Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2009 Dec 21.
Published in final edited form as: Emotion. 2009 Dec;9(6):847–854. doi: 10.1037/a0017745

The Unconscious Regulation of Emotion: Nonconscious Reappraisal Goals Modulate Emotional Reactivity

Lawrence E Williams 1, John A Bargh 1, Christopher C Nocera 1, Jeremy R Gray 1
PMCID: PMC2796590  NIHMSID: NIHMS135211  PMID: 20001127

Abstract

People often encounter difficulty when making conscious attempts to regulate their emotions. We propose that nonconscious self-regulatory processes may be of help in these difficult circumstances, because nonconscious processes are not subject to the same set of limitations as are conscious processes. Two experiments examined the effects of nonconsciously operating goals on people's emotion-regulatory success. In Experiment 1, participants engaged in an anxiety-eliciting task who had a reappraisal emotion-control goal primed and operating nonconsciously achieved the same decrease in physiological reactivity as those explicitly instructed to reappraise, compared to a control group. In Experiment 2, the effect of nonconscious reappraisal priming on physiological reactivity was shown to be most pronounced for those who do not habitually use reappraisal strategies. The findings highlight the potential importance of nonconscious goals for facilitating emotional control in complex real-world environments, and have implications for contemporary models of emotion-regulation.

Keywords: nonconscious goals, emotion regulation, reappraisal, dual-process

Every day, people are bombarded with emotionally stimulating information. Details in the media, busy streets, and one's family life all have the potential to influence people's emotional lives, whether these influences are welcome or not. Although emotions serve important functions, such as informing people of their goal progress and to signal that one's attention is needed elsewhere (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1990; Mandler, 1975; Simon, 1967), people often attempt to regulate intense, distressing, or otherwise disruptive emotional states. The present research examines a potential way to help people increase their control over their emotions, via the operation of nonconscious regulatory goals.

When conscious attempts at emotion regulation are difficult, we suggest that nonconscious self-regulatory processes can augment these efforts and increase the likelihood of self-regulatory success. First, across a variety of goal contents, nonconsciously operating goals have been found to produce the same outcomes, and in the same manner, as when those same goals are consciously pursued (see reviews in Bargh, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, in press; Bargh & Huang, 2009; Bargh & Morsella, 2008). Extending this principle to the domain of emotion regulation, we would expect nonconscious emotion-regulation goals to be capable of successfully regulating emotional experience. Second, as it is the nature of nonconscious processes to operate more efficiently, in parallel with other ongoing processes, and thus to not be subject to the same memory and attention limitations as are conscious thought processes (e.g., Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006; Posner & Snyder, 1975; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977), we would predict that nonconscious emotion regulation goals might be of particular value under circumstances that prevent or hinder the operation of conscious emotion regulation strategies. In what follows, we present empirical evidence bearing on the effectiveness of nonconscious goals in helping people overcome their emotion regulatory challenges.

Nonconscious operation of the higher mental processes

Over the past 25 years, social psychologists have produced numerous demonstrations of nonconscious processes producing the same outcomes as their conscious counterparts, across a variety of research domains (see Bargh & Morsella, in press). Nonconsciously primed concepts can exert significant directive influences over behavior (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2003; Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996; Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar, & Troetschel, 2001; Dijksterhuis & van Knippenberg, 1998), suggesting that an important source of the impulses from which human action springs may be automatic environmental priming effects. For example, the simple presence of a briefcase in a room leads people to behave more competitively than they do when there happens to be a backpack in the room instead (Kay, Wheeler, Bargh, & Ross, 2004). Moreover, nonconsciously operating goal pursuits manifest the same underlying phenomenal qualities as when the same goal is operating consciously: not only does nonconscious priming of the achievement goal improve participants' subsequent task performance, such as when finding solutions to anagrams, it also increases participants' perseverance on the task, the strength of their desire to complete it (Bargh et al., 2001), and even the flexibility of their responding in the face of sudden and unannounced changes in the task rules (e.g., in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; Hassin, Bargh, & Zimerman, in press).

Nonconscious processes have thus been shown to attain the same outcomes as conscious versions of the same process, through the same processing steps and with the same phenomenal qualities. In fact, some researchers now claim that in domains such as decision making, nonconscious thought might even produce better outcomes than conscious processing of the same information (Dijksterhuis, Bos, Nordgren, & van Baaren, 2006; Dijksterhuis & Nordgren, 2006), because conscious thinking is constrained both by the limited capacity of working memory and the serial nature of conscious processing, whereas unconscious thought is not. Although that conclusion of superior outcomes remains somewhat controversial (Lassiter, Lindberg, Gonzalez-Vallego, Bellezza, & Phillips, 2009), there is no question that nonconscious processes operate more efficiently than conscious processes, in that they are not as affected by the same distractions and divisions of attention that are able to block their conscious brethren from operating (e.g., Bargh & Thein, 1985; Jacoby, 1991; Spencer, Fein, Wolfe, & Dunn, 1998).

