Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Dec 9.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Sci Med. 2008 Jul 16;67(6):982–987. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.06.002

Do social comparisons explain the association between income inequality and health?: Relative deprivation and perceived health among male and female Japanese individuals

Naoki Kondo a,b,*, Ichiro Kawachi a, SV Subramanian a, Yasuhisa Takeda c, Zentaro Yamagata b
PMCID: PMC2791046  NIHMSID: NIHMS160387  PMID: 18632196

Abstract

Relative deprivation has been hypothesized as one of the pathways accounting for the link between income inequality and health. We tested this hypothesis in a large national sample of men and women in Japan. Our survey included a probability sample of 22,871 men and 24,243 women aged 25–64, from whom information was gathered on demographic variables, household income, occupation or employment status, and self-rated health. Our measure of relative deprivation was the Yitzhaki Index, which calculates the deprivation suffered by each individual as a function of the aggregate income shortfall for each person relative to everyone else with higher incomes in that person’s reference group. We modeled several alternative reference groups, including others with the same occupation, others of the same age group, and others living in the same geographic area (prefecture), as well as combinations of these. Generalized estimating equations demonstrated that higher relative deprivation was associated with worse self-rated health. Even after controlling for absolute income as well as other sociodemographic factors, the odds ratio and its 95% confidence intervals (CI) for poor health ranged from 1.09 (95% CI: 1.02–1.16) to 1.18 (95% CI: 1.11–1.26) for men and from 1.10 (95% CI: 1.04–1.16) to 1.16 (95% CI: 1.09–1.23) for women per 1 million increase in the Yitzhaki Index. As such, relative income deprivation is associated with poor self-rated health independently of absolute income, and relative deprivation may be a mechanism underlying the link between income inequality and population health.

Keywords: Japan, Psychosocial deprivation, Relative deprivation, Income inequality, Pathways

Introduction

It is widely accepted that income poverty or lower absolute income adversely affects health (Lynch & Kaplan, 2000; Marmot & Wilkinson, 2005). Although controversial, many papers have also suggested that income inequality or relative income affects health (Subramanian & Kawachi, 2004; Wilkinson & Pickett, 2006). The empirical evidence linking income inequality to health outcomes is strongest in the case of U.S. state level analyses (Backlund, Rowe, Lynch, Wolfson, Kaplan, & Sorlie, 2007; Lochner, Pamuk, Makuc, Kennedy, & Kawachi, 2001). Outside the United States, the evidence showing a link between income inequality and population health is less secure, with some positive studies (Larrea & Kawachi, 2005; Subramanian, Delgado, Jadue, Vega, & Kawachi, 2003; Subramanian, Kawachi, & Smith, 2007), but also several null studies (Blakely, Kawachi, Atkinson, & Fawcett, 2004; Gerdtham & Johannesson, 2004; Osler, Prescott, Gronbak, Christensen, Due, & Engholm, 2002). In Japan, Shibuya, Hashimoto, and Yano (2002) previously reported that prefectural level income inequality in that country was not associated with poor self-rated health (“prefecture” refers to the geographical/administrative unit of local government in Japan). On the other hand, they found that a measure of relative income (calculated as the difference between an individual’s income and median prefectural income) was associated with worse health status. The problem, however, is that this measure of relative income is collinear with absolute income.

Until relatively recently, few studies have attempted to tease out the mechanisms underlying the relationship between income inequality and health. Two distinct pathways have been proposed through which income inequality is believed to affect population health: a macro policy-related pathway and an individual-level psychosocial pathway (Kawachi, Fujisawa, & Takao, 2007). At the societal level, income inequality is believed to erode social cohesion, cooperation, and support for the provision of public goods (Kawachi & Kennedy, 2006). Recent evidence from experimental economics – in which income inequality was manipulated in the context of trust games – supports this mechanism (Anderson, Mellor, & Milyo, 2004). Alternatively, the psychosocial pathway positsthat income inequality will heighten individuals’ sense of relative deprivation, resulting in frustration, shame, stress, and adverse health consequences (Wilkinson, 2001). The theory of social comparison, initially proposed by Festinger (1954), supports this hypothesized mechanism. Empirical support for this pathway was provided recently by studies in the United States, which examined individual relative deprivation as a predictor of increased risks of mortality, as well as smoking, obesity, and mental health services utilization (Eibner & Evans, 2005; Eibner, Sturn, & Gresenz, 2004). However, few other studies have been reported on the association between relative deprivation and health outside the United States (Gravelle & Sutton, in press; Jones & Wildman, 2008). Evidence is particularly sparse among Asian countries, even though the region has experienced widening income inequalities since the 1990s (Khang, Lynch, Yun, & Lee, 2004; Kondo, Subramanian, Kawachi, Takeda, & Yamagata, in press).

In the present study, we sought to provide a test of the association between relative deprivation and health in Japan. Although previous studies have not found an association between aggregated measures of prefecture-level income inequality and health in Japan (Nakaya & Dorling, 2005; Shibuya et al., 2002), this may be due to the timing of these studies, i.e., they may have presented a limited snapshot at an early stage of the surge in income inequality in Japan. The situation may change in the future. Meanwhile, we are not aware of a previous study that has formally tested the association between individual-level sense of relative deprivation and health in Japanese society.

Methods

Data

Data on demographic variables, household income, occupation, and perceived health status were obtained from the 2001 Comprehensive Survey of the Living Conditions of People on Health and Welfare (CSLC) conducted by the Japanese government (Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, 2001). Trained investigators visited households and interviewed all household members within census tracts which were randomly selected from all prefectures in the nation (N = 47). A health-related questionnaire was mailed to participants and collected by investigators at the time of home visiting. The survey was conducted across 5240 census tracts including 247,278 households (response rate = 87.4%), from which 31,871 households were further randomly selected and surveyed regarding income and savings (response rate = 79.5%). The total sample size available for our analyses was 22,871 men and 24,243 women, aged 25–64. Participants who completed the income questionnaire were on average 0.5 years older, 0.6% more likely to be women, and 1.0% more likely to be married compared to the group who did not provide income data (including both selected and not selected for income survey). This study was based on the secondary analysis of the data in which any individual identifiers were removed.

Relative deprivation

Following the recently adopted method by Eibner and colleagues (Eibner & Evans, 2005; Eibner et al., 2004), relative deprivation was operationalized in the present study using the Yitzhaki Index (Yitzhaki, 1979), which is itself based on the theory of relative deprivation articulated by Runciman (1966). In brief, relative deprivation for each individual is calculated as the aggregated shortfall in income between that individual and everyone else with higher incomes in that person’s reference group.

YitzhakiIndexi=1Nj(yjyi)yj>yi

where the amount of relative deprivation for individual i is the sum of the income gap between individuals i and j (yjyi, where j has higher income than i) divided by the total number of people in the reference group (N).

Since we cannot know the reference group for each individual (i.e., to whom each person compares him/herself), our approach is to fit alternative definitions of reference groups. For the present study, we used three different reference groups – others with the same occupation, others in the same age group, and others living in the same geographical area (prefecture) – as well as combinations of these. Occupations were categorized into the following groups: professional/technician, manager/administrator, sales/service/clerical workers, security/transportation/laboring workers, farming/fishery/forestry workers, and unknown jobs, as well as economically inactive including homemaker (female only) and unemployed. We categorized each subject into one of four age groups: 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, and 55–64 years old.

Self-rated health

The CSLC elicited respondents’ self-rated health with the single item: “What is your current health status: excellent, very good, good, fair, or poor?”. From this question, we created a dichotomous outcome measure with poor perceived health if the respondent answered fair or poor. It has been reported that self-rated health is associated with various objective health measures and strongly predicts future onset of mortality (Idler & Benyamini, 1997; Kondo et al., 2005; Kopp, Skrabski, Rethelyi, Kawachi, & Adler, 2004).

Covariates

To demonstrate the independent contribution of relative deprivation on health, it is critical to control for absolute income. Household income was calculated by summing up the exact individual incomes of all household members. The income was pre-tax, included benefits and inheritances, and adjusted for household size (equivalence elasticity = 0.5). We used the continuous income variable in the multivariate model. A previous study reported on the sufficient reliability of CSLC income data (e.g., 1.3% standard error of median income) (Ishii & Furuya, 2005). In our regression models, we further controlled for age, marital status, and employment status. Marital status was categorized as married, never married, separated, or divorced. We did not include behavioral risk factors such as smoking, alcohol drinking, exercise habits, and health check-ups since we considered these variables as potentially mediating the association between relative deprivation and health status (Lynch & Kaplan, 2000).

Statistical analysis

All analyses were stratified by gender. To address potential clustering of data arising from the stratified sampling strategy, we used a multivariate generalized estimating equation approach with a logit link function (PROC GEN-MOD, the SAS statistical package version 9.1, SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Municipalities were selected as cluster units. We calculated odds ratio (OR) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for reporting poor health according to the increase in relative deprivation adjusting for sociodemographic confounders (including absolute income). To evaluate the trend in the association between relative deprivation and self-rated health, we also divided Yitzhaki Index by quintile and carried out a trend test.

Results

Descriptive analysis showed that people in older age groups were more likely to report poor health. Perceived health status also varied across marital status and occupations (Table 1). The proportion of poor perceived health was higher in the lower absolute income categories (data not shown).

Table 1.

Percent reported poor health by the sociodemographic characteristics and summary of economic indicators in Japanese males and females, 2001 CLSC

Variable Male (n = 22,871)
Female (n = 24,243)
Respondents (%) No. (%) of poor self-rated health or median [25%, 75%] Respondents (%) No. (%) of poor self-rated health or median [25%, 75%]
Age group (years old)
25–34 4913 (23.0) 334 (6.8) 5305 (23.4) 474 (8.9)
35–44 4905 (23.0) 435 (8.9) 5235 (23.1) 534 (10.2)
45–55 5690 (26.6) 601 (10.6) 5914 (26.0) 792 (13.4)
55–64 5856 (27.4) 762 (13.0) 6259 (27.6) 959 (15.3)
Marital status
Married 17,295 (75.6) 1650 (10.1) 18,667 (77.0) 2126 (12.1)
Never married 4691 (20.5) 369 (8.7) 3321 (13.7) 302 (9.8)
Separated 244 (1.1) 28 (13.0) 992 (4.1) 135 (15.3)
Divorced 641 (2.8) 85 (14.5) 1263 (5.2) 196 (17.0)
Occupation/employment status
Employed
 Professional/technician 4000 (17.5) 314 (8.2) 2328 (9.6) 245 (11.0)
 Manager/administrator 1967 (8.6) 179 (9.4) 343 (1.4) 36 (11.0)
 Sales/service/clerical 5879 (25.7) 501 (9.0) 7547 (31.1) 760 (10.7)
 Security/transportation/labour 6853 (30.0) 575 (9.1) 2560 (10.6) 232 (9.6)
 Farming/fishery/forestry 938 (4.1) 87 (10.2) 729 (3.0) 74 (11.3)
 Unknown Job 1680 (7.4) 179 (11.7) 2461 (10.2) 295 (13.0)
Unemployed
 Homemakera 7502 (31.0) 971 (13.8)
 Unemployed 1554 (6.8) 297 (21.9) 773 (3.2) 146 (22.3)
Household income (10,000 Japanese Yen)
351.1 [231.5, 509.1] 334.0 [214.7, 490.8]
Relative income deprivation by the definition of reference group (10,000 Japanese yen)
Prefecture of residence 121.1 [64.7, 195.1] 121.5 [66.0, 194.6]
Occupation 116.4 [61.2, 188.4] 118.6 [63.5, 191.9]
Age group 118.5 [61.9, 198.0] 120.3 [64.2, 196.4]
Prefecture and age 114.6 [57.6, 192.2] 116.4 [60.6, 191.8]
Occupation and age 109.6 [57.2, 184.3] 111.6 [57.5, 191.0]

CSLC: Comprehensive Survey of Living Condition of People on Health and Welfare.

a

Male homemakers were categorized in unemployed due to the small number of male homemakers.

Univariate regression models demonstrated that higher relative deprivation was significantly associated with poor reported health (Table 2). ORs (95% CI) of poor health by 1 million increase in relative deprivation varied from 1.08 (1.03–1.13) to 1.27 (1.21–1.32) in men and from 1.08 (1.03–1.13) to 1.21 (1.16–1.26) in women, depending on the type of reference group that was fitted. Regardless of the reference group assumed, we found a statistically significant trend between increasing quintile of relative deprivation and higher ORs of poor self-rated health. When the models were further adjusted for potential confounding variables including absolute income, the trend between increasing relative deprivation and worse health remained statistically significant across all reference group comparisons. When prefecture of residence was used as a reference group, relative deprivation showed the strongest association with poor self-rated health. Adjusted OR (95% CI) of the deprivation by the increase in one million Japanese yen per person was 1.18 (1.11–1.26) in men and 1.16 (1.09–1.23) in women, while the ORs with other reference groups ranged from 1.11 to 1.16 in men and from 1.10 to 1.14 in women. We did not find clear gender differences.

Table 2.

Crude and adjusted odds ratio (95% confidence intervals) for reporting poor health by the level of relative deprivation in Japanese men and women, 2001 CLSC

Reference group defined by
Prefecture of residence Occupation Age group Prefecture and age Occupation and age
Male
Crude
 Continuousa 1.21 (1.15–1.27) 1.08 (1.03–1.13) 1.27 (1.21–1.32) 1.26 (1.21–1.32) 1.11 (1.07–1.17)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.53 (1.33–1.75) 1.20 (1.04–1.38) 1.75 (1.52–2.02) 1.73 (1.50–1.99) 1.35 (1.17–1.56)
p for trend <0.0001 0.005 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001
Model 1
 Continuousa 1.23 (1.17–1.29) 1.11 (1.06–1.16) 1.20 (1.15–1.26) 1.20 (1.15–1.26) 1.07 (1.02–1.12)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.62 (1.41–1.87) 1.30 (1.13–1.50) 1.54 (1.33–1.78) 1.53 (1.33–1.77) 1.22 (1.06–1.41)
p for trend <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.003
Model 2
 Continuousa 1.18 (1.11–1.26) 1.11 (1.04–1.19) 1.16 (1.08–1.24) 1.16 (1.09–1.23) 1.09 (1.02–1.16)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.37 (1.12–1.67) 1.22 (1.01–1.49) 1.25 (1.00–1.58) 1.25 (1.02–1.53) 1.26 (1.04–1.54)
p for trend 0.0008 0.006 0.04 0.01 0.01
Female
Crude
 Continuousa 1.17 (1.12–1.22) 1.08 (1.03–1.13) 1.20 (1.15–1.25) 1.21 (1.16–1.26) 1.13 (1.09–1.18)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.39 (1.23–1.58) 1.15 (1.02–1.30) 1.61 (1.42–1.82) 1.58 (1.39–1.79) 1.43 (1.26–1.62)
p for trend <0.0001 0.0008 <0.0001 <0.0001 <0.0001
Model 1
 Continuousa 1.16 (1.11–1.21) 1.08 (1.03–1.13) 1.13 (1.08–1.18) 1.15 (1.10–1.20) 1.07 (1.02–1.11)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.39 (1.23–1.58) 1.18 (1.04–1.34) 1.38 (1.21–1.57) 1.37 (1.20–1.56) 1.23 (1.08–1.40)
p for trend <0.0001 0.0003 <0.0001 <0.0001 0.002
Model 2
 Continuousa 1.16 (1.09–1.23) 1.12 (1.06–1.19) 1.12 (1.05–1.19) 1.14 (1.08–1.21) 1.10 (1.04–1.16)
 Top vs bottom quintile 1.30 (1.10–1.54) 1.24 (1.05–1.48) 1.28 (1.06–1.54) 1.27 (1.07–1.51) 1.33 (1.12–1.58)
p for trend 0.0004 0.0002 0.01 0.005 0.001

Model 1 was adjusted for age group and marital status and model 2 was adjusted for age group, marital status, absolute income, and employment status.

CSLC: Comprehensive Survey of Living Condition of People on Health and Welfare.

a

Odds ratios by the increase in 1 million Japanese yen per person of relative deprivation.

Discussion

To date, empirical studies of income inequality and health have yielded mixed findings (Lynch et al., 2004; Subramanian & Kawachi, 2004). An association between income inequality and worse health (including mortality and poor self-rated health) has been reported most consistently in the United States when income distribution was measured at the state level (Wilkinson & Pickett, 2006). Studies carried out in countries that are more egalitarian than the U.S. – such as Sweden and Denmark – have failed to find an association between income inequality and mortality (Gerdtham & Johannesson, 2004; Osler et al., 2002). A possible explanation for this discrepancy is that states with strong welfare protections (such as the Nordic countries) have managed to buffer their citizens against the deleterious health consequences of inequality. Alternatively, the association between income inequality and poor health is non-linear, so that the adverse health impact of inequality becomes evident only when a certain threshold of inequality is exceeded (Subramanian & Kawachi, 2004).

The mixed findings of previous studies of income inequality and health in Japan could be explained by either or both of the foregoing scenarios. Thus Shibuya et al. (2002) found that there was no association between prefectural income inequality and poor self-rated health, using an earlier release (in 1995) of the CSLC (the same survey used in the present study). Their null findings could have been due to the relatively egalitarian distribution of incomes in Japan compared to the United States (the threshold hypothesis), and/or due to Japanese welfare protection policies (e.g., universal health care insurance coverage established in 1961). The Japanese situation is changing, however, due to the decade-long economic recession since the early 1990s following the collapse of the so-called bubble economy (Kondo et al., in press). Although income inequality at the prefecture level does not appear to be associated with poor self-rated health, the findings of the present study suggest that individual relative deprivation is associated with worse health status among working-age Japanese men and women, irrespective of absolute income. Relative deprivation is closely linked to income inequality to the extent that the Yitzhaki Index would be expected to rise as inequality grows even though individuals’ absolute incomes do not change. Our study may be therefore viewed as a test of the individual-level psychosocial mechanism linking income inequality to health outcomes. The principal limitation of the relative deprivation approach is that we do not know which reference groups individuals choose in making their social comparisons. It is likely that individuals make several kinds of comparisons in their daily lives, including comparisons to co-workers in the same workplace, their neighbors, and parents at a similar stage in their life-course, as well as other people’s lifestyles portrayed in the national media. Unfortunately, we lacked individual information about these bases of social comparison. Instead, we adopted the approach of modeling a series of reference groups based on data available within our survey, including others in the same occupation, others belonging to the same age group, and others residing in the same prefecture as the respondent. Each of our models could be interpreted as sensitivity analyses that test the theory of relative deprivation. As our results indicate, regardless of the reference group fitted, we found consistent evidence for an association between relative deprivation and poor health status. Moreover, when we defined relative deprivation by prefectures of residence, relative deprivation was most strongly associated with health, possibly supporting the importance of geographical proximity when social comparisons take a role in the pathway linking income inequality to health. However, because we have insufficient statistical support (i.e., overlapped confidence intervals), further confirmation is needed for the differences among reference groups.

Although our study did not address the specific mechanisms linking relative deprivation to poor health status, some clues are provided by the study by Eibner and colleagues (Eibner & Evans, 2005; Eibner et al., 2004), which found that relative deprivation in a U.S. sample was linked to stress-related outcomes, including smoking, obesity, and mental health services utilization.

Recent evidence has also linked subjective social status with worse health outcomes (Adler, Epel, Castellazzo, & Ickovics, 2000; Singh-Manoux, Marmot, & Adler, 2005), where social position was assessed by the respondent’s rating of where they stood on an imaginary 10-rung ladder of social hierarchy. Subjective social status may be interpreted as a gauge of relative deprivation. However, the ladder instrument does not have the same quantitative interpretation as the Yitzhaki Index. For example, if the incomes of all other people in the reference group rise relative to the respondent, the Yitzhaki Index is expected to increase. However, the response of the same person within the domain of subjective social status is unknown, since it has not been determined how individuals integrate information on the resulting income shortfall when they are asked to position themselves on the 10-rung ladder instrument.

Our study has limitations. Because of the cross-sectional design, we cannot rule out reverse causation. Even though we controlled for absolute income, it is nonetheless possible that the association between the Yitzhaki Index and poor self-rated health reflected the unmeasured influence of poor health on an individual’s ability to earn income. The association between relative deprivation and health could also reflect other omitted variables such as individual variations in ability, temperament, and personality, which were not measured in our survey. In addition, we found mild variance inflation in some multivariate models based on quintiled Yitzhaki Index values when covariates (including absolute income) were added to the models: i.e., variance inflation factors (VIFs) between 2.0 and 3.3. This suggested the existence of collinearity between the two income-related variables. Although the models using continuous Yitzhaki Index allowed us to conclude that high relative deprivation was consistently associated with poor health throughout the reference groups tested, caution must be applied in interpreting the shape of the association between quintiles of the Yitzhaki Index and self-rated health. Future prospective investigations are warranted to test these ideas, including ideally, examining the association between exogenous changes in relative deprivation (for example, induced by economic shocks) and resulting changes in health status.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a grant from the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare, Japan and Pfizer Health Research Foundation. N. Kondo is a recipient of the fellowship grant in the 2006–07 Takemi Program in International Health at the Harvard School of Public Health, funded by the Japan Foundation for the Promotion of International Medical Research Cooperation; and in the 2007 Abe Fellowship Program administered by the Social Science Research Council and the American Council for Learned Societies in cooperation and with funds provided by the Japan Foundation Center for Global Partnership. S.V. Subramanian is supported by the National Institutes of Health Career Development Award (NHLBI 1 K25 HL081275). These sponsors did not have any involvements in this study’s design, data collection, analysis, data interpretation, and writing paper. We thank Malavika Subramanyam for her helpful suggestions and input during the design of the study.

Contributor Information

Naoki Kondo, Email: nkondo@hsph.harvard.edu.

Ichiro Kawachi, Email: ikawachi@hsph.harvard.edu.

S.V. Subramanian, Email: svsubram@hsph.harvard.edu.

Yasuhisa Takeda, Email: takeda-yasuhisa@mhlw.go.jp.

Zentaro Yamagata, Email: zenymgt@yamanashi.ac.jp.

References

  1. Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology. 2000;19(6):586–592. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.6.586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson LR, Mellor JM, Milyo J. Inequality and public good provision: an experimental analysis. College of William and Mary Department of Economics Working paper #14 2004 [Google Scholar]
  3. Backlund E, Rowe G, Lynch J, Wolfson MC, Kaplan GA, Sorlie PD. Income inequality and mortality: a multilevel prospective study of 521 248 individuals in 50 US states. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36(3):590–596. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blakely T, Kawachi I, Atkinson J, Fawcett J. Income and mortality: the shape of the association and confounding New Zealand Census-Mortality Study, 1981–1999. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;33(4):874–883. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Eibner C, Evans WN. Relative deprivation, poor health habits, and mortality. The Journal of Human Resources. 2005;40(3):591–620. [Google Scholar]
  6. Eibner C, Sturn R, Gresenz CR. Does relative deprivation predict the need for mental health services? The Journal of Mental Health Policy and Economics. 2004;7(4):167–175. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Festinger L. A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations. 1954;7(2):117–140. [Google Scholar]
  8. Gerdtham UG, Johannesson M. Absolute income, relative income, income inequality, and mortality. The Journal of Human Resources. 2004;39(1):228–247. [Google Scholar]
  9. Gravelle H, Sutton M. Income, relative income, and self-reported health in Britain 1979–2000. Health Economics. 2008 doi: 10.1002/hec.1354. in press. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Idler EL, Benyamini Y. Self-rated health and mortality: a review of twenty-seven community studies. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1997;38(1):21–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ishii F, Furuya H. Precision of income distribution in the Comprehensive Survey of the Living Conditions of People on Health and Welfare (kokumin seikatsu kiso tyousa ni okeru shotoku bunpu no seido ni tsuite) Kosei no Shihyou. 2005;52(12):7–12. [Google Scholar]
  12. Jones AM, Wildman J. Health, income and relative deprivation: evidence from the BHPS. Journal of Health Economics. 2008;27(2):308–324. doi: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2007.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kawachi I, Fujisawa Y, Takao S. The health of Japanese – what can we learn from America? Journal of National Institute of Public Health. 2007;56(2):114–121. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kawachi I, Kennedy BP. The Health of Nations: Why Inequality is Harmful to Your Health. 2. New York: New Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  15. Khang YH, Lynch JW, Yun S, Lee SI. Trends in socioeconomic health inequalities in Korea: use of mortality and morbidity measures. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2004;58(4):308–314. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.012989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Kondo N, Mizutani T, Minai J, Kazama M, Imai H, Takeda Y, et al. Factors explaining disability-free life expectancy in Japan: the proportion of older workers, self-reported health status, and the number of public health nurses. Journal of Epidemiology. 2005;15(6):219–227. doi: 10.2188/jea.15.219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kondo N, Subramanian SV, Kawachi I, Takeda Y, Yamagata Z. Economic recession and health inequalities in Japan: analysis with a national sample, 1986–2001. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2008 doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.070334. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Kopp M, Skrabski A, Rethelyi J, Kawachi I, Adler NE. Self-rated health, subjective social status, and middle-aged mortality in a changing society. Behavioral Medicine. 2004;30(2):65–70. doi: 10.3200/BMED.30.2.65-72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Larrea C, Kawachi I. Does economic inequality affect child malnutrition? The case of Ecuador. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;60(1):165–178. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lochner K, Pamuk E, Makuc D, Kennedy BP, Kawachi I. State-level income inequality and individual mortality risk: a prospective, multilevel study. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91(3):385–391. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.3.385. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Lynch J, Kaplan GA. Socioeconomic Position. In: Berkman LF, Kawachi I, editors. Social Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
  22. Lynch J, Smith GD, Harper S, Hillemeier M, Ross N, Kaplan GA, et al. Is income inequality a determinant of population health? Part 1. A systematic review. Milbank Quarterly. 2004;82(1):5–99. doi: 10.1111/j.0887-378X.2004.00302.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Marmot MG, Wilkinson RG. Social Determinants of Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  24. Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare. Comprehensive survey of living condition of people on health and welfare. Tokyo: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  25. Nakaya T, Dorling D. Geographical inequalities of mortality by income in two developed island countries: a cross-national comparison of Britain and Japan. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;60(12):2865–2875. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.11.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Osler M, Prescott E, Gronbak M, Christensen U, Due P, Engholm G. Income inequality, individual income, and mortality in Danish adults: analysis of pooled data from two cohort studies. British Medical Journal. 2002;324(7328):1–4. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7328.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Runciman WG. Relative Deprivation and Social Justice: A Study of Attitudes to Social Inequality in Twentieth-century. England: University of California Press; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  28. Shibuya K, Hashimoto H, Yano E. Individual income, income distribution, and self rated health in Japan: cross sectional analysis of nationally representative sample. British Medical Journal. 2002;324(7328):16–19. doi: 10.1136/bmj.324.7328.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG, Adler NE. Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosomatic Medicine. 2005;67(6):855–861. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000188434.52941.a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Subramanian SV, Delgado I, Jadue L, Vega J, Kawachi I. Income inequality and health: multilevel analysis of Chilean communities. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2003;57(11):844–848. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.11.844. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Subramanian SV, Kawachi I. Income inequality and health: what have we learned so far? Epidemiologic Reviews. 2004;26(1):78–91. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Subramanian SV, Kawachi I, Smith GD. Income inequality and the double burden of under- and overnutrition in India. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2007;61(9):802–809. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.053801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Wilkinson RG. Mind the Gap: Hierarchies, Health, and Human Evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  34. Wilkinson RG, Pickett KE. Income inequality and population health: a review and explanation of the evidence. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62(7):1768–1784. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Yitzhaki S. Relative deprivation and the Gini coefficient. The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 1979;93(2):321–324. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES