Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as: Sex Transm Dis. 2009 Dec;36(12):750–756. doi: 10.1097/OLQ.0b013e3181b0f311

Using Respondent Driven Sampling in a Hidden Population at Risk of HIV Infection: Who do HIV-positive recruiters recruit?

Daniela Abramovitz *, Erik M Volz , Steffanie A Strathdee *, Thomas L Patterson , Alicia Vera *, Simon DW Frost §,, for Proyecto ElCuete
PMCID: PMC2788103  NIHMSID: NIHMS139718  PMID: 19704394

Abstract

Background

Respondent driven sampling (RDS) is a network-based method used to recruit hidden populations. Since it is respondent-driven, RDS is prone to bias. However, these biases could facilitate recruitment of high risk networks. We examined recruitment patterns of HIV-positive injection drug users (IDUs) and identified factors associated with being recruited by an HIV-positive IDU in a RDS-based study.

Methods

IDUs aged >=18, who injected within the last month and resided in Tijuana, Mexico, were recruited using RDS and underwent interviews and testing for HIV, syphilis, and TB. Weighted logistic regression was used to identify predictors of being recruited by an HIV-positive IDU.

Results

Of 1056 IDUs, HIV-positive subjects comprised 4.4% of the sample and generated 4.7% of recruits, indicating that recruitment effectiveness did not vary by HIV-status. However, 10% of the subjects recruited by HIV-positive recruiters were infected with HIV as compared to 4.1% of subjects recruited by HIV-negative recruiters, (P=0.06), a difference that, after controlling for whether the recruiter and recruit injected drugs together, attained statistical significance (P=0.04), indicating that recruitment patterns differed by HIV-status. Factors independently associated with being recruited by an HIV-positive IDU included lifetime syphilis infection, ever having sex with an HIV-positive person, knowing someone with HIV/AIDS, being recruited at a shooting gallery, having recently used the local needle exchange program, and having a larger number of recent arrests for track-marks.

Conclusion

HIV-positive IDUs have different recruitment patterns than HIV-negative IDUs, with HIV-positive IDUs tending to recruit other HIV-positive IDUs. Social and environmental factors along with risk behaviors were independently associated with being the recruit of an HIV-positive IDU in Tijuana. While the goal of this study was not to recruit HIV+ or other high-risk persons, our results suggest that RDS has the potential to successfully be used in the identification of HIV+ or other high risk individuals.


Respondent driven sampling (RDS) is a network-based method to recruit hidden populations1 that is increasingly used in HIV-related studies of persons who engage in illicit drug use, commercial sex work, and men having sex with men.27, 9 RDS involves direct recruitment of peers by their peers, a dual system of incentives, and a coupon system. Recruitment starts with an initial set of subjects known as “seeds”, and continues in waves, with “seeds” recruiting first-wave respondents, first-wave respondents recruiting the second-wave respondents, and so on, until the final sample size is achieved. Respondents are typically monetarily compensated for interview completion as well as for each peer that they successfully recruit. A coupon system is used to monitor the recruitment quota (i.e. the number of peers one can recruit into the study) and recruitment information is used to link recruiters to recruits.

RDS is an adaptation of traditional chain-referral sampling methods first introduced by Coleman10 to study characteristics of social networks. It was specifically designed to eliminate some of the biases associated with these methods, such as bias due to non-random selection of seeds, volunteerism, and masking.1, 1113 Although RDS can be successful in eliminating these biases, it is prone to other sources of bias such as differential recruitment effectiveness, differential recruitment patterns, and heterogeneity in degree.1112, 1415

Differential recruitment effectiveness occurs when some groups are better at recruiting than others. When this occurs, the group with better recruitment effectiveness usually becomes over-represented in the sample.11 Over-representation takes place when the population is homophilous (i.e. its members are more likely to connect with other individuals who are similar to themselves), the opposite being true for heterophilous populations (i.e. its members are more likely to connect with other individuals who are dissimilar to themselves). However, since most populations are homophilous, over-representation of groups with better recruitment effectiveness is much more common than under-representation.

