Abstract
Legislation mandates that all children, including children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) be taught to read in ways that are consistent with reading research, and target the five components of evidence-based reading instruction: phonemic awareness, phonics, reading fluency, vocabulary and comprehension strategies. The purpose of this review is to synthesize the literature on reading instruction for children with ASD that encompasses one or more of the five components of reading. The review yielded 11 studies with 61 participants ages 4 to 17. Results indicate that children with ASD can benefit from reading instruction consistent with reading research. Research in this area is still preliminary, and more research is needed to guide practice. Possible directions for future research are provided.
Keywords: autism spectrum disorders, reading, literacy
Generally, converging evidence from a handful of studies describes the reading skills of children with ASD as having relative strengths in decoding while experiencing greater difficultly with language and reading comprehension (Calhoon, 2001; Frith, 2003; Lord & Paul, 1997; Nation, Clarke, Wright, & Williams, 2006). Across two large studies involving a total of 280 participants with ASD, Mayes and Calhoun (2003a, 2003b) found that all of their participants with average IQ scores and approximately half of the children who had IQ scores below 80 achieved average scores on decoding and spelling measures. Notably, children with average IQ scores had comprehension scores within average range, but most children with IQ scores below 80 were unable to complete comprehension subtests. All participants, regardless of IQ scores, demonstrated difficulty with language comprehension.
To gain a broader understanding of the reading capabilities of children representative of the autism spectrum, Nation et al. (2006) examined the reading skills of 41 children with ASD ages 6–15 including 16 identified with autism, 13 with pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS), and 12 with Asperger syndrome. Inclusion criteria included “measurable language skills” even if language skills were limited. Children were assessed on measures of single word recognition in isolation, pseudoword or nonword recognition, text reading accuracy, and text comprehension; on average, they demonstrated good word reading ability and poor comprehension. Their vocabulary and oral language comprehension scores were highly correlated with their scores on the reading comprehension measure (i.e., .72 and .67 respectively). However, the authors noted large individual differences in performance with some children scoring far above average, and others unable to complete the task. This level of variance demonstrates the heterogeneity in reading ability across the autism spectrum, and suggests using caution when interpreting mean scores for this population of students (Nation et al.).
Another study specifically examined the degree to which students with ASD could master phonics rules. Calhoon (2001) studied the word recognition skills of ten children with autism who obtained varied IQ scores ranging from 60 to 100, and were able to identify sight words on a second grade level at the onset of the study. The author assessed each child’s understanding of word parts, grapheme-phonemes, onsets and rime, and recognition of high frequency words. Results indicated that the children had developed phonics skills and that they attended to word parts that provide cues such as rimes. The author suggested phonics instruction that encompasses word families, word parts, and structural analysis (e.g., prefixes and suffixes) may prove beneficial for students with autism. Two further studies have shown that children with Asperger syndrome who developed grade level decoding skills could comprehend material containing factual information, but had trouble making inferences (Griswold, Barnhill, Myles, Hagiwara, & Simpson, 2002; Myles et al., 2002).
The No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB) and IDEA 2004 mandate that all children, including children with ASD be taught to read in ways that are consistent with reading research. More specifically, findings from the National Reading Panel (NICHD, 2000) are cited in NCLB language requiring that all students be provided explicit and systematic classroom reading instruction that includes five essential components of reading: phonemic awareness, phonics, oral reading fluency, vocabulary and comprehension strategies. See Table 1 for descriptions and examples of each identified component.
Table 1.
