Abstract
Background
Among survivors of childhood cancer, those with Central Nervous System (CNS) malignancies have been found to be at greatest risk for neuropsychological dysfunction in the first few years following diagnosis and treatment. This study follows survivors to adulthood to assess the long term impact of childhood CNS malignancy and its treatment on neurocognitive functioning.
Participants & Methods
As part of the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study (CCSS), 802 survivors of childhood CNS malignancy, 5937 survivors of non-CNS malignancy and 382 siblings without cancer completed a 25 item Neurocognitive Questionnaire (CCSS-NCQ) at least 16 years post cancer diagnosis assessing task efficiency, emotional regulation, organizational skills and memory. Neurocognitive functioning in survivors of CNS malignancy was compared to that of non-CNS malignancy survivors and a sibling cohort. Within the group of CNS malignancy survivors, multiple linear regression was used to assess the contribution of demographic, illness and treatment variables to reported neurocognitive functioning and the relationship of reported neurocognitive functioning to educational, employment and income status.
Results
Survivors of CNS malignancy reported significantly greater neurocognitive impairment on all factors assessed by the CCSS-NCQ than non-CNS cancer survivors or siblings (p<.01), with mean T scores of CNS malignancy survivors substantially more impaired that those of the sibling cohort (p<.001), with a large effect size for Task Efficiency (1.16) and a medium effect size for Memory (.68). Within the CNS malignancy group, medical complications, including hearing deficits, paralysis and cerebrovascular incidents resulted in a greater likelihood of reported deficits on all of the CCSS-NCQ factors, with generally small effect sizes (.22-.50). Total brain irradiation predicted greater impairment on Task Efficiency and Memory (Effect sizes: .65 and .63, respectively), as did partial brain irradiation, with smaller effect sizes (.49 and .43, respectively). Ventriculoperitoneal (VP) shunt placement was associated with small deficits on the same scales (Effect sizes: Task Efficiency .26, Memory .32). Female gender predicted a greater likelihood of impaired scores on 2 scales, with small effect sizes (Task Efficiency .38, Emotional Regulation .45), while diagnosis before age 2 years resulted in less likelihood of reported impairment on the Memory factor with a moderate effect size (.64). CNS malignancy survivors with more impaired CCSS-NCQ scores demonstrated significantly lower educational attainment (p<.01), less household income (p<.001) and less full time employment (p<.001).
Conclusions
Survivors of childhood CNS malignancy are at significant risk for impairment in neurocognitive functioning in adulthood, particularly if they have received cranial radiation, had a VP shunt placed, suffered a cerebrovascular incident or are left with hearing or motor impairments. Reported neurocognitive impairment adversely affected important adult outcomes, including education, employment, income and marital status.
Keywords: Neurocognitive functioning, brain tumors, CNS malignancies, Childhood Cancer Survivor Study
Cancers of the central nervous system (CNS), the most common solid malignancies in childhood, are associated with a number of sequelae including dysfunction in neurologic, endocrine, social, psychological and neurocognitive areas (Anderson et al, 2001; Packer et al, 2003; Schultz et al, 2007). In the neurocognitive domain, reduced global IQ compared to controls and normative samples has been reported (Ellenberg et al, 1987; Palmer et al, 2003), as have deficits in specific cognitive functions including verbal skills, visual spatial skills, attention, memory, psychomotor speed and learning (Kiehna et al, 2006; Mabbott et al, 2008; Mulhern et al, 2004; Nagel et al, 2006). In particular, recent research has focused on attention, processing speed and working memory as domains of significant dysfunction in survivors of childhood CNS cancer, perhaps because of the sensitivity of these functions to white matter damage resulting from cranial irradiation (Ris, 2005).
Factors within the population of pediatric CNS malignancy patients that have been associated with neurocognitive outcome include tumor site (Mulhern et al, 1992), age at diagnosis (Sands et al, 2001), and radiation treatment (Reamers et al, 2003). Cortical tumors have been reported to result in more cognitive late effects than 3rd or 4th ventricle tumors (Ellenberg et al, 1987, Mulhern et al, 1992). However, pediatric posterior fossa tumors have also been associated with neurocognitive sequelae including deficits in attention, planning, sequencing, executive functioning, memory, processing speed, visual-spatial organization, modulation of affect and behavior (Beebe et al, 2005; Karatekin et al, 2000; Riva & Giorgi, 2000; Steinlin et al, 2003; Schmahmann & Sherman, 1998; Levisohn et al, 2000).
Radiation therapy is a well established risk factor for neurocognitive difficulties of CNS cancer patients as well as of children with leukemia treated with prophylactic cranial radiation (Smibert et al, 1996; Spiegler et al, 2004). Correlation between the amount of radiation received and IQ declines have been demonstrated (Grill et al, 1999; Kieffer-Renaux et al, 2000). MRI studies suggest that radiation may be associated with changes to white matter, including calcification and reduction in white matter volume that may contribute to these difficulties (Mulhern et al, 1999). Animal studies of the pathophysiology of late effects of cranial radiation therapy suggest primary apoptosis of endothelial and oligodendroglial cells as well as secondary cell injury and death mediated by hypoxia/ischemia and neuro-inflamation that results in a cascade of events that alter the microenvironment, leading to further endothelial dysfunction, disruption of the blood brain barrier, inhibition of neurogenesis, demyelination and tissue necrosis (Wong and Van der Kogel, 2004).
CNS malignancy survivors treated with radiation therapy at younger ages have frequently been reported to show greater neurocognitive impairment (Radcliffe et al, 1994; Ris et al, 2001). For example, Palmer et al (2001), in a longitudinal study of intellectual development in medulloblastoma patients, found patients treated at ≤8.02 years of age had significantly greater declines in IQ, knowledge and nonverbal abstract thinking than older patients. Mulhern and Palmer (2003) suggest that the effect may be due to the differential effect of cranial irradiation on white matter, which continues to develop until age 20, but seems to be lost at a similar rate by younger and older patients. The younger patients, then, may have more white matter loss over time, and reduced white matter volume in younger CNS malignancy patients has been associated with greater intellectual deficits (Mulhern et al, 1999). In a recent study, Mabbott et al (2008) reported no effect of age of diagnosis on attention and working memory in children treated for posterior fossa tumors with radiation therapy, although the authors caution that their sample was small and within a narrow age range, which may have skewed their results.
Most of the available literature on neuropsychological outcomes in pediatric CNS malignancy survivors followed patients for a limited period after treatment. The Childhood Cancer Survivors Study (CCSS) was designed to assess adults who survived childhood cancers. Based on CCSS data, adolescent and adult survivors of CNS malignancy, compared to other cancer survivors, show increased neurological deficits (Packer et al, 2003), problems with depression (Zebrack et al, 2004), anxiety, attention, social behavior (Schultz et al, 2007) and reduced reported quality of life (Zeltzer et al, 2008).
The main goal of the present study was to examine and quantify neurocognitive functioning and adaptive outcome in adult survivors of childhood CNS malignancies who are 16-34 years from their original diagnosis. We evaluated a large group of CCSS enrolled CNS malignancy survivors to determine their degree of reported neurocognitive dysfunction compared to survivors of non-CNS malignancy and a sibling cohort. Within the group of CNS malignancy survivors, we assessed factors related to treatment (cranial radiation, surgery, ventriculoperitoneal shunt), illness or therapy residuals (sensory and/or motor deficits, stroke), and demographic variables (gender, age at diagnosis) that might be associated with greater neurocognitive dysfunction. Moreover, we evaluated the degree of correlation between neurocognitive dysfunction and the adult socioeconomic outcomes of educational attainment, employment status and household income. We hypothesized the following: Hypothesis 1: Survivors of CNS malignancies will report greater neurocognitive dysfunction than survivors of non-CNS malignancies and siblings. Hypothesis 2: The greatest reported neurocognitive dysfunction will be in processing speed, memory, planning and organization, aspects of task efficiency and executive functioning. Hypothesis 3: There will be greater reported neurocognitive deficits in CNS malignancy survivors who had significant motor or sensory residuals, cranial radiation, cortical tumors or were younger at diagnosis. Hypothesis 4: Reported neurocognitive impairment will be associated with poorer adaptive outcome in adulthood as assessed by lower achievement in education, full time employment and income, and less likelihood of being married.