Differential recruitment patterns are usually the result of individuals’ tendencies to associate with other individuals who are similar to them, also known as homophily. This causes personal networks to be homogeneous with regard to many socio-demographic, behavioral, and intrapersonal characteristics.16 The presence of homophily will cause a greater correlation between the sample and its seeds. In the presence of differential recruitment, homophily may bias the sample because recruitment patterns will reflect affiliation patterns, with preference for ties within a group. 11

Heterogeneity in degree refers to differences between groups with respect to network size. When such differences exist, subjects with larger network sizes are over-sampled because more recruitment paths lead to them. 11

In public health, the notion that biased samples can yield benefits is rarely entertained. A biased sample can be problematic if valid statistical inference cannot be made. However, when sources of bias can be identified and quantified, bias becomes less problematic because one may correct for it and obtain unbiased (or at least asymptotically unbiased) estimates of parameters of interest. Bias in RDS has been extensively studied and methods to assess, quantify, and correct for it have been developed and thoroughly described.1115, 1718 However, the potential benefits that RDS bias may yield have been under-studied. We explored bias in an RDS-based study by examining patterns of recruitment of HIV-positive and HIV-negative recruiters. We first determined to what extent differential recruitment effectiveness, differential recruitment patterns, and heterogeneity in degree, by HIV-status, were present in our sample. We subsequently identified factors associated with being recruited by an HIV-positive IDU. While the goal of this study was not to recruit HIV+ or other high-risk persons, our results have implications for intervention studies that may consider using RDS to identify HIV+ or other high risk individuals.

Methods

Study population

Study subjects were 1056 IDUs (1024 recruits and 32 seeds) recruited between April, 2006 and April, 2007, in Tijuana, Mexico into a prospective study of behavioral and contextual factors associated with HIV, syphilis, and tuberculosis (TB) infection. Eligibility criteria included: age ≥18 years; having injected illicit drugs within the past month as confirmed by inspection of injection stigmata (track-marks); ability to speak Spanish or English; willingness and ability to provide informed consent; and having no plans to permanently move out of the city in the following 18 months. Methods were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of California, San Diego and the Ethics Board of the Tijuana General Hospital.

Study design and procedures

Participants were recruited via RDS, whereby a diverse group of seeds, heterogeneous in age, gender, and geographic location underwent an interview, were educated on how to refer other eligible IDUs, and were given uniquely coded coupons to refer their peers to the study, as described previously.7 Of the 32 subjects treated as seeds, 24 were productive at recruiting other individuals into the study. Recruitment continued in waves as subjects returning with coupons were given coupons to recruit members from their social networks. While men were given three coupons, women received anywhere between six and twelve coupons in an unsuccessful attempt to recruit more women. Computerized interviews, produced via Questionnaire Development System (QDS), were conducted by indigenous outreach workers through the use of a modified recreational vehicle and a storefront office. IDUs completed an interviewer-administered survey that elicited information on sociodemographic, behavioral and contextual characteristics.

Study staff recorded serial number of coupons given to respondents and those from respondents who were enrolled into the study. This information helped us link each recruit with his/her recruiter, to enable adjustments for correlations between recruiter and recruit. We also collected network size information with respect to injection drug use and used this information for a multiplicity adjustment, in which the study participants were weighted by the reciprocal of their network sizes.

Laboratory Testing

The “Determine”® rapid HIV antibody test was administered to determine the presence of HIV antibodies (Abbott Pharmaceuticals, Boston, MA). All reactive samples were then tested using an HIV-1 enzyme immunoassay and immunofluorescence assay. Syphilis serology used the rapid plasma reagin (RPR) test (Macro-Vue, Becton Dickenson, Cockeysville, MD, USA). RPR-positive samples were subjected to confirmatory testing using the Treponema pallidum particle agglutination assay (TPPA) (Fujirebio, Wilmington, DE, USA). Quantitative nontreponemal test titers were obtained for subjects who tested positive to the RPR test. Titers ≥1:8 were considered to be consistent with active syphilis infection. QuantiFERON® TB Gold In-Tube (QFT-G) was used for detecting Mycobacterium tuberculosis infection. Specimen testing was conducted at the San Diego County Health Department. HIV/STI test results were provided to participants after confirmation; those testing positive were referred to the municipal health clinic for free medical care.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses compared primarily recruits of HIV-positive and of HIV-negative IDUs. Since “seeds” did not have a recruit, they were excluded from these comparisons. Additionally, in order to assess recruitment effectiveness and recruitment patterns, by HIV status, we compared HIV-positive subjects with HIV-negative subjects. The entire sample was used for these comparisons. Depending on whether distributional assumptions were met or not, continuous outcomes were examined using either t-tests or Wilcoxon rank sum tests. Similarly, binary outcomes were examined using either Pearson’s χ2 test or Fisher’s exact test. To control for multiple testing, the raw p-values associated with outcomes within each area of interest (i.e. sociodemographics, social influence, individual behaviors/risks, and structural/environmental factors) were adjusted for false discovery rate (FDR) by using the Hochberg and Benjamini’s method. 8 While both, raw and FDR adjusted p-values are listed in table 1, the corresponding statistical inferences are based on FDR adjusted p-values. To identify factors associated with being the recruit of an HIV-positive IDU, we performed univariate and multivariate logistic regressions. For model building we used a manual procedure, where all of the variables that had attained a significance level <=10% in the univariate models were considered for inclusion in a multivariate model. Lack of multicollinearity between the predictor variables in the final model was confirmed by appropriate values of the largest condition index and of the variance inflation factors.

TABLE 1.

Characteristics of IDUs recruited by HIV-positive and HIV-negative recruiters in Tijuana, Mexico: 2006–2007

Baseline Characteristics Recruitees of HIV Positive Recruiters (n1=50) Recruitees of HIV Negative Recruiters (n2=974) Total (N=1024) Raw P-value FDR Adjusted P-value
Sociodemographics
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) age 35.5 (31–41) 37 (31–42) 36.5 (31–42) 0.906 0.906
37.0 (8.3) 37.0 (8.3) 37.0 (8.3)
Female 6 (12.0%) 145 (14.9%) 151 (14.7%) 0.574 0.861
Thinks of oneself as gay/lesbian 1(2.0%) 9(0.9%) 10(1.0%) 0.396 0.713
Thinks of oneself as bisexual 2(4.0%) 16(1.6%) 18(1.8%) 0.219 0.678
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 7(6–9) 8 (6–9) 8 (6–9) 0.331 0.713
number of years of education completed 7.0 (3.6) 7.4 (3.4) 7.4 (3.4)
Speaks some English 20 (40.0%) 475 (48.8%) 495 (48.3%) 0.226 0.678
Average monthly income ≥2500 pesos 35 (71.4%) 775 (81.5%) 810 (81.0%) 0.079 0.678
Married/common-law 15 (30.0%) 304 (31.2%) 319 (31.2%) 0.857 0.906
Born outside of Baja California 34(68.0%) 653(67.0%) 687(67.0%) 0.888 0.906
Social Influence
Sex partner is an IDU* 0 (0.0%) 24 (3.2%) 24 (3.0%) 0.635 0.635
Median (IQR) and Mean (SD) 40 (21–70) 22 (6–50) 22 (6–50) 0.008 0.015
number of IDUs in social network 58.1 (9.2) 38.4 (9.9) 39.1 (9.9)
Came in contact with recruiter on the street 8 (16.0%) 438 (45.0%) 446 (43.6%) <0.001 <0.001
Came in contact with recruiter in a shooting gallery 15 (30.0%) 116 (11.9%) 131 (12.8%) <0.001 <0.001
Relationship with the recruiter: friend 40 (80.0%) 595 (61.1%) 635 (62.0%) 0.007 0.015
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) # 12 (8–18) 10 (7–12) 10 (7–12.5) 0.039 0.061
hours spent daily on the street* 12.7 (6.7) 10.7 (5.5) 10.8 (5.6)
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 2(0–3) 2 (1–3) 2 (1–3) 0.29 0.319
number of people usually injected with* 2.6 (4.0) 2.6 (4.9) 2.6 (4.9)
Ever been forced to have sex 4 (8.0%) 44 (4.5%) 48 (4.7%) 0.288 0.319
High perceived risk of HIV infection compared to others 26 (53.1%) 419 (43.6%) 445 (44.1%) 0.195 0.268
Knows personally at least one HIV+ person 30(60.0%) 375(38.5%) 405(39.6%) 0.003 0.008
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 1.5 (0–4) 0 (0–2) 0 (0–2) <0.001 <0.001
number of HIV+ people known personally 3.4 (5.9) 1.7 (4.9) 1.8 (5.0)
Individual Behaviors/Risks
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 14(10–19) 15 (9–22) 14.5 (9–22) 0.812 0.869
duration (years) of injection 16.0 (9.5) 15.5 (9.0) 15.6 (9.0)
Injected > once per day* 48(96.0%) 893(92.0%) 941(92.2%) 0.422 0.057
Any receptive needle sharing* 27 (54.0%) 576 (59.1%) 603 (58.9%) 0.472 0.551
Obtained syringes from needle exchange program* 17 (34.0%) 155 (15.9%) 172 (16.8%) <0.001 0.001
Male having sex with male 8(18.2%) 216(26.1%) 224(25.7) 0.242 0.424
Has had any sex in the past 6 months 10(20.0%) 259(26.6%) 269(26.3%) 0.298 0.464
Has had sex with a regular sex partner*(among those who had sex during the past 6 months) 1(10.0%) 155(59.9%) 156(58.0%) 0.002 0.014
Has had sex with casual partner(s)*(among those who had sex during past 6 months) 10(100%) 163(63.2%) 173(64.6%) 0.016 0.045
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 4(3–7) 1.5(0–4) 2(0–4) 0.006 0.021
number of casual sex partners* 5.3(4.0) 6.0(21.7) 6.0(21.3)
Never used a condom during sex with a casual sex partner*(among those who reported having sex with casual partners) 7(70.0%) 59(36.2%) 66(38.2%) 0.044 0.088
Ever traded sex in exchange for money, drugs, goods or shelter 12 (24.0%) 223 (23.0%) 235 (23.0%) 0.869 0.869
Ever had unprotected sex with HIV infected person 5 (10.0%) 19 (2.0%) 24 (2.4%) 0.005 0.021
Ever tested for HIV 23 (46.0%) 391 (40.1%) 414 (40.4%) 0.411 0.537
Previously unaware of the HIV- positive status (among those diagnosed as HIV-positive) 3(60.0%) 39(97.5%) 42(93.3%) 0.03 0.070
HIV status 5(10.0%) 40(4.1%) 45(4.4%) 0.063 NA
Positive for syphilis antibodies 16 (32.0%) 142 (14.6%) 158 (15.5%) <0.001 NA
Syphilis titer ≥1:8 6 (12.0%) 72 (7.4%) 78 (7.7%) 0.267 NA
Structural/Environmental Factors
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 14 (8–29) 15 (5–30) 14 (5–30) 0.497 0.547
number of years lived in Tijuana (IQR) 18.3 (14.5) 17.8(14.6) 17.8 (14.6)
Homeless* 5 (10.2%) 128 (13.1%) 133 (13.0%) 0.551 0.551
Normally injected drugs outside* 10(20.0%) 239 (24.5%) 249 (24.3%) 0.466 0.547
Normally injected drugs at shooting gallery* 15 (30.0%) 377 (38.7%) 392 (38.3%) 0.212 0.333
Ever traveled to the U.S. 43 (86.0%) 753 (77.3%) 796 (77.7%) 0.150 0.275
Has been forced by police to leave the place where lived* 14 (32.6%) 111(12.6%) 125(13.5%) <0.001 0.001
Ever forced by police to rush an injection 25(50.0%) 312(32.0%) 337(32.9%) 0.008 0.040
Police affected access to a new syringe* 10(20.0%) 109(11.2%) 119(11.7%) 0.059 0.158
Ever been arrested 41 (82.0%) 849 (87.3%) 890 (87.0%) 0.281 0.386
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) 5(0, 15) 3 (0, 10) 3 (0, 10) 0.065 0.158
number of times arrested for having track marks ¥ 12.5 (17.7) 7.5 (12.0) 7.7 (12.3)
Median (IQR) and mean (SD) # 2(1–4) 1 (0–3) 2 (0–3) 0.072 0.158
times in jail/prison 3.4 (5.0) 2.9 (6.2) 2.9 (6.1)
*