Nonconscious emotion-regulation

Historically in the self-regulation literature, success or positive outcomes have been associated with conscious control, while the blame for negative outcomes has been laid at the feet of automatic influences (e.g., Mischel & Ayduk, 2004). For example, for individuals who are trying to lose weight, automatic impulses are seen to lead to the overconsumption of fattening foods, while controlled, conscious processes are seen as necessary to prevent these impulses from unduly affecting behavior (Baumeister, Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). However, research on nonconscious goal pursuit has demonstrated positive outcomes as well, such as better performance on a variety of tasks (with a primed achievement goal; Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994), increased cooperation in a commons dilemma (with a primed cooperation goal; Bargh et al., 2001), and greater concern for the welfare of others (with indirect priming of a communal relationship orientation; Chen, Lee-Chai, & Bargh, 2001). Therefore, just as positive interpersonal goals (e.g., cooperation) can be primed to operate outside of people's awareness, we expect that helpful emotion-regulatory goals can also be nonconsciously instigated, such that people need not rely solely on conscious, volitional regulation strategies to navigate their emotional lives.

Nonconscious emotion regulation can be defined as the unintentional, automatic, and relatively effortless control of one's exposure to, processing of, and response to emotionally evocative events (Bargh & Williams, 2007). The mechanism underlying nonconscious emotion regulation is posited to be nonconscious goal pursuit, which has been shown to be flexibly adaptive to dynamic, changing situational demands (Bargh & Morsella, 2008; Hassin, 2005; Hassin et al., in press). We expect nonconscious emotion regulation to occur when a person's successful pursuit of currently active goals depends on either the up- or down-regulation of phenomenal affect.

Consistent with these hypotheses, Mauss, Cook, and Gross (2007) recently found that priming people with an emotional control goal leads to reduced anger after an experimental provocation, compared to people primed with an emotional expression goal. In these studies, participants were nonconsciously primed with words related to either the full expression of emotions (volatile, boiled), or words related to emotional control (stable, restrains). After being subjected to an anger provocation, participants primed with emotional expression exhibited greater anger compared to participants primed with emotional control. These results provide initial evidence for the view that nonconscious goals can help people control their emotional states without subjective feelings of effort, intentionality, or awareness.

Research on emotion regulation has shown that people try to regulate their emotions by directly modifying their responses to emotionally evocative stimuli (a response-focused strategy) or by reappraising the meaning of those emotional stimuli before experiencing the emotion (an antecedent-focused strategy; Gross, 1998). A number of investigations have shown a link between the habitual use of reappraisal strategies for emotion regulation and positive psychosocial outcomes (Gross & John, 2003; Magar, Phillips, & Hosie, 2008; Richards & Gross, 2000). People who habitually use reappraisal strategies to control their emotions have better interpersonal relationships and report higher levels of well-being, compared to people who habitually use suppression strategies (Gross & John, 2003). Also, recent research has found that habitual use of reappraisal strategies is negatively correlated with potentially dangerous behaviors such as smoking and alcohol-induced fights (Magar et al., 2008). Accordingly, the present studies focused on the effect of nonconscious reappraisal priming on the individual's reactivity to an emotionally evocative stressor.

We suggest that nonconscious goal pursuit can bolster a person's ability to regulate their emotions. For nonconscious goal pursuit to be initiated, all that is required is the perception of relevant stimuli to activate the goal representation; the goal will then be pursued even though there is no conscious awareness of the primes, active intention towards the goal, or active guidance of goal-directed thought and behavior (Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994). We propose that such nonconscious goal pursuit can augment the human capacity for conscious emotion-regulation, especially under conditions in which people are less likely or able to use effective conscious strategies. In Experiment 1, we assessed the benefits of a nonconscious reappraisal goal for people facing an anxiety-inducing task, a situation (e.g., delivering a counter-attitudinal speech) in which emotion regulation is necessary but difficult. In the second experiment, we tested the hypothesis that the benefits of nonconscious emotion regulation (reappraisal) would be most pronounced for those individuals who are less likely to naturally or spontaneously use a conscious reappraisal strategy.

Experiment 1: Nonconscious versus conscious emotion regulation

Recent investigations have shown that people can nonconsciously regulate their emotions (Mauss, Cook, & Gross, 2007). The present experiment builds upon and extends this research in several important ways. First, it moves beyond the general goal of ‘emotion control’ to test for the existence of nonconscious goals related to more specific emotion-regulation strategies such as reappraisal (Gross, 1998). Second, it provides a first direct comparison of the effectiveness of nonconscious and conscious emotion-regulation. Third, it includes a control group in which no emotion regulation goal is induced by the experimental procedures, either through explicit instruction or implicit priming, in order to compare the effects of experimentally manipulated conscious and nonconscious emotion regulation to those of the regulatory efforts people spontaneously and naturally use when faced with a stressor.

In Experiment 1, a third of participants were nonconsciously primed with a reappraisal goal. A second third of participants were explicitly instructed to reappraise their emotions (Gross, 1998). The remaining participants were neither primed nor received explicit reappraisal instructions, and served as a control group. All participants then mentally prepared for a surprise oral speech on a counter-attitudinal topic, during which we collected continuous heart rate recordings. We hypothesized that nonconsciously priming the reappraisal goal would help people regulate their emotional reactivity, as reflected in their heart rate.

Method

Participants and Design

Thirty-nine undergraduates (25 female, age range: 18 – 39 years, M = 20.6) participated in a 3 groups (nonconscious reappraisal vs. conscious reappraisal vs. control) between subjects design in exchange for ten dollars. The experimental session lasted approximately 45 minutes.

Procedure

On arrival at the lab, participants were escorted into a separate room containing the psychophysiological recording equipment and were informed that the experiment was concerned with emotions and verbal abilities. Participants were then prepared for psychophysiological recordings. We attached two sensors to participants' wrists. Participants were then instructed to “sit back, clear your mind of thoughts and feelings” while a 3-minute baseline assessment was recorded (pre-stressor recording).

Next, participants were primed with either a reappraisal goal or neutral concepts using a scrambled sentences task (Srull & Wyer, 1979). Those primed with reappraisal unscrambled five neutral sentences and five sentences containing words or phrases related to reappraisal (reassessed, perspective, appraised again, carefully analyzed, strategy). Participants in the conscious reappraisal and control conditions unscrambled ten neutral sentences.

Afterwards, participants were given the oral speech instructions. Specifically, they were told that they would have “three minutes to mentally prepare a timed two-minute oral speech which will be graded on the quality of various aspects. The topic will be why the increasing cost of tuition is a good thing.” Participants in the nonconscious reappraisal and control conditions received no further instructions. Participants in the conscious reappraisal condition were provided with the following additional instructions (adapted from Gross, 1998):

“As you prepare the speech try to adopt a detached and unemotional attitude. Try strategically to reassess the situation and adopt a neutral attitude toward the speech task. In other words, as you are preparing the speech, try to think about the task objectively, in a way that is unemotionally relevant to you.”

All participants were then given three minutes to prepare a speech, during which heart rate recordings were taken (post-stressor recording). Following the speech preparation period, each participant delivered the speech to the experimenter. Following the speech itself, a final 3-minute post speech heart rate recording was taken to monitor participants' recovery to their baseline levels of physiological reactivity. Finally, participants were probed for suspicion and debriefed using the funneled debriefing method (see Bargh & Chartrand, 2000). No participants indicated suspicion of the prime, or guessed the hypotheses of the study.

Physiological measures

During the session, physiological channels were sampled at 400 Hz utilizing a BIOPAC Universal Interface Module (MP100). We used heart rate (HR) as a measure of physiological activity related to emotional response, consistent with recent investigations of the effect of emotion regulation on cardiovascular reactivity (Mauss, Cook, Cheng, & Gross, 2007). Cardiac R-R wave interval measurements provided mean heart rate activity for each 3-minute stage. A BIOPAC electrocardiogram amplifier module was connected directly with two Ag-AgCl lead adhesive snap electrodes placed on both wrists.

Three 3-minute psychophysiology recordings were taken at baseline (pre-stressor), during speech task preparation (post-stressor), and post speech. Heart rates (beats per minute) were averaged within each recording across 3-second intervals. The pre-stressor and post-stressor recordings were used to calculate percent change in physiological reactivity (see Min, Chung & Min, 2005, for a similar treatment of psychophysiological data). This index served as the primary dependent variable.

Results And Discussion

Table 1 shows the mean heart rates for the baseline, speech preparation, and post-speech recording periods. An ANOVA on the mean baseline heart rate (pre-stressor) indicated that there was no initial difference in reactivity among the experimental groups, F(2, 36) = 1.31, ns. Next, we examined whether nonconsciously priming people with a reappraisal goal affects emotional reactivity to the stressful task, by means of an ANOVA on the percent change observed in heart rate activity from baseline to the speech preparation phase. As seen in Figure 1, there was a significant effect of experimental condition on percent change in HR, F(2, 36) = 4.56, p < .05, ηp2 = .20. Post hoc comparisons using Fisher's LSD indicated that both the nonconscious reappraisal (M = .04) and the conscious reappraisal groups (M = .05) had significantly less HR reactivity compared to control participants (M = .11), both p's < .05, with the means of the two reappraisal conditions not significantly different from each other, p > .5. Thus, the nonconscious reappraisal prime and the conscious reappraisal instructions were equally effective in helping people manage their emotions, compared to the control group, supporting the conclusion that nonconsciously operating emotion regulation goals can assist in self-regulatory activities.

