Abstract
Perception relies on the response of populations of neurons in sensory cortex. How the response profile of a neuronal population gives rise to perception and perceptual discrimination has been conceptualized in various ways. Here we suggest that neuronal population responses represent information about our environment explicitly as Fisher information (FI), which is a local measure of the variance estimate of the sensory input. We show how this sensory information can be read out and combined to infer from the available information profile which stimulus value is perceived during a fine discrimination task. In particular, we propose that the perceived stimulus corresponds to the stimulus value that leads to the same information for each of the alternative directions, and compare the model prediction to standard models considered in the literature (population vector, maximum likelihood, maximum-a-posteriori Bayesian inference). The models are applied to human performance in a motion discrimination task that induces perceptual misjudgements of a target direction of motion by task irrelevant motion in the spatial surround of the target stimulus (motion repulsion). By using the neurophysiological insight that surround motion suppresses neuronal responses to the target motion in the center, all models predicted the pattern of perceptual misjudgements. The variation of discrimination thresholds (error on the perceived value) was also explained through the changes of the total FI content with varying surround motion directions. The proposed FI decoding scheme incorporates recent neurophysiological evidence from macaque visual cortex showing that perceptual decisions do not rely on the most active neurons, but rather on the most informative neuronal responses. We statistically compare the prediction capability of the FI decoding approach and the standard decoding models. Notably, all models reproduced the variation of the perceived stimulus values for different surrounds, but with different neuronal tuning characteristics underlying perception. Compared to the FI approach the prediction power of the standard models was based on neurons with far wider tuning width and stronger surround suppression. Our study demonstrates that perceptual misjudgements can be based on neuronal populations encoding explicitly the available sensory information, and provides testable neurophysiological predictions on neuronal tuning characteristics underlying human perceptual decisions.
Keywords: Fisher Information, Computational model, Psychophysics, Discrimination, Motion repulsion, Bayes, Maximum likelihood
1 Introduction
Human perceptual skills rely on the activity of thousands of neurons within sensory cortex. The activity of each of these neurons contributes to the perception of sensory information in our environment. Exactly how the pattern of neuronal activities are combined to inform perceptual decisions is one of the central problems in theoretical and system neuroscience (Dayan and Abbott 2001). Solutions to this problem generally need to address (1) how sensory information is encoded in the pattern of activated neurons, and (2) how this information is read out to ultimately inform a perceptual decision. Here we propose a new theoretical framework providing insights into both aspects of neuronal decoding and apply it to perceptual performance in a fine discrimination task.
Neurons convey information about sensory inputs by producing graded responses to a continuously varying stimulus feature such as the direction of visual motion. The response profile of a neuron is typically characterized by its tuning function. For visual motion processing, tuning functions are well described by a bell-shaped Gaussian profile indicating that a motion sensitive neuron responds with a peak amplitude only for a narrow range of motion directions, and that response strength levels off for directions of motion offset from the preferred direction (Britten 2003; Born and Bradley 2005). The peak region and width parameter provide a complete description of neuron’s response to different directions of motion.
Based on these tuning characteristics, various theoretical approaches have been proposed to decode neuronal population activity in order to predict the sensory stimulus evoking particular neuronal responses and to infer the perceptual decision about the input (Fig. 1). However, it is far from clear how the neuronal response characteristics are used by the brain to extract the perceptually relevant information. Most commonly used approaches rely on vector averaging of neuronal responses, maximum-likelihood or Bayesian probability estimation (Paradiso 1988; Vogels 1990; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Kim and Wilson 1997; Zemel et al. 1998; Pouget et al. 2000; Jazayeri and Movshon 2006). In population vector models the relevant sensory information is derived directly from the neuronal firing rate, which is considered as the “weight” of a neuron in a global population vector of firing rates (Vogels 1990; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993). In contrast, likelihood based models rely on the calculation of the probability that a particular response is observed in response to an input stimulus and Bayesian approaches compute the probability that an input was presented given the observed response. Individual neurons contribute by means of their probability function of observing a neuronal response to a given stimulus (Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Zemel et al. 1998; Pouget et al. 2000; Jazayeri and Movshon 2006).
However, it is also possible to calculate information conveyed by a neuronal population about the sensory input explicitly as an information profile representing directly the statistical knowledge of the input. It can be represented through a measure known as Fisher information (FI) (Fisher 1925; Paradiso 1988; Kass et al. 2005) that is used for arriving at an optimal estimate of available sensory information. The interest of using the FI measure is that its definition is related to the underlying variance of the estimate (Fisher 1925; Paradiso 1988) and therefore to the error on the predicted stimulus (the discrimination threshold, e.g. Paradiso 1988). In other words, extracting FI for a given neuronal system provides a representation of the minimum variance estimate within the neuronal population. The FI encoded by neurons is directly related to their tuning functions. The most informative part of the tuning function for estimating the sensory stimulus is a region offset from the peak, i.e. the neurons most sensitive to the stimulus are not the ones with the highest firing rate (Paradiso 1988; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Dayan and Abbott 2001). However, FI definition implies that it should allow estimating the identity of the sensory stimulus that has evoked a neuronal population response. In human perceptual terms, decoding of FI from the neuronal population should allow to predict which stimulus is actually perceived.
In the following we calculate information profiles of theoretical neuronal population activity during a motion discrimination task. We show that actual perceptual decisions directly correspond to the stimulus value that leads to the same information content for the different alternative motion directions, which need to be discriminated. The proposed model is based on the assumptions (1) that behaviorally relevant information in neuronal population responses is represented as FI and (2) that decoding this information can be achieved by a simple equalization procedure. The model is used to predict perceived stimulus values and it is compared to the prediction of standard decoding methods considered in the literature (population vector average, maximum-likelihood and maximum a posteriori inferences). All models provide testable predictions of the neuronal tuning parameters underlying the perceptual performance. Additionally, the variation of discrimination sensitivity is also explained through the modulation of the total Fisher information content (e.g. Paradiso 1988).
We apply the models to perceptual performance during motion discrimination of a foveally presented target stimulus surrounded by task irrelevant motion of varying directions across trials (Fig. 1). The paradigm is known to give rise to motion repulsion reflecting a systematic overestimation of the angular difference of two motion directions (Marshak and Sekuler 1979; Kim and Wilson 1997). In particular, subjects misperceive the physical direction of the target motion when the task irrelevant surround moves at directions around 30-60° away from the target direction (Kim and Wilson 1997). Motion repulsion is often explained by inhibitory interactions between motion-tuned neurons (Marshak and Sekuler 1979; Allman et al. 1985; Born and Bradley 2005), and in particular to neurons in area MT/V5 tuned to visual unidirectional motion (Britten 2003; Born and Bradley 2005). In the following we utilize these tuning properties to propose a novel approach to decode the information content of neuronal population responses during a motion repulsion paradigm, and we compare its prediction power to standard decoding schemes.
2 Modeling human perception
In the following we describe the theoretical background of the various decoding methods applied to the psychophysical results. We first provide in detail the rationale underlying our novel approach to decode perceptual decisions from neuronal information profiles. Secondly, we summarize the models based on population vector average, maximum-likelihood and Bayesian maximum-a-posteriori inference for performing the decision process. We use the term standard models for these last three models because they are commonly considered in the literature and we demonstrate that in the experimental paradigm considered here they give rise to identical estimates for the value of the stimulus despite different theoretical origins.
2.1 Theoretical background
All decoding models are based on theoretical and functional hypotheses. Here, we assume that the decision process is based on the response of those neurons tuned to the main stimulus feature—here the direction of motion, and these neuronal responses are used to decode the visual input with a given theoretical decoding scheme. Furthermore, we assume a continuous population of direction of motion sensitive neurons based on the insights from cortical area MT/V5 (see Fig. 2a). This visual area contains a hypercolumn representation of motion direction at each location of the visual field and the neuronal activity is modulated by surround-to-center spatial interactions (Britten 2003; Born and Bradley 2005). Importantly, the neurons’ response to various directions of visual motion follows a characteristic unimodal bell-shaped tuning function that is well represented by a Gaussian function written as:
1 |
. In this equation r i (θ 0) is the mean firing rate of the neurons with preferred direction of motion θ i in response to a motion direction θ 0, σ i is the standard deviation of the tuning curve, and A i is the mean maximum firing rate of the neurons tuned to θ i . Therefore, for a single presented stimulus the hypercolumn population response follows the same Gaussian function (Fig. 2b, thick solid line) (Pouget et al. 2000). To simplify the subsequent calculations, we assumed that all neurons could be modelled by a characteristic tuning curve with amplitude A 0 and tuning width σ 0 and that the responses of neurons follow an independent Poisson process (cf. Dayan and Abbott 2001; Jin et al. 2005).
We were particularly interested in the modulation of perceived motion directions of the center stimulus in a center surround stimulus configuration with the surround being task-irrelevant (Fig. 1a).Motion in the surround does not activate directly the population of neurons within the directional hypercolumn responding to the central target, but indirectly modulates the strength of the neuronal response (Allman et al. 1985; Born and Bradley 2005). According to neurophysiological evidence (Allman et al. 1985), the surround motion (subscript i 0) is modelled to reduce the maximal response amplitude of neurons responding to the target motion. The response modulation varies as a function of the difference between the surround motion directions () and the neuron’s preferred direction (θ i ), with the surround assumed to have a multiplicative effect on neuronal responses to the target motion when they have similar preferred directions (no effect from opponent motion directions is considered, see Appendix A). We capture this amplitude modulation effect with a Gaussian function centred at and a standard deviation , written as:
2 |
, with corresponding to the maximum amplitude inhibition. Figure 2c illustrates an amplitude suppressive effect of a second irrelevant motion on the population tuning curves.
In deriving most models, we need to define the probability distribution P(R|θ 0) of observing a pattern of activity R = (x 1, x 2 … x n) in the hypercolumn population given the presence of the stimulus θ 0. Based on the independent Poisson process for neuronal firing rates and assuming a reading time T of the neuronal response giving individual firing rates x i in the trial, the probability can be written as (e.g. Dayan and Abbott 2001):
3 |
4 |
. This equation represents the likelihood function for observing a population response R for the presented stimulus θ 0 with known mean tuning characteristics r i (θ 0) (here x i T represents the number of spikes fired by neuron i during the reading time of the neuronal population).
2.2 Fisher information decoding method
To calculate information profiles of a theoretical neuronal population we derive Fisher information (FI) across the whole neuronal population (as in Paradiso 1988; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Dayan and Abbott 2001) (cf. Fig. 2). When the neuronal population is activated by a single stimulus with motion in one direction (Fig. 2b), the total FI represented by the neuronal population response is reflected in the mean curvature of the log-likelihood function (Fisher 1925; Paradiso 1988; Dayan and Abbott 2001; Kass et al. 2005). It is defined as:
5 |
. For neuronal responses based on Poisson-like noise processes, it can be shown that the FI about a single presented stimulus conveyed by neurons having the same preferred stimulus θ i is proportional to the square of the tuning curve derivative divided by the tuning curve itself (Paradiso 1988; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Dayan and Abbott 2001):
6 |
. The above total FI (Eq. 5) is the sum of the FI across the neuronal population with different preferred values.
Neuronal tuning functions for the direction of motion in area MT/V5 are known to followa Gaussian function (Eq. 1). Thus, the mean FI carried by the neurons with a preferred direction θ i about the stimulus θ 0 is:
7 |
. The neurons that provide the most FI about a given direction of motion are thosewith preferred directions at from the stimulus direction, i.e. where small changes of motion direction results in the strongest differences in neuronal response compared to the underlying variability of the firing rate (see Fig. 2b) (Dayan and Abbott 2001).
We were particularly interested in the modulation of neuronal information profiles induced by a second task-irrelevant motion direction in the spatial surround of the target motion (cf. Fig. 1a). Motion in the surround does not activate directly the population of neurons within the directional hypercolumn responding to the central target, but indirectly modulates the strength of the neuronal population response (Allman et al. 1985; Born and Bradley 2005). As a consequence, this spatial segregation leads to a single FI term for the hypercolumn model. Figure 2c–d illustrate the suppressive effect of a second irrelevant motion on the population tuning curves together with the modulation of the population response and its information content.
We model the subjectively estimated perceived motion direction as an information equalization process. It is obtained by assuming that human subjects perceive the stimulus value that lead to the same amount of information present for either alternative. It can be written in different forms (ratio, squared or absolute value of difference), and here we use the mathematical expression:
8 |
9 |
G(θ) represents the difference in information content conveyed by the neuronal populations “left” and “right” of the reference value θ.
For example, when a single direction of motion is presented to the neuronal hypercolumn population without a surround motion (Fig. 2b), it is clear that the amount of information for directions of motion leftward and rightward from the exact physical direction of motion is equal (cf. dark and light grey areas in Fig. 2b). When a second stimulus is presented in the surround and changes the response strength of a part of the neuronal population (Fig. 2c), the equalization point is moved further away from the surround direction (white vertical line in Fig. 2d).
The FI computation can be implemented by postsynaptic neurons in a higher-level neuronal population coding for direction of motion. For example, calculation of the individual FI terms require neurons to compute the square of the tuning curve derivative divided by the tuning curve (see Eq. 6). In neuronal terms it involves basic computations: subtracting the responses of neurons with adjacent stimulus preference and squaring ((r i+1 − r i−1)2)), and dividing the outcome by the neuronal response (r i). These calculations could easily be implemented in dendritic trees equivalent to two-layer neural networks (London and Häusser 2005). As a final step, the read-out of this FI population representation is performed through Eq. 9.
2.3 Standard models of decoding
2.3.1 Population vector average
The population vector average model assumes that the final percept θ perc is obtained by performing a weighted-average across firing rates of neurons’ with preferred direction θ i (e.g. Dayan and Abbott 2001). It is written as:
10 |
2.3.2 Maximum-Likelihood estimate
Themaximum likelihood estimate of the input is obtained by computing the log-likelihood function L(θ) = ln(P(R|θ)) for the observed firing pattern R in the neuronal population encoding the direction of motion. The likelihood function for different motion directions θ can be represented in a higher level neuronal population (e.g. Jazayeri and Movshon 2006) and therefore could allow to extract the stimulus value that maximizes this function, i.e. the most probable input value. The final perceived value is mathematically tractable, and gives:
11 |
2.3.3 Maximum-a-posteriori estimate
The Bayes rule relates the conditional probability that a stimulus was presented given the neuronal response to the global probabilities of neuronal responses and stimulus presentations and their conditional relation. It is written as:
12 |
where P(θ|R) is the conditional probability that stimulus θ was presented given the observed activity pattern R across the neuronal population, P(R|θ) is the conditional probability of observing a pattern of activity R given the presence of stimulus θ, and P(θ) and P(R) are respectively the probabilities of stimulus θ being presented and observing response R (e.g. Chap. 3, “Neural decoding”, in Dayan and Abbott 2001).
By using the theoretical hypotheses (Sect. 2.1) plus the assumption that the probability of a stimulus θ being presented is unimodal and can be represented with a Gaussian probability density function centred on θ prior and width σ prior, one obtains the maximum-a-posteriori estimate θ MAP as (cf. Dayan and Abbott 2001):
13 |
If the prior distribution is centred on the MAP estimate, i.e. θ MAP = θ prior , which is equivalent to saying that the prior distribution of the stimulus is centred on the mean perceived value by the subject, it can be easily shown that Eq. 13 becomes:
14 |
This demonstrates an interesting result for the condition described above, that the maximum-a-posteriori estimate is independent of the prior width. In Sect. 4, we show that the staircase psychophysical method used in our experimental design entails exactly this property, that is, the global a priori probability distribution of the target direction of motion is centred on the mean perceived value by the subject. Therefore, a direct comparison of the MAP estimate to the human perceptual data is possible.
2.3.4 Decision process
All these standard decoding processes have the same mathematical description under different theoretical assumptions. The PV model is based on the assumption that each neuron codes its preferred direction of motion and contributes to the perceived value with a weight equal to its firing rate. The ML estimate is based on the assumption that a higher-level neuronal population computes the different probabilities of observing the activity profile in MT/V5 for different possible input values and the decision process simply reads-out the stimulus value with the highest probability (e.g. Jazayeri and Movshon 2006). The MAP estimate could be implemented by assuming that a higher-level neuronal population calculates the a posteriori distribution based on knowledge of both stimulus distribution across the experiment and firing rate distribution of the neuronal population (e.g. Zemel et al. 1998).
As demonstrated in the model derivations these three decision processes provide the same prediction of the stimulus value, and therefore the exact modulation of the perceived value follows the same behaviour in the three cases. When a single stimulus is presented to the hypercolumn population without a surround, the firing rate weighted-average of the preferred directions is representing the stimulus estimate. It is the exact presented stimulus value (Fig. 2b, peak of the Gaussian population response). When a second stimulus is presented in the surround and changes the response amplitude of a part of the neuronal population, the estimate is moved further away from the surround direction, consistent with the psychophysically observed repulsion effect (Fig. 2d, black vertical line).
2.4 Discrimination thresholds: error on the perceived value of a stimulus
In addition to the measured perceived direction of motion we can infer the discrimination threshold, i.e. the amount of direction change required for a subject to report the correct direction difference with respect to his/her perceived reference (e.g. Paradiso 1988; Dayan and Abbott 2001). We can estimate a lower bound on the discrimination thresholds through the total Fisher information representing the best decoding error one can achieve with an unbiased decoding algorithm. The squared discrimination threshold is inversely proportional to the total FI present in the neuronal population, and it is written as (Cramer-Rao bound, cf. Dayan and Abbott 2001):
15 |
In the described center-surround paradigm, the mathematical computations for the total FI contained in the neuronal population activity are practicable (integral over θ i of Eq. 7), and lead to the following expression:
16 |
by replacing:
17 |
18 |
Equation 16 represents the total FI present in the neuronal hypercolumn population about the reference direction θ 0 given the presence of the modulatory surround direction . Here, I 0F is the FI contained in the total population activity when no surround direction of motion is present (cf. Dayan and Abbott 2001). Thus, it is also possible to predict the exact variation of the discrimination thresholds for the central target motion with different surround motion directions.
2.5 Predictions of human perceptual parameters modulation
The described decision processes predict a systematic variation of the perceived upward reference direction of motion as a function of the direction of motion in the spatial surround (Fig. 3a). It is important to note that for a given theoretical population characteristics (σ 0, and fixed) the repulsion amplitude of the models are substantially different (see also Fig. 2d). This is due to the high sensitivity of the FI decoding method to both tuning curve parameters: width and amplitude of firing rate, whereas the Standard models need strong amplitude modulation for creating a repulsion effect. As a consequence, for predicting similar repulsion amplitudes the models need very different population characteristics. The modulation of the total FI content predicts variation of the discrimination threshold of the target stimulus as a function of surround motion direction (Fig. 3b). We assumed that k = 1, i.e. σ 0 = and optimal decoding for the total variance estimate (these assumptions are taken throughout the paper).
3 Psychophysics of center-surround motion discrimination
3.1 Methods
3.1.1 Subjects
We collected data from 20 subjects (including the first author) for fitting and extracting model parameters. Subjects had normal, or corrected to normal, vision and gave written consent for participating in the experiment (mean age: 27.6 ± 5.6 years).
3.1.2 Apparatus and stimuli
The experiment was conducted on a 21-inch CRT monitor at a refresh rate of 85 Hz and a resolution of 40 pixels per degree of visual angle, controlled by a MacIntosh G4 computer. Stimuli were random dot patterns (RDP) presented at the center of a white screen (luminance: 80.2 cd/m2). RDPs contained 8 dots per square degree with each dot extending 4 pixels and set to the lowest luminance resulting in a contrast of 22.6 cd/m2 (the contrast of an RDP is defined as ; the sum is over all dots present in the RDP; p i is the proportion of surface in the RDP of dot i; L i is its luminance; L 0 is the luminance of the background). Dots moved through a circular or annular aperture at a speed of 8°/s in an unidirectional translational motion for the center stimulus and the surround in the test condition. For the noise-control condition, the surround contained dots moving at 8°/s in random directions, with each dot being assigned a fixed direction of motion until it disappeared from the screen. Upward motion was defined as zero degree with “leftward” motion directions as negative values. The target RDP had a radius of 1.5°. In the target-alone control condition, its allowed directions of motion were in the ±30° range sampled in one degree steps. In the other two experimental conditions the range of the target RDP directions was ±29° sampled every 2°, and the target RDP was surrounded by an annular aperture (inner/outer radius: 1.5°/4.5°) (see Fig. 1a). For the test condition, the surround motion was 100% coherent in one of the 36 directions of motion spanning the whole 360°. The variation allowed us to test the influence of different angles between target and surround motion on the perceived upward reference direction of the central target RDP, i.e. the motion repulsion effect.
3.1.3 Procedure
Subjects were seated in a dimly lit room 57 cm in front of the monitor. A chin rest was used to stabilize the head. They were instructed to fixate a small dark square centered on the screen and to attend the central target stimulus, the surround being task irrelevant. They started each trial by pressing the space bar, and 353ms after the offset of the fixation square the stimulus was presented for 117ms (10 frames) at the center of the screen. Subjects had to report if the direction of motion of the small central RDP (the target) was to the left or right relative to his/her internal reference direction of upward motion by pressing either the “4” or “6” keys on the computer keyboard.
Two control and one test condition were presented to each subject. In the control conditions the target stimulus was presented either alone or with random motion in the surround. Each of these two conditions had a total of 120 trials. In the test condition, the target was shown together with the surround containing 100% coherent motion. The test condition was split in two experimental blocks, one containing 18 directions of motion of the surround (from −170 to +170° with steps of 20°), the second the remaining directions of motion (from −160 to +180° with steps of 20°). The test design contained a small number of total presentations of each annular stimulus direction (30 times) combined with a short presentation of the stimuli to the subject at each trial (117 ms).
For measuring the subject’s response curve to the direction of motion of the central RDP, a weighted up-down staircase procedure for stimulus presentation was used (Kaernbach 1991). The theoretical convergence hit rate of a given staircase algorithm was either 75 or 25%, corresponding to step sizes Up-Down along the psychometric function of 3/1 and 1/3 respectively. For example, in the test condition using the convergence point of 75%, the step was 3×2° to the left when “right” responses were given, and 1×2° to the right when a “left” response was present. In the test and noisecontrol conditions, each staircase run started at ±21° deviation from zero degrees upward motion, at the opposite side of the convergence point when compared to the midpoint. Such a staircase run for a 75% convergence point can be visualized in Fig. 4a where the run starts at +21° and the algorithm converges rapidly to the stochastic region. In addition, the convergence points of 25 and 75% were equally distributed through the different staircases within a given experimental block in order to avoid biases due to unequal number of “left” and “right” answers. In the control conditions, four independent staircase algorithms were interleaved in order to avoid that subjects learned the experimental procedure (Cornsweet 1962). Within one test block, the eighteen staircases, corresponding respectively to each motion direction of the surround, were presented in a pseudo-random order on a trial-by-trial basis (Bonnet 1986). This allowed avoiding any building-up of information (learning or adaptation) due to the shortness of the presentation (117 ms), the randomness of surround directions from trial-to-trial, and the small number of total trials (30) per surround direction. This global design prevented potential problems coming from asymmetric sampling of motion directions (Rauber and Treue 1999), adaptation effects, eye tracking (Rauber and Treue 1999), or stimulus statistics (Mahani et al. 2005) as explanation of motion repulsion.
3.1.4 Raw data extraction
We extracted the staircase runs and obtained the response curves in the control conditions, and for each surround motion direction in the test condition. The response curve represents the number of “left” answers of the subject as a function of each motion direction of the target RDP. Figure 4a,b presents a staircase run and the pooled “left” responses at each target direction of motion. Using the maximum likelihood method and the simplex algorithm (Press et al. 1997), each response curve was fitted with a logistic model of the form:
19 |
, where x is the direction of motion of the target RDP, p(x) is the corresponding hit rate, a corresponds to the midpoint of the function and b is related to its steepness. Thus, a reflects the internal reference direction of upward motion, i.e. the direction for which subjects are equally likely to give a “left” or “right” response. Parameter b allows to compute the discrimination threshold defined as σ exp = x p=0.84 − x p=0.50 = (1/b)log(21/4). It represents the amount of direction deviation in degrees that allows the subject to discriminate between the target direction relative to his/her internal zero degrees upward reference in 84% of the trials (for an example, see Fig. 4a,b). By analyzing the raw results, in one case out of the 720 experimental values (36 surround directions × 20 subjects) the fitted psychometric function was flat with correspondingly very high discrimination threshold (>100°). This psychometric function was not included in the data analysis.
Previous studies have demonstrated that the slope, and thus the discrimination threshold, is a biased and very noisy parameter (Leek et al. 1992; Vågerö and Sundberg 1999; Kaernbach 2001). Therefore, we conducted Mont Carlo simulations using the staircase procedure described above for assessing the biases and variability of both parameters in the experimental design. As reported previously, we observed that the slope parameter b has a strong bias and variability. Additionally, the simulations showed that the midpoint a has a small bias corresponding to less than5%of its total variance (see Sect. 4).
3.2 Results
In the experiments, subjects had to judge whether the direction of motion of a foveally presented stimulus deviated either rightward or leftward from upward motion (defined to be 0° and rightward deviations as positive) (see Fig. 1). Varying the direction of a task irrelevant motion in the spatial surround induced changes in the perceived upward motion direction in all 20 subjects (motion repulsion, cf. Figs. 4 and 5), complementing previous reports (Kim and Wilson 1997; Tzvetanov et al. 2006). Figure 5a shows the between-subjects mean value of the perceived vertical reference direction of motion across different surround motion directions. The figure plots the direction of motion of the center target that the subjects reported to perceive as moving upward. The physical direction of motion perceived as moving upward is closer to the surround motion direction, thus demonstrating the repulsion effect. Subjects misperceive the upward direction particularly for surround directions around ±30–60° from the true zero degrees upward direction. Interestingly, the average results showed a small bias induced by surround stimuli moving at ±130–150°, i.e. opposite to the direction that caused the strongest motion repulsion. This influence of motion opponency was evident in only a subset of the subjects and is integrated into the model in more detail in Appendix A.
Surround motion also modulated the threshold for discriminating the target motion direction (Fig. 5b) with elevated thresholds for surround directions around ±40–50° (test thresholds normalized to the noise-control condition; noise-control mean discrimination threshold of 2.7 ± 0.2°, SEM; n = 20). This is in the range of themaximum repulsion effect (Pearson correlation coefficient computed between the absolute value of perceived upward motion and discrimination thresholds: r = 0.29, p < 0.001).
4 Model fit to the data
4.1 Methods
4.1.1 Perceived value
The logistic fits to the psychophysical results provided the perceived vertical upward reference direction of motion and the threshold in the a and b parameters respectively. After subtracting the individual mean of the data for adjusting to zero, the variation of the a parameter with different modulatory directions of motion in the surround can be directly compared to the models.
The FI and standard models predictions were fit to the data by minimizing the mean squared error between the theoretically predicted perceived upward motion direction by the model and the physical true 0° upward direction (using the simplex algorithm, cf. Press et al. 1997). For a given set of theoretical model parameters, the perceived upward direction predicted by the model is obtained by the following procedure. The two values of (1) the measured physical direction of motion reported as upward (parameter a) and (2) the corresponding surround direction (θ surr) are introduced in Eqs. 1–2 and then the neuronal activity and information profiles computed. Models predictions were extracted through the standard models estimate and the equalization point of the FI profile (computing the cumulative of the population information profile and extracting the stimulus value giving half of the total information content). These calculations were performed by discretizing the theoretical hypercolumn population in steps of 0.01° (this method is computationally intensive). Each model has 2 parameters to be adjusted: σ 0, . Interestingly, we found that some individual subjects’ data showed clear attraction effects for opposite directions of motion that was only marginally evident in the group average. To account for this finding we extended the model to incorporate motion opponency, described in detail in Appendix A.
For fitting the MAP model to the data, we first tested for the assumption that the prior distribution of the target stimulus, which is the experimental distribution of the presented target motion direction, is centered on the perceived value. This condition needs to be met in order to have the MAP estimate independent of the target distribution width and therefore allowing a simple Bayesian prediction of human perceived value. Figure 4c presents for all trials and across subjects (21,600 trials) the distribution of presented target directions of motion. The mean is −0.073 ± 0.054 (SE, n = 21, 600) showing that the distribution of presented target directions is well centred on the real vertical direction across all trials and subjects, as expected due to the staircase algorithms used in the experiment. Therefore, the mathematical conditions for applying the MAP estimate (Eq. 14) are met.
4.1.2 Discrimination threshold
We also predicted changes in motion discrimination thresholds as a function of the direction of motion in the surround. We computed the ratio of thresholds in the test condition to those in the control condition. According to Eqs. 15 and 16 and using σ 0exp = 1/I 0F (the discrimination threshold of the control condition), we can express the above mentioned ratio as:
20 |
with B 0 reflecting a global constant shift of the curve. This model prediction was applied on the average normalized thresholds across subjects, normalized to the noise-control condition, and fit with the Levenberg-Marquardt algorithm (Press et al. 1997). Additionally, we considered optimal information extraction, i.e. the smallest attainable discrimination threshold by assuming equality of left and right sides in Eq. 15 and no bias of the curve. Equation 20 provides three parameter estimates: B 0, , σ 0. Using the second target-alone condition for normalization of threshold estimates gave nearly identical results (not shown).
4.1.3 Statistical tests
The between-subject mean repulsion data were compared to the mean fitted model with a standard χ 2 test for how good a model predicts the data points. In an initial fit of the mean repulsion curve and Discrimination thresholds variation obtained across subjects, wefound a statistical difference between models and data (see Appendix B). Therefore, we decided to restrict all model fits and tests to 32 out of the 36 data points where the repulsion effect is due to inhibitory interactions and not affected by yet unknown supplementary interactions (discarding surround angular deviations of ±10° and ±20°, see Fig. 5, grey data points).
It should be noted that any influence from the small bias of the midpoint estimate mentioned in Sect. 3 (“Raw data extraction”) on the main statistical outcome would decrease the match between the model and data. The bias is always in a direction away from the midpoint and toward the theoretical convergence point. As a consequence of the fixed experimental design, with half points slightly biased toward the 25% and half toward the 75% convergence points, the χ2 test would show less statistically reliable model prediction.
The discrimination threshold fit was assessed with an F-test between global mean residual variance of the data and model residual variance. This test allows estimating if the model predicts the data better than the global data mean.
4.2 Results
We evaluated the outlined models by fitting them to the psychophysical data. The resulting fits of the data pooled across subjects are shown in Fig. 5a (FI in black solid line; standard models in grey solid line) and reveals that the models captured the observed behavioural motion repulsion across varying surround motion directions (FI: χ 2 = 28.0, df = 30, p > 0.05; standard models: χ 2 = 20.2, df = 30, p > 0.05). The FI model parameters underlying the significant fit corresponded to an underlying tuning width of σ = 18.2° and surround modulation of the response strength to the target motion of 2.4% ( = 0.024). The parameter of the standard models showed σ = 30.1° of tuning width and a surround modulation of the response strength to the target motion of 50% ( = 0.50). Comparing the capability to predict the data by the FI and by the standard models showed no statistically different predictions (F(30,30)=1.38, p > 0.05).
In addition to changes in the perceived upward motion we also fit the variation of the discrimination threshold as a function of the surround motion direction. Despite the inherent variability of discrimination thresholds the model reproduces the basic shape of the surround influence on human sensitivity to motion of the center stimulus (Fig. 5b; comparison to the global data mean: F(31, 29) = 2.48, p < 0.01; parameter estimates: σ = 25.3, = 1.34, B 0 = 0.14).
5 Discussion
This study delineated a novel approach to decode sensory information from neuronal population responses based on the extraction of local Fisher information (FI) representing the underlying variance estimate of the sensory input. We applied the model to a theoretical hypercolumn population of area MT/V5 tuned to direction of motion, and predicted for a motion discrimination paradigm the global perceptual misjudgements of the center motion direction when flanked with surround motion. For comparison, we applied more commonly used standard models (Population vector average, maximum-likelihood, and maximum-a-posteriori (Bayesian) inference) to the same data and found that these models showed similar statistical prediction power for the global perceived direction of motion. However, the standard approaches and the FI decoding algorithm provided clearly divergent estimates of the tuning parameters of the neuronal population that could underlie the psychophysically observed effect. In addition, our approach demonstrated that the same surround modulation of the population response also gave rise to a systematic change in the sensitivity to motion direction.
Our psychophysical results revealed a systematic change of the perceived motion direction and discrimination thresholds of a target stimulus due to the presence of a task irrelevant motion signal in the spatial surround. Subjects misperceived the vertical upward motion maximally by on average 6° for surround direction of motion deviating by about ±40° from the upward reference direction. The magnitude and direction of this effect agrees with previous reports of motion repulsion (Hiris and Blake 1996; Kim and Wilson 1997; Rauber and Treue 1999; Tzvetanov et al. 2006). Moreover, the average thresholds of human subjects for discriminating a target motion varied in accordance with the repulsion effect. Elevations of discrimination thresholds were strongest when the angle between center and surround motion was ±40–50° (similar to results of a previous study which used an adaptation paradigm, Hol and Treue 2001).
The psychophysically observed effects could be predicted by decoding the neuronal population response as information profile. The motion direction that was subjectively perceived was calculated as the value that lead to the same information content for either behavioural alternative (rightward vs. leftward direction of motion) across the whole neuronal population. This finding together with the representation of Fisher information within the neuronal population (Paradiso 1988; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Dayan and Abbott 2001) suggests that human responses can be conceived of as reading information explicitly from the neuronal response profiles. This finding extends the more standard views on neuronal decoding, which also accounted for the observed effects. However, FI and standard models clearly differed with respect to the neurophysiological population parameters that underlie motion discrimination in the center-surround configuration: compared to the standard models the FI model predicted far narrower neuronal tuning width (about 40% smaller) and required only a fraction of the surround amplitude modulation to account for the perceptual results.
The discrimination threshold variations with surround motion directions were well predicted based on modulation of the total FI content. This finding parallels an earlier report from the orientation domain (Paradiso 1988) that demonstrated how the experimental modulation of orientation sensitivity to a target line by a superimposed irrelevant line segment (Westheimer et al. 1976) could be well predicted by changes in the total FI in a theoretical neuronal population tuned to orientation. We applied these insights to the center-surround configuration in a fine motion discrimination task that allowed simplified mathematical computations.
The combined computational modelling of both human perceptual parameters, that is, the exact perceived value as well as the sensitivity to this value, demonstrates that a theoretical framework is available for fully predicting human perceptual decisions. Regarding the prediction of perceived stimulus value, it is still contentious how decoding schemes could be implemented in the brain (for example, see the different propositions in Vogels 1990; Seung and Sompolinsky 1993; Pouget et al. 2000; Jazayeri and Movshon 2006). The proposed Fisher information decoding could be implemented in a higher-level neuronal population similar to maximum- likelihood inference, but by reading-out the Fisher information represented in the neuronal population instead of probability distributions. It should be noted that the FI model and the various standard models share common simplifying neurophysiological assumptions allowing tractable mathematical derivations: identical Gaussian tuning curve characteristics across neurons (amplitude and width), firing rate variability following independent Poisson noise, a theoretical hypercolumn arrangement of neuronal tuning. A challenging task for future research is to consider the biological complexity in more detail and explore the influence of various noise functions or correlation structures on decoding performance of the various models (Vogels 1990; Zemel et al. 1998; Abbott and Dayan 1999; Pouget et al. 2000).
The proposed FI model is built around two essential ingredients that allowed us to bridge the gap from neuronal encoding of sensory information to exact predictions of human perceptual performance. First, the model assumes that sensory responses are decoded in the theoretically optimal way by extracting the FI contained in the population of motion-tuned neurons in area MT/V5. Second, we propose in the context of a two-alternative forced choice discrimination task, that the subjective decision about the perceived stimulus is achieved by finding the stimulus value that lead to equal FI available for either alternative of the discrimination task (“leftward” versus “rightward” from the upward direction of motion).
The described mechanism critically depends on the insight that maximal information about a stimulus is conveyed by neurons with tuning preferences offset from the stimulus value (Regan and Beverley 1985; Hol and Treue 2001). In particular, for neurons in macaque area MT/V5 with known tuning width for the direction of motion of about 42–51° (Britten 2003), the most “Fisher” informative neurons are those with tuning preferences 60–72° away from the presented motion (at ). Importantly, these FI theoretical values gain strong support from neuronal recording in macaque area MT/V5 (Purushothaman and Bradley 2005). Using a fine motion discrimination task this study revealed that most information about discrimination performance are obtained from neurons tuned to directions that were around 60–70° away from the task direction, which exactly corresponds to the prediction of the FI model. These neurophysiological findings along with the FI model suggest that the peaks of misperception correspond to those pools of neurons that contribute the most to the Fisher information. The individual neuronal contribution from the remaining population is lower but also conveys information and cannot be discarded. Thus, perceptual decisions are based on the whole population of activated neurons, while particular subsets of neurons contribute more than others due to their tuning parameters in relation to the incoming sensory stimulus.
Using FI as the critical decoding variable clearly diverges from models using maximum-likelihood and Bayesian inference methodologies to match perceptual decisions. These approaches generally involve computing probability distributions from the observed neuronal activity pattern. In contrast, we propose that the variance coded locally in the corresponding neuronal population is the only relevant variable used for the decision. This provides a new concept to imagine how the brain processes information. If response variance serves as the neuronal parameter critical for the read-out and decision mechanisms, then the brain may actually implement statistical variance tests on neuronal population responses and finds the stimulus value equalizing the variances. This can be considered as a novel theoretical framework of how the brain processes information, complementing a recently proposed general framework based on weight of evidence (Gold and Shadlen 2001, 2002).
In summary,we delineated an information theoretic model to account for human perceptual performance and compared it to standard decoding methods. The good performance of the models highlights the relevance of theoretical constraints on neuronal processing. In particular, it demonstrates that human perception could be the result of computational strategies that are optimal given the known tuning properties of neurons in cortical areas. This finding bridges the existing gap between theoretical accounts of neuronal information processing and perceptual performance of humans and suggests that humans can extract sensory information at their best given the neuronal population dynamics.
Acknowledgments
Parts of these results have been presented at the conference “NeuroComp—Neurosciences Computationnelles”, 2006, Pont-a-Mousson, France. The authors are grateful to Dr. Alexander Gail for helpful discussion and comments. This research was supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research, grant 016–071–079 (T.W.). T.T. is currently a Bernstein Fellow at the Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience, Göttingen, grant 01GQ0433.
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Appendix A: Opponent motion effects
A.1 Psychophysical results
Visual inspection of the individual experimental results suggested that about seven subjects showed opponent direction of motion effects, with five subjects showing attractive effects between the surround and center motion and 2 subjects showing repulsive effects. Figure 6 presents two typical curves for motion repulsion with different peak types across subjects, one with a simple repulsion effect (subject B) and one of the strongest opposite directions effects (subject H). The figure plots the physical direction of motion of the center target the subject reported to perceive as upward motion (dots; note that each datum corresponds to a unique measurement of the perceived upward direction). The graph also shows the prediction of the FI model (solid line). Figure 6 presents attraction effects for one subject, showing that surround motion of about ±140° resulted in perceived vertical motion as being further than the surround motion direction, in contrast to the first peak where it was seen closer to the surround direction. This second peak was visually evident in seven of the twenty subjects.
A.2 FI model with opponent motion effects
The model presented in the main text assumes that the maximal amplitude modulation effect due to an irrelevant direction of motion is exactly at its direction (equation 2). However, it has been described that motion processing is also influenced by opponency, i.e. by mutual excitation/inhibition effects between motion signals with opposite directions (Allman et al. 1985; Snowden et al. 1991; Heeger et al. 1999). Since we also observed opponency effects in 7 out of 20 subjects, this led us to consider these effects in more detail. We therefore extended the model to incorporate plausible effects from opponent motion . This was done by adjusting Eq. 2 (main text) according to:
21 |
The additional term introduces the possible suppression of information content for the perception of the target motion from motions opponent to the first peak motion (). The extended-model fits to the individual data curves resulted in generally low residuals despite the small number of model parameters (between subjects mean±SE of residuals standard deviation: 1.76 ± 0.11°; n = 20). The individual fits for all 20 subjects data captured the global curve variation (F-test between model variance and global mean variance: 19 subjects had p<0.01; one subject had p = 0.053). Even if the introduced opponent motion model seems to catch the global effect it also induced small discontinuities in the model prediction as it is evident in Fig. 6.
Appendix B: Motion repulsion in center-surround paradigms
In the psychophysical literature, we know of few studies that reported the motion repulsion phenomenon for center-surround configuration. The study of Kim and Wilson (1997) reports a full repulsion curve for four subjects, i.e. for the whole 360° of surround directions of motion sampled at roughly every 22.6° by using sine-wave gratings (see Fig. 7; data extracted from Fig. 3 in their article; mean of 4 subjects). We performed a fit of the FI and standard models to their data but it resulted in a statistically poor fit. It seems that biases are present in this data, with the repulsion curve being strongly asymmetric for clockwise and counter-clockwise surroundto-center direction differences (see Fig. 7, solid black curve for FI, solid grey curve for standard models). Additionally, the standard models hardly predicted themaximum repulsion amplitude (about 30°) even with an almost full surround-tocenter inhibition ( ∼ 0.93).
Our psychophysical results revealed clear motion repulsion for surround motion directions deviating by 20 up to about 100° from the center motion direction. In contrast, smaller angular deviations between center and surround motion directions (±10°) did not show any motion repulsion. To our knowledge, this is the first report of such an effect for this spatial layout and it was unexpected from our side. Since our experimental design allowed a finer sampling of the surround motion directions than Kim and Wilson’s, the effects that we observed in our data at direction deviation between center and surround of ±10 and ±20° cannot be seen in their data. A fit of the FI model to the whole set of 36 data points showed that the model could not predict the overall curve variation (χ 2 = 185.9, df = 34, p < 0.0001; fit not shown). The same was true for the standard models (χ 2 = 99.6, df = 34, p < 0.001; fit not shown). The four data points present for a restricted range of surround motion directions contributed strongly to the model fit (at ±10 and ±20°, see Fig. 5, grey dots), and were deviating substantially from the repulsion effect predicted by inhibitory surround-to-center interactions (see the fits to the 32 points, Fig. 5). Given that the FI and standard models were built explicitly on a theoretical neuronal hypercolumn in area MT/V5 with surround-to-center inhibitory interactions, we believe that any model using only these hypotheses cannot account for the observed differences at these small angular deviations. For addressing directly the results at small direction differences, it remains to be seen how additional model assumptions (e.g. including recurrent neuronal interactions in MT/V5 or between MT/V5 and earlier processing stages) will change the model predictions about the perception of the target stimulus.
Contributor Information
Tzvetomir Tzvetanov, Email: ttzvetanov@dpz.gwdg.de.
Thilo Womelsdorf, Email: t.womelsdorf@fcdonders.ru.nl.
References
- Abbott L, Dayan P. The effect of correlated variability on the accuracy of a population code. Neural Comput. 1999;11:91–101. doi: 10.1162/089976699300016827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allman J, Miezin F, McGuiness E. Direction- and velocity-specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field in the middle temporal visual area (MT) Perception. 1985;14:105–126. doi: 10.1068/p140105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bonnet C (1986) Manuel pratique de psychophysique. édition U. Armand Colin
- Born R, Bradley D. Structure and function of visual area MT. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:157–189. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.26.041002.131052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Britten K (2003) The visual neurosciences, vol 2, chapter The middle temporal area: motion processing and the link to perception (Chap. 81), pp 1203–216. Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2003.
- Cornsweet T. The staircase method in psychophysics. Am J Psychol. 1962;75:485–491. doi: 10.2307/1419876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Abbott L. Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. Cambridge: MIT Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA. Theory of statistical estimation. Proc Cambridge Phil Soc. 1925;22:700–725. doi: 10.1017/S0305004100009580. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JI, Shadlen MN. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli. Trends Cogn Sci. 2001;5:10–16. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01567-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold JI, Shadlen MN. Banburismus and the brain: decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward. Neuron. 2002;36:299–308. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00971-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heeger D, Boynton G, Demb J, Seidemann E, Newsome W. Motion opponency in visual cortex. J Neurosci. 1999;19:7162–7174. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-16-07162.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hiris E, Blake R. Direction repulsion in motion transparency. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13:187–197. doi: 10.1017/S0952523800007227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hol K, Treue S. Different populations of neurons contribute to the detection and discrimination of visual motion. Vis Res. 2001;41:685–689. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00314-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jazayeri M, Movshon A. Optimal representation of sensory information by neural populations. Nature Neurosci. 2006;9:690–696. doi: 10.1038/nn1691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jin DZ, Dragoi V, Sur M, Seung HS. Tilt aftereffect and adaptation-induced changes in orientation tuning in visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:4038–4050. doi: 10.1152/jn.00571.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaernbach C. Simple adaptive testing with the weighted up-down method. Percept Psychophys. 1991;49:227–229. doi: 10.3758/bf03214307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaernbach C. Slope bias of psychometric functions derived from adaptive data. Percept Psychophys. 2001;63:1389–1398. doi: 10.3758/bf03194550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kass RE, Ventura V, Brown EN. Statistical issues in the analysis of neuronal data. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:8–25. doi: 10.1152/jn.00648.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J, Wilson H. Motion integration over space: interaction of the center and surround motion. Vis Res. 1997;37:991–1005. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00254-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leek MR, Hanna TE, Marshall L. Estimation of psychometric functions from adaptive tracking procedures. Percept Psychophys. 1992;51:247–256. doi: 10.3758/bf03212251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- London M, Häusser M. Dendritic Computation. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2005;28:503–532. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mahani AS, Carlsson AE, Wessel R. Motion repulsion arises from stimulus statistics when analyzed with a clustering algorithm. Biol Cybern. 2005;92:288–291. doi: 10.1007/s00422-005-0556-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshak W, Sekuler R. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. Science. 1979;205:1399–1401. doi: 10.1126/science.472756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paradiso MA. A theory for the use of visual orientation information which exploits the columnar structure of striate cortex. Biol Cybern. 1988;58:35–49. doi: 10.1007/BF00363954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pouget A, Dayan P, Zemel R. Information processing with population codes. Nature Rev Neurosci. 2000;1:125–132. doi: 10.1038/35039062. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Press W, Teukolsky S, Vetterling W, Flannery B (1997) Numerical recipes in C: the art of scientific computing. Cambridge University Press
- Purushothaman G, Bradley DC. Neural population code for fine perceptual decisions in area MT. Nature Neurosci. 2005;8:99–106. doi: 10.1038/nn1373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rauber H-J, Treue S. Revisiting motion repulsion: evidence for a general phenomenon? Vis Res. 1999;39:3187–3196. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00025-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regan D, Beverley KI. Postadaptation orientation discrimination. J Opt Soc Am A. 1985;2:147–155. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.2.000147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seung H, Sompolinsky H. Simple models for reading neuronal population codes. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. 1993;90:10749–10753. doi: 10.1073/pnas.90.22.10749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snowden R, Treue S, Erickson R, Andersen R. The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion. J Neurosci. 1991;11:2768–2785. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02768.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tzvetanov T, Womelsdorf T, Niebergall R, Treue S. Feature-based attention influences contextual interactions during motion repulsion. Vis Res. 2006;46:3651–3658. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vågerö M, Sundberg R. The distribution of the maximum likelihood estimator in up-and-down experiments for quantal dose-response data. J Biopharmaceut Statist. 1999;9:499–519. doi: 10.1081/BIP-100101190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vogels R. Population coding of stimulus orientation by striate cortical cells. Biol Cybern. 1990;64:25–31. doi: 10.1007/BF00203627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westheimer G, Shimamura K, McKee SP. Interference with line-orientation sensitivity. J Opt Soc Am. 1976;66:332–338. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.66.000332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zemel RS, Dayan P, Pouget A. Probabilistic interpretation of population codes. Neural Comput. 1998;10:403–430. doi: 10.1162/089976698300017818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]