Past 6 months;

Values adjusted for multiplicity;

¥

Among those ever arrested (n=890)

To correct for differential recruitment effectiveness by HIV status, we calculated inverse probability weights based on individualized recruitment weights. The weights include a factor to control for respondent’s heterogeneity of degree (i.e. multiplicity) and were derived via RDS Analytical Tool (RDSAT). 19 The variable containing the weights was used as a covariate in the logistic regression models. Interactions between this covariate and the predictors were also explored. To account for correlation between recruiter and recruit, we created a variable indicating who the recruiter of each subject was, and used this variable as a cluster variable in the GEE algorithm. An exchangeable correlation structure within each cluster was assumed (i.e. correlation between any two subjects recruited by the same recruiter was assumed to be the same).

To estimate the model’s rate of classification accuracy, we conducted Monte-Carlo cross-validation. 20 The Monte-Carlo procedure randomly split the data into model fitting and model testing subsets. For each iteration, the proportion of observations for which the model agreed with the outcome was calculated and averaged over the 10, 000 iterations to obtain the estimate for the classification accuracy of the model.

Results

Description of HIV-positive and HIV-negative recruits

Our sample consisted of 1056 (32 seeds and 1024 recruits) participants. Table 1 summarizes the baseline characteristics of the 1024 recruits (50 recruits of HIV-positive IDUs and 974 recruits of HIV-negative IDUs). Most recruits were males (85.2%), the median age was 37, and 67.2% were born outside the Mexican state of Baja California. Overall, 45 recruits were diagnosed as HIV-positive and the vast majority (93.3%) were previously unaware of their own serostatus. However, the percentage of subjects unaware of their ownserostatus was marginally higher among those recruited by HIV-negative subjects as compared to those recruited by HIV-positive subjects (97.5% vs. 60%; PFDR-adj.=0.07). Recruits of HIV-positive versus HIV-negative recruiters did not differ with respect to age, gender, educational attainment, income, marital status, birthplace, or sexual orientation.

We next examined group differences in terms of social influences. Recruits of HIV- positive IDUs had larger numbers of IDUs in their social network (median = 40 vs. 22; PFDR-adj. =0.02), personally knew a larger number of HIV-positive individuals (median=1.5 vs. 0; PFDR-adj. <0.001), were more likely to be friends with the recruiter (80% vs. 61.1%; PFDR-adj. =0.02), and were more likely to come in contact with the recruiter in a shooting gallery (30% vs. 11.9%; PFDR-adj. <0.001). They also spent marginally more time on the street (median=12 vs. 10 hrs per day; PFDR-adj. =0.06), but were less likely to come in contact with the recruiter on the street (16% vs. 45%; PFDR-adj. <0.001). The two groups did not differ in terms of the proportion ever having been forced to have sex, the proportion with high perceived risk of HIV infection, the proportion with an IDU sex partner or the number of people that they inject with.

We next compared recruits of HIV-positive versus HIV-negative recruiters in terms of risk behaviors, protective behaviors and infectious disease status. During the previous 6 months, recruits of HIV-positive IDUs were less likely than their HIV-negative counterparts to have had sex with a regular sex partner (10% vs. 59.9%; PFDR-adj. =0.01), more likely to have had sex with casual sex partner(s) (100% vs. 63.2%; PFDR-adj. =0.05), and marginally more likely to have never used a condom during sex with their casual partner(s) (70% vs. 36%; PFDR-adj. =0.09). They also were more likely to report having had unprotected sex with an HIV-infected partner (10% vs. 2%; PFDR-adj. =0.02) and to report obtaining syringes from the Tijuana needle exchange program (34% vs. 15.9%; PFDR-adj. =0.001). Last but not least, they were more likely to test positive for syphilis antibodies (32% vs. 14.6%; P<0.001), and marginally more likely to test positive for HIV (10% vs. 4%; P=0.06). The proportion of subjects with syphilis titers ≥1:8 was greater among the recruits of HIV-positive (12% vs. 7.7%) but this difference did not reach statistical significance (P=0.27). Also, groups did not differ in their reported years of injection; frequency of injection; receptive needle sharing; male having sex with male status; ever trading sex; and having been tested previously for HIV.

Finally, we examined group differences for a variety of structural influences. Compared to recruits of HIV-negative IDUs, recruits of HIV-positive IDUs were significantly more likely to report having been forced by police to rush an injection (50% vs. 32%; PFDR-adj. 0.04), and to having been forced by police to leave the place where they lived in the previous 6 months (32.6% vs. 12.6%; PFDR-adj.= 0.001). No differences between groups were found with respect to number of years lived in Tijuana; homelessness; places where they inject drugs; ever being arrested; and the number of times in jail/prison.

All variables attaining P values ≤0.10 in univariate regressions were considered as candidates for multivariate models (Table 1). The following six factors remained independently associated with being the recruit of an HIV positive recruiter: personally knowing someone with HIV/AIDS (Adj.OR = 2.4); having had unprotected sex with an HIV-infected person (Adj.OR = 6.7) lifetime syphilis infection (Adj.OR = 2.8), meeting the recruiter in a shooting gallery (Adj.OR = 4.5), having obtained needles from a needle exchange program in the previous 6 months (Adj.OR = 2.3), and having a larger number of arrests for track marks (Adj.OR = 1.11) (Table 2). The average classification rate of accuracy for this model yielded by Monte-Carlo cross-validation was 94.96% with a standard error of 0.01%.

TABLE 2.

Factors independently associated with being the recruit of a HIV positive recruiter among IDUs in Tijuana, Mexico

Variable Adjusted Odds Ratio 95% Confidence Interval
Knows someone with HIV/AIDS 2.40 1.29–4.46
Ever had unprotected sex with HIV infected person 6.69 2.17–19.65
Positive for syphilis antibodies 2.80 1.44–5.48
Came in contact with recruiter at shooting gallery 4.50 2.28–8.89
Obtained syringes from needle exchange program* 2.34 1.22–4.50
Number of times arrested for track marks (per 5 arrests)* 1.11 1.01–1.22
*

Past 6 months

Recruitment effectiveness

Among the 1056 study participants, 47 (4.5%) were diagnosed as HIV-positive. Twenty of the 47 (42.6%) HIV-positive and 449 of the 1009 (44.5%) HIV-negative subjects were recruiters, with HIV-positive subjects generating 4.9% (50/1024) of the recruits. Since HIV-positive subjects comprised 4.5% of the sample and generated 4.9% of the recruits we found no evidence that recruitment effectiveness varied by HIV status.

Differential recruitment patterns

HIV-positive subjects recruited 10% (5/50) other HIV-positive subjects and HIV-negative subjects recruited 4.1% (40/974) HIV-positive subjects (P=0.06). After controlling for whether the recruiter and the recruit injected drugs together, the odds of an HIV-positive recruiter recruiting an HIV-positive individual were 2.8 times greater than the corresponding odds of an HIV-negative recruiter (P=0.04). We also found that 20% of the HIV-positive recruiters as compared to 0% of the HIV-negative recruiters, recruited more than one HIV-positive subject into the study (P=0.002), indicating that recruitment patterns differed significantly by HIV status. Only one of the 20 HIV-positive recruiters was aware of his HIV-positive serostatus, suggesting that HIV-positive recruiters were more likely to recruit other HIV positive IDUs even though they were not aware of their own serostatus.

Heterogeneity in degree

As chain referral samples are biased towards individuals with larger network sizes, we adjusted the distribution of network size by weighting the distribution of network sizes by the inverse of the network size.13 This led to a significant drop in estimated network size, from a median of 95 and 70 for recruits of HIV-positive and HIV-negative recruiters to corresponding values of 40 and 22, respectively. Recruits of HIV-positive recruiters had significantly larger network sizes than recruits of HIV-negative recruiters (PFDR-adj. =0.02) and thus had a higher probability of being recruited into the study.

Discussion

An important contribution of this study is the finding that HIV-positive IDUs were significantly more likely than HIV-negative IDUs to recruit other HIV-positive IDUs into a research study, even though they were unaware of their own HIV serostatus. While this finding reinforces the existing knowledge that in order to draw valid statistical inferences from RDS-based studies, one has to assess and possibly adjust for differential recruitment bias, our study suggests that RDS could be successfully used to identify HIV-positive individuals who are unaware of their HIV-positive status and refer them to counseling and medical services. One has to keep in mind that the goal of our study was not to recruit HIV-positive or other high-risk individuals, and as such, only 2 of the seeds in our study were HIV-positive. However, RDS can be easily adapted to over-sample individuals, such as undiagnosed HIV cases, if we know certain characteristics of the people who are more likely to recruit them. For instance, if the goal is to recruit high-risk individuals, one strategy may be to initiate RDS recruitment with most or all seeds consisting of HIV-positive and/or other high risk individuals.

A second important finding is that compared to recruits of HIV-negative IDUs, recruits of HIV-positive IDUs have larger IDU networks. This indicates not only that recruits of HIV-positive IDUs have a higher probability of selection but also suggests that they have a heightened vulnerability to HIV infection. For instance, having a larger number of peers who are IDU has been associated with higher levels of needle sharing21, overdose22, and lower drug use cessation23, which are known risk factors for HIV infection. Thus, RDS can potentially be used to identify not only undiagnosed HIV cases but also HIV-negative individuals at high risk of HIV acquisition.

Finally, compared to recruits of HIV-negative IDUs, recruits of HIV-positive IDUs were more likely to know someone infected with HIV, to have unprotected sex with an HIV-infected person, to have a higher lifetime prevalence of syphilis antibodies, and to have been more frequently arrested for track marks. These factors have been associated with an increased risk of acquiring HIV in our population7, and others.2426

Our study was limited by the fact that it only analyzed characteristics and behaviors of the HIV-positive and HIV-negative recruits cross-sectionally, allowing us to identify factors associated with being recruited by an HIV-positive recruit, without being able to ascribe causal interpretations to the data. Longitudinal studies are needed to ascribe such interpretations. We were also limited by the fact that only two of the seeds in our sample were HIV-positive which precluded the comparison of recruitment patterns between HIV-positive and HIV-negative seeds. Another limitation was that only one of the twenty HIV-positive recruiters was aware of his HIV-positive serostatus, so our finding that HIV positive recruiters are more likely than HIV-negative recruiters to recruit other HIV-positive individuals can only be generalized to HIV-positive recruiters who were unaware of their HIV serostatus. Further studies including HIV-positive respondents who are aware of their HIV serostatus are needed to determine their recruitment patterns. Similarly, our sample included a low proportion of female participants. However, an RDS convergence analysis with respect to gender indicated that the sample composition reached equilibrium, thus supporting the robustness of our results.

Overall, our findings suggest that beyond success in recruiting hidden populations, RDS may facilitate identification of persons with undiagnosed HIV infection and high risk networks. As such, RDS could become a vehicle for what has been referred to as “the new generation of network-based interventions.”29 An example of such intervention is the Peer Driven Intervention (PDI) of the Eastern Connecticut Health Outreach Project, whereby IDUs were recruited through RDS and successfully motivated to recruit and educate each other about HIV prevention through a voucher-based incentive system.293029 Heckathorn and his colleagues29 found that compared to the traditional street-based outreach intervention, the PDI not only accessed a larger number of people and was more effective in reducing their levels of HIV risk behaviors, but it did so at a lower cost. Similarly, in an evaluation of four different sampling methods (targeted, stratified, time-space, and respondent driven sampling) Semaan31 concluded that RDS uses the “least amount of formative research and resources.”

These findings suggest that RDS-based network interventions may be especially useful in resource-constrained settings with emerging HIV epidemics, like Tijuana. Our study opens the doors to a very practical application that can be used in fields other than HIV research. If our results can be generalized to other populations, RDS can be easily adapted to over-sample individuals if we know certain characteristics of the people who are more likely to recruit them.

Acknowledgments

Sources of support: The authors gratefully acknowledge the contributions of study participants and binational staff and investigators from the University of California San Diego and Pro-COMUSIDA, Tijuana, Mexico. Proyecto ElCuete is funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA; grant R01DA019829). During the study period Dr. Volz was funded under grant 4 T32 AI07384 from National Institutes of Health, and Dr. Frost was funded under grant NR10961 from National Institute of Nursing Research and by a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award.

