Abstract
Background
Seizures are common in children admitted with severe falciparum malaria and are associated with neuro-cognitive impairments. Prolonged febrile seizures are associated with hippocampal damage and impaired memory. It was hypothesized that severe malaria causes impaired everyday memory which may be associated with hippocampal damage.
Methods
An everyday memory battery was administered on 152 children with cerebral malaria (CM) (mean age, 7 y 4 months [SD 13 months]; 77 males) 156 children (mean age, 7 y 4 months [SD, 14 months]; 72 males) with malaria plus complex seizures (MS) and 179 children (mean age, 7 y 6 months [SD, 13 months]; 93 males) unexposed to either condition.
Results
CM was associated with poorer everyday memory [95% CI, -2.46 to -0.36, p = 0.004] but not MS [95% CI, -0.91 to 1.16, p = 1.00] compared to unexposed children. Children with exposure to CM performed more poorly in recall [95% CI, -0.79 to -0.04, p = 0.024] and recognition subtests [95% CI, -0.90 to -0.17, p = 0.001] but not in prospective memory tests compared to controls. The health factors that predicted impaired everyday memory outcome in children with exposure to CM was profound coma [95% CI, 0.02 to 0.88, p = 0.037] and multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia [95% CI, 0.05 to 0.78, p = 0.020], but not seizures.
Discussion
The findings show that exposure to CM was associated with a specific impairment of everyday memory. Seizures commonly observed in severe malaria may not have a causal relationship with poor outcome, but rather be associated with profound coma and repeated metabolic insults (multi-hypoglycaemia) that are strongly associated with impaired everyday memory.
Background
Plasmodium falciparum is the most common parasitic infection of the central nervous system. Every year, over two billion people are infected and over a million die [1]. Children living in sub-Saharan Africa bear the brunt of the disease in which the most common neurological complications are seizures and impaired consciousness i.e. cerebral malaria (CM) [2]. These neurological manifestations are associated with neurocognitive sequelae [3]. Previous reports show that between 5 and 26% of children exposed to severe falciparum malaria have cognitive impairment, in particular affecting language, executive functioning [4-6] and memory [3]. It is important to establish whether cognitive and memory deficits in children with cerebral malaria are a consequence of seizures, the underlying CM or a combination of factors as this would guide therapeutic strategies that aim to minimize adverse memory outcomes following CM.
Seizures are a common feature in children with severe or cerebral falciparum malaria; on admission, over 80% have a history of seizures and these recur in over 60% during the course of admission [7]. In the context of acute malaria, it is unclear what proportion of seizures are caused by fever, by metabolic stress or by the sequestration of the parasites within the vasculature of the brain [8]. Although fever commonly occurs in malaria, the seizures observed in children admitted to hospital are not regarded as simple febrile seizures, since over 50% occur when the child is afebrile [9], and they are often complex (i.e. focal, prolonged or repetitive) [10]. Prolonged febrile seizures not precipitated by malaria, have been associated with hippocampal injury [11-13], which may present later as temporal lobe seizures associated with mesial temporal sclerosis (MTL), impaired memory [14], and/or behavioural problems [15]. It is therefore possible that prolonged seizures within the context of acute malaria could also result in hippocampal injury associated with later episodic memory impairments (i.e. memory for everyday episodes) [16,17]. Direct evidence linking cerebral malaria to hippocampal damage is confined to two single cases, one with pathology following surgery [18] and the other a cognitive study [19]. It remains uncertain in these cases whether the MTS was present prior to the episode of malaria, or whether it was acquired during the acute episode either as a function of the seizures or of direct effects of the parasite. There is, however, little data available that examines the association of severe malaria and hippocampal function, particularly in African children who bear the brunt of the disease.
The hypothesis was that children exposed to severe malaria may have hippocampal injury and therefore will have impaired everyday memory. The aims of the current study therefore were to;
1. Define memory impairments associated with cerebral malaria in African children
2. Investigate whether memory outcomes are worse in children with seizures
3. To evaluate clinical risk factors for memory impairments in children exposed to malaria
Methods
The study took place in the catchment area of Kilifi District Hospital (KDH), a malaria-endemic area on the coast of Kenya, where the population is largely dependent upon subsistence farming. Three groups of children were examined: those discharged following recovery from cerebral malaria (CM) (defined as a Blantyre coma score (BCS) of less or equal to 2 for 4 or more hours, a peripheral falciparum malaria parasitaemia and exclusion of other causes of encephalopathy [20]), Children discharged following malaria plus complex seizures (MS) (defined as more than two seizures within 24 hours or focal or prolonged for more than 30 minutes but did not lapse into coma), and a comparison group of children unexposed to either condition living in the same community.
The children had a neurological examination and underwent cognitive testing as part of a larger study, the results of which have been reported elsewhere [4,21,22]. Children were excluded if their parents or guardians refused informed written consent or the child refused verbal informed consent. The Kenya Medical Research Institute Scientific Steering and Ethical Review Committees approved the study.
Memory assessment
The Kilifi Creek Behavioural Memory Test for Children (KCBMT), an everyday memory battery adapted from the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT-C) [23] was administered. It was developed to examine everyday memory i.e. memory needed to lead everyday life independently. It is composed of sub-tests of recall (memory of past events), recognition (memory of past events given clues) and prospective memory (remembering to carry out some action in the future). The RBMT-C was modified to make the test materials more familiar to the local children. Objects familiar to the local children and photographs of local people were used to replace those in the RBMT-C. The content of the orienting questions were also changed to reflect local experience.
In order to ensure equal weighting, performance on each subtest was ranked on scale ranging from 0 (impaired), 1 (borderline) to 2 (normal), yielding a total "profile" score. In addition, three composite scores were created by summing the rankings of each of the following measures: (i) delayed recall subtests (i.e. story, route, message recall) (ii) recognition subtests (i.e. picture recognition and face recognition, referred to as 'recognition memory') and (iii) prospective memory (appointment, name, hidden object). Impaired everyday memory was defined as 2 standard deviations below the age-group total profile score.
Underlying Cognitive Measures
In the present study, it was not possible to use a standardized intelligence (IQ) tests since none are available appropriate for use in the target population. Instead, the cognitive language domains of a speech and language test to control for underlying cognitive abilities [24] were used. In particular, receptive vocabulary (Picture Vocabulary Test) and expressive language are used as a proxy for IQ.
Other Background Factors (Health and Environmental)
These factors were age (6, 7, 8 or 9 years) sex (male or female), socio-economic status (high or low), schooling (yes or no) and IQ (high or low). Clinical factors were denoted as either 'yes' or 'no' and included; profound coma (BCS = 0 or 1), prolonged coma (> 24 hours), deep breathing, hypoglycaemia (blood glucose < 2.2 mmol/L), multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia (≥ 3 episodes), severe anaemia (Hb < 5 g/dl), history of seizures before admission, prolonged seizures (> 30 minutes), multiple seizures (> 3 seizures), height for age (stunting) (z-score < -3SD) and weight for age (wasting) (z-score < -3SD). These were all obtained from the child's hospital records at admission.
Statistical analysis
Analysis was performed using SPSS for Windows version 15 (Chicago, Illinois, USA). The total profile score of the KBMTC was computed for each child and it represented their everyday memory. The everyday memory outcome and three composite measures of recall, recognition and prospective memory, of the three diagnostic groups i.e. CM, MS and unexposed children, were compared using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with age and schooling as covariates. The Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons was used in the post hoc comparisons (t-tests) for simple effects. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05. A multivariable logistic regression model, with impaired memory as a dependent variable, was used to determine those factors that are independently associated with poor everyday memory performance.
Results
All the children were born between 1991 and 1995 and were between 6:0 and 9:11 years during assessment which took place a minimum of 20 months post-discharge. A total of 152 children with a history of CM (mean age 7 y 4 months [SD 13 months]; 77 males) and 156 with a history of MS (mean age, 7 y 4 months [SD, 14 months]; 72 males) were selected from the admission database at the KDH. A random sample of 179 children unexposed to either condition (mean age, 7 y 6 months [SD, 13 months]; 93 males) was sampled from the census database of children living in the same study area.
Underlying Cognitive Abilities (Cognitive language measures)
An analysis of variance of the cognitive language measures by diagnosis revealed significant differences between groups for both receptive vocabulary (PVT) [F (2, 483) = 5.980, p = 0.003] and expressive language [F (2, 483) = 6.689, p = 0.001]. The mean receptive vocabulary score in CM was 20.6 (95% CI, 20.1 to 21.2), in MS was 21.5 (95% CI, 21.0 to 22.0) and in controls was 22.0 (95% CI, 21.5 to 22.5). After Bonferroni adjustment, there was a significant difference between CM and controls (p = 0.002). There was no significant difference between MS and controls or between the two malaria groups. The mean expressive language score in CM was 34.9 (95% CI, 34.1 to 35.8), in MS was 36.7 (95% CI, 35.8 to 37.5) and in controls was 37.0 (95% CI, 36.2 to 37.7). The Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons there was applied and there was significant difference between CM and controls (p = 0.002) and between CM and MS (p = 0.013).
Everyday Memory (Total Profile Score)
The diagnostic groups differed significantly by schooling [F (2, 484) = 4.904, p = 0.008], marginally by age (p = 0.069) but however did not differ by sex (p = 0.702) or socioeconomic status (p = 0.957).
The analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with total profile score (everyday memory) as dependent variable, diagnostic group (i.e CM, MS and unexposed) as fixed factor and, age, cognitive language scores and schooling as covariates revealed that diagnostic category was associated with impaired everyday memory [F (2, 482) = 7.377, p = 0.001]. Children with a history of CM had a mean total profile score of 15.7 (95% CI, 15.1 to 16.4), those with MS had 17.1 (95% CI, 16.5 to 17.8) and unexposed children 17.2 (95% CI, 16.6 to 17.8). Post-hoc comparisons for simple effects of disease exposure revealed significantly poorer performance in those children with a history of CM (p = 0.004) but not of those exposed to MS (p = 1.000) compared to unexposed children. Children with a history of CM performed significantly poorer than those exposed to MS (p = 0.002). Figure 1 illustrates the mean total profile scores for the three diagnostic groups.
Children's performance on composite measures
An ANCOVA of the composite measures revealed statistically significant group differences in recall memory [F (2, 482) = 4.128, p = 0.017] and recognition memory [F (2, 482) = 11.687, p < 0.001] but not in prospective memory items [p = 0.552, ns] (Figure 2). The mean scores for recall, recognition and prospective memory for the children with a history of CM [4.9 (95% CI, 4.7 to 5.1), 2.8 (95% CI, 2.6 to 3.0) and 2.9 (95% CI, 2.6 to 3.2) respectively], MS [5.2 (95% CI, 5.0 to 5.4), 3.4 (95% CI, 3.2 to 3.6) and 3.0 (95% CI, 2.7 to 3.2)] and unexposed children [5.3 (95% CI, 5.0 to 5.5), 3.2 (95% CI, 2.9 to 3.3) and 3.3 (95% CI, 3.0 to 3.6)] were compared.
Post-hoc pair-wise comparisons of diagnostic groups again revealed that these differences were largely accounted for by poorer performance in children with a history of CM compared to unexposed children. In both recall and recognition memory children with exposure to CM performed significantly more poorly (p = 0.024 and p = 0.001 respectively). The difference between children with a diagnosis of CM and those with a history of MS only reached significance for recognition memory (p < 0.001) but not recall (p = 0.064, ns). The difference between children exposed to MS and unexposed children did not reach significance on either composite measure [recall p = 1.00, ns and recognition p = 0.552, ns].
Background factors independently associated with performance in everyday memory
Univariable analysis showed that diagnostic group, nutrition, schooling, cognitive language score (used as IQ proxy), duration of seizures and number of seizures (Table 1) were associated with impaired everyday memory. Children with CM performed four times more poorly than unexposed children [OR 4.5, 95% CI 1.692 to 12.913]. Having a seizure that lasted at least five minutes increased the odds for poor performance approximately three-fold [OR 2.8, 95% CI 1.049 to 7.626], as did those who had seizures that lasted longer than 30 minutes [OR 2.8, 95% CI 1.064 to 7.740]. Children who had more than 9 seizures performed significantly poorer than those without seizures [OR 4.0, 95% CI 1.426 to 11.291]. These factors were then entered into a multivariable logistic regression analysis to determine which factors independently predicted poor outcome. In the full model, a significant association remained between diagnostic category (i.e. CM) and impaired everyday memory [B = -1.17, Wald χ2 = 4.650, 95% CI 0.108 to 0.899, p = 0.030]. Additional factors with a significant association were nutrition [B = 1.35, Wald χ2 = 9.373, 95% CI 1.628 to 9.215, p = 0.006] and schooling [B = -1.55, Wald χ2 = 10.612, 95% CI 0.083 to 0.539, p = 0.001]. The number of seizures or seizure duration did not independently predict everyday memory.
Table 1.
Variable | P-value | OR associated with impairment | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|
Sex (female) | 0.125 | 1.803 | 0.850 to 3.826 |
Age | |||
7 years | 0.652 | 0.791 | 0.285 to 2.193 |
8 years | 0.836 | 0.897 | 0.323 to 2.494 |
9 years | 0.902 | 1.062 | 0.407 to 2.771 |
Diagnosis | |||
Cerebral malaria | 0.003 | 4.675 | 1.692 to 12.913 |
Malaria plus seizures | 0.184 | 2.131 | 0.699 to 6.498 |
Nutrition | |||
weight for age | < 0.001 | 5.434 | 2.373 to 12.445 |
Height for weight | < 0.001 | 4.861 | 2.249 to 10.507 |
SES (high) | 0.235 | 0.618 | 0.280 to 1.367 |
Schooling | < 0.001 | 0.154 | 0.062 to 0.381 |
Cognitive language (IQ proxy)* | < 0.001 | 26.444 | 0.781 to 0.919 |
Epilepsy | 0.438 | 0.549 | 0.121 to 2.501 |
Seizure duration (at least 5 min) | 0.040 | 2.829 | 1.049 to 7.626 |
Seizure duration (5> × >30 min) | 0.936 | 0.957 | 0.324 to 2.821 |
Seizure duration (more than 30 min) | 0.037 | 2.870 | 1.064 to 7.740 |
Multiple seizures (less than 3) | 0.557 | 1.373 | 0.477 to 3.948 |
Multiple seizures (between 4 and 9) | 0.246 | 2.063 | 0.607 to 7.012 |
Multiple seizures (more than 9) | 0.008 | 4.012 | 1.426 to 11.291 |
*Cognitive language scores were used as a proxy for IQ. Age was compared to children aged 6 years old and diagnosis compared to unexposed children
Clinical health factors of CM children and everyday memory
Backward stepwise multivariable logistic regression analysis with impaired everyday memory as a dependent variable to examine the health factors associated with poor outcome in the children with a diagnosis of CM was used. The clinical factors were prolonged seizures, multiple seizures, deep coma, profound coma, severe hypoglycaemia, multiple hypoglycaemia, stunting (height for age) and wasting (weight for age). The results showed that impaired everyday memory was associated with profound coma [(B = -2.58, Wald χ2 = 7.210, 95% CI 0.02 to 0.88, p = 0.007] and multi-hypoglycaemic episodes [B = -1.62, Wald χ2 = 5.397, 95% CI 0.05 to 0.78, p = 0.020]. Again, the seizures variables did not significantly affect the outcome of children who had a history of CM.
Discussion
This study investigated the association between severe falciparum malaria in children and everyday memory impairment. To understand better the pathogenesis of cognitive impairment in severe malaria, we were interested in exploring whether impairment in everyday memory is hippocampal dependent. The results showed that everyday memory was significantly impaired in children with a history of CM. Children exposed to CM not only performed significantly more poorly on the total profile score but also on recall and recognition sub-tests of the everyday memory battery. It may be likely that the deficit in everyday memory in children with exposure to CM was related to impaired cognition as they were significantly different with controls in the expressive and receptive language tests, which acted as markers of underlying cognitive skills. However, children with a history of MS also have poorer language scores but their performance on the everyday memory test did not differ from unexposed children.
Two factors, however, suggest that the impairment of everyday memory may not be related to an underlying involvement of the hippocampus. Firstly, a pattern of everyday memory impairment (impaired recall memory and recognition memory but preserved prospective memory) not previously associated with specific damage to the extended hippocampal system [16,25-27] is observed. Then, poor recall in children exposed to CM may suggest a retrieval deficit associated with executive dysfunction and mediated by the frontal lobes [28]. In the absence of structural neuro-imaging, which was unavailable at the study setting, it remained difficult to associate poor performance directly to hippocampal impairment.
Overall, poor everyday memory was associated with poorer nutrition, lack of schooling and a diagnosis of CM. These factors have been found to have a great impact on the cognitive development of school-age children in developing countries [22,29,30]. The factors that were associated with poor outcome in children with a history of CM in the present study, according to the logistic regression model, were profound coma and multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia. Multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia may affect everyday memory through hippocampal damage [31,32]. The hippocampus is critical to the formation of long-term memory, and may be particularly sensitive to hypoglycaemia [33]. Coma in cerebral malaria may result from either sequestration of the infected erythrocytes or from metabolic dysfunctions [34]. The pathways of memory impairments could thus be diverse depending on the pathogenesis of coma. Previous studies, both human and animal, have suggested that cerebral malaria may affect the medial temporal lobe structures of the hippocampus [18,19] causing impaired functioning.
Previous studies in Africa on the neuro-cognitive outcomes of CM have also shown that duration of coma [35,36], depth of coma [36,37], seizures [4,35-38], and hypoglycaemia [36,37] are predictors of poor outcome. Conversely, a recent prospective, but smaller study in Uganda, did not find an association between these clinical factors and poor cognitive outcome [6]. The present study suggests impairment of structures involved with recall memory, which could include hippocampal structures that may have been compromised by an interaction of seizures and coma or hypoglycaemia. However, in the absence of neuro-imaging data, it is impossible to localize which structures were impaired in children with a history of cerebral malaria. However, from the present results, it is difficult to associate seizures with poor outcome, though they may have contributed.
Cognitive outcome post-severe malaria seems to depend on a range of risk factors, some of which are dependent on the social and economic environment of the child [3]. Further, the severity of illness at admission and during hospitalization is also important. The difference in everyday memory outcome between CM and MS suggests that memory deficits have originated from sources other than seizures per se, possibly the mechanisms that cause prolonged and profound coma or hypoglycaemia [21]. A particularly interesting finding of the study was that multiple seizures or seizure duration did not independently predict poor outcome of everyday memory. Our findings suggest that multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia and profound coma in CM are important factors in determining impairment of everyday memory.
Limitations of the study
The results of the present study should be interpreted within the limits of lack of data on the children's IQ. We used cognitive language scores as a proxy for IQ and assumed that nutritional and socio-economic effects were equally distributed amongst both exposed and unexposed children. There are some factors the present study was unable to ascertain. It cannot be ascertained that children with no recorded seizures post admission had seizures during their illness since they did not have continuous electroencephalographic monitoring. It is also possible that the coma was caused by severe metabolic imbalances especially multiple episodes of hypoglycaemia. Further it is possible that time spent under the glucose curve was more important than the lowest blood glucose concentration.
Conclusion
It is concluded that CM may be associated with everyday memory deficits, but the present results don't suggest specific hippocampal involvement. The impairment is associated with profound coma and multiple hypoglycaemic episodes and these may be important determinants of poor memory post infection. Studies incorporating standardised IQ and neuroimaging would help define this consequence further. Further studies should seek to determine why the effects of the number of seizures are not linear as would be expected.
List of abbreviations used
KCBMT: Kilifi Creek Behavioural Memory Test; CM: Cerebral malaria; MS: Malaria plus complicated seizures; RBMT-C: Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test for Children.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors' contributions
JAC, BGRN and CRJCN were responsible for the conception of the study, its design, data collection and critical review of the manuscript. PAH was involved in the adapting of the test battery to the local context and was involved in reviewing the manuscript. RI was involved in reviewing malaria cases to fulfil the strict criteria and reviewing the draft. MdH and FVK were involved in the interpretation of the memory data and reviewing of the manuscript. RCS and GWF offered statistical help as well as reviewing the manuscript. MK wrote the initial draft of the manuscript and all other authors contributed substantially to the content. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.
Financial support
CRJC Newton (grant no. 070114) and Julie A. Carter (grant no. 059336) are supported by The Wellcome Trust, UK. The opinions expressed here are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of Wellcome Trust. All authors have no conflict of interest to declare.
Contributor Information
Michael Kihara, Email: mkihara@kilifi.kemri-wellcome.org.
Julie A Carter, Email: j.carter@ich.ucl.ac.uk.
Penny A Holding, Email: penny.holding@uclmail.net.
Faraneh Vargha-Khadem, Email: f.khadem@ich.ucl.ac.uk.
Rod C Scott, Email: rscott@ich.ucl.ac.uk.
Richard Idro, Email: ridro@kilifi.kemri-wellcome.org.
Greg W Fegan, Email: gfegan@kilifi.kemri-wellcome.org.
Michelle de Haan, Email: m.de-haan@ich.ucl.ac.uk.
Brian GR Neville, Email: b.neville@ich.ucl.ac.uk.
Charles RJC Newton, Email: cnewton@kilifi.kemri-wellcome.org.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Dr Richard Chin for his insightful comments and Victor Mung'ala, Gladys Murira, Neema Mturi, Chris Musumba, Elizabeth Obiero and Khamisi Katana for their contribution in the collection of data. This paper was published with the permission of the Director of the Kenya Medical Research Institute, KEMRI. We confirm that we have read the Journal's position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.
References
- Snow RW, Guerra CA, Noor AM, Myint HY, Hay SI. The Global Distribution of Clinical Episodes of Plasmodium Falciparum malaria. Nature. 2005;434:214–217. doi: 10.1038/nature03342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newton CR, Warrell DA. Neurological Manifestations of Falciparum Malaria. Ann Neurol. 1998;43:695–702. doi: 10.1002/ana.410430603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kihara M, Carter JA, Newton CR. The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition: a systematic review. Trop Med Int Health. 2006;11:386–397. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2006.01579.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter JA, Mung'ala-Odera V, Neville BG, Murira G, Mturi N, Musumba C, Newton CR. Persistent neurocognitive impairments associated with severe falciparum malaria in Kenyan children. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:476–481. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.043893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holding PA, Taylor HG, Kazungu SD, Mkala T, Gona J, Mwamuye B, Mbonani L, Stevenson J. Assessing cognitive outcomes in a rural African population: development of a neuropsychological battery in Kilifi District, Kenya. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:246–260. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704102166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- John CC, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, Wu B, Boivin MJ. Cerebral malaria in children is associated with long-term cognitive impairment. Pediatrics. 2008;122:e92–99. doi: 10.1542/peds.2007-3709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley J, Smith S, Kirkham F, Muthinji P, Waruiru C, Marsh K. Seizures and status epilepticus in childhood cerebral malaria. QJM. 1996. pp. 591–597. [DOI] [PubMed]
- Newton CR, Krishna S. Severe Falciparum Malaria in Children: Current Understanding of Pathophysiology and Supportive Treatment. Pharmacology. 1998;79:1–53. doi: 10.1016/s0163-7258(98)00008-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waruiru C, Newton CR, Forster D, New L, Winstanley P, Mwangi I, Marsh V, Winstanley M, Snow RW, Marsh K. Epileptic seizures and malaria in Kenyan children. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 1996;90:152–155. doi: 10.1016/S0035-9203(96)90120-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mung'ala-Odera V, Snow RW, Newton CR. The Burden of the Neuro-cognitive Impairment associated with Falciparum Malaria in sub-Saharan Africa. American Journal of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene. 2004;71(2):64–70. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott R. Febrile seizures and mesial temporal sclerosis: no association in a long-term follow-up study. Neurology. 2003;61(4):588. doi: 10.1212/wnl.61.4.588. author reply 588-589. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott RC, Gadian DG, King MD, Chong WK, Cox TC, Neville BGR, Connelly A. Magnetic resonance imaging findings within 5 days of status epilepticus in childhood. Brain. 2002;12:1951–1959. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanLandingham KE, Heinz ER, Cavazos JE, Lewis DV. Magnetic resonance imaging evidence of hippocampal injury after prolonged focal febrile convulsions. Ann Neurol. 1998;43(4):413–426. doi: 10.1002/ana.410430403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Viskontas IV, McAndrews MP, Moscovitch M. Remote Episodic Memory Deficits in Patients with Uniletaral Temporal Lobe Epilepsy and Excisions. J Neuroscience. 2000;20:5853–5857. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-15-05853.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin JK, Dunn DW. Progressive behavioral changes in children with epilepsy. Prog in Brain Res. 2002;135:419–427. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(02)35039-8. full_text. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Watkins KE, Connelly A, Paesschen W, Mishkin M. Differential Effects of Early Hippocampal Pathology on Episodic and Semantic Memory. Science. 1997;277:376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.277.5324.376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons JS, Spiers HJ. Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:637–648. doi: 10.1038/nrn1178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schijns OE, Visser-Vandewalle V, Lemmens EM, Janssen A, Hoogland G. Surgery for temporal lobe epilepsy after cerebral malaria. Seizure. 2008;17:731–734. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2008.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grote CL, Pierre-Louis SJC, Durward WF. Deficits in delayed memory following cerebral malaria: A case study. Cortex. 1997;33:385–388. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70014-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WHO. Severe falciparum malaria. World Health Organization, Communicable Diseases Cluster. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 2000;94(Suppl 1):S1–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Idro R, Carter JA, Fegan G, Neville BG, Newton CR. Risk factors for persisting neurological and cognitive impairments following cerebral malaria. Arch Dis Child. 2006;91:142–148. doi: 10.1136/adc.2005.077784. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter JA, Ross AJ, Neville BG, Obiero E, Katana K, Mung'ala-Odera V, Lees JA, Newton CR. Developmental impairments following severe falciparum malaria in children. Trop Med Int Health. 2005;10:3–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3156.2004.01345.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson BA, Ivani-Chalian R, Besag FM, Bryant T. Adapting the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test for use with Children aged 5 to 10 years. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1993;15:474–486. doi: 10.1080/01688639308402572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carter JA, Lees JA, Gona JK, Murira G, Rimba K, Neville BG, Newton CR. Severe falciparum malaria and acquired childhood language disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2006;48:51–57. doi: 10.1017/S0012162206000107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Briscoe J, Gathercole SE. Everyday Memory and Cognitive Ability in Children Born Very Prematurely. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42:749–754. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Isaacs EB, Lucas A, Chong WK, Wood SJ, Johnson CL, Marshall C, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG. Hippocampal Volume and Everyday Memory in Children of Very Low Birth Weight. Pediatr Res. 2000;47:713–720. doi: 10.1203/00006450-200006000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mishkin M, Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG. Amnesia and the organization of the hippocampal system. Hippocampus. 1998;8:212–216. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:3<212::AID-HIPO4>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley AD. In: Handbook of memory disorders. Baddeley AD, Wilson BA, Watts FN, editor. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 1995. The Psychology of Memory. [Google Scholar]
- Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet. 2007;369:60–70. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60032-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olness K. Effects on Brain Development Leading to Cognitive Impairment: A Worldwide Epidemic. Dev Beh Pediatr. 2003;24:120–130. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommerfield AJ, Deary IJ, McAulay V, Frier BM. Moderate hypoglycemia impairs multiple memory functions in healthy adults. Neuropsychology. 2003;17:125–132. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.1.125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sommerfield AJ, Deary IJ, McAulay V, Frier BM. Short-term, delayed, and working memory are impaired during hypoglycemia in individuals with type 1 diabetes. Diabetes care. 2003;26:390–396. doi: 10.2337/diacare.26.2.390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warren RE, Zammitt NN, Deary IJ, Frier BM. The effects of acute hypoglycaemia on memory acquisition and recall and prospective memory in type 1 diabetes. Diabetologia. 2007;50:178–185. doi: 10.1007/s00125-006-0535-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Idro R. Severe anaemia in childhood cerebral malaria is associated with profound coma. African health sciences. 2003;3:15–18. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boivin MJ, Bangirana P, Byarugaba J, Opoka RO, Idro R, Jurek AM, John CC. Cognitive impairment after cerebral malaria in children: a prospective study. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e360–366. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-2027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Hensbroek MB, Palmer A, Jaffar S, Schneider G, Kwiatkowski D. Residual neurologic sequelae after childhood cerebral malaria. J Pediatr. 1997;131:125–129. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(97)70135-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holding PA, Stevenson J, Peshu N, Marsh K. Cognitive sequelae of severe malaria with impaired consciousness. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 1999;93:529–534. doi: 10.1016/S0035-9203(99)90368-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bondi FS. The incidence and outcome of neurological abnormalities in childhood cerebral malaria: a long-term follow-up of 62 survivors. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg. 1992;86:17–19. doi: 10.1016/0035-9203(92)90420-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]