Table 1.

Experiment 1 Mean Heart Rates (Standard Deviations) across Recording Periods as a Function of Condition

Experimental Condition Pre-stressor: Baseline Period Post-stressor: Speech Preparation Period Recovery: Post-Speech Period
Nonconscious reappraisal group 67.9 (9.81)a 70.6 (9.99)a 66.4 (8.81)a
Conscious reappraisal group 74.1 (9.22)a 78.6 (11.8)b 73.5 (5.63)b
Control group 71.4 (10.1)a 80.3 (11.6)b 70.1 (8.58)a

Note. Mean heart rates collected over 3-minute recording periods. Means in the same column with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05 in the Fisher's least significant difference test.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mean percent change in heart rate between baseline and speech preparation phase as a function of priming condition. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

In Experiment 1, participants primed with a reappraisal goal showed less emotional reactivity than a control group to a stressful task, as measured by the change in their mean heart rate from baseline. In harmony with Mauss, Cook and Gross (2007), these findings support the hypothesis that nonconscious processes can be a boon to successful self-regulation. They also extend prior research by demonstrating the effectiveness in nonconscious form of a specific emotion-regulatory strategy, reappraisal, and by furnishing a comparison of nonconscious and conscious emotion-reappraisal goal effectiveness. As in many previous studies, the same outcomes were observed with nonconscious operation of the goal as when it is consciously pursued: those primed with the reappraisal goal managed their emotions as well as did participants who were explicitly instructed to reappraise their emotional situation.

In the second experiment, we assessed whether chronic, trait differences in the use of reappraisal strategies for emotion control moderate the effect of nonconscious reappraisal priming. For habitual reappraisers, the repeated pursuit of the reappraisal goal should lead that goal to become automatically associated with those situational settings requiring emotion regulation (Bargh, 1990, 1997; Bargh & Chartrand, 1999; Wood & Neal, 2007). When this occurs, the reappraisal goal will become active and operative merely upon the individual entering those situations, without having to be consciously chosen and pursued at that time. In harmony with this analysis, it has been shown that the habitual use of reappraisal strategies does not incur high costs in terms of limited cognitive resources, compared to a suppression strategy (Richards & Gross, 2000).

Therefore, for individuals who chronically use reappraisal strategies, the activation of the reappraisal goal should occur automatically when the person is faced with an emotionally evocative stressor, without needing temporary priming to be put into operation. For those who do not habitually reappraise, on the other hand, reappraisal priming should have a more pronounced effect, because for these individuals the reappraisal goal is not already chronically accessible. Past research comparing chronic and temporary forms of accessibility in the domain of impression formation has shown temporary priming effects to mimic chronic accessibility effects in those individuals for whom the construct is not chronically accessible (see Bargh, 1989; Bargh, Bond, Lombardi, & Tota, 1986).

Because chronically accessible constructs operate more efficiently than do other constructs, they are better able to function under conditions of cognitive load, such as distraction, arousal, or time pressure (e.g., Bargh & Thein, 1985). Thus, primed and nonconsciously operating reappraisal goals should be most helpful to those who do not chronically or habitually engage in reappraisal, when faced with the difficult situations requiring emotion regulation. Nonconsciously activated goals should operate more efficiently (i.e., use less in the way of limited information processing resources) compared to deliberately activated goals, and thus the nonconscious mode of emotion regulation should be better able to function under conditions known to prevent conscious goals from operating (i.e., conditions requiring active intention, attention, or limited ego resources; Baumeister, Muraven, & Tice, 2000). In short, nonconscious emotion regulation goals may be especially useful for those who do not habitually use reappraisal strategies to meet their self-regulatory challenges (Bargh & Williams, 2007; Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2004), and so in Experiment 2 we examined trait tendencies towards the use of reappraisal strategies as a potential moderator of the effect of nonconscious goal pursuit on emotion regulation.

Experiment 2: Trait reappraisal and nonconscious emotion regulation

Recent data suggests that people vary in their ability to successfully control their emotions. Mauss, Cook, Cheng, and Gross (2007) demonstrated that people who habitually reappraise their emotions respond more adaptively to an emotionally evocative event, compared to people who use reappraisal strategies less frequently. After an anger provocation, low reappraisers report experiencing more anger and negative emotions, and have a worse cardiac output profile than high reappraisers.

Based on these findings, we hypothesized that nonconscious reappraisal should especially benefit those who do not frequently engage in conscious reappraisal. Participants were assigned to the same conditions as in Experiment 1 (nonconscious reappraisal vs. conscious reappraisal vs. control), and completed the same speech task while having their heart rate recorded. However, after delivering the speech, participants completed the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003), which measures trait tendencies to use reappraisal and suppression emotion control strategies. Because nonconscious goal primes can produce the same effects as in automatized, habitual goal pursuit, and because people who report a low frequency of using reappraisal strategies for emotional control do not use those strategies habitually, we hypothesized that nonconscious reappraisal priming will help those who do not spontaneously use reappraisal strategies in their emotion regulation attempts to a greater extent than would explicit instructions to reappraise.

Method

Participants and Design

Forty-seven undergraduate and graduate students (31 female; age range 18-40 years, M = 21.3) participated in a 3 groups (nonconscious reappraisal vs. conscious reappraisal vs. control) between subjects design in exchange for 10 dollars. The experimental session lasted approximately 45 minutes.

Procedure and Materials

The procedure for this experiment was identical to the first experiment, with the sole exception being the administration of the ERQ (Gross & John, 2003) at the end of the experimental session. We averaged and centered the six reappraisal items on the ERQ into a scale to assess people's habitual use of reappraisal strategies (ERQ-R; α = .79). Participants indicated the extent to which they agree with statements such as “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I'm in” using a scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). We expected that the effects of the priming manipulation would be moderated by individual differences in people's reappraisal tendencies. We also averaged and centered the four suppression items on the ERQ into a scale to assess people's habitual use of reappraisal strategies (ERQ-S; α = .73); however, we did not expect suppression scores to moderate the effects of the manipulations. At the end of the experimental session, participants were debriefed using the funneled debriefing technique. Again, no participants indicated suspicion of the prime, or guessed the hypotheses of the study.

Results And Discussion

Table 2 shows the mean heart rates for the baseline, speech preparation, and post-speech recording periods. As in Experiment 1, we first conducted an ANOVA on mean baseline heart rate between conditions, which revealed no significant difference between groups, F(2, 44) = 1.55, ns. Next, we conducted an ANOVA which indicated that the experimental groups differed in the amount of physiological reactivity in response to the stressor, reflected in the percent change in heart rate, F(2, 44) = 12.06, p < .01, ηp2 = .35. Post hoc comparisons using Fisher's LSD indicated that both the nonconscious reappraisal (M = .03) and the conscious reappraisal groups (M = .08) had significantly less HR reactivity compared to control participants (M = .14), both p's < .01, replicating the pattern of results in Experiment 1. Moreover, the nonconscious reappraisal group had marginally less change in HR reactivity compared to the conscious reappraisal group, p = .06.

Table 2.

Experiment 2 Mean Heart Rates (Standard Deviations) across Recording Periods as a Function of Condition

Experimental Condition Pre-stressor: Baseline Period Post-stressor: Speech Preparation Period Recovery: Post-Speech Period
Nonconscious reappraisal group 71.2 (9.40)a 73.9 (12.5)a 68.9 (9.19)a
Conscious reappraisal group 73.2 (10.8)a 79.2 (10.7)a 72.3 (9.60)a
Control group 67.3 (8.23)a 78.8 (8.60)a 68.9 (8.75)a

Note. Mean heart rates collected over 3-minute recording periods. Means in the same column with different subscripts differ significantly at p < .05 in the Fisher's least significant difference test.

To examine whether the impact of nonconscious priming is moderated by trait tendency to use reappraisal or suppression strategies, we conducted a series of regression analyses predicting HR reactivity (percent change) using experimental conditions, ERQ scores, and their interactions as predictors. Given the three levels in our primary predictor variable (experimental condition), we created two dummy-coded variables, one indicating whether or not participants were primed, and the other indicating whether or not participants were explicitly instructed to reappraise. We then use these in separate regressions to analyze whether habitual use of reappraisal (suppression) strategies moderates the effect of experimental conditions on emotional reactivity.

Trait reappraisal

In the first analysis comparing the nonconscious reappraisal group to the conscious reappraisal and control groups, the overall model was significant, F(3, 43) = 7.00, p < .01, r2 = .33. Priming condition significantly predicted HR reactivity, β = -0.46, t(43) = -3.65, p < .01, replicating the effects of Experiment 1, but trait reappraisal scores did not, β = -0.19, ns. Critically, the interaction between priming condition and ERQ-reappraisal scores significantly contributed to the model, β = 0.42, t(43) = 2.28, p = .03 (see Figure 2, which for the sake of clarity presents this interaction as a median split between high and low reappraisers).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Mean percent change in heart rate between baseline and speech preparation phase as a function of priming condition and ERQ-reappraisal score (median split). Error bars represent standard errors of the mean.

Simple effects analyses clarify the nature of this interaction effect. For participants with high reappraisal scores on the ERQ, there was no difference between the nonconscious reappraisal and conscious reappraisal groups in terms of HR reactivity, F < 1, ns. However, amongst participants who had low reappraisal scores on the ERQ, those nonconsciously primed to reappraise had significantly less HR reactivity than participants consciously instructed to do so, F(1, 27) = 5.34, p < .05. The second model comparing the conscious reappraisal group to the nonconscious reappraisal and control groups was not significant, F(3, 43) = 1.08, ns, and there was no interaction between experimental condition and ERQ-reappraisal scores, β = -0.20, t(43) = -1.11, p > .25. These results lend support to the conclusion that nonconscious implementation of a reappraisal strategy is most beneficial for those individuals who tend not to reappraise their emotional situations naturally on their own.

Trait suppression

We conducted an identical set of analyses examining the interactive effects of experimental conditions and ERQ-suppression scores. In the first model comparing the nonconscious reappraisal group to the conscious reappraisal and control groups, the overall model was significant, F(3, 43) = 4.84, p < .01, r2 = .25. Priming condition significantly predicted HR reactivity, β = -0.47, t(43) = -3.51, p < .01. However, in contrast to the reappraisal analysis, the interaction between priming condition and ERQ-suppression scores did not significantly contribute to the model, β = .17, t(43) = 1.01, p > .30. Further, the second model comparing the conscious reappraisal group to the nonconscious reappraisal and control groups was not significant, F(3, 43) = 0.17, ns and there was no interaction between experimental condition and ERQ-suppression scores, β = -0.08, t(43) = -0.45, p > .30. Although caution must be used in arguing from null findings, the overall pattern of data supports the conclusion that the present nonconscious priming manipulation activated a specific emotion-reappraisal goal and not merely a general emotion-control goal, because only the individual difference measure of habitual use of reappraisal strategies -- not of emotion-suppression strategies -- moderated the effect of the reappraisal prime on reactivity.

Thus nonconscious reappraisal goals can help people successfully control their emotions. Furthermore, the effects of the nonconscious reappraisal goal were moderated by self-reported tendencies to use reappraisal strategies, benefiting those who do not reappraise habitually more than people who do. This finding is consistent with previous investigations in which nonconscious priming effects are most pronounced in those for whom the primed concepts are not chronically activated (cf. Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee, 1999, Experiment 2). Given the common claim that many of the challenges to successful self-regulation are rooted in nonconscious or automatic impulses (e.g., Mischel & Ayduk, 2004), these results suggest a way to “fight fire with fire” by harnessing the power of nonconscious goals to control unwanted nonconscious influences (Bargh, 1999).

General Discussion

When people attempt to control their emotions, they often fail. The present results have important implications for people's ability to withstand the emotion-regulatory challenges they routinely face in daily life. Nonconscious prompts to reappraise one's emotional situation can reduce reactivity to emotionally evocative events. The present experiments are in harmony with other recent findings that nonconsciously activated emotion regulation goals can help people meet such challenges (Mauss, Cook, & Gross, 2007). Nonconscious reappraisal priming was found to be significantly more effective than people's spontaneous regulatory efforts (in the control condition). The nonconsciously operating reappraisal goal enabled participants to control their emotional reactions on a stressful task, and in Study 2 even marginally better than those who were consciously attempting to reappraise. Though participants in the priming condition were not aware of the activation and operation of this goal, it nonetheless was successful in attenuating the emotional impact of the giving-a-speech task.

The present findings also revealed that the nonconscious activation and operation of reappraisal goals help those most who do not habitually use reappraisal strategies. This is encouraging evidence that the efficiency of nonconscious goal pursuit (Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994) can help us self-regulate effectively especially when we are not able to do so consciously and intentionally. Thus environmental cues associated with situational reappraisal goals can be a potentially powerful aid to the effective navigation of one's emotional world.

Limitations and future directions

Although there are clear theoretical reasons to expect that nonconscious emotion regulation might be even more effective than conscious regulation, because automatic processes are less limited by constraints on information processing resources, support for this prediction was mixed: we found in Experiment 1 that the effect of nonconscious reappraisal priming on emotional reactivity was not significantly greater than the effect of conscious (explicit) reappraisal instructions, while in Experiment 2 there was a marginal difference such that the nonconscious reappraisal group showed less reactivity compared to the conscious reappraisal group. Future investigations could profitably examine other indices of emotion regulatory benefits, such as the amount of effort one experiences while controlling one's emotions, or the extent to which nonconscious versus conscious emotion regulation depletes self-regulatory resources (see Baumeister et al., 2000). Related, the present studies featured just one index of emotional reactivity (changes in heart rate across the experimental sessions), and the use of other subjective (self-report) and physiological indices (skin conductance) could shed additional light on the effectiveness of nonconscious emotion regulation. Finally, the present research focused on emotion reappraisal strategies, but there are of course other methods for controlling one's emotions (e.g., attentional deployment; suppression of emotional response), and it remains an empirical issue whether environmental priming can activate these other methods as well. For example, it is possible that nonconscious priming of a suppression goal can have paradoxical effects on people's emotional reactivity to a stressful situation (cf. Gross, 1998; Wegner, Ansfield, & Pilloff, 1998).

In conclusion, it appears that the nonconscious mode of goal pursuit can help people control their emotions, even when they are not expert at using psychologically beneficial strategies, such as reappraisal, to do so. Traditionally, successful self-regulation has been associated with exerting conscious control over one's actions, whereas acting on automatic impulses has been associated with self-regulatory failures (Vohs & Baumeister, 2004). However, there are common conditions of daily life, such as distraction, arousal, stress, and time pressure (i.e., cognitive load) under which conscious processes are unable to operate but nonconscious processes can (e.g., Bargh & Thein 1985; Spencer, Fein, Wolfe, & Dunn, 1998). Especially for these circumstances, therefore, nonconscious emotion-regulatory goals can be an effective aid to self-control – and as the present Experiment 2 found, mainly of help to those who need it most: those less skilled at conscious deployment of the most effective regulatory strategy. That successful emotional control can be attained through nonconscious as well as conscious processes expands both the human repertoire of emotion regulation capacities and the sphere of influence of nonconscious processes in everyday life.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a National Science Foundation Predoctoral Fellowship to Williams, and by NIMH Grant R01 MH60767 to Bargh.

References

  1. Aarts H, Dijksterhuis A. The silence of the library: Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:18–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bargh JA. Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence on social perception and cognition. In: Uleman J, Bargh J, editors. Unintended thought. New York: Guilford; 1989. pp. 3–51. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bargh JA. Auto-motives: Preconscious determinants of social interaction. In: Higgins ET, Sorrentino RM, editors. Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior. Vol. 2. New York: Guilford; 1990. pp. 93–130. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bargh JA. The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In: Wyer RS Jr, Srull TK, editors. Handbook of social cognition. 2nd. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 1–40. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bargh JA. The automaticity of everyday life. In: Wyer RS, editor. The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition. Vol. 10. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1997. pp. 1–61. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bargh JA. The cognitive monster: The case against controllability of automatic stereotype effects. In: Chaiken S, Trope Y, editors. Dual process theories in social psychology. New York: Guilford; 1999. pp. 361–382. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bargh JA. Social psychological approaches to consciousness. In: Zelazo P, Moscovitch M, Thompson E, editors. The Cambridge handbook of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. pp. 553–567. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bargh JA, Bond R, Lombardi W, Tota ME. The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;50:869–878. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bargh JA, Chartrand TL. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist. 1999;54:462–479. [Google Scholar]
  10. Bargh JA, Chartrand TL. The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In: Reis HT, Judd CM, editors. Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2000. pp. 253–285. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bargh JA, Chen M, Burrows L. Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;71:230–244. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM. Environmental control of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In: Spaulding WD, editor. Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press; 1994. pp. 71–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Lee-Chai A, Barndollar K, Troetschel R. The automated will: Nonconscious activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:1014–1027. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Bargh JA, Gollwitzer PM, Oettingen G. Motivation. In: Fiske S, Gilbert D, Lindzey G, editors. Handbook of social psychology. 5th. Boston: McGraw-Hill; in press. [Google Scholar]
  15. Bargh JA, Huang JY. The selfish goal. In: Moskowitz G, Grant H, editors. The psychology of goals. New York: Guilford; 2009. pp. 127–150. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bargh JA, Morsella E. The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2008;3:73–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00064.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bargh JA, Morsella E. Unconscious behavioral guidance systems. In: Agnew CR, Carlston D, editors. Then a miracle occurs: Focusing on behavior in social psychological theory and research. New York: Oxford University Press; in press. [Google Scholar]
  18. Bargh JA, Thein RD. Individual construct accessibility, person memory, and the recall-judgment link: The case of information overload. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1985;49:1129–1146. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bargh JA, Williams LE. The nonconscious regulation of emotion. In: Gross J, editor. Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford; 2007. pp. 429–445. [Google Scholar]
  20. Baumeister RF, Heatherton TF, Tice DM. Losing control: How and why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  21. Baumeister RF, Muraven M, Tice DM. Ego depletion: A resource model of volition, self-regulation, and controlled processing. Social Cognition. 2000;18:130–150. [Google Scholar]
  22. Carver CS, Scheier MF. Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review. 1990;97:19–35. [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen S, Lee-Chai AY, Bargh JA. Relationship orientation as a moderator of the effects of social power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;80:173–187. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science. 2006;311:1005–1007. doi: 10.1126/science.1121629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dijksterhuis A, Chartrand TL, Aarts H. Effects of priming and perception on social behavior and goal pursuit. In: Bargh JA, editor. Social psychology and the unconscious: The automaticity of higher mental processes. Philadephia: Psychology Press; 2007. pp. 51–132. [Google Scholar]
  26. Dijksterhuis A, Nordgren LF. A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives in Psychological Science. 2006;1:95–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00007.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Dijksterhuis A, van Knippenberg A. The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:865–877. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Fitzsimons G, Bargh JA. Automatic self-regulation. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, editors. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford; 2004. pp. 151–170. [Google Scholar]
  29. Gardner WL, Gabriel S, Lee AY. “I” value freedom, but “We” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science. 1999;10:321–326. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gross JJ. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:224–237. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.1.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Gross JJ, John OP. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;85:348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hassin R. Nonconscious control and implicit working memory. In: Hassin R, Uleman J, Bargh J, editors. The new unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press; 2005. pp. 196–222. [Google Scholar]
  33. Hassin RR, Bargh JA, Zimmerman S. Automatic and flexible: The case of non-conscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.1.20. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Higgins ET. Knowledge activation: Acessibility, applicability, and salience. In: Higgins ET, Kruglanski AW, editors. Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New York: Guilford; 1996. pp. 133–168. [Google Scholar]
  35. Jacoby LL. A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language. 1991;30:513–541. [Google Scholar]
  36. Kay AC, Wheeler SC, Bargh JA, Ross L. Material priming: The influence of mundane physical objects on situational construal and competitive behavioral choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2004;95:83–96. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lassiter GD, Lindberg MJ, Gonzalez-Vallego C, Bellezza FS, Phillips ND. The deliberation-without-attention effect: Evidence for an artifactual interpretation. Psychological Science. 2009;20:671–675. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02347.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Magar ECE, Phillips LH, Hosie JA. Self-regulation and risk-taking. Personality and Individual Differences. 2008;45:153–159. [Google Scholar]
  39. Mandler G. Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley; 1975. [Google Scholar]
  40. Mauss IB, Cook CL, Gross JJ. Automatic emotion regulation during anger provocation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2007;43:698–711. [Google Scholar]
  41. Mauss IB, Evers C, Wilhelm FH, Gross JJ. How to bite your tongue without blowing your top: Implicit evaluation of emotion regulation predicts affective responding to anger provocation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32:389–402. doi: 10.1177/0146167205283841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Min Y, Chung S, Min B. Physiological evaluation on emotional change induced by imagination. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 2005;30:137–150. doi: 10.1007/s10484-005-4310-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Mischel W, Ayduk O. Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of delay of gratification. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, editors. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford; 2004. pp. 99–129. [Google Scholar]
  44. Posner MI, Snyder CRR. Attention and cognitive control. In: Solso R, editor. Information processing and cognition: The Loyola symposium. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1975. pp. 55–85. [Google Scholar]
  45. Richards JM, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and memory: The cognitive costs of keeping one's cool. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:410–424. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.3.410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shah JY, Kruglanski AW. When opportunity knocks: Bottom-up priming of goals by means and its effects on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:1109–1122. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Shiffrin RM, Schneider W. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review. 1977;84:127–190. [Google Scholar]
  48. Simon H. Motivational and emotional controls of cognition. Psychological Review. 1967;74:29–39. doi: 10.1037/h0024127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Spencer SJ, Fein S, Wolfe CT, Dunn MA. Automatic activation of stereotypes: The role of self-image threat. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1998;24:1139–1152. [Google Scholar]
  50. Srull TK, Wyer RS. The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1979;37:1660–1672. [Google Scholar]
  51. Vohs KD, Baumeister RF. Understanding self-regulation: An introduction. In: Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, editors. Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications. New York: Guilford; 2004. pp. 1–12. [Google Scholar]
  52. Wegner DM, Ansfield M, Pilloff D. The putt and the pendulum: Ironic effects of the mental control of action. Psychological Science. 1998;9:196–199. [Google Scholar]
  53. Wood W, Neal DT. A new look at habits and the habit-goal interface. Psychological Review. 2007;114:843–863. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.4.843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES