 − 14 1264 0.01761
Cerebellum R Dcl 34 − 72 − 20 1184 0.01903
Fusiform gyrus L 37 − 40 − 48 − 16 1112 0.01617
Cerebellum L Dcl − 20 − 78 − 20 984 0.01673
Lingual gyrus L 17 − 16 − 94 − 8 976 0.01757
Medial frontal gyrus L 6 0 0 56 928 0.01695
Middle frontal gyrus R 46 46 30 18 784 0.01677
Precentral gyrus R 6 42 − 4 34 544 0.01555
Fusiform gyrus R 37 40 − 46 − 16 512 0.01234
Inferior occipital gyrus R 18 24 − 90 − 12 384 0.01374
Precentral gyrus R 9 38 12 34 384 0.01344
Insula L 13 − 30 22 2 288 0.01258
Lentiform nucleus R Gp 18 − 8 − 8 272 0.01314
Angry
Cingulate gyrus R 32 6 12 40 5432 0.04209
Claustrum R 28 20 1 3048 0.02863
Inferior frontal gyrus L 47 − 44 26 2 2528 0.01677
Middle frontal gyrus R 9 50 16 30 832 0.01220
Parahippocampal gyrus R 34 16 − 8 − 16 712 0.01344
Lentiform nucleus L Gp − 20 − 8 − 6 432 0.01046
Cerebellum L Cu − 40 − 52 − 22 392 0.01040
Inferior frontal gyrus R 47 46 18 0 376 0.01115
Anterior cingulate R 24 2 24 18 352 0.01171
Middle temporal gyrus R 22 50 − 38 0 304 0.01061
Fusiform gyrus L 19 − 46 − 76 − 12 288 0.01061
Disgusted
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 22 − 6 − 16 1824 0.01643
Middle frontal gyrus L 9 − 36 20 26 1808 0.01230
Middle temporal gyrus L 21 − 58 − 58 6 864 0.01265
Middle temporal gyrus R 22 56 − 46 2 712 0.01217
Middle temporal gyrus L 21 − 56 − 30 − 10 640 0.01322
Fusiform gyrus R 19 40 − 76 − 10 576 0.01286
Precentral gyrus L 4 − 36 − 20 40 480 0.01031
Insula R 44 42 15 16 456 0.01083
Inferior frontal gyrus R 46 42 32 6 392 0.01004
Thalamus L − 4 − 14 0 304 0.00907
Inferior parietal lobule L 40 − 50 − 56 38 280 0.00842

ALE = activation likelihood estimation; Amy = amygdale; BA = Brodmann area; Cu = culmen; Dcl = declive; FDR = false discovery rate; Gp = globus pallidus; L = left; Ptm = Putamen; R = right.

*

FDR = 0.01, cluster p < 0.001.

Emotional processing as compared with baseline

Processing neutral faces

The processing of neutral faces compared with baseline (165 foci), was associated with an increased activation of visual areas (bilateral fusiform gyrus, left lingual gyrus, inferior occipital gyrus), the cerebellum (bilateral declive), limbic areas (left amygdala and left cingulate gyrus), subcortical areas (right lentiform nucleus), prefrontal regions (left medial frontal gyrus, right middle frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus) and the left insula (cluster p < 0.001, false discovery rate [FDR] = 0.01; Table 2).

Processing happy faces

The processing of happy faces compared with baseline (112 foci) was associated with an increased activation in the right middle occipital gyrus, left precuneus, left amygdala, left insula, left medial frontal gyrus, left putamen, left cerebellum, bilateral supramarginal gyrus and left middle temporal gyrus (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 2).

Processing sad faces

The processing of sad faces compared with baseline (37 foci) showed an increased activation in the right superior occipital gyrus, left insula and left thalamus (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 2).

Processing angry faces

The processing of angry faces compared with baseline (77 foci) activated the right cingulate and anterior cingulate gyri, the right parahippocampal gyrus, left cerebellum, subcortical regions such as the left globus pallidus and the right claustrum and prefrontal regions such as the bilateral inferior frontal gyrus and the right middle frontal gyrus (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 2).

Processing fearful faces

During the processing of fearful faces compared with baseline (338 foci), there was a significant increase of neural activation in the bilateral amygdala and fusiform gyrus, right cerebellum, left inferior parietal lobule, left inferior frontal and right medial frontal gyrus (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 2).

Processing disgusted faces

When processing disgusted faces compared with baseline (48 foci), there was an increased neural response in the left amygdala, fusiform gyrus, bilateral middle temporal gyrus, left middle frontal and right inferior frontal gyri, right insula, left precentral gyrus, left inferior parietal lobule and left thalamus (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 2).

Differential emotional processing compared with neutral faces

Processing happy faces (65 foci) was associated with neural activation in the bilateral amygdala, left fusiform gyrus and right anterior cingulate cortex. Sad faces (39 foci) activated the right amygdala and the left lingual gyrus. Fear (257 foci) was associated with activation in the bilateral amygdala and the fusiform and medial frontal gyri. Processing angry faces (66 foci) was associated with increased neural response in the left insula and right inferior occipital gyrus. Disgust (150 foci) was associated with neural activation in the insula bilaterally, right thalamus and left fusiform gyrus (Table 3 and Fig. 2).

Table 3.

Differential activation during emotional faces processing compared with neutral faces*

Emotional face; brain region Side BA Site of maximum ALE
Volume, mm3 Maximum ALE value
x y z
Disgusted
Insula R 13 42 − 8 4 4232 0.0153
L 13 − 38 3 6 1512 0.0131
Thalamus R 2 − 18 4 3944 0.0139
Fusiform gyrus L 37 − 38 − 46 − 12 2760 0.0161
Happy
Parahippocampal gyrus R Amy 24 0 − 14 1248 0.0161
L Amy − 20 − 6 − 12 1664 0.0171
Anterior cingulate cortex R 32 0 40 8 1568 0.0132
Fusiform gyrus L 19 − 40 − 65 − 11 1088 0.0097
Sad
Lingual gyrus L 18 − 18 − 78 − 6 952 0.0078
Parahippocampal gyrus R Amy 22 0 − 18 784 0.0093
Angry
Insula L 13 − 38 14 − 4 1432 0.0095
Inferior occipital gyrus R 19 40 − 74 − 8 792 0.0084
Fearful
Parahippocampal gyrus R Amy 20 − 4 − 14 7304 0.0350
L Amy − 22 − 4 − 10 8288 0.0541
Fusiform gyrus R 37 30 − 46 − 12 6192 0.0235
L 19 − 20 − 61 − 6 2920 0.0196
Medial frontal gyrus R 10 6 52 0 1824 0.0184

ALE = activation likelihood estimation; Amy = amygdala; BA = Brodmann area; FDR = false discovery rate; L = left; R = right.

*

FDR = 0.001, cluster p < 0.001.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Brain maps of neural activation in response to happy, sad, angry, fearful and disgusted human faces compared with neutral faces (false discovery rate = 0.001, cluster p < 0.001). Amy = amygdala; FG = fusiform gyrus; MFG = medial frontal gyrus.

Specific contrasts

Effect for implicit or explicit emotional processing

When healthy participants were explicitly processing the emotional faces, there was an increased activation in the temporal part of the right fusiform gyrus, bilateral amygdala, left cerebellum (declive), right claustrum and caudate, and the right inferior occipital, left inferior frontal and left medial frontal gyri, as compared with the implicit emotional processing (734 foci). Conversely, during the implicit processing of the emotional faces, healthy participants showed more activation in the occipital parts of the left lingual and right fusiform gyri, left post-central gyrus and right insula, as compared with the explicit emotional processing of faces (cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01; Table 4, Fig. 3).

Table 4.

Effect of implicit or explicit facial emotional processing*

Emotional processing; brain region Side BA Site of maximum ALE
Volume, mm3 Maximum ALE value
x y z
Explicit > implicit
Fusiform gyrus R 37 40 − 46 − 18 6880 0.07251
Parahippocampal gyrus L Amy − 20 − 6 − 10 5248 0.10330
Cerebellum L Dcl − 42 − 56 − 20 4448 0.05498
Parahippocampal gyrus R 34 16 − 8 − 16 3776 0.06769
Medial frontal gyrus L 6 − 2 12 44 2584 0.05677
Inferior occipital gyrus R 19 42 − 74 − 8 1032 0.03858
Inferior frontal gyrus L 47 − 44 24 2 896 0.03167
Claustrum R 30 20 4 520 0.03487
Inferior frontal gyrus R 47 48 20 − 2 472 0.03139
Inferior frontal gyrus L 9 − 42 10 24 432 0.02986
Cerebellum L Dcl − 22 − 78 − 20 232 0.02890
Caudate R 12 − 6 16 232 0.02620
Implicit > explicit
Postcentral gyrus L 3 − 34 − 26 44 416 − 0.02583
Lingual gyrus L 17 − 10 − 92 − 18 328 − 0.03550
Fusiform gyrus R 18 28 − 80 − 22 288 − 0.02496
Insula R 13 47 8 7 216 − 0.02462

ALE = activation likelihood estimation; Amy = amygdale; BA = Brodmann area; Dcl = declive; FDR = false discovery rate; L = left; R = right.

*

FDR = 0.001, cluster p < 0.001.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Neural correlates of explicit versus implicit facial emotional processing (yellow clusters: explicit > implicit; blue clusters: implicit > explicit; false discovery rate = 0.01 cluster p < 0.001).

Effect of age on human facial emotional processing

When processing human emotional faces, older participants showed greater neural response in the temporal part (Brodmann area [BA] 37) of the bilateral fusiform gyrus, left cerebellum and left hippocampus as compared with young healthy participants (196 foci). Conversely, younger participants showed greater activation in a more occipital part (BA19) of the bilateral fusiform gyrus than older participants (cluster p < 0.0001, FDR = 0.01; Table 5, Fig. 4).

Table 5.

Effect of age on human facial emotional processing

Activation by age; brain region Side BA Site of maximum ALE
Volume, mm3 Maximum ALE value
x y z
Old > young
Fusiform gyrus R 37 40 − 46 − 18 3000 0.02905
L 37 − 40 − 46 − 16 2208 0.02174
Cerebellum L Tuber − 42 − 68 − 24 1224 0.01629
Parahippocampal gyrus L Hippo − 28 − 10 − 16 1008 0.01371
Young > old
Fusiform gyrus R 19 32 − 72 − 18 2624 − 0.01836
L 19 − 18 − 78 − 18 1112 − 0.01608

ALE = activation likelihood estimation; BA = Brodmann area; Hippo = hippocampus; L = left; R = right; Tuber = tuber vermis.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Age modulation of human emotional processing (yellow clusters: old > young; blue clusters: young > old; false discovery rate = 0.01, cluster p < 0.001).

Effect of sex on human facial emotional processing

When processing human emotional faces (613 foci), male participants showed greater neural response than female participants in a cluster spanning the right amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus (x = 25, y = − 12, z = − 10, activation likelihood estimation = 0.01681, voxels = 1024), in the right medial frontal gyrus (BA6, x = 1, y = 2, z = 52, activation likelihood estimation = 0.02018, voxels = 760) and in the left fusiform gyrus (BA19, x = − 38, y = − 76, z = − 13, activation likelihood estimation = 0.01836, voxels = 360). Conversely, female participants showed greater activation than male participants in the right subcallosal gyrus (BA34, x = 19, y = 3, z = − 13, activation likelihood estimation = − 0.02225, voxels = 696; cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01).

Effect of magnetic field strength

We detected a significant effect for the magnetic field strength on the detection of neural correlates of emotional processing. When using a magnet ≥ than 3 T, as compared with magnets of 1.5 T or less (1675 foci), increased activation was observable in the bilateral fusiform gyri (BA37, right: x = 36, y = − 54, z = − 13, activation likelihood estimation = 0.06837, voxels = 6656; left: x = − 38, y = − 55, z = − 13, activation likelihood estimation = 0.06011), bilateral amygdala (left: x = − 27, y = − 9, z = − 11, activation likelihood estimation = 0.06529, voxels = 2024; right: x = 22, y = − 13, z = − 10, activation likelihood estimation = 0.03178, voxels1408) and left medial frontal gyrus (BA6, x = − 2, y = − 2, z = 54, activation likelihood estimation = 0.04081, voxels = 1104; cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01). We detected no clusters for the opposite contrast.

Happy versus sad

When processing happy faces compared with sad faces (149 foci), participants showed greater activation in the left amygdala (x = − 22, y = − 8, z = − 11, activation likelihood estimation = 0.02463, voxels = 1472) and right middle occipital gyrus (BA18, x = 40, y = − 72, z = − 9, activation likelihood estimation = 0.01953, voxels = 1336; cluster p < 0.001, FDR = 0.01). We detected no clusters during the processing of sad compared with happy faces (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Differential neurofunctional response to emotions. (A) Increased activation during processing of happy faces compared with sad faces. (B) Differential activation between processing fearful and happy faces (yellow: happy > fearful; blue: fearful > happy). (C) Differential activation between processing angry and disgusted faces (yellow: angry > disgust; blue: disgust > angry). False discovery rate = 0.001, cluster p < 0.001.

Angry versus disgusted

When processing angry faces compared with disgusted faces (38 foci) there was increased activation in the right amygdala (x = 17, y = − 5, z = − 18) and right fusiform gyrus and deactivation in the right insula (x = 40, y = − 5, z = 2; Fig. 5).

Happy versus fearful

When processing fearful faces compared with happy faces (59 foci) there was increased activation in the bilateral amygdala (left x = − 19, y = − 2, z = − 13; right x = 16, y = 4, z = − 18) and decreased activation in the anterior cingulate gyrus (x = 2, y = 39, z = 11; Fig. 5).

Sad versus fearful

When processing fearful faces compared with sad faces (68 foci) there was increased activation in the bilateral amygdala (x = − 20, y = − 6, z = − 14) and in the right fusiform gyrus (x = 45, y = − 46, z = − 18). No brain regions were more activated by the sad faces compared with the fearful faces.

Discussion

The aim of the present meta-analysis was to provide a neurofunctional map of human emotional face processing in healthy individuals.

Methodological issues

We adopted a multiple-steps approach with the encompassing objective of reducing heterogeneity across fMRI findings. In a first step (study selection), we chose to include fMRI studies only and avoid positron emission tomography studies given the profound methodological differences of these methods. In addition, to control for considerable variation observed in the results of imaging experiments addressing even closely related experimental paradigms,32 our meta-analysis focused on a single paradigm (i.e., the presentation of human faces and emotional picture) during emotional processing (attentive or mnestic studies were not considered). To overcome the lack of statistical power associated with single fMRI studies, we selected a large sample of 105 studies relating to 1600 healthy volunteers and 1785 foci. Although there are no community-accepted criteria for interpreting the activation likelihood estimation results, for a study of this size if 6 or more foci contribute to a cluster it is considered very robust, and if 3–5 foci contribute to a cluster, it is acceptable.144 It is not convincing if only 1 or 2 foci contribute to a cluster. As recent activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses have been published with samples of 12 studies and fewer than 10 foci,144 our results are particularly robust. We have chosen a function–location statistical approach (voxel-based, activation likelihood estimation) in place of the standard effect-size meta analysis, because it is the location, rather than the magnitude, of the effect that is of interest in the present study.32 Specifically, activation likelihood estimation assumes that although each study reports the specific coordinates of activations, issues such as intersubject variability in brain anatomy and differences in investigators’ labels for anatomic regions may lead to some uncertainty as to the actual locations of these peaks. In fact, one of the difficulties when comparing imaging studies is that there is considerable variability when labelling neuroanatomical regions, and differences in nomenclature could obscure findings. An advantage of the activation likelihood estimation technique is that because it uses the coordinates of reported foci (rather than anatomic labels) for meta-analysis, it avoids the problem of any mislabelling of regions in the primary literature.145 A further benefit is that the exclusion of negative data has very little effect on the results.32

Neurofunctional map of human emotional face processing

The present study has produced a reliable normative map of brain response to human emotional faces processing. We found that processing of emotional faces was associated with increased activation in a number of visual areas (fusiform gyrus, inferior and middle occipital gyri, lingual gyrus), limbic areas (amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus, posterior cingulate), temporal areas (middle/superior temporal gyrus), temporoparietal areas (parietal lobule, middle temporal gyrus, insula), prefrontal areas (medial frontal gyrus), subcortical areas (putamen) and the cerebellum (declive). This neural network is the most likely to be activated during the processing of a human emotional face. These findings confirm that processing emotion from facial expressions draws on diverse psychological processes implemented in a large array of neural structures.26 Although the exact functional interplay between these areas is not clear, some authors suggest that early perceptual processing of faces draws on cortices in occipital and temporal lobes that construct detailed representations from the configuration of facial features.5 Subsequent recognition requires a set of structures, including the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex, that links perceptual representations of the face to the generation of knowledge about the emotion signaled.5

Differential neural response to emotions

Our second aim was to identify discrete neural subsystems underlying processing of “basic” emotional expressions (fear, disgust, anger, happiness and sadness).35 As human signals of basic emotion are universal, fMRI studies that explore the neural substrates of emotion recognition are free of the reliance on vague subjective measures that plagued earlier emotion research.146 When we compared processing of emotional faces with baseline, we uncovered wide and overlapping neural networks. Thus, to better disentangle the specific neural substrates of emotions, we contrasted emotional faces against neutral faces.

With respect to the limbic system, we found a differential sensitivity to emotional conditions. Whereas happy and fearful faces activated the amygdala bilaterally, sad faces showed a laterality effect, and angry or disgusted faces had no effect on this region. In a second step, we tested the differential amygdala sensitivity to happy, fearful and sad faces by computing direct contrasts between these emotional conditions. The intensity of the activation likelihood estimation scores confirmed that amygdala sensitivity was greater during the fearful stimulus than in the other 2 conditions. In other words, it is possible to conclude that amygdala engagement during processing of fearful faces is a strong and consistent finding in the available fMRI literature. Such a result is in line with evidence suggesting that the amygdala is specifically sensitive to fearful emotional processing.147 However, we found the amygdala was also activated while processing neutral faces; as there are face-responsive neurons in the amygdala,68 this brain region may have an additional role for vigilance or for processing salience.148

The second interesting finding of the present study is insular activation during processing of disgusted and angry faces. Although the insula was activated not only by disgusted but also by angry faces, the intensity of activation likelihood estimation scores and the direct contrast between angry and disgusted faces clearly indicated that its sensitivity is greater to disgusted than to angry faces. A number of behavioural and neurobiological accounts have suggested that this brain region is relevant to the neurobiological models of disgust,149 as the insula in primates contains neurons that respond to pleasant and unpleasant tastes.146 Some authors have speculated that whereas limbic (amygdala–hippocampus) regions are particularly involved in the emotional response to exteroceptive sensory stimuli, the insular cortex is preferentially involved in the emotional response to potentially distressing stimuli, interoceptive sensory stimuli and body sensations.146 In fact, the insula is part of the gustatory cortex and is connected to the ventro–posterior–medial thalamic nucleus.146 Interestingly, we have uncovered additional thalamic activation in response to disgusted faces, indicating that the insular–thalamic pathway may represent a core relay of this neural network.

We also uncovered neural activation in different areas of the visual cortex (fusiform gyrus, lingual gyrus, inferior occipital gyrus). This response was observable across all emotional conditions. As discussed in the previous paragraph, it is possible to speculate that these areas are engaged in early perceptual processing of facial stimuli,5 which may be independent from emotional valence. The medial frontal cortex, on the other hand, participates in the conscious experience of emotion, inhibition of excessive emotion, or monitoring one’s own emotional state to make relevant decisions. This region was activated by fearful faces, whereas happy faces activated the anterior cingulate cortex, a region that is specifically involved in the arousal to an emotive visual stimulus.146

Finally, we found no differential brain activation across emotions in the cerebellum. This area is of particular interest in the field of affective neuroscience as it is strongly connected with the reticular system (arousal), cortical association areas (cognitive processing of emotions) and limbic structures (emotional experience and expression) such as the amygdala, the hippocampus and the septal nuclei.150 Previous studies have indicated that cerebellar lesions result in flattening or blunting of emotions151 and cerebellar activation has been observed in response to different emotions.152,153 The lack of differential activation in this area is in line with evidence that the cerebellum plays a general role in emotional processing.154

These findings taken together suggest that emotional conditions are implemented by neural systems that are at least partially separable, although they are not represented by entirely distinct neural circuits. Certainly, at least at level of resolution of fMRI there appear to be interesting limbic and insular differences between the basic emotions.

Modulatory effect of age, sex and explicit/implicit emotional facial processing

The present meta-analysis has also identified the neurofunctional maps of implicit and explicit emotional faces processing. Of particular interest, we found that the explicit processing of stimuli was associated with activation of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala. In particular, the medial prefrontal cortex is thought to be involved not only in general emotional processing31 but also in emotional self-awareness.155 Specifically, this region plays a key role in complex aspects of emotional processing such as social interaction by virtue of its connections with the discrete parts of the temporal lobe and subcortical structures that control autonomic responses.156 Given the anatomic connections between the medial pre-frontal cortex and the amygdala,157 the coactivation of these 2 structures may reflect possible influence of cortical control on explicit processing. The amygdala–medial prefrontal circuitry has been termed as the emotion generation–regulation circuit158 and is implicated in attention to threat and interpretation of emotional stimuli.159 Previous lesion studies have confirmed that insults to these areas result in emotion dysregulation.160

Conversely, the implicit processing was associated with activation in more ventral regions spanning the inferior pre-frontal cortex and insula. The insular cortex plays a key role in emotional processing owing to its abundant connections with other association and primary sensory areas and its involvement in language, vestibular and pain perception (for a review see Nagai et al.161). In particular, insular projections to the inferior prefrontal cortex and amygdala may convey social information from emotional expressions.22 The anterior part of the insular cortex is considered to be a limbic-related cortex and part of the interoceptive system, which associates (with a mirror neuron link) external and internal experience.25 This insular subregion provides information about aversive body states associated with conditional stimuli, signalling this information to prefrontal brain areas that are critical for the allocation of attention and the execution of actions.162 The cognitive control of appraisal and emotional relevance is not mediated by the orbitofrontal cortex alone, but arises from large-scale systems that include the amygdala and the insular cortices.163 In line with such evidence our findings suggest that the explicit or implicit emotional processing is regulated by the functional interaction of a network comprising these brain regions.

With respect to age, we demonstrated that the neural response to emotional faces in the fusiform gyrus, the cerebellum and the hippocampus is modulated by this factor. The hippocampus shows extensive connections with extrastriate visual areas, including the fusiform gyrus. Previous fMRI paradigms have confirmed that brain activity in this area is affected by age.164 The observed age-related differences could reflect functional compensation within the neural system involved in perception of facial affect or the fact that older adults process emotional information in a different manner than young adults. Finally, we found that male participants activated the limbic (amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus) and the prefrontal cortices more than female participants during emotional processing; conversely, female participants showed greater activation in the right subcallosal gyrus. Previous studies found that male and female participants engage the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex in different ways while passively viewing emotional faces.15

Implications for studies of emotional processing in psychiatric populations

Over the past decade, a number of behavioural findings have shown that alterations of emotional faces processing play a role in a range of psychiatric disorders spanning affective diseases (e.g., major depression, bipolar disorders, anxiety-related disorders)24 and the psychotic spectrum (e.g., schizophrenia,165167 autism168). For instance, depressed participants show a state-related positive bias toward negative emotional cues (e.g., sad faces) and a bias away from positive emotional cues (e.g., happy faces).24 These findings are in line with cognitive theories of depression, which emphasizes the role of negative biases in information processing in the etiology and maintenance of depression.169 Consistent with these observations, there are data to suggest there may be an analogous, state-related negative recognition bias for negative emotions in mania.91 On the other hand, patients with schizophrenia have difficulty recognizing the emotion that corresponds with a given facial expression.2,170172 Early deficit in visual processing underlying emotion recognition170,173 is a hallmark of schizophrenia, with consequences for cognitive function,174 social function2 and subjective well-being.171 Specific alterations of emotional faces processing of schizophrenic patients include bias for threat-related emotional material, which may be regarded with increased significance by delusion-prone individuals, and it is possible that this bias is involved in the formation of delusional beliefs.2,175,176

Facial emotional stimuli may serve as valid tools tapping on neural networks implicated in emotional processing. Thus,30 fMRI has been exponentially employed to address the neurophysiological substrates of impairments in emotional faces processing under different psychiatric conditions.177 Although many of the brain regions activated during facial emotional processing in healthy participants are also implicated in the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders,24 current imaging literature indicates contrasting findings and a variable picture8 and is unable to definitively characterize what brain region is etiologically associated with each psychiatric disease. For this reason, the use of the physiologic maps provided herein will help to strengthen future study hypotheses and designs by robustly identifying regions of normal activation.

Limitations

There are several limitations to our study. The first is the heterogeneity of the studies included. Factors such as behavioural performance, demographic information, substance abuse, cognitive function and personality traits, which could potentially influence the results, vary across the different samples. Although we have attempted to address the effect of age, sex, implicit/explicit emotional processing and intensity of magnet, at the present time it is not possible to directly evaluate the influence of all these factors on the results. Second, our methods did not allow for weighting of the results based on the level of statistical significance reported in each study. This means that we cannot exactly determine the relative strengths of activation differences. Third, although the quantitative meta-analytic method used herein represents a substantial advance for integrating functional neuroimaging data, the method remains subject to the basic limitation of literature reviews, in particular the “file drawer” problem. However, a benefit of activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses is that the exclusion of negative data has very little effect on the results.32 Conversely, it is possible that studies employing ROI analysis of brain activation may have influenced the resulting activation patterns and potentially biased the results. Previous activation likelihood estimation studies144 did not acknowledge such a methodological limitation, including in the same database whole brain and ROI studies. To our knowledge, the present study is the first activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis to address this point by providing details of the methodological approach adopted in each study.

Issues for future research

Functional neuroimaging is currently advancing from the simple detection and localization of cortical activation to the investigation of complex cortical processes and associated functional relations between cortical areas. Such research questions can no longer be addressed by the isolated analysis of single experiments alone, but necessitate the consolidation of results across different cognitive tasks and experimental paradigms. This again makes meta-analyses an increasingly important part in the evaluation of functional imaging results.8 A combination of functional imaging and electrophysiological techniques (i.e., fMRI and electroencephalography) may also represent the future research instrument to combine the high spatial resolution of the first technique with the high temporal resolution of the second. Finally, although the evidence suggests that defined areas of the brain are responsive to specific facial expressions, recent evidence indicates neuronal subspecialization in face-specific brain regions for different components of facial perception such as facial expression, viewing of familiar and unfamiliar faces, and discrimination of spatial relations of facial features (i.e., eyes, lips or nose).28 Investigations into the neural systems underlying processing of these cues for each of the basic emotions may be helpful to further elucidate their neural representation.

Conclusion

The wide neurofunctional network underlying human faces processing includes a number of visual, limbic, temporoparietal, prefrontal and subcortical areas as well as the cerebellum. Whereas occipital areas and the cerebellum were commonly activated across different emotions, a discrete response to valence has been reported for the limbic system and insular cortex. Although the basic emotions are not represented by entirely distinct neural circuits, they are at least partially separable. Sex, age and consciousness modulate the neurophysiological response to human emotional faces.

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr. Angela Laird for her valuable advice with the meta-analytical package.

Footnotes

Competing interests: None declared.

Contributors: Drs. Fusar-Poli, Placentino, Allen, Barale, Perez, McGuire and Politi designed the study. Drs. Fusar-Poli, Carletti, Landi, Abbamonte and Gasparotti acquired the data, which Drs. Fusar-Poli, Surguladze, Benedetti and McGuire analyzed. Drs. Fusar-Poli, Landi and Perez wrote the article, which all other authors reviewed. All authors approved the final version for publication.

References

  • 1.Tate AJ, Fischer H, Leigh AE, et al. Behavioural and neurophysiological evidence for face identity and face emotion processing in animals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361:2155–72. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Phillips ML, David AS. Facial processing in schizophrenia and delusional misidentification: cognitive neuropsychiatric approaches. Schizophr Res. 1995;17:109–14. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(95)00035-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Frank MG, Stennett J. The forced-choice paradigm and the perception of facial expressions of emotion. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;80:75–85. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.80.1.75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Grossmann T, Johnson MH. The development of the social brain in human infancy. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;25:909–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05379.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:169–77. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00301-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci. 1997;17:4302–11. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Peelen MV, Downing PE. The neural basis of visual body perception. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8:636–48. doi: 10.1038/nrn2195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Neumann J, von Cramon DY, Lohmann G. Model-based clustering of meta-analytic functional imaging data. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:177–92. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fischer H, Sandblom J, Gavazzeni J, et al. Age-differential patterns of brain activation during perception of angry faces. Neurosci Lett. 2005;386:99–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Deeley Q, Daly EM, Azuma R, et al. Changes in male brain responses to emotional faces from adolescence to middle age. Neuroimage. 2008;40:389–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Iidaka T, Okada T, Murata T, et al. Age-related differences in the medial temporal lobe responses to emotional faces as revealed by fMRI. Hippocampus. 2002;12:352–62. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pine DS, Grun J, Zarahn E, et al. Cortical brain regions engaged by masked emotional faces in adolescents and adults: an fMRI study. Emotion. 2001;1:137–47. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.2.137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Guyer AE, Monk CS, McClure-Tone EB, et al. A developmental examination of amygdala response to facial expressions. J Cogn Neurosci. 2008;20:1565–82. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Schulte-Rüther M, Markowitsch HJ, Shah NJ, et al. Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy. Neuroimage. 2008;42:393–403. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, et al. A developmental examination of gender differences in brain engagement during evaluation of threat. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:1047–55. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kranz F, Ishai A. Face perception is modulated by sexual preference. Curr Biol. 2006;16:63–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.10.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee TM, Liu HL, Hoosain R, et al. Gender differences in neural correlates of recognition of happy and sad faces in humans assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosci Lett. 2002;333:13–6. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)00965-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Habel U, Windischberger C, Derntl B, et al. Amygdala activation and facial expressions: explicit emotion discrimination versus implicit emotion processing. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2369–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gorno-Tempini ML, Pradelli S, Serafini M, et al. Explicit and incidental facial expression processing: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2001;14:465–73. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0811. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Whalen PJ, Rauch SL, Etcoff NL, et al. Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. J Neurosci. 1998;18:411–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00411.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Chen CH, Lennox B, Jacob R, et al. Explicit and implicit facial affect recognition in manic and depressed states of bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;59:31–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Critchley H, Daly E, Phillips M, et al. Explicit and implicit neural mechanisms for processing of social information from facial expressions: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;9:93–105. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200002)9:2&#x0003c;93::AID-HBM4&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Posamentier MT, Abdi H. Processing faces and facial expressions. Neuropsychol Rev. 2003;13:113–43. doi: 10.1023/a:1025519712569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Leppanen JM. Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2006;19:34–9. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.46411.00. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gobbini MI, Haxby JV. Neural systems for recognition of familiar faces. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Vuilleumier P, Pourtois G. Distributed and interactive brain mechanisms during emotion face perception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:174–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Pourtois G, Vuilleumier P. Dynamics of emotional effects on spatial attention in the human visual cortex. Prog Brain Res. 2006;156:67–91. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)56004-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Masella RS, Meister M. The neuroanatomic basis of facial perception and variable facial discrimination ability: implications for orthodontics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop. 2007;132:293–301. doi: 10.1016/j.ajodo.2006.04.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Davidson RJ, Irwin W. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3:11–21. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01265-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol Psychiatry. 2002;51:59–67. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01330-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage. 2002;16:331–48. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Murphy FC, Nimmo-Smith I, Lawrence AD. Functional neuroanatomy of emotions: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2003;3:207–33. doi: 10.3758/cabn.3.3.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fox PT, Laird AR, Lancaster JL. Coordinate-based voxel-wise meta-analysis: dividends of spatial normalization. Report of a virtual workshop. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:1–5. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Wild B, Erb M, Bartels M. Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Res. 2001;102:109–24. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(01)00225-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ekman P, Friesen W. Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto (CA): Consulting Psychologists Press; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gur RC, Sara R, Hagendoorn M, et al. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J Neurosci Methods. 2002;115:137–43. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Laird AR, Fox PM, Price CJ, et al. ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:155–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Turkeltaub PE, Eden GF, Jones KM, et al. Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation. Neuroimage. 2002;16:765–80. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rama P, Courtney SM. Functional topography of working memory for face or voice identity. Neuroimage. 2005;24:224–34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wolfensberger SP, Veltman DJ, Hoogendijk WJ, et al. Amygdala responses to emotional faces in twins discordant or concordant for the risk for anxiety and depression. Neuroimage. 2008;41:544–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Payer DE, Lieberman MD, Monterosso JR, et al. Differences in cortical activity between methamphetamine-dependent and healthy individuals performing a facial affect matching task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2008;93:93–102. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.De Martino B, Kalisch R, Rees G, et al. Enhanced processing of threat stimuli under limited attentional resources. Cereb Cortex. 2008;19:127–33. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Surguladze SA, Elkin A, Ecker C, et al. Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter modulates neural system-wide response to fearful faces. Genes Brain Behav. 2008;7:543–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2008.00390.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bryant RA, Kemp AH, Felmingham KL, et al. Enhanced amygdala and medial prefrontal activation during nonconscious processing of fear in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29:517–23. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Seitz RJ, Schafer R, Scherfeld D, et al. Valuating other people’s emotional face expression: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography study. Neuroscience. 2008;152:713–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.10.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Loughead J, Gur RC, Elliott M, et al. Neural circuitry for accurate identification of facial emotions. Brain Res. 2008;1194:37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Miskowiak K, Inkster B, Selvaraj S, et al. Erythropoietin improves mood and modulates the cognitive and neural processing of emotion 3 days post administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:611–8. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hermans EJ, Ramsey NF, van Honk J. Exogenous testosterone enhances responsiveness to social threat in the neural circuitry of social aggression in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:263–70. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Andersson F, Glaser B, Spiridon M, et al. Impaired activation of face processing networks revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2008;63:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pavuluri MN, O’Connor MM, Harral E, et al. Affective neural circuitry during facial emotion processing in pediatric bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:158–67. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Coccaro EF, McCloskey MS, Fitzgerald DA, et al. Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:168–78. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Hessl D, Rivera S, Koldewyn K, et al. Amygdala dysfunction in men with the fragile X premutation. Brain. 2007;130:404–16. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, et al. Amygdala reactivity predicts automatic negative evaluations for facial emotions. Psychiatry Res. 2007;154:13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.05.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Deeley Q, Daly EM, Surguladze S, et al. An event related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of facial emotion processing in Asperger syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:207–17. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Salloum JB, Ramchandani VA, Bodurka J, et al. Blunted rostral anterior cingulate response during a simplified decoding task of negative emotional facial expressions in alcoholic patients. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2007;31:1490–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00447.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Anderson IM, Del-Ben CM, McKie S, et al. Citalopram modulation of neuronal responses to aversive face emotions: a functional MRI study. Neuroreport. 2007;18:1351–5. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282742115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rauch AV, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, et al. Cognitive coping style modulates neural responses to emotional faces in healthy humans: a 3-T FMRI study. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:2526–35. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ashwin C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, et al. Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:2–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gur RE, Loughead J, Kohler CG, et al. Limbic activation associated with misidentification of fearful faces and flat affect in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:1356–66. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Miskowiak K, O’Sullivan U, Harmer CJ. Erythropoietin reduces neural and cognitive processing of fear in human models of anti-depressant drug action. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1244–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kleinhans NM, Johnson LC, Mahurin R, et al. Increased amygdala activation to neutral faces is associated with better face memory performance. Neuroreport. 2007;18:987–91. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328165d189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Fusar-Poli P, Allen P, Lee F, et al. Modulation of neural response to happy and sad faces by acute tryptophan depletion. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;193:31–44. doi: 10.1007/s00213-007-0757-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Pelphrey KA, Morris JP, McCarthy G, et al. Perception of dynamic changes in facial affect and identity in autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:140–9. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Benuzzi F, Pugnaghi M, Meletti S, et al. Processing the socially relevant parts of faces. Brain Res Bull. 2007;74:344–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lee TW, Josephs O, Dolan RJ, et al. Imitating expressions: emotion-specific neural substrates in facial mimicry. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1:122–35. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Williams LM, Liddell BJ, Kemp AH, et al. Amygdala-prefrontal dissociation of subliminal and supraliminal fear. Hum Brain Mapp. 2006;27:652–61. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bird G, Catmur C, Silani G, et al. Attention does not modulate neural responses to social stimuli in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage. 2006;31:1614–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Fitzgerald DA, Angstadt M, Jelsone LM, et al. Beyond threat: amygdala reactivity across multiple expressions of facial affect. Neuroimage. 2006;30:1441–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Grosbras MH, Paus T. Brain networks involved in viewing angry hands or faces. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:1087–96. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Somerville LH, Wig GS, Whalen PJ, et al. Dissociable medial temporal lobe contributions to social memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2006;18:1253–65. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Britton JC, Taylor SF, Sudheimer KD, et al. Facial expressions and complex IAPS pictures: common and differential networks. Neuroimage. 2006;31:906–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Holt DJ, Kunkel L, Weiss AP, et al. Increased medial temporal lobe activation during the passive viewing of emotional and neutral facial expressions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2006;82:153–62. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Taylor SE, Eisenberger NI, Saxbe D, et al. Neural responses to emotional stimuli are associated with childhood family stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:296–301. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.09.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Batut AC, Gounot D, Namer IJ, et al. Neural responses associated with positive and negative emotion processing in patients with left versus right temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2006;9:415–23. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2006.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Wright CI, Wedig MM, Williams D, et al. Novel fearful faces activate the amygdala in healthy young and elderly adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:361–74. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sambataro F, Dimalta S, Di Giorgio A, et al. Preferential responses in amygdala and insula during presentation of facial contempt and disgust. Eur J Neurosci. 2006;24:2355–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.05120.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Pessoa L, Japee S, Sturman D, et al. Target visibility and visual awareness modulate amygdala responses to fearful faces. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16:366–75. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Williams LM, Kemp AH, Felmingham K, et al. Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage. 2006;29:347–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, et al. Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:28–30. doi: 10.1038/nn1611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wright P, Liu Y. Neutral faces activate the amygdala during identity matching. Neuroimage. 2006;29:628–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62:273–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Noesselt T, Driver J, Heinze HJ, et al. Asymmetrical activation in the human brain during processing of fearful faces. Curr Biol. 2005;15:424–9. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Straube T, Mentzel HJ, Miltner WH. Common and distinct brain activation to threat and safety signals in social phobia. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;52:163–8. doi: 10.1159/000087987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Williams MA, McGlone F, Abbott DF, et al. Differential amygdala responses to happy and fearful facial expressions depend on selective attention. Neuroimage. 2005;24:417–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Strauss MM, Makris N, Aharon I, et al. fMRI of sensitization to angry faces. Neuroimage. 2005;26:389–413. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Cools R, Calder AJ, Lawrence AD, et al. Individual differences in threat sensitivity predict serotonergic modulation of amygdala response to fearful faces. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2005;180:670–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2215-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Habel U, Klein M, Kellermann T, et al. Same or different? Neural correlates of happy and sad mood in healthy males. Neuroimage. 2005;26:206–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Reinders AA, den Boer JA, Buchel C. The robustness of perception. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;22:524–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04212.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Kawachi T, et al. Specific brain activation in Japanese and Caucasian people to fearful faces. Neuroreport. 2005;16:133–6. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200502080-00012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Cannistraro PA, Wright CI, Wedig MM, et al. Amygdala responses to human faces in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:916–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Lennox BR, Jacob R, Calder AJ, et al. Behavioural and neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with mania. Psychol Med. 2004;34:795–802. doi: 10.1017/s0033291704002557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Singer T, Kiebel SJ, Winston JS, et al. Brain responses to the acquired moral status of faces. Neuron. 2004;41:653–62. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Williams LM, Das P, Harris AW, et al. Dysregulation of arousal and amygdala-prefrontal systems in paranoid schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:480–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Phillips ML, Williams LM, Heining M, et al. Differential neural responses to overt and covert presentations of facial expressions of fear and disgust. Neuroimage. 2004;21:1484–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, et al. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7:1271–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Straube T, Kolassa IT, Glauer M, et al. Effect of task conditions on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;56:921–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Glascher J, Tuscher O, Weiller C, et al. Elevated responses to constant facial emotions in different faces in the human amygdala: an fMRI study of facial identity and expression. BMC Neurosci. 2004;5:45. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-5-45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Nomura M, Ohira H, Haneda K, et al. Functional association of the amygdala and ventral prefrontal cortex during cognitive evaluation of facial expressions primed by masked angry faces: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2004;21:352–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Schroeder U, Hennenlotter A, Erhard P, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of perceiving surprised faces. Hum Brain Mapp. 2004;23:181–7. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Somerville LH, Kim H, Johnstone T, et al. Human amygdala responses during presentation of happy and neutral faces: correlations with state anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:897–903. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Etkin A, Klemenhagen KC, Dudman JT, et al. Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron. 2004;44:1043–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Bishop SJ, Duncan J, Lawrence AD. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli. J Neurosci. 2004;24:10364–8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2550-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Surguladze SA, Brammer MJ, Young AW, et al. A preferential increase in the extrastriate response to signals of danger. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1317–28. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00085-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Killgore WD, Young AD, Femia LA, et al. Cortical and limbic activation during viewing of high- versus low-calorie foods. Neuroimage. 2003;19:1381–94. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00191-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Bradley MM, Sabatinelli D, Lang PJ, et al. Activation of the visual cortex in motivated attention. Behav Neurosci. 2003;117:369–80. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.2.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Yang TT, Menon V, Reid AJ, et al. Amygdalar activation associated with happy facial expressions in adolescents: a 3-T functional MRI study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42:979–85. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046886.27264.BA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Wright CI, Martis B, McMullin K, et al. Amygdala and insular responses to emotionally valenced human faces in small animal specific phobia. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;54:1067–76. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00548-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, et al. Both of us disgusted in My in-sula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron. 2003;40:655–64. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Nelson EE, McClure EB, Monk CS, et al. Developmental differences in neuronal engagement during implicit encoding of emotional faces: an event-related fMRI study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2003;44:1015–24. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Schwartz CE, Wright CI, Shin LM, et al. Differential amygdalar response to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: a functional MRI probe developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:854–62. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01906-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Wright CI, Martis B, Schwartz CE, et al. Novelty responses and differential effects of order in the amygdala, substantia innominata, and inferior temporal cortex. Neuroimage. 2003;18:660–9. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)00037-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Lange K, Williams LM, Young AW, et al. Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53:226–32. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01455-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Phillips ML, Gregory LJ, Cullen S, et al. The effect of negative emotional context on neural and behavioural responses to oesophageal stimulation. Brain. 2003;126:669–84. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Wild B, Erb M, Eyb M, et al. Why are smiles contagious? An fMRI study of the interaction between perception of facial affect and facial movements. Psychiatry Res. 2003;123:17–36. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(03)00006-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Canli T, Sivers H, Whitfield SL, et al. Amygdala response to happy faces as a function of extraversion. Science. 2002;296:2191. doi: 10.1126/science.1068749. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Yang TT, Menon V, Eliez S, et al. Amygdalar activation associated with positive and negative facial expressions. Neuroreport. 2002;13:1737–41. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210070-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Gur RE, McGrath C, Chan RM, et al. An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159:1992–9. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.12.1992. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Gur RC, Schroeder L, Turner T, et al. Brain activation during facial emotion processing. Neuroimage. 2002;16:651–62. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Kosaka H, Omori M, Murata T, et al. Differential amygdala response during facial recognition in patients with schizophrenia: an fMRI study. Schizophr Res. 2002;57:87–95. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00324-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Hariri AR, Tessitore A, Mattay VS, et al. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: a comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage. 2002;17:317–23. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Whalen PJ, et al. Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;49:309–16. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01066-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, et al. Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:1057–63. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Williams LM, Phillips ML, Brammer MJ, et al. Arousal dissociates amygdala and hippocampal fear responses: evidence from simultaneous fMRI and skin conductance recording. Neuroimage. 2001;14:1070–9. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Narumoto J, Okada T, Sadato N, et al. Attention to emotion modulates fMRI activity in human right superior temporal sulcus. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001;12:225–31. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(01)00053-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Vuilleumier P, Armony JL, Driver J, et al. Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron. 2001;30:829–41. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00328-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Iidaka T, Omori M, Murata T, et al. Neural interaction of the amygdala with the prefrontal and temporal cortices in the processing of facial expressions as revealed by fMRI. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13:1035–47. doi: 10.1162/089892901753294338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Kesler-West ML, Andersen AH, Smith CD, et al. Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2001;11:213–26. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(00)00073-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, et al. A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion. 2001;1:70–83. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, et al. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47:769–76. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00828-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Phillips ML, Williams L, Senior C, et al. A differential neural response to threatening and non-threatening negative facial expressions in paranoid and non-paranoid schizophrenics. Psychiatry Res. 1999;92:11–31. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(99)00031-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Phillips ML, Bullmore ET, Howard R, et al. Investigation of facial recognition memory and happy and sad facial expression perception: an fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 1998;83:127–38. doi: 10.1016/s0925-4927(98)00036-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Phillips ML, Young AW, Scott SK, et al. Neural responses to facial and vocal expressions of fear and disgust. Proc Biol Sci. 1998;265:1809–17. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Sprengelmeyer R, Rausch M, Eysel UT, et al. Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions. Proc Biol Sci. 1998;265:1927–31. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1998.0522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Phillips ML, Young AW, Senior C, et al. A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature. 1997;389:495–8. doi: 10.1038/39051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, et al. Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron. 1996;17:875–87. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Brett M, Christoff K, Cusack R, et al. Using the Talairach atlas with the MNI space. Neuroimage. 2001;13:S85. [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Lancaster JL, Tordesillas-Gutierrez D, Martinez M, et al. Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1194–205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Ellison-Wright I, Glahn DC, Laird AR, et al. The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: an anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:1015–23. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Chikazoe J, Konishi S, Asari T, et al. Activation of right inferior frontal gyrus during response inhibition across response modalities. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:69–80. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, et al. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:1051–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Owen AM, McMillan KM, Laird AR, et al. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.McMillan KM, Laird AR, Witt ST, et al. Self-paced working memory: validation of verbal variations of the n-back paradigm. Brain Res. 2007;1139:133–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Witt ST, Laird AR, Meyerand ME. Functional neuroimaging correlates of finger-tapping task variations: an ALE meta-analysis. Neuroimage. 2008;42:343–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Li H, Chan RC, McAlonan GM, et al. Facial emotion processing in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Schizophr Bull. 2009 May 20; doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn190. [Epub ahead of print] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Laird AR, McMillan KM, Lancaster JL, et al. A comparison of label-based review and ALE meta-analysis in the Stroop task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25:6–21. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Husted DS, Shapira NA, Goodman WK. The neurocircuitry of obsessive-compulsive disorder and disgust. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006;30:389–99. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Adolphs R. Fear, faces, and the human amygdala. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18:166–72. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Davis M, Whalen PJ. The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6:13–34. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Calder AJ, Keane J, Manes F, et al. Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:1077–8. doi: 10.1038/80586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Baillieux H, De Smet HJ, Paquier PF, et al. Cerebellar neurocognition: insights into the bottom of the brain. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110:763–73. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2008.05.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Schmahmann J. The role of cerebellum in affect and psychosis. J Neurolinguistics. 2000;13:189–214. [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Reiman EM, Lane R, Ahern G, et al. Neuroanatomical correlates of externally and internally generated human emotion. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:918–25. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.7.918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Sacchetti B, Baldi E, Lorenzini CA, et al. Cerebellar role in fear-conditioning consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:8406–11. doi: 10.1073/pnas.112660399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Turner BM, Paradiso S, Marvel CL, et al. The cerebellum and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45:1331–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Scheuerecker J, Frodl T, Koutsouleris N, et al. Cerebral differences in explicit and implicit emotional processing–an fMRI study. Neuropsychobiology. 2007;56:32–9. doi: 10.1159/000110726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Rudebeck PH, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MF. The contribution of distinct subregions of the ventromedial frontal cortex to emotion, social behavior, and decision making. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8:485–97. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.4.485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Ongur D, Price JL. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:206–19. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Ghashghaei HT, Hilgetag CC, Barbas H. Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Neuroimage. 2007;34:905–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Bishop SJ. Neurocognitive mechanisms of anxiety: an integrative account. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11:307–16. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Banks SJ, Eddy KT, Angstadt M, et al. Amygdala-frontal connectivity during emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2007;2:303–12. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Nagai M, Kishi K, Kato S. Insular cortex and neuropsychiatric disorders: a review of recent literature. Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22:387–94. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:383–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2000;10:295–307. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Miller SL, Celone K, DePeau K, et al. Age-related memory impairment associated with loss of parietal deactivation but preserved hippocampal activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:2181–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706818105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE. Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22:789–832. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00130-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Tremeau F. A review of emotion deficits in schizophrenia. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2006;8:59–70. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2006.8.1/ftremeau. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Marwick K, Hall J. Social cognition in schizophrenia: a review of face processing. Br Med Bull. 2008;88:43–58. doi: 10.1093/bmb/ldn035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Jemel B, Mottron L, Dawson M. Impaired face processing in autism: Fact or artifact? J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:91–106. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0050-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Norbury R, Mackay CE, Cowen PJ, et al. Short-term antidepressant treatment and facial processing. Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Br J Psychiatry. 2007;190:531–2. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.031393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Sergi MJ, Green MF. Social perception and early visual processing in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2003;59:233–41. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00405-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Tsoi DT, Lee KH, Khokhar WA, et al. Is facial emotion recognition impairment in schizophrenia identical for different emotions? A signal detection analysis. Schizophr Res. 2008;99:263–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Whittaker JF, Deakin JF, Tomenson B. Face processing in schizophrenia: defining the deficit. Psychol Med. 2001;31:499–507. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701003701. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Sergi MJ, Rassovsky Y, Nuechterlein KH, et al. Social perception as a mediator of the influence of early visual processing on functional status in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:448–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Sachs G, Steger-Wuchse D, Kryspin-Exner I, et al. Facial recognition deficits and cognition in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004;68:27–35. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00131-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Green MJ, Williams LM, Davidson DJ. Processing of threat-related affect is delayed in delusion-prone individuals. Br J Clin Psychol. 2001;40:157–65. doi: 10.1348/014466501163607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Kline JS, Smith JE, Ellis HC. Paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenic processing of facially displayed affect. J Psychiatr Res. 1992;26:169–82. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(92)90021-f. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Drevets WC, Price JL, Furey ML. Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression. Brain Struct Funct. 2008;213:93–118. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0189-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience : JPN are provided here courtesy of Canadian Medical Association

RESOURCES