Methods
Participants
The methodology for the CCSS and the characteristics of the study population has been published previously (Robison et al, 2002). The CCSS is a retrospective cohort of children and adolescents treated for cancer at 26 collaborating institutions in the US and Canada (Appendix I). Individuals below the age of 21 diagnosed between 1970-1985 with leukemia, CNS malignancy, Hodgkin's disease, non-Hodgkin's lymphoma, kidney cancer, neuroblastoma, soft tissue sarcoma or malignant bone tumor who survived over 5 years were recruited. Respective institutional review boards of participating centers reviewed and approved the CCSS protocol. Of the 20,691 eligible individuals, 17,568 were located and 14,363 survivors (81.8%) and 3,899 siblings were enrolled. Of the survivors and siblings who participated in the baseline survey, mostly tested between 1992 and 1998, 9308 survivors (64.8%) and 2951 siblings (75.7%) participated in the 2003 follow-up survey (2002-2005).
The Childhood Cancer Survivor Study Neurocognitive Questionnaire (CCSS-NCQ), described below, was included in the 2003 Follow-up Survey and sent to all survivors and a selected sub-sample of 500 siblings, chosen using simple random sampling from those siblings who completed the baseline survey, were not lost to follow-up and had not refused further participation. The CCSS-NCQ was fully completed with no missing data for 802 (68.1%) of the 1177 survivors of CNS cancer who participated in the follow-up survey as well as 5937 (73.0%) of the 8130 survivors of non-CNS cancers and 382 (76.4%) of the selected group of 500 siblings. Comparison of the 802 CNS participants to the 375 CNS non-participants demonstrated that non-participants were more likely to be from ethnic or racial minority backgrounds (13.9% vs. 7.1%; p<0.001). Also, non-participants were more likely to be younger at time of CNS diagnosis (6.9 years vs. 8.5 years; p<0.001). No statistically significant differences were detected between participants and non-participants in gender, treatment with cranial radiation therapy or time since diagnosis (16.1 to 34.6 years). The diagnoses of the survivors of non-CNS childhood cancers were as follows: Leukemia (37.6%), Hodgkin's disease (15.3%), Non-Hodgkin's lymphoma (8.6%), neuroblastoma (7.3%), soft tissue sarcoma (10.3%), osteosarcoma (6.4%), Ewings and other bone tumors (3.6%) and Wilm's tumor (10.9%).
The survey forms were sent directly to any participant 18 years or older and to the parents of individuals under 18 years of age. Parents completed the forms for all participants under 18 years of age. Otherwise, forms were completed by the subject or by someone else who was familiar with the subject's neurocognitive functioning (proxy), with a box on the survey form to indicate whether the form was completed by self or proxy. Not all completed forms were appropriately marked. Data were available for 785 of the CNS malignancy survivors, and it was indicated that 136 of the CCSS-NCQs were completed by proxy (spouse: 3, friend: 1, sibling: 2, in-law: 4, legal guardian: 1, caseworker: 1, parent or not otherwise specified: 124). For the non-CNS cancer survivors, 243 of 5870 for whom data were available were completed by proxy (spouse: 33, friend: 4, sibling: 1, in-law: 1, legal guardian: 3, grandparent: 1, parent or not otherwise specified: 200) and for the sibling group, 3 of 379 for whom data were available were completed by proxy (spouse: 1, parent or not otherwise specified: 2). While there was no code for the reason forms were completed by proxy, many of the proxy respondents for CNS survivors indicated by writing on the form that they completed it due to cognitive problems of the participant. Other reasons informally provided by proxy respondents for all groups included mental health or drug abuse issues, participant away at school or military and participant not interested.
Instrument
To assess self-reported neurocognitive functioning, an instrument was developed for the CCSS population based on the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning (BRIEF), a multidimensional standardized behavior rating inventory for children, adolescents and adults with scales labeled Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials and Monitor (Gioia, Isquith & Guy, 2000; Guy, Isquith & Gioia, 2004; Roth, Isquith & Gioia, 2005). The two items of each BRIEF scale with the highest item-total correlation were combined with independently derived items designed to assess the neurocognitive domains of processing speed, memory and academic functioning. The resulting 25 items were factor analyzed in a group of 382 siblings of CCSS survivors (Krull et al, 2008). Four reliable and valid factors were found based on 19 of the original 25 items, labeled Task Efficiency, Emotional Regulation, Organization and Memory, and these 19 items constitute the resulting instrument, the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study Neurocognitive Questionnaire (CCSS-NCQ), presented in Appendix 2.
Although the 25 item questionnaire was also given to a group of 900 individuals as part of the standardization of the adult version of the BRIEF, the 382 siblings were used as the normative group for ultimate development of the CCSS-NCQ because they were demographically more similar to the cancer survivors in terms of ethnic group and education (compared to the non-CNS malignancy survivors). The sums of item endorsements by the siblings (i.e., “Never a problem” [scored 1], “Sometimes a problem” [scored 2], “Often a problem” [scored 3]) on each factor were converted to T scores, such that the sibling group had a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 on each of the 4 factors, with higher scores indicative of greater reported neurocognitive impairment.
Treatment and Radiation Exposure
Medical records were abstracted for treatment-related information including chemotherapy, surgery and radiation therapy. To quantify radiation exposure, the brain was partitioned into four segments, posterior fossa (Field 1), temporal lobe (Field 2), frontal cortex (Field 3), and parietal or occipital lobe (Field 4) and maximum radiation dosages were assigned for each field utilizing radiation oncology records from the treating institutions. A segment was said to be included in a radiation field if at least 50% of the segment was in the primary radiation volume. Nearly all CNS malignancy survivors who received cranial radiation therapy received high doses, at or above 40 gray (the Standard International Unit for absorbed ionizing radiation, abbreviated Gy), with only 2% receiving a lower dose.
Data analysis
The first set of analyses compared survivors of CNS malignancy with survivors of non-CNS malignancy and the sibling group. Descriptive statistics for gender, age, ethnicity, education, household income, and employment status were generated for the three groups. The groups were compared on each of the four CCSS-NCQ factor scores via multiple linear regression, adjusting for age at the time of the study, gender and ethnicity. When survivors and siblings were compared, the modification of linear regression by Generalized Estimating Equations was used to account for potential within-family correlation (Zeger & Liang, 1986).
The remaining analyses involved only survivors of CNS malignancy. Within the CNS malignancy patient group, multiple linear regression was used to assess the effects of the following potential independent variables on each of the four CCSS-NCQ factor scores, adjusted for race: age at survey; age at diagnosis; VP shunt; gender; sensory/motor complications (hearing loss [reduced hearing, deafness in one or both ears], visual difficulty [blindness, cataracts], paralysis, cerebrovascular incident); surgery (yes/no); cranial radiation therapy (whole brain, partial [less than all 4 dosimetry fields], none); and chemotherapy (yes/no). Only treatments provided within the first five years from the original diagnosis of cancer were considered. We also adjusted for whether or not the questionnaire was completed by proxy.
To assess the contribution of more specific factors to outcome, multiple linear regressions adjusted for age at the time of the study, gender and ethnicity were used for individual comparisons. CCSS-NCQ scores of CNS malignancy survivors with and without paralysis, hearing and/or visual difficulties, and stroke were examined. Since gender was significantly associated with CCSS-NCQ factor scores in CNS malignancy survivors, gender was examined in the sibling group to assess whether the effects were specific to CNS malignancy survivors or a more general gender effect. To assess the effect of tumor site in radiated CNS malignancy survivors, those with cortical vs. subcortical radiation boosts were compared. To assess the effect of radiation dose on neurocognitive outcome, CNS malignancy survivors who received cortical radiation without a cortical tumor were compared to leukemia survivors who received 24 Gy of cranial radiation and non-irradiated leukemia patients. Finally, to assess the relationship between neurocognitive outcome and socioeconomic variables in adulthood, individual multiple linear regressions were used to examine the association between age, gender and ethnicity adjusted CCSS-NCQ scores of CNS malignancy survivors and educational attainment (< high school degree, high school graduate, college graduate), household income (<$20,000, $20,000-$39,999, $40,000-$59,999 and ≥$60,000) and employment status (<full time, full time). All statistical analyses were performed using SAS Version 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) and 2-sided statistical inferences were employed throughout the analyses.
Results
Characteristics of the Study Population
Demographic characteristics of survivors of CNS malignancy, non-CNS cancers and the sibling group are presented in Table 1. Compared to survivors of CNS malignancy, the sibling group was older (p=.002), better educated (p=.003), more likely to be employed (p<.001) had higher family income (p<.001) and were more likely to be married (p<.001). Siblings were also older (p<.001), had higher household incomes (p<.001) and were more likely to be married (p<.001) than survivors of non-CNS cancers. Compared to survivors of non-CNS malignancies, survivors of CNS malignancy were more likely to be male (p=.04), less educated (p<.001), less fully employed (p<.001) and have lower household incomes (p<.001). With age at diagnosis divided into developmentally based categories (infancy, early and middle childhood, early and late adolescence), more non-CNS malignancy survivors than CNS malignancy survivors were observed in the 0-2 and 16-20 year old age categories. In terms of treatment, CNS malignancy survivors were more likely to receive surgery (p<.001) and less likely to receive chemotherapy (p<.001) than were survivors of non-CNS cancers.
Table 1.
Characteristics 1,2 | CNS Survivors (n=802) | Non-CNS Survivors (n=5937) | Siblings (n=382) | P(CNS vs. non-CNS) | P (CNS vs. sibs) | P(non-CNS vs. sibs) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex | 0.04 | 0.14 | 0.78 | |||
Male | 419 (52.2%) | 2876 (48.4%) | 182 (47.6%) | |||
Female | 383 (47.8%) | 3061 (51.6%) | 200 (52.4%) | |||
Age at time of study | ||||||
Mean (SD) | 31.5 (7.1) | 32.2 (7.6) | 34.1 (8.4) | 0.01 | 0.002 | <.001 |
Range | 17.4-51.8 | 17.0-54.1 | 17.8-58.4 | |||
Median | 31.2 | 31.9 | 33.7 | |||
Ethnicity | 0.36 | 0.99 | 0.65 | |||
White | 745 (93.2%) | 5397 (91.2%) | 336 (93.9%) | |||
Black | 17 (2.1%) | 147 (2.5%) | 8 (2.2%) | |||
Hispanic/Latino | 20 (2.5%) | 222 (3.8%) | 9 (2.5%) | |||
Other | 17 (2.1%) | 152 (2.6%) | 5 (1.4%) | |||
Education | <.001 | 0.003 | 0.37 | |||
<12 years | 43 (5.4%) | 219 (3.7%) | 9 (2.4%) | |||
High School Grad. | 455 (56.8%) | 2886 (48.6%) | 181 (47.5%) | |||
College Grad. | 303 (37.8%) | 2831 (47.7%) | 191 (50.1%) | |||
Employment | <.001 | <.001 | 0.13 | |||
Full time | 353 (44.0%) | 3931 (66.2%) | 267 (69.9%) | |||
Other | 449 (56.0%) | 2006 (33.8%) | 115 (30.1%) | |||
Household income | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | |||
<$19,999 | 163 (21.2%) | 663 (11.4%) | 26 (6.9%) | |||
$20,000-39,999 | 199 (25.9%) | 1335 (22.9%) | 62 (16.5%) | |||
$40,000-59,999 | 165 (21.5%) | 1232 (21.1%) | 82 (21.8%) | |||
Over 60,000 | 241 (31.4%) | 2604 (44.6%) | 206 (54.8%) | |||
Ever married | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | |||
Yes | 277 (34.8%) | 3489 (59.3%) | 271 (71.7%) | |||
No | 518 (65.2%) | 2391 (40.7%) | 107 (28.3%) | |||
Age at Diagnosis | <.001 | |||||
0-2 | 136 (17.0%) | 1355 (22.8%) | ||||
3-5 | 165 (20.6%) | 1325 (22.3%) | ||||
6-10 | 232 (28.9%) | 1101 (18.5%) | ||||
11-15 | 201 (25.1%) | 1302 (21.9%) | ||||
16-20 | 68 (8.5%) | 854 (14.4%) | ||||
Treatment | <.001 | |||||
Surgery only | 225 (30.1%) | 280 (5.0%) | ||||
Chemotherapy only | 0 (0.0%) | 499 (9.0%) | ||||
Radiation only | 13 (1.7%) | 11 (0.2%) | ||||
Chemo. + Radiation | 3 (0.4%) | 850 (15.3%) | ||||
Chemo. + Surgery | 12 (1.6%) | 1164 (20.9%) | ||||
Radiation + Surgery | 344 (46.1%) | 441 (7.9%) | ||||
Chemo. +Rad + Surgery | 150 (20.1%) | 2322 (41.7%) | ||||
Cranial Radiation3 | <.001 | |||||
None | 268 (37.1 %) | 3910 (71.5%) | ||||
Whole brain | 218 (30.2%) | 1436 (26.3%) | ||||
Partial brain | 236 (32.7%) | 124 (2.2%) | ||||
Cranial Radiation Dose | <.001 | |||||
None | 268 (37.1%) | 3910 (71.5%) | ||||
0.1-19 Gy | 2 (0.3%) | 674 (12.3%) | ||||
20-39 Gy | 7 (1.0%) | 762 (13.9%) | ||||
40-59 Gy | 423 (58.6%) | 112 (2.0%) | ||||
>=60 Gy | 22 (3.0%) | 12 (0.2%) | ||||
Tumor Type | ||||||
Astrocytoma | 495(61.7%) | |||||
Medulloblastoma, PNET | 172(21.5%) | |||||
Other CNS | 135(16.8%) |
Note: Percentages are based on the total with available data for each variable.
For categorical characteristics, bootstrap of the families was used to account for the family effect when the p-values were obtained.
For continuous characteristics GEE was used to account for the family effect when the p-values were obtained.
Does not include patients for whom radiation status was unknown.
Neurocognitive Outcomes In Survivors of Childhood CNS Malignancies, non-CNS Malignancies and Siblings
As presented in Table 2, CNS malignancy survivors reported significantly greater neurocognitive dysfunction than the sibling cohort or non CNS malignancy survivors on all CCSS-NCQ factors. Effect sizes for the differences between CNS cancer survivors and siblings as well as between CNS and non-CNS cancer survivors were small for Organization (CNS v. sibs=.22, CNS v. non-CNS=.23) and Emotional Regulation (CNS v. sibs=.29, CNS v. non-CNS=.11), medium for Memory (CNS v. sibs=.68, CNS v. non-CNS=.53) and large for Task Efficiency (CNS v. sibs=1.16, CNS v. non-CNS=.90). Non-CNS malignancy survivors scored closer to the sibling cohort; while they showed more impaired scores on 3 of the 4 CCSS-NCQ scales, the effect sizes were small (Task Efficiency= .26, Emotional Control=.18, Memory=.15).
Table 2. CCSS-NCQ scores of childhood cancer survivors and siblings.
Characteristics | CNS Survivors (n=802) | Non-CNS Survivors (n=5937) | Siblings (n=382) | P-values* |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CNS vs. Non-CNS | CNS vs. Sibs | Non-CNS vs. sibs | ||||
Task Efficiency | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 | |||
Mean (SD) | 61.6 (16.0) | 52.6 (12.6) | 50.0 (10.0) | |||
Effect size | 1.16 | 0.26 | ||||
Emotional Regulation | 0.004 | <.001 | 0.002 | |||
Mean (SD) | 52.9 (11.1) | 51.8 (10.9) | 50.0 (10.0) | |||
Effect size | 0.29 | 0.18 | ||||
Organization | <.001 | 0.007 | 0.82 | |||
Mean (SD) | 52.2 (10.8) | 49.9 (10.0) | 50.0 (10.0) | |||
Effect size | 0.22 | -0.01 | ||||
Memory | <.001 | <.001 | 0.002 | |||
Mean (SD) | 56.8 (13.5) | 51.5 (11.8) | 50.0 (10.0) | |||
Effect size | 0.68 | 0.15 |
P-values are age, gender and race adjusted.
Factors Predicting Neurocognitive Dysfunction in CNS Cancer Survivors
Factors that predicted self and/or proxy reported neurocognitive dysfunction in survivors of CNS malignancy are presented in Table 3. Medical complications, including visual and hearing difficulties, paralysis and stroke, were associated with greater reported deficits on all of the CCSS-NCQ factors, with generally small effect sizes (.22-.50). Cranial radiation was correlated with greater impairment on Task Efficiency and Memory, with medium effect sizes for total brain irradiation (65 and .63, respectively), and smaller effect sizes for partial brain irradiation (.49 and .43, respectively). VP shunt predicted more impaired Task Efficiency and Memory scores, as well, but to a lesser degree (Effect sizes: Task Efficiency .26, Memory .32). Female gender predicted more impaired scores on the Task Efficiency and Emotional Regulation scales, with small effect sizes (Task Efficiency .38, Emotional Regulation .45), while diagnosis before age 2 predicted less impairment on the Memory scale with a moderate effect size (.64). Completion of the form by proxy was associated with more impairment on all CCSS-NCQ scales. The effect size was very large for the Task Efficiency scale (1.39), medium for Organization (.51) and Memory (.57) and small for Emotional Regulation (.34).
Table 3. Multiple linear regression analysis of factors predicting CCSS-NCQ scores for CNS malignancy survivors*.
Variables | Task efficiency | Emotional regulation | Organization | Memory | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean diff† (95% CI) | p | Mean diff† (95% CI) | p | Mean diff† (95% CI) | p | Mean diff† (95% CI) | p | |
Age at dx | ||||||||
0-2 | 1.1 (-4.8, 7.0) | 0.72 | 0.5 (-4.1, 5.1) | 0.84 | -1.1 (-5.5, 3.3) | 0.63 | -6.4 (-11.7, -1.0) | 0.02 |
3-5 | 0.6 (-5.0, 6.1) | 0.84 | 0.2 (-4.0, 4.5) | 0.91 | 1.3 (-2.8, 5.4) | 0.54 | -4.8 (-9.7, 0.2) | 0.06 |
6-10 | -0.1 (-5.2, 4.9) | 0.96 | 0.3 (-3.6, 4.2) | 0.89 | 2.0 (-1.7, 5.8) | 0.29 | -3.2 (-7.7, 1.4) | 0.17 |
11-15 | -0.7 (-5.4, 4.0) | 0.78 | 0.8 (-2.9, 4.4) | 0.67 | 2.0 (-1.5, 5.5) | 0.25 | -1.7 (-5.9, 2.5) | 0.43 |
16-20 | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
VP shunt | ||||||||
Yes | 2.6 (0.2, 5.0) | 0.03 | 0.7 (-1.2, 2.6) | 0.46 | 0.7 (-1.0, 2.5) | 0.41 | 3.2 (1.1, 5.4) | 0.003 |
No | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Brain RT | ||||||||
Whole brain | 6.5 (3.7, 9.4) | <.001 | 1.6 (-0.6, 3.8) | 0.16 | 0.5 (-1.6, 2.6) | 0.64 | 6.3 (3.8, 8.9) | <.001 |
Partial brain | 4.9 (2.3, 7.4) | <.001 | 0.8 (-1.2, 2.8) | 0.45 | 1.5 (-0.4, 3.4) | 0.13 | 4.3 (2.0, 6.6) | <.001 |
None | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Surgery | ||||||||
Yes | 1.9 (-3.9, 7.8) | 0.51 | 2.4 (-2.2, 6.9) | 0.31 | -2.4 (-6.7, 2.0) | 0.29 | 2.7 (-2.6, 7.9) | 0.32 |
No | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Chemotherapy | ||||||||
Yes | 2.1 (-0.6, 4.9) | 0.13 | 0.4 (-1.8, 2.5) | 0.73 | 0.4 (-1.6, 2.5) | 0.69 | 1.4 (-1.1, 3.9) | 0.27 |
No | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Complications | ||||||||
Yes | 5.0 (2.8, 7.2) | <.001 | 2.2 (0.5, 3.9) | 0.01 | 2.4 (0.8, 4.0) | 0.004 | 3.9 (2.0, 5.9) | <.001 |
No | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Gender | ||||||||
Female | 3.8 (1.7, 5.9) | <.001 | 4.5 (2.8, 6.1) | <.001 | 1.0 (-0.6, 2.6) | 0.22 | 1.8 (-0.1, 3.8) | 0.06 |
Male | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Completed by self | ||||||||
No | 13.9 (10.9, 16.8) | <.001 | 3.4 (1.1, 5.6) | 0.004 | 5.1 (2.9, 7.2) | <.001 | 5.7 (3.1, 8.3) | <.001 |
Yes | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Age at study | ||||||||
17-24 | 2.0 (-3.1, 7.1) | 0.43 | 2.6 (-1.4, 6.5) | 0.20 | 2.3 (-1.5, 6.1) | 0.24 | 2.0 (-2.6, 6.6) | 0.39 |
25-29 | 0.1 (-4.4, 4.7) | 0.95 | 2.1 (-1.4, 5.6) | 0.25 | -1.0 (-4.3, 2.4) | 0.57 | -0.4 (-4.5, 3.7) | 0.84 |
30-34 | 1.0 (-3.1, 5.1) | 0.63 | 1.1 (-2.1, 4.3) | 0.52 | 0.3 (-2.8, 3.4) | 0.85 | -0.6 (-4.3, 3.1) | 0.74 |
35-39 | 0.4 (-3.7, 4.5) | 0.84 | 1.4 (-1.7, 4.6) | 0.37 | 0.3 (-2.7, 3.4) | 0.84 | -0.4 (-4.1, 3.3) | 0.84 |
40+ | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref | ||||
Race | ||||||||
Other | 3.1 (-1.3, 7.5) | 0.17 | 0.4 (-3.0, 3.8) | 0.82 | 2.8 (-0.5, 6.1) | 0.09 | 3.8 (-0.2, 7.8) | 0.06 |
White | Ref | Ref | Ref | Ref |
Ref = Reference group
effect size = the mean difference divided by 10.
Specific Predictor Variables
Complications
Table 4 provides results of multivariate analysis investigating selected medical complications and domain-specific scores. CNS malignancy survivors with concomitant hearing and visual complications scored significantly poorer on each of the four CCSS-NCQ scales than other CNS survivors, with medium effect sizes for Task Efficiency (.86) and Memory (.69) and smaller effect sizes for Emotional Regulation (.43) and Organization (.47), while those with hearing deficits alone scored worse on all scales except Emotional Regulation (Effect sizes: Task Efficiency .80, Memory .50, Organization .32). Visual complications alone were not significantly associated with poorer scores. History of stroke was significantly associated with poorer mean scores in Task Efficiency (Effect size .78), Emotional Regulation (Effect size .39) and Memory (Effect size .45) and reported paralysis was associated with significantly more impairment in Task Efficiency (Effect size .45) and Organization (Effect size .30).
Table 4. CCSS-NCQ scores of CNS malignancy survivors with and without individual medical complications*.
Task efficiency | Emotional regulation | Organization | Memory | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Freq | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | |
Sensory Deficits | |||||||||||||
Hearing + Visual | 27 | 68.6 (16.3) | 0.88 | 0.004 | 57.0 (11.1) | 0.46 | 0.03 | 56.2 (10.7) | 0.47 | 0.03 | 63.0 (12.9) | 0.69 | 0.008 |
Hearing | 89 | 68.0 (15.5) | 0.86 | <.001 | 53.7 (11.0) | 0.17 | 0.17 | 54.7 (10.7) | 0.34 | 0.006 | 61.1 (14.1) | 0.54 | <.001 |
Visual | 101 | 63.4 (15.7) | 0.26 | 0.12 | 52.4 (10.3) | -0.07 | 0.52 | 53.3 (10.7) | 0.16 | 0.16 | 55.6 (12.9) | -0.06 | 0.66 |
Neither | 573 | 60.0 (15.8) | Ref | 52.7 (11.2) | Ref | 51.5 (10.9) | Ref | 56.1 (13.3) | Ref | ||||
Stroke | |||||||||||||
Yes | 35 | 70.0 (17.5) | 0.87 | 0.001 | 56.6 (12.0) | 0.37 | 0.05 | 53.8 (10.4) | 0.17 | 0.37 | 61.1 (14.7) | 0.47 | 0.04 |
No | 763 | 61.2 (15.8) | Ref | 52.7 (11.0) | Ref | 52.0 (10.8) | Ref | 56.6 (13.4) | Ref | ||||
Paralysis | |||||||||||||
Yes | 114 | 65.5 (17.4) | 0.40 | 0.01 | 54.5 (12.2) | 0.14 | 0.19 | 54.7 (11.1) | 0.28 | 0.01 | 58.3 (13.2) | 0.16 | 0.25 |
No | 686 | 61.0 (15.7) | Ref | 52.7 (10.8) | Ref | 51.7 (10.7) | Ref | 56.6 (13.5) | Ref |
Sensory deficits, stroke, and paralysis were analyzed separately adjusting for age, gender and race or ethnic group. Mean and SD were given for descriptive purposes and unadjusted, while effect sizes were estimated with the adjustment. Ref=Reference group
Cranial radiation dose/Tumor Location
Precise site of CNS tumor was not available for the CCSS cohort. However, many CNS malignancy survivors who had radiation therapy had a “boost” field, a smaller field inside a region already treated with a larger field, used to treat the specific tumor site with additional irradiation. Patients with cortical radiation focus (Boosts to Fields 2, 3, and/or 4; n = 119) were compared to patients with subcortical radiation focus (Boost to posterior fossa Field 1; n = 102) on CCSS-NCQ scale scores. The groups were not significantly different.
To assess the association of cranial radiation dose with neurocognitive impairment, CCSS-NCQ scores of 65 CNS malignancy patients who received whole brain radiation (mean dose=36.3 Gy, s.d.=5.97 Gy), with the highest dose (boost) to Field 1, indicating a subcortical tumor, were compared to those of 510 leukemia survivors who received 24 Gy of cranial radiation and 744 non-radiated leukemics. The CNS survivors reported significantly more impairment (p<.001) on the Task Efficiency factor (T=62.6) than leukemia survivors who received 24 Gy of cranial radiation (T=56.2), who in turn reported significantly more impairment than non-radiated leukemia survivors (T=51.7). On the Memory factor, the CNS malignancy group (T=57.6) and the radiated leukemics (T=54.0) did not differ from each other, but both showed significantly more impairment (p<.001) than the non-radiated leukemics (T=50.5).
Gender
In survivors of CNS malignancy, female gender predicted more impairment on Task Efficiency and Emotional Regulation in the multivariate analysis. To ascertain whether this was unique to CNS survivors, males and females in the sibling cohort were compared on CCSS-NCQ scale scores via univariate analysis. Female siblings had higher CCSS-NCQ scores than their male counterparts on the Emotional Regulation scale (p<.001), with similarly small effect size (CNS survivors: .45, sibling group: .34), suggesting that females, in general, may report slightly more difficulty with Emotional Regulation than males.
CCSS-NCQ scores and indicators of adaptive outcome
Within the group of CNS malignancy survivors, CCSS-NCQ scores were highly correlated with variables predictive of success in the achievement of adult tasks (Table 5). Lower educational attainment was associated with more impairment on CCSS-NCQ Task Efficiency, Emotional Regulation and Memory scales. Less than full time employment was associated with greater impairment on all CCSS-NCQ scales and household income under $20,000 per year was associated with more impaired CCSS-NCQ scores on all scales compared to household income over $60,000. Never having been married correlated with poorer Task Efficiency, Memory and Organization scores.
Table 5. CCSS-NCQ scores of CNS malignancy survivors by categories of education, household income, ad employment and marital status*.
Task efficiency | Emotional regulation | Organization | Memory | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Freq | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | Mean (SD) | Effect size | P | ||
Education | ||||||||||||||
Not HS Grad | 43 | 75.8 (14.4) | 1.87 | <.001 | 57.9 (12.7) | 0.68 | <.001 | 54.6 (12.7) | 0.26 | 0.15 | 63.7 (12.1) | 1.09 | <.001 | |
HS Grad | 455 | 64.0 (16.3) | 0.73 | <.001 | 53.7 (10.9) | 0.28 | <.001 | 52.5 (10.9) | 0.09 | 0.29 | 58.7 (13.6) | 0.57 | <.001 | |
College Grad | 303 | 56.1 (13.6) | Ref | 51.0 (10.6) | Ref | 51.3 (10.4) | Ref | 53.1 (12.4) | Ref | |||||
Income | ||||||||||||||
<$20,000 | 163 | 66.3 (16.9) | 0.85 | <.001 | 55.7 (11.2) | 0.52 | <.001 | 53.8 (11.6) | 0.27 | 0.014 | 60.1 (13.3) | 0.54 | <.001 | |
20,000-39,999 | 199 | 61.8 (15.8) | 0.40 | 0.007 | 52.9 (11.7) | 0.27 | 0.008 | 52.0 (11.4) | 0.09 | 0.401 | 57.0 (13.6) | 0.22 | 0.084 | |
40,000-59,999 | 165 | 60.5 (15.0) | 0.20 | 0.208 | 52.4 (11.3) | 0.18 | 0.098 | 51.9 (9.7) | 0.06 | 0.576 | 55.9 (13.1) | 0.10 | 0.439 | |
Over 60,000 | 241 | 58.8 (15.8) | Ref | 51.0 (9.8) | Ref | 51.5 (10.8) | Ref | 55.1 (13.4) | Ref | |||||
Employment | ||||||||||||||
Full time | 353 | 54.7 (12.7) | -1.18 | <.001 | 50.5 (10.3) | -0.34 | <.001 | 49.9 (9.7) | -0.37 | <.001 | 53.0 (12.4) | -0.69 | <.001 | |
Other** | 449 | 67.1 (16.3) | Ref | 54.7 (11.3) | Ref | 53.9 (11.4) | Ref | 59.9 (13.5) | Ref | |||||
Ever married | ||||||||||||||
Yes | 277 | 56.0 (14.1) | -.82 | <.001 | 52.4 (10.9) | -0.05 | 0.54 | 51.0 (10.2) | -0.17 | 0.053 | 54.7 (12.9) | -0.33 | 0.003 | |
No | 518 | 64.7 (16.3) | Ref | 53.2 (11.1) | Ref | 52.8 (11.2) | Ref | 57.8 (13.6) | Ref |
Education, income, employment and marital status were analyzed separately adjusting for age, gender and race or ethnic group. Mean and SD were given for descriptive purposes and unadjusted, while effect sizes were estimated with the adjustment. Ref=Reference group
other includes: Part time (141); Unemployed, looking for a job (62); Retired (only 1 person), care for home and/or unable to work due to illness or disabilities and/or student (216), Other (29, such as Volunteer, non-profit; Self employed, NOS; Leave of absence, maternity; Unemployed; Seasonal lay-off)
Discussion
Survivors of childhood CNS malignancies are at known risk for deficits in neuropsychological functioning. The current study was the first to follow a large cohort of CNS malignancy survivors to adulthood, using large non-CNS malignancy and sibling control groups, in order to quantify the degree and assess the nature of the neurocognitive dysfunction in these survivors, determine associated risk factors and evaluate the impact of neurocognitive impairment in adulthood. Among CNS malignancy survivors in the CCSS cohort, risk of neurocognitive dysfunction was significantly associated with treatment involving cranial irradiation or placement of a ventriculoperitoneal shunt, as well as a history of stroke, paralysis or auditory difficulties.
Of the factors identified on the CCSS NCQ in the present study, CNS malignancy survivors with cognitive impairment were most likely to report dysfunction on the Task Efficiency scale, which contains items related to speed of performance, self initiation and multi-tasking, as well as on the Memory scale, which assesses both long term and working memory. The two scales are highly correlated with each other (.71), while correlations between the other scales are less robust (.36 to .57). In contrast to some studies of selected subgroups of CNS cancer patients (Mabbot et al, 2008; Reddick et al, 2003), our results suggest that the combined CNS malignancy survivor group is left with deficits in both information processing speed and working memory, rather than showing impairment in only one of the two variables.
The complications of paralysis, stroke or hearing impairment were found to be highly predictive of neuropsychological dysfunction in survivors of CNS malignancy. The mean scores for Task Efficiency of CNS survivors in each of these groups was in the impaired range compared to the normative sibling group (>1.5 s.d. from the mean). The high degree of neurocognitive impairment among CNS malignancy survivors with hearing difficulties, but not visual deficits, is surprising. Compared to other CNS cancer survivors, those with hearing problems did not have significantly different diagnoses. However, they were more likely that other CNS survivors to have received cranial radiation (93% vs. 64%) or chemotherapy (40% vs. 20%), and to have been treated with Cisplatinum, which is known to affect hearing (13% vs. 4%), although in our multiple regression model, hearing difficulties emerged as an independent contributor to reported neurocognitive dysfunction when controlling for treatment variables. To our knowledge, this association has not been previously reported. Our study did not consider tumor site as a variable. It may be that hearing loss is associated with CNS malignancies in areas of the brain where irradiation produces significant neurocognitive deficits. This possibility can be investigated using the current CCSS cohort, since data regarding tumor site will be available.
Because of the general uniformity of whole brain radiation dose in CNS malignancy patients, a relationship between radiation dose and degree of neuropsychological impairment could not be established using the CNS group alone in the current study, but comparison with a group of leukemia patients who received 24Gy of cranial radiation, however, yielded data suggestive of a dose/response relationship between amount of cranial irradiation and neurocognitive impairment. Although CNS tumor and leukemia patients differ in many other ways, these data are in agreement with a number of previous studies correlating dose of cranial irradiation with neurocognitive outcome in survivors of ALL (Halberg et al, 1992) and medulloblastoma (Mulhern et al, 1998; Grill et al, 1999; Kieffer-Renaux et al, 2000).
Contrary to previous studies (Ellenberg et al, 1987; Sands et al, 2001), younger age at diagnosis was not correlated with greater reported neurocognitive dysfunction in the current large sample of adult CNS malignancy survivors. The present study assessed individuals who were significantly older and farther from the age at which they were diagnosed with and treated for cancer than other studies. It is possible that children with CNS malignancies that are diagnosed and treated at younger ages show more difficulty with childhood outcome measures, such as academic achievement, than those diagnosed and tested when they are older. However, both groups appear to report similar difficulties with neurocognitive functioning in adulthood. As maturity demands more complex neurocognitive skills, including executive functions and the management of multiple simultaneous tasks, childhood CNS malignancy survivors may “grow into” neurocognitive deficits that carry a greater impact in adult life. It may be that the white matter abnormalities, including calcifications, other lesions and smaller white matter volumes, which have been implicated in the neurotoxicity of cranial radiation therapy (Fouladi et al, 2004, Wilburn et al, 2006), result in deficits in integrative neurocognitive functions that lead to greater impairment in complex adult tasks compared to those assessed in survivors during childhood. In addition, previous studies have relied on test data, while the current study used a self-report measure. It is possible that individuals experience neurocognitive deficits even when objective tests do not show commensurate impairment.
Mabbott et al (2008) failed to find an age effect in information processing speed among childhood medulloblastoma survivors, in a recent small study. Most studies in the past have used more general measures of neuropsychological dysfunction (e.g. IQ). It may be that certain deficits, such as processing speed, are robust determinants of neurocognitive impairments in adulthood, so that even though older survivors do not show lower IQ scores for the first few years after diagnosis/treatment, deficits in key neurocognitive functions predict significant impairment in CNS malignancy survivors as adults. Finally, CNS malignancy survivors within the CCSS cohort who responded to the CCSS-NCQ were significantly older at diagnosis than those who declined to participate. It is possible that those survivors diagnosed younger who were more impaired refused more often to continue with CCSS participation, thereby skewing the results.
In the current study, CNS survivors diagnosed at the youngest ages (0-2) actually reported less memory impairment than other groups. This youngest age group did not differ from other patient groups in diagnosis or type of treatment. However, when analyzed by diagnosis, only patients diagnosed at ages 0-2 with astrocytomas had lower (less impaired) Memory scale scores, not those with medulloblastoma/PNET or other CNS tumors. Since our study design did not permit comparison of site or grade of tumor, these variables may have contributed to the better outcome in the youngest group. In addition, while the proportion of patients receiving cranial radiation did not differ by age group, patients diagnosed between birth and age 2 who underwent CRT received lower average doses than other patients (ages 0-2 mean=48.1 Gy, ages 3-20 mean=52.3 Gy; p<0.001), which may have resulted in less residual memory impairment.
Compared to control groups of siblings and survivors of non-CNS childhood cancers, adult survivors of childhood CNS malignancy have been reported to show deficits in IQ, educational attainment, income, employment, and marital status (Mostow et al, 1991; Zebrack et al, 2004). The current study indicates that adverse outcomes in indicators of successful adult adaptation (educational attainment, income, employment, marital status) were most likely in survivors who report neurocognitive dysfunction on the CCSS-NCQ.
It is worth noting that significantly more CCSS-NCQ questionnaires were completed by proxy for CNS malignancy survivors than other study participants, and completion by proxy was highly correlated with reported impairment. The group for whom reports were by proxy was much more likely to have received radiation therapy, chemotherapy, or VP shunt placement, suffered hearing and/or visual impairments, paralysis, or stroke and to have been younger at diagnosis (p<.001). They were also more likely than other CNS survivors to have had an astrocytoma rather than a medulloblastoma/PNET (p=.02). Groups with these risk factors require careful early monitoring and intervention, since without it, they may have severe neurocognitive deficits as adults, to the point that they cannot independently complete a self-report questionnaire.
While the current study has many strengths, including the large and well-characterized patient population who is a decade or more from their cancer diagnosis, the findings may be limited by the fact that neurocognitive dysfunction was evaluated by report rather than direct assessment. Self-reported and other-reported deficits in an important domain of cognition, executive functioning, have been shown to be well correlated with outcome measures (Rabin et al, 2006), and the current findings likely provide extremely useful information about everyday functioning. However, direct neuropsychological evaluation of at risk subgroups of CNS malignancy survivors is important to gain insight into the mechanisms underlying observed neurocognitive impairments, providing further direction for the development of treatment strategies. In addition, a significantly larger percentage of the CCSS-NCQs were completed by proxy for the CNS malignancy survivors than for the other groups. While this is no doubt a reflection of the higher level of neurocognitive impairment in this group, specific items endorsed may differ in a systematic way depending on whether the questionnaire was completed by self or proxy. Also, compared to CCSS study non-participants, participants were less likely to be from ethnic minorities, which may limit generalization to minority group survivors. Another study limitation was the absence of information about precise location of CNS malignancy. Using radiation dosimetry to infer tumor site helped correct for this lack to a certain extent, but precluded more detailed analysis of radiation and surgical factors within study subgroups. Finally, since study participants were diagnosed between 1970 and 1985, it is not possible in this study to evaluate the effects of improved treatments on neuropsychological outcome in CNS malignancy survivors. A new cohort is currently being recruited by CCSS that will provide data to address this question.
Survivors of CNS malignancy have consistently been shown to be at great risk for residual neurocognitive impairment in a large number of previous studies. The current study indicates that deficits extend to adulthood in many patients. Over 50% of survivors of childhood CNS malignancy reported significant impairment on at least one CCSS-NCQ Task Efficiency item, more than three times as many as in the sibling cohort. This underscores the need for continued attention to mitigating the long term negative effects of CNS malignancies and their treatment. Cognitive rehabilitation aimed at improving attention and working memory in irradiated survivors of CNS cancer has shown some success in terms of outcome variables, with small to medium effect sizes, but little improvement on measures of neurocognitive functioning (Butler et al, 2008). Since neurocognitive skills are difficult to improve directly, it will be important to investigate the benefits of early and consistent use of compensatory strategies, including assistive technology, transitional facilities to promote independent living, and job placement and coaching, to enhance functional outcomes in at risk CNS malignancy survivors.
Also of interest is the study of resilience in CNS malignancy survivors, and exploration of protective factors among survivors in high risk groups who had good neurocognitive outcomes. A recent study in survivors of childhood ALL, for example, suggested that folate pathway genetic polymorphisms may be related to attentional disorders (Krull et al, 2008). In survivors of childhood traumatic brain injury, social advantage and low family stress may be associated with better neurocognitive outcome (Taylor et al, 2002). Better prediction of outcome could lead to more focused treatments.
Acknowledgments
Funding:
This research was funded by a grant from the National Institute of Health (U24 CA55727 to LLR). Additional support was provided to St. Jude Children's Research Hospital by the American Lebanese Syrian Associated Charities. NIH played no role in the design of the study, the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data, the decision to submit the manuscript for publication or the writing of the manuscript.
Appendix 1
The Childhood Cancer Survivor Study (CCSS) is a collaborative, multi-institutional project, funded as a resource by the National Cancer Institute, of individuals who survived five or more years after diagnosis of childhood cancer.
CCSS is a retrospectively ascertained cohort of 20,346 childhood cancer survivors diagnosed before age 21 between 1970 and 1986 and approximately 4,000 siblings of survivors, who serve as a control group. The cohort was assembled through the efforts of 26 participating clinical research centers in the United States and Canada. The study is currently funded by a U24 resource grant (NCI grant # U24 CA55727) awarded to St. Jude Children's Research Hospital. Currently, we are in the process of expanding the cohort to include an additional 14,000 childhood cancer survivors diagnosed before age 21 between 1987 and 1999. For information on how to access and utilize the CCSS resource, visit www.stjude.org/ccss
CCSS Institutions and Investigators | |
St. Jude Children's Research Hospital, Memphis, TN | Leslie L. Robison, Ph.D.#‡, Melissa Hudson, M.D.*‡ |
Greg Armstrong, M.D. ‡, Daniel M. Green, M.D. ‡ | |
Children's Healthcare of Atlanta/Emory University Atlanta, GA | Lillian Meacham, M.D. *, Ann Mertens, Ph.D. ‡ |
Children's Hospitals and Clinics of Minnesota Minneapolis St. Paul, MN | Joanna Perkins, M.D.* |
Children's Hospital and Medical Center, Seattle, WA | Douglas Hawkins, M.D.*, Eric Chow, M.D. ‡ |
Children's Hospital, Denver, CO | Brian Greffe, M.D.* |
Children's Hospital Los Angeles, CA | Kathy Ruccione, RN, MPH* |
Children's Hospital, Oklahoma City, OK | John Mulvihill, M.D.‡ |
Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, PA | Jill Ginsberg, M.D.*, Anna Meadows, M.D. ‡ |
Children's Hospital of Pittsburgh, PA | Jean Tersak, M.D.*, |
Children's National Medical Center, Washington, DC | Gregory Reaman, M.D.*, Roger Packer, M.D.‡ |
Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center | Stella Davies, M.D., Ph.D.‡ |
City of Hope- Los Angeles, CA | Smita Bhatia, M.D. *‡ |
Dana-Farber Cancer Institute/Children's Hospital Boston, MA | Lisa Diller, M.D.* †, |
Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, WA | Wendy Leisenring, Sc.D.*‡ |
Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON | Mark Greenberg, MBChB.*, Paul C. Nathan, M.D. *‡ |
International Epidemiology Institute, Rockville, MD | John Boice, Sc.D.‡ |
Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN | Vilmarie Rodriguez, M.D.* |
Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center New York | Charles Sklar, M.D.*‡, Kevin Oeffinger, M.D.‡ |
Miller Children's Hospital | Jerry Finklestein, MD† |
National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD | Roy Wu, Ph.D.†, Nita Sibel, M.D. †, Preetha Rajaraman, Ph.D.† |
Nationwide Children's Hospital, Columbus, Ohio | Amanda Termuhlen, M.D.*, Sue Hammond, M.D.‡ |
Riley Hospital for Children, Indianapolis, IN | Terry A. Vik, M.D.* |
Roswell Park Cancer Institute, Buffalo, NY | Martin Brecher, M.D.* |
St. Louis Children's Hospital, MO | Robert Hayashi, M.D.* |
Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA | Neyssa Marina, M.D. *, Sarah S. Donaldson, M.D. ‡, |
Texas Children's Hospital, Houston, TX | Zoann Dreyer, M.D.* |
University of Alabama, Birmingham, AL | Kimberly Whelan, M.D., MSPH* |
University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB | Yutaka Yasui, Ph.D.‡ |
University of California-Los Angeles, CA | Jacqueline Casillas, MD MSHS*, Lonnie Zeltzer, M.D. †‡ |
University of California-San Francisco, CA | Robert Goldsby, M.D.* |
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI | Raymond Hutchinson, M.D.* |
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN | Joseph Neglia, M.D., MPH‡*, |
University of Southern California | Dennis Deapen, Dr. P.H. ‡ |
UT-Southwestern Medical Center at Dallas, TX | Dan Bowers, M.D.* |
U.T.M.D. Anderson Cancer Center, Houston, TX | Louise Strong, M.D.*‡, Marilyn Stovall, MPH, Ph.D.‡ |
* Institutional Principal Investigator
‡ Member CCSS Steering Committee
† Former Institutional Principal Investigator
# Project Principal Investigator (U24 CA55727)
Appendix 2: Factors and items of the Childhood Cancer Survivors Study Neurocognitive Questionnaire (CCSS-NCQ)
Task Efficiency
It takes me longer to complete my work
I have problems completing my work
I have problems getting started on my own
I am easily overwhelmed
I have trouble doing more than one thing at a time
I have trouble prioritizing my activities
I read slowly
I am slower than others when completing my work
I don't work well under pressure
Emotional Regulation
I get upset easily
I get frustrated easily
My mood changes frequently
Organization
I am disorganized
I have trouble finding things in my bedroom, closet or desk
My desk/workspace is a mess
Memory
I forget instructions easily
I have difficulty recalling things I learned before
I forget what I am doing in the middle of things
I have trouble remembering things, even for a few minutes
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/journals/neu.
References
- Anderson DM, Rennie KM, Ziegler RS, Neglia JP, Robison LR, Gurney JG. Medical and Neurocognitive Late Effects among Survivors of Childhood Central Nervous System Tumors. Cancer. 2001;92:2709–2719. doi: 10.1002/1097-0142(20011115)92:10<2709::aid-cncr1625>3.0.co;2-d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beebe DW, Ris MD, Armstrong FD, Fontanesi J, Mulhern R, Holmes E, Wisoff JH. Cognitive and adaptive outcome in low-grade pediatric cerebellar astrocytomas: evidence of diminished cognitive and adaptive functioning in National Collaborative Research Studies (CCG 9891/POG 9130) Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2005;23(22):5198–204. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.06.117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, Mulhern RK, Katz ER, Kazak AE, Noll RB, Patel SK, Sahler OJ. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2008;76(3):367–78. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellenberg L, McComb JG, Siegel SE, Stowe S. Factors affecting intellectual outcome in pediatric brain tumor patients. Neurosurgery. 1987;21:638–44. doi: 10.1227/00006123-198711000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Odessa, Fla.: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Grill J, Renaux VK, Bulteau C, Viguier D, Levy-Piebois C, Sainte-Rose C, Dellatolas G, Raquin MA, Jambaque I, Kalifa C. Long-term intellectual outcome in children with posterior fossa tumors according to radiation doses and volumes. International Journal of Radiation Oncology Biology Physics. 1999;45:137–45. doi: 10.1016/s0360-3016(99)00177-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guy SC, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Self Report Version. Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Halberg FE, Kramer JH, Moore IM, Wara WM, Matthay KK, Ablin AR. Prophylactic cranial irradiation dose effects on late cognitive function in children treated for acute lymphoblastic leukemia. International Journal of Radiation Oncology Biology Physics. 1992;22:13–6. doi: 10.1016/0360-3016(92)90976-o. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karatekin C, Lazareff JA, Asarnow RF. Relevance of the cerebellar hemispheres for executive functions. Pediatric Neurology. 2000;22:106–12. doi: 10.1016/s0887-8994(99)00128-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kieffer-Renaux V, Bulteau C, Grill J, Kalifa C, Viguier D, Jambaque I. Patterns of neuropsychological deficits in children with medulloblastoma according to craniospatial irradiation doses. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology. 2000;42:741–5. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200001377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiehna EN, Mulhern RK, Li C, Xiong X, Merchant TE. Changes in attentional performance of children and young adults with localized primary brain tumors after conformal radiation therapy. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2006;24(33):5283–90. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.03.8547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krull KR, Brouwers P, Jain N, Zhang L, Bomgaars L, Dreyer Z, Mahoney D, Bottomley S, Okcu MF. Folate pathway genetic polymorphisms are related to attention disorders in childhood leukemia survivors. The Journal of Pediatrics. 152:101–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.05.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krull KR, Gioia G, Ness KK, Ellenberg L, Recklitis C, Leisenring W, Huang S, Stovall M, Robison LL, Zeltzer L. Reliability and validity of the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study Neurocognitive Questionnaire. Cancer. 2008 Sep 12; doi: 10.1002/cncr.23809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levisohn L, Cronin-Golomb A, Schmahmann JD. Neuropsychological consequences of cerebellar tumour resection in a paediatric population. Brain. 2000;123:1041–50. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A, Strother D, Bouffet E. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22:159–68. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.2.159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mostow EN, Byrne J, Connelly RR, Mulvihill JJ. Quality of life in long-term survivors of CNS tumors of childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 1991;9:592–9. doi: 10.1200/JCO.1991.9.4.592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulhern RK, Hancock J, Fairclough D, Kun L. Neuropsychological status of children treated for brain tumors: A critical review and integrative analysis. Medical and Pediatric Oncology. 1992;20:181–191. doi: 10.1002/mpo.2950200302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulhern RK, Kepner JL, Thomas PR, Armstrong FD, Friedman HS, Kun LE. Neuropsychologic functioning of survivors of childhood medulloblastoma randomized to receive conventional or reduced-dose craniospinal irradiation: a Pediatric Oncology Group study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 1998;16:1723–8. doi: 10.1200/JCO.1998.16.5.1723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Kun LE. Late neurocognitive sequelae in survivors of brain tumours in childhood. Lancet Oncology. 2004;5:399–408. doi: 10.1016/S1470-2045(04)01507-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulhern RK, Palmer SL. Neurocognitive late effects in pediatric cancer. Current Problems in Cancer. 2003;4:177–197. doi: 10.1016/s0147-0272(03)00026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulhern RK, Reddick WE, Palmer SL, Glass JO, Elkin TD, Kun LE, Taylor J, Langston J, Gajjar A. Neurocognitive deficits in medulloblastoma survivors and white matter loss. Annals of Neurology. 1999;46:834–41. doi: 10.1002/1531-8249(199912)46:6<834::aid-ana5>3.0.co;2-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagel BJ, Delis DC, Palmer SL, Reeves C, Gajjar A, Mulhern RK. Early patterns of verbal memory impairment in children treated for medulloblastoma. Neuropsychology. 2006;20:105–12. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Packer RJ, Gurner JG, Punyko JA, Donaldson SS, Inskip PD, Stovall M, Yasui Y, Mertens AC, Sklar CA, Nicholson HS, Zeltzer LK, Neglia JP, Robison LL. Long-term neurologic and neurosensory sequelae in adult survivors of a childhood brain tumor: Childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2003;21:3255–61. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2003.01.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer SL, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Glass JO, Kun LE, Wu S, Xiong X, Mulhern RK. Predicting intellectual outcome among children treated with 35-40 Gy craniospinal irradiation for medulloblastoma. Neuropsychology. 2003;17:35–40. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.4.548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Palmer SL, Goloubeva O, Reddick WE, Palmer SL, Glass JO, Gajjar A, Kun L, Merchant TE, Mulhern RK. Patterns of intellectual development among survivors of pediatric medulloblastoma: a longitudinal analysis. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2001;19:2302–2308. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2001.19.8.2302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rabin LA, Roth RM, Isquith PK, Wishart HA, Nutter-Upham KE, Pare N, Flashman LA, Saykin AJ. Self- and informant reports of executive function on the BRIEF-A in MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2006;21(7):721–32. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radcliffe J, Bunin GR, Sutton LN, Goldwein JW, Phillips PC. Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood medulloblastoma and other noncortical tumors: age-dependent effects of whole brain radiation. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 1994;12(4):327–34. doi: 10.1016/0736-5748(94)90081-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reamers TS, Ehrenfels S, Mortensen EL, Schmiegelow M, Sonderkaer S, Carstensen H, Schmiegelow K, Muller J. Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood brain tumors: Identification of predictive factors. Medical and Pediatric Oncology. 2003;40:26–34. doi: 10.1002/mpo.10211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reddick WE, White HA, Glass JO, Wheeler GC, Thompson SJ, Gajjar A, Leigh L, Mulhern RK. Developmental model relating white matter volume to neurocognitive deficits in pediatric brain tumor survivors. Cancer. 2003;97:2512–9. doi: 10.1002/cncr.11355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ris MD. Lessons in pediatric neuropsycho-oncology: What we have learned since Johnny Gunther. Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 2007;32:1029–1057. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ris MD, Packer R, Goldwein J, Jones-Wallace D, Boyett JM. Intellectual outcome after reduced-dose radiation therapy plus adjuvant chemotherapy for medulloblastoma: a Children's Cancer Group study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2001;19(15):3470–6. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2001.19.15.3470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riva D, Giorgi C. The cerebellum contributes to higher functions during development: evidence from a series of children surgically treated for posterior fossa tumours. Brain. 2000;123:1051–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robison LL, Mertens AC, Boice JD, Breslow NE, Donaldson SS, Green DM, Li FP, Meadows AT, Mulvihill JJ, Neglia JP, Nesbit ME, Packer RJ, Potter JD, Sklar CA, Smith MA, Stovall M, Strong LC, Yasui Y, Zeltzer LK. Study design and cohort characteristics of the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study: a multi-institutional collaborative project. Medical and Pediatric Oncology. 2002;38(4):229–39. doi: 10.1002/mpo.1316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function –Adult Version. Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Sands SA, Kellie SJ, Davidow AL, Diez B, Villablanca J, Weiner HL, Pietanza MC, Balmaceda C, Finlay JL. Long-term quality of life and neuropsychologic functioning for patients with CNS germ-cell tumors: From the First International CNS Germ-Cell Tumor Study. Neuro-Oncology. 2001;3:175–83. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/3.3.174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schatz J, Kramer JH, Ablin A, Matthay KK. Processing speed, working memory, and IQ: A developmental model of cognitive deficits following cranial radiation therapy. Neuropsychology. 2000;14:189–200. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain. 1998;121:561–79. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.4.561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz KA, Ness KK, Whitton J, Recklitis C, Zebrack B, Robison LL, Zeltzer L, Mertens AC. Behavioral and social outcomes in adolescent survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2007;25(24):3649–56. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2006.09.2486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smibert E, Anderson V, Godber T, Ekert H. Risk factors for intellectual and educational sequelae of cranial irradiation in childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Br J Cancer. 1996;73:825–30. doi: 10.1038/bjc.1996.145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiegler BJ, Bouffet E, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Mabbott DJ. Change in neurocognitive functioning after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2004;22(4):706–13. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2004.05.186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinlin M, Imfeld S, Zulauf P, Boltshauser E, Lovblad K, Luthy AR, Perrig W, Kaufmann F. Neuropsychological long-term sequelas after posterior fossa tumour resection during childhood. Brain. 2003;126:1998–2008. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor G, Yeates KO, Wade SL, Drotar D, Stancin T, Minich N. A prospective study of short- and long-term outcomes after traumatic brain injury in children: behavior and achievement. Neuropsychology. 16:15–27. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.16.1.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong CS, Van der Kogel AJ. Mechanisms of radiation injury to the central nervous system: Implications for neuroprotection. Molecular Interventions. 2004;4:273–284. doi: 10.1124/mi.4.5.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zebrack BJ, Gurney JG, Oeffinger K, Whitton J, Packer RJ, Mertens A, Turk N, Castleberry R, Dreyer Z, Robison LL, Zeltzer LK. Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Journal of Clinical Oncology. 2004;22(6):999–1006. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2004.06.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeger SL, Liang KY. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics. 1986;42(1):121–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeltzer LK, Lu Q, Leisenring W, Tsao JCI, Recklitis C, Armstrong G, Mertens AC, Robison LL, Ness KK. Psychosocial outcomes and health-related quality of life in adult childhood cancer survivors: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention. 2008;17:435–446. doi: 10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-07-2541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]