Summary of NRP Essential Components of Reading Instruction
Component | Definition | Examples |
---|---|---|
Phonemic Awareness | Recognizing and manipulating spoken words in language. | Tell me: “the first sound in the word cat.” /c/; “the sound that is the same tide, toy, toss.” /t/; “what word these sounds make /c/ /a/ /t/ ?” cat; “all the sounds you hear in cat?” /c/ /a/ /t/ |
Phonics | Understanding letter-sound correspondences in reading and spelling. | Mapping letters to corresponding sound/phoneme and blending these sounds to form words; analyzing letter-sound relationships from whole to part (e.g., chop, chin, catch contain digraph ch-); segmenting words into phonemes and writing corresponding letters to form words; using parts of known words to identify new words; using sound letter correspondences and context cues to identify unfamiliar words in text. |
Oral Reading Fluency | Reading text with speed, accuracy, and expression | Repeatedly reading a text orally with guidance from the teacher (i.e., choral reading, echo reading, etc.), or through independent reading practice with feedback. |
Vocabulary | Understanding words read by linking the word to oral vocabulary. | Active learning of vocabulary required for reading a specific text, or likely to be seen in a variety of texts. Instruction in multiple contexts to include: making mental images of words, acting out definitions, using words in writing, and searching and using context clues to aid understanding. |
Comprehension strategy instruction | Directly teaching students to be aware of the cognitive processes involved in reading. | Teacher models monitoring reading for understanding using think aloud through graphic organizers, story maps, questioning, summarization and/or multiple strategies. Guided practice is provided and faded as readers become increasingly more independent in employing the strategies during reading. |
Although interventions including these five components have been effective in preventing or remediating reading difficulties for most children, none of these studies have included children with ASD. Findings from a recent and seminal review established the efficacy of sight word reading approaches for most students with significant cognitive disabilities including children with autism (Browder, Wakeman, Spooner, Ahlgrim-Delzell, & Algozzine, 2006). At present no review has summarized the effects of interventions that target one or more of the essential components of reading mandated by NCLB and IDEA on the reading skills of children with ASD. Therefore, the purpose of this review is to examine the effects of such reading interventions for school-aged children with ASD.
The conceptual framework for the present review is the Simple View of Reading (Gough, Hoover, & Peterson, 1996) as it is widely supported in the literature (Catts & Kamhi, 2005; Kamhi, 2005; Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Perfetti, Landi, & Oakhill, 2005), and useful in categorizing the essential reading instructional components recommended by the NRP into two broader sets of skills. The first set includes code-focused skills, or phonological awareness, phonics, and fluency, which are required to accurately and fluently identify words in text. The second set is comprised of meaning-focused skills, namely vocabulary and comprehension, required for comprehending language in oral or written form.
In considering the research base on reading development of students with ASD (Calhoon, 2001; Frith, 2003; Lord & Paul, 1997; Mayes & Calhoun 2003a, 2003b; Nation et al., 2006), this population of learners appear to have relative strengths in the area of phonics or decoding. However, if children with ASD can read text accurately, but don’t know the meaning of key vocabulary, or cannot comprehend the concepts expressed, then reading comprehension will suffer. As the Simple View of Reading states, students use word recognition skills to identify written words while at the same time they are using general verbal knowledge and language comprehension abilities to construct the meaning of what they are reading (Gough et al., 1996). Because some students with ASD may experience difficulty with word level reading, and an even greater number with reading comprehension, both code-focused and meaning-focused reading instruction is a priority. Therefore, the literature incorporating reading instruction as defined by NCLB for children with ASD was reviewed to determine the extent to which code-and meaning-focused reading interventions affect the reading skills of children with ASD.
Method
To be included in this review, articles had to meet the following criteria: (a) published in a peer reviewed journal, (b) included one or more school-age participants (ages 5–18) identified as having an ASD (i.e., autism, pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified, or Asperger syndrome), and (c) described research studies that tested the effectiveness of interventions in code-focused and/or meaning-focused skills as defined by the NRP. The search began with online ERIC and Psych Info databases. Key words pertained to autism and reading (i.e., autism, Asperger, developmental, moderate, disabilities, reading, literacy, phonics, phonic instruction, word recognition, word identification, sight words, vocabulary, fluency, oral reading, decode, strategy instruction, comprehension). From this preliminary search, journals frequently publishing intervention research with a focus on children with ASD were identified: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, Autism, Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Research and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, and Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities. These were hand searched for abstracts of interventions implemented to increase the reading skills of children with ASD. Third, references were gathered and obtained from previous literature reviews (Browder et al., 2006; Conners, 1992), and book chapters (Browder, Courtade-Little, Wakeman, & Rickelman, 2006; Houston, Al Otaiba, & Torgesen, 2006). Studies were excluded from the review that (a) lacked a formal research design (e.g., Colasent & Griffith, 1998; Colby, 1973; Hewett, 1964; Lanquetot, 1984), (b) provided only sight word instruction (see Browder et al., 2006 for a review of these studies), and/or (c) included a reading measure, but an intervention targeting a skill other than reading (e.g., visual stress in Ludlow, Wilkins & Heaton, 2006).
Results
A total of 11 studies met the aforementioned criteria. Table 2 shows the design, participants, setting and intervention, duration, measures and treatment effects of each included study. Code-focused and meaning-focused interventions will be examined first, followed by interventions that combine multiple components. Implications for practitioners and directions for future research will be discussed.
Table 2.
Reviewed Studies by Instructional Focus
Author | Design | Participants | Setting & Intervention | Duration | Measures | Treatment Effects |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Code-Focused Studies | ||||||
Basil & Reyes, 2003 | Pre- post-test design | 2 students with ASD: 1. 13-yr. old; MA = 4 yrs; Reading: limited sight word reading/writing. 2. 8-yr. old with intellectual disability; MA = 7 yrs; Reading: limited sight word reading/writing, described as difficult to involve in reading tasks. |
Self-contained classroom (Spain). One to one computer – assisted instruction |
30 minutes, twice a week for 3 months; total = 12 hrs. | End of lesson test. Spanish standardized test measuring writing (i.e., dictation, free composition, copying) and reading (i.e., letters, syllables, words, text, and comprehension) | Mastered all 10 lessons and passed the end of lesson tests at the end of 3 months. One participant demonstrated gains in phonological awareness, word spelling and spontaneous written composition on standardized measure. |
Coleman-Martin, Heller, Chihak, & Irvine, 2005 | Multiple conditions design with drop-down baselines | 1 12-yr. old with ASD; MA NR; Reading: informal assessments indicated 2 grade level word recognition skills | Self-contained classroom. One to one computer-assisted sight word instruction. |
Instruction provided until 80% correct for 2 consecutive sessions. Minimum of 4 sessions; total = NR | Sight word measure (identified a target word from 3 similar distracter words). | Criterion reached in 3 sessions in computer-assisted condition; in 4 sessions during teacher only condition; and in 6 sessions during teacher + computer condition. |
Heiman, Nelson, Tjus, Gillberg (1995) | Pre- post- test with follow-up | 11 children with ASD Mean CA = 9:4 yrs. (range of 6–13) Mean MA = 7 yrs. (SD = 2) Reading: NR |
Self-contained school (Sweden) One to one computer-assisted instruction. |
Averaged 26 sessions (range 21–32 minutes); approximate total = 13 hrs. | End of lesson test. Swedish standardized test of phonological awareness, word identification, sentence reading, and sentence imitation. Observations of communication. |
Completed end of lesson tests with a mean of 92% correct and SD of 8.4. Increased mean scores on reading measures from pre- to post-test and at follow-up; increased mean scores on PA measures from pre- to post-test, but decreased at follow-up; sentence imitation results inconclusive. Observations revealed increased interaction during intervention |
Tjus, Heimann & Nelson (1998) | Pre- post- test with follow-up | 13 students with ASD Mean CA = 10 yrs. (range of 4–11) Mean MA = 7.3 yrs. (SD = 2) Reading: NR |
Self-contained classroom (Sweden) One to one computer-assisted instruction. |
Averaged 15 sessions (range 15–30 minutes); approximate total = 7.5 hrs. | End of lesson test. Swedish standardized test of phonological awareness, word identification, sentence reading, and sentence imitation. |
Increased mean scores from pre- to posttest on reading measures; decline at the follow-up; increased mean scores on PA from pre- to post-test and at follow-up; proportion of correct sentences increased minimally, but response time significantly decreased |
Meaning Focused Studies | ||||||
Dugan, et al., 1995 | Reversal Design | 16 4th grade general education peers 2 4th grade students with ASD; 1. 10-yr. old female with moderate cognitive disability (MA: NR); Reading: Difficulty with comprehension 2. 9-yr. old male w/HFA; Reading: difficulty with comprehension |
4th grade general education social studies class. 10-minute teacher-directed social studies lecture; heterogeneous peer tutoring and cooperative learning activities. |
40 minute class periods, 4 days a week for 8 weeks; total = 21 hrs. | Researcher-made social studies curriculum tests: 15 vocabulary terms and sentence creation using terms. Observations of academic and social engagement |
Mean gain scores of participants with ASD increased from pre (m = 0–2) to posttest (m = 5 to 8.6). Mean gain scores of general education peers also increased from pre to posttest. Levels of engagement of participants with ASD and peers increased. |
Kamps, Leonard, Potucek & Garrison-Harrell (1995) | Reversal design with random assignment | 3 children with ASD 1. 8 yr old w/HFA; IQ Score: 101; 3rd grade; Reading: difficulty with comprehension 2. 13 yr old with autism; IQ score = 50; 5th grade; Reading: Able to read and write, but difficulty with comprehension 3. 12 yr old with autism; IQ score: 46; 5th grade; Reading: primary level; difficulty with comprehension of language and text. |
General education 3rd and 5th grade classroom Participants randomly assigned roles during cooperative learning groups (n = 4 students): (1) reviewing vocabulary words, (2) responding to wh comprehension questions following reading, (3) comprehension game of characters and facts from stories read in class. |
30–40 minute sessions for approximately 14 weeks; approximate total = 35–47 hours | Weekly researcher-made vocabulary and comprehension tests on reading chapters with 15 items including, comprehension, vocabulary, and sequencing questions. Academic and social engagement. Teacher satisfaction survey. |
Participant with HFA increased gain scores on weekly tests from baseline (m = 4; 3.7) to intervention (m = 7.8; 8.7); General education peers increased mean gain scores on weekly tests after intervention; Greater variability for 2 participants with autism with gains from baseline (range 0–3; 0–4) to intervention (range 1–6; 0–4). All participants increased levels of engagement. Teachers reported high levels of satisfaction for whole class and for the child with HFA, but lower levels for the 2 children with cognitive disability. |
O’Connor & Klein, 2004 | Repeated measures within subjects design | 20 children with ASD 10 w/autism, 6 w/Asperger syndrome, and 4 w/PDD Mean age = 15; range = 14–17; SD = 1; Reading: all participants have average or above decoding skills but lower levels of reading comprehension. |
Home or school setting One to one instruction. Students read stories aloud under 2 control and 3 treatment conditions: prereading question; anaphoric cuing; cloze. |
10-minute conditions Total = 1 hour. | Researcher-made test of comprehension questions. | Anaphoric cuing resulted in medium effect size gains (1/2 SD above results in control condition) of more than half of participants. Gains from prereading questions and cloze conditions were small and not statistically significant. |
Rosenbaum & Breiling (1976) | ABA Reversal Design | 12 yr old female with ASD; MA and IQ NR; Reading: limited decoding ability and comprehension. | Clinical setting. One to one instruction in reading directions (e.g., stand up, point to your mouth, point to the man writing, etc.) presented for 10 second trials. |
Initially 17 daily 15–20 minute sessions; followed by 17 twice a day sessions; approximate total = 15 hrs. | Researcher-made observational recording scale based on level of prompting required for a correct response. | 100% mastery by 12th session and maintained when reward removed; Generalized across experimenters and conditions-except when researcher absent, but when reentered room performance near 100% |
Whalon & Hanline, in press | Multiple baseline across participants | 3 children w/ASD 1. 7-yr. old with Asperger syndrome; IQ = 101; Reading: difficulty with retell; 2. 8-yr. old with ASD; Nonverbal IQ = 112; 3. 7 – yr. old with PDD; Nonverbal IQ = 92; Reading: difficulty with retell |
Small group setting outside of general education classroom. Reciprocal questioning in cooperative pairs. |
30–40 minutes 4 days a week for 3 weeks; approximate total = 7 hrs. | Frequency of question generation and unprompted responses to questions generated by peers. Participant satisfaction survey. |
Increased generation of and response to questions. Prompting occurred throughout intervention but decreased. Questions became less generic and more varied and specific; Participants perceived the intervention as helpful and parents noted a difference in their children’s reading |
Multi-component Studies | ||||||
Kamps, Barbetta, Leonard & Delquadri (1994) | Multiple Baseline across subjects | 3 children w/HFA 1. 8-yr. old; IQ = 101; 1/2nd grade classroom; Reading: at or above 2nd grade level 2. 8-yr. old; IQ = 71; 2nd grade; Reading: 2nd grade but difficulty with reading comprehension and completing tasks 3. 9-yr. old; IQ NR; 3rd grade; Reading: grade level on most academic tasks |
General education 1st/2nd, 2nd and 3rd grade classrooms. Classwide peer tutoring. |
25–30 minutes; 3–4 times per week; Number of sessions varied (range of 14 to 35 sessions); total = unclear or a range of 6–17 hours | Researcher-made measures of words read correctly per minute (and errors); response to comprehension questions. Duration of social interaction (i.e., initiation – response sequence) |
Participants increased mean number of words read correctly per minute (19, 31, & 12 words); Number of errors decreased by one student (from 6 to 2) and remained low at 2–3 words per minute for the other 2 participants. Participants increased the percentage of correct responses to comprehension questions (47%, 24% & 67% at baseline to 76%, 68% & 90%). Increased duration of social communication. |
Kamps, Locke, Delquadri & Hall, 1989 | Multiple baseline across tasks | 2 children with autism 1. 9-yr. old; IQ =50; Reading: good decoding but poor comprehension 2. 11-yr. old; IQ =50 and 50; Reading: good decoding but poor comprehension. |
Self-contained 5th grade general education class. Peer tutoring in money skills, expressive language, and oral reading/comprehension. Oral reading fluency and comprehension. |
20 minutes 3 times a week for approximately 6 weeks. Total = approximately 5–6 hours. |
Number of words read correctly and errors per minute from a 2 min timed reading. Number of correct responses to factual comprehension questions |
Participant mean reading rates increased following intervention (m = 19.6 and 30.2 at baseline to 34.2 and 39.7) and the number of errors decreased (m=3.7; m=1.9at baseline to m = 1.3;, m = .9) Participants increased the number of correct responses to factual comprehension questions (1.9; 1.3 during baseline to 3.8 and 3.5) |
Of the 11 studies, four targeted code-focused skills (Basil & Reyes, 2003; Coleman-Martin, Heller, Chihak, & Irvine, 2005; Heiman, Nelson, Tjus, & Gillberg, 1995; Tjus, Heimann, & Nelson, 1998), five targeted meaning-focused skills (Dugan et al., 1995; Kamps, Leonard, Potucek, & Garrison-Harrell, 1995; O’Conner & Klein, 2004; Rosenbaum & Breiling, 1976; Whalon & Hanline, 2008), and the remaining two studies addressed both code-focused and meaning-focused skills (Kamps, Barbetta, Leonard, & Delquadri, 1994; Kamps, Locke, Delquadri, & Hall, 1989). In all, 61 children with ASD participated including, 44 (72%) identified with autism, 5 (8%) with high functioning autism (HFA), 7 with Asperger syndrome (12%), and 5 (8%) with pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified (PDD-NOS). Participants ranged in age from 4 to 17 years.
Code-Focused Interventions
Four studies addressed the question of whether code-focused interventions can improve the reading ability of students with ASD (Basil & Reyes, 2003; Coleman-Martin et al., 2005; Heiman et al., 1995; Tjus et al., 1998). Each of these studies used computer-assisted instruction with a pre/post-test design. Three shared the same software, developed in Sweden that evolved from an early version called “Alpha” (Heiman et al.) to a later version “Delta” (Basil & Reyes; Tjus et al.). The program administered in the Basil and Reyes study was changed from Swedish to accommodate students speaking Castilian Spanish. The program includes 10 lessons that teach students to build sentences in growing difficulty by clicking on a word or group of words. Once a sentence is created, a corresponding picture is generated, and students click on the sentence, and the program reads the sentence aloud. This is an errorless learning task in that all possible words the child could select form a meaningful sentence that can be animated. This task builds in complexity beginning with noun and verb sentences and adding prepositions, adjectives, and conjunctions in future lessons. The program also includes a computer-delivered assessment task (used in each study as one of the pre/post-test measures) that required participants to create a sentence representing a presented animated action from a choice of preselected words at 80% accuracy.
Heiman et al. (1995) provided training to 30 children, including 11 with ASD. The intervention was conducted across 3–4 months. Results suggested that on average students mastered the computer-assisted task and also increased their reading and phonological scores on researcher-administered standardized tests with some maintenance at follow up.
Tjus et al. (1998) targeted only students with ASD. In this study, students received fewer sessions (15) conducted over a briefer time frame (1–2 months). Similar to the earlier study, students mastered the computer-assisted task and achieved generally positive gains with some demonstration of maintenance in phonological awareness.
The findings from both of these studies (Heiman et al., 1995; Tjus et al., 1998) are encouraging, yet ambiguous. Because of the small sample size, age range of included participants, absence of information describing participant reading ability prior to the intervention, and the variable reading skills associated with ASD (Nation et al., 2006), mean scores can obscure findings. Thus, it is possible that reporting means could mask important and potentially large individual differences in response to intervention.
In the third computer study (Basil & Reyes, 2003), two students successfully completed 10 lessons and passed the computer-assisted task with 80% accuracy or better; however, only one student was administered the post-test battery, the other “did not cooperate in the test tasks” (p. 40). Thus, findings for only one student suggest improvement from pre to post-test on measures of phonological awareness and spelling.
The fourth study (Coleman-Martin et al., 2005) used a single subject design to determine the effectiveness of the nonverbal reading approach delivered through computer assisted instruction on participants’ ability to decode new words. This study was conducted in English in the US. One student with was taught phonic skills across three conditions: (1) teacher only, (2) teacher plus computer assisted instruction, and (3) computer assisted instruction alone. In all three conditions the teaching method was constant, but differed in the mode of instruction. First, the teacher taught the whole word (man), and then the teacher segmented the word by stretching out each individual sound (e.g., mmmmaaaannnn). In the final step, the teacher showed the student the whole word again and asked him to say it fast. The process was the same during the computer-assisted condition except instruction was delivered through PowerPoint. Throughout each step, the teacher or computer verbalized the word as the student was instructed to use internal speech, or say the sound “in your head” (Coleman-Martin et al., p. 84). As in the prior three studies, the scope of training was limited with only a total of 15 words taught to include five in each condition. The findings suggest that computer assisted instruction may be a promising and flexible way to provide children with autism additional decoding practice.
Summary
In these four code-focused computer-assisted instruction studies, the authors reported gains across a variety of code reading skills and spelling measures including: phonological awareness (Basil & Reyes, 2003; Heiman et al., 1995; Tjus et al., 1998), word recognition (Basil & Reyes; Coleman-Martin et al., 2005; Heiman et al.; Tjus et al.), word spelling (Basil & Reyes), sentence reading (Heiman et al.; Tjus et al.), and sentence imitation (Heiman et al.; Tjus et al.). These results suggest that children with ASD can develop phonemic awareness and/or phonics skills regardless of IQ score (Tjus et al.). However, it is important to consider that the Swedish and Spanish languages are more transparent than English so that replications are needed.
Consistent across all included studies were gains made by children with ASD in code-focused skills. The duration or intensity of instruction needed for children with ASD to master skills is less clear. Participants who received instruction through Delta Messages did not receive direct instruction in phonemic awareness or phonics, yet made incidental gains on phonological awareness and phonics measures. It may be possible that the degree and intensity of phonological awareness and phonics instruction provided to the general education population, and consistent with the NRP recommendations, may be sufficient for some children with ASD.
Meaning-Focused Interventions
Five studies investigated the impact of meaning-focused interventions on the reading skills of participants with ASD. Two targeted vocabulary (Dugan et al., 1995; Rosenbaum & Breiling, 1976), two comprehension (O’Connor & Klein, 2004; Whalon & Hanline, 2008), and one vocabulary and comprehension (Kamps et al., 1995). Across these five studies, two instructional delivery methods were used: peer-mediated (Dugan et al., 1995; Kamps et al., 1995; Whalon & Hanline, 2008), and one-to one (O’Connor & Klein, 2004; Rosenbaum & Breiling, 1976). Participants in all five studies were students with ASD described as having word recognition skills of at least a second grade level, and experiencing problems with reading comprehension.
Dugan et al. (1995) formed cooperative groups in an inclusive 4th grade social studies class. Each group consisted of one high academic achiever, 2 moderate level achievers, and one below level achiever or one of the two students with ASD. The authors described one participant with ASD as “high functioning” and the other as “functioning at a moderate level” (p. 177). Cooperative learning groups followed a 10 min social studies lecture and included key word peer tutoring for 10 min, fact card tutoring for 8 min, and a 5 min team worksheet or research activity. Groups occurred four times a week for three weeks during a 40 min social studies class and again for five weeks following a reversal to baseline conditions. Weekly quizzes tested each participant’s ability to identify and use 15 key vocabulary terms in a grammatically correct sentence to include the key word and a referent to that term. When asked about satisfaction with the intervention, teachers reported high levels regarding ease of implementation and social benefits of the intervention, but reports of academic gains were mixed as one teacher indicated dissatisfaction with academic progress of students with ASD.
While the intervention produced positive gains, the authors reported low reliability when participants exhibited few social interaction and engagement behaviors, and on some social studies quiz items. The mean percentage interobserver agreement on target students’ social studies quizzes was 63%. The authors explained that although reliability was acceptable for most items, items that required students to make inferences resulted in lower levels of agreement (Dugan et al., 1995).
Kamps et al. (1995) also incorporated cooperative learning groups. Participants with ASD included one child identified with HFA and two children with autism and a moderate cognitive disability. While in cooperative learning groups, participants completed three, 10 min learning activities based on a novel. Teacher-led instruction followed cooperative learning groups and focused on new vocabulary, reading a story, main ideas and sequencing. The participant with HFA participated in teacher-led instruction, and students with autism were given independent tasks based on their separate reading material. The participant identified as having HFA responded to both fact- and inferential- based questions, and participants with autism were administered only fact-based items.
Kamps et al. (1995) did not report the number of fact-based and inferential items, and vocabulary and comprehension questions posed to students. In addition, reported results do not mention student performance on fact-based versus inferential questions. Also, by administering only fact-based items to participants with autism and a moderate cognitive disability, their ability to respond to inference related questions is unknown.
Whalon and Hanline (2008) randomly assigned participants with ASD to one of three general education peers (n=9) from their mainstream classroom setting to work in cooperative pairs. On each successive day, general education participants were rotated. In cooperative pairs, students were taught to generate and respond to wh- questions using a story grammar framework (i.e., setting, characters, events, problem, and solution) as they took turns reading a book aloud. During the initial session, the researchers used a think aloud procedure to teach question generation by verbally “walking” the students through each mental process used to construct a question during reading. During the initial session the researcher and participants took turns generating and responding to questions. By session two, the researcher no longer generated questions with participants, and participants monitored their participation through the use of a visual checklist and an interactive, visual storyboard. The researcher provided praise following appropriate question generation and responding, and scaffolding to include verbal prompts, corrective feedback, and modeling when participants had difficulty forming/responding to questions. Verbal prompting included a verbal reminder to generate or respond to a question, and when giving corrective feedback the researcher explained why a question or response was unclear/inappropriate and modeled an appropriate question/response.
Gains were immediate for 2 participants, and the participant with PDD-NOS showed greater variability. Levels of prompting were similar among participants with ASD and their peers. For example, both participants with ASD and their general education peers required more prompting when a question required an answer beyond text recall or what was explicitly stated in text. Measures of treatment fidelity reveal the intervention was implemented as intended. Despite positive results, the lack of a generalization measure makes it unclear whether student gains would be realized on a distant curriculum based assessment (e.g., a weekly pre and post-test, story retell after the completing a book).
The two one-on-one studies used very different approaches to enhance understanding of text. Rosenbaum and Breiling (1976) taught a girl with autism to demonstrate understanding of a single direction presented on an index card by acting out the action or pointing to a picture of someone completing the action. When the participant successfully interpreted the action, the researcher provided praise and candy. Following an incorrect response, the researcher provided a series of prompts until the participant responded correctly: (1) a verbal prompt, (2) model of the correct response, and (3) physical guidance. Despite mastery