References

  • 1.Heckathorn DD. Respondent driven sampling: a new approach to study hidden populations. Soc Probl. 1997;44:174–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Abdul-Quader AS, Heckathorn DD, McKnight C, Bramson H, Nemeth C, Sabin K, Gallagher K, Des Jarlais DC. Effectiveness of respondent-driven sampling for recruiting drug users in New York City: findings from a pilot study. J Urban Health. 2006;83(3):458–76. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9052-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Abdul-Quader AS, Heckathorn DD, Sabin K, Saidel T. Implementation and analysis of respondent driven sampling: lessons learned from the field. J Urban Health. 2006;83(7):i1–5. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9108-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Yeka W, Maibani-Michie G, Prybylski D, Colby D. Application of Respondent Driven Sampling to collect baseline data on FSW and MSM for HIV risk reduction interventions in two urban centres in Papua New Guinea. J Urban Health. 2006;83(7 Suppl 1):i60–72. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9103-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Johnston LG, Sabin K, Hien MT, Huong PT. Effectiveness of respondent-driven sampling to recruit female sex workers in two cities in Vietnam. J Urban Health. 2006;83(Suppl 7):16–28. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9099-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Magnani R, Sabin K, Saidel T, Heckathorn DD. Review of sampling hard-to-reach and hidden populations for HIV surveillance. AIDS. 2005;19(Suppl 2):S67–S72. doi: 10.1097/01.aids.0000172879.20628.e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Strathdee SA, Lozada R, Pollini RA, Brouwer KC, Mantsios A, Abramovitz D, Rhodes T, Latkin CA, Loza O, Alvelais J, Magis-Rodriguez CMD, Patterson TL. Individual, social, and environmental influences associated with HIV infection among injection drug users in Tijuana, Mexico. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2008;47(3):369–76. doi: 10.1097/QAI.0b013e318160d5ae. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ramirez-Valles J, Heckathorn DD, Vazquez R, Diaz RM, Campbell RT. From networks to populations: the development and application of respondent-driven sampling among IDUs and Latino gay men. AIDS Behav. 2005;9(4):387–402. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-9012-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Coleman JS. Relational analysis: the study of social organization with survey methods. Hum Organ. 1958;17:28–36. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Heckathorn DD. Respondent driven sampling II: deriving valid populations estimates from chain-referral samples of hidden populations. Soc Probl. 2002;49(1):11–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Heckathorn DD, Semaan S, Broadhead RS, Hughes JJ. Extensions of respondent-driven sampling: a new approach to the study of injection drug users aged 18–25. AIDS Behav. 2002;6:55–67. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Salganik MJ, Heckathorn DD. Sampling and estimation in hidden populations using respondent-driven sampling. Sociol Method. 2004;34:193–240. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Heckathorn DD. Extensions of respondent-driven sampling: analyzing continuous variables and controlling for differential recruitment. Sociol Methodol. 2007;6:151–208. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ramirez-Valles J, Heckathorn DD, Vasquez R, Diaz RM, Campbell R. The fit between theory and data in respondent-driven sampling: response to Heimer. AIDS Behav. 2005;9(4):409–14. doi: 10.1007/s10461-005-9012-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks. Annual Review of Sociology. 2001;27:415–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Goel S, Salganik M. [Accessed 10 September 2008];Respondent driven sampling as Markov Chain Monte Carlo. doi: 10.1002/sim.3613. Available at: http://www.cam.cornell.edu/~sharad/papers/RDSasMCMC.pdf. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 18.Volz E, Heckathorn DD. Probability based estimation theory for Respondent Driven Sampling. Journal of Official Statistics. 2008;24(1):79–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Volz E, Wejnert C, Degani I, Heckathorn DD. Respondent-driven sampling analysis tool (RDSAT) version 5.6. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shao J. Linear model selection by cross-validation. JASA. 1993;88:486–494. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Costenbader EC, Astone NM, Latkin CA. The dynamics of injection drug users’ personal networks and HIV risk behaviors. Addiction. 2006;101:1003–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01431.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tobin KE, Hua W, Costenbader EC, Latkin CA. The association between change in social network characteristics and non-fatal overdose: results from the SHIELD study in Baltimore, MD, USA. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;87:63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Latkin CA, Knowlton AR, Hoover D, Mandell W. Drug network characteristics as a predictor of cessation of drug use among adult injection drug users: a prospective study. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 1999;25:463–473. doi: 10.1081/ada-100101873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.El-Bassel N, Gilbert L, Wu E, Chang M. A social network profile and HIV risk among men on Methadone: Do social networks matter? J Urban Health. 2006;8(4):602–13. doi: 10.1007/s11524-006-9075-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Buchacz K, Klausner JD, Kerndt PR, Shouse LR, Onorato I, McElroy PD, Schwendenmann J, Tambe PB, Allen M, Coye F, Kent CK, Park MN, Hawkins K, Samoff E, Brooks JT. HIV incidence among men diagnosed with early syphilis in Atlanta, San Fransisco, and Los Angeles, 2004 to 2005. J Acquir Immune Deffic Syndr. 2008;47(2):234–240. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Service SK, Blower SM. HIV transmission in sexual networks: an empirical analysis. Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 1995;260(1359):237–44. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1995.0086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Erickson BH. The relational basis of attitudes. In: Wellman B, Berkowitz SD, editors. Social Structures: A Network Approach. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge, U.K: 1988. pp. 99–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Leenders R. Longitudinal Behavior of Network Structure and Actor Attributes: Modeling Interdependence of Contagion and Selection. In: Patrick Doreian, Frans Stokman., editors. Evolution of Social Networks. Gordon and Breach Publishers; 1997. pp. 165–84. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Heckathorn DD, Broadhead RS, Anthony DL. Aids and social networks: HIV prevention thorough network mobilization. Sociological Focus. 1999;32(2):159–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Broadhead RS, Heckathorn DD, Weakliem DL, Anthony DL, Madray H, Mills RJ, Hugheset J. Harnessing peer networks as an instrument for AIDS prevention: Results from a peer-driven intervention. Public Health Reports. 1998;113(Suppl 1):42–57. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Semaan S, Lauby J, Liebman J. Street and Network Sampling in Evaluation Studies of HIV Risk-Reduction Interventions. AIDS Rev. 2002;4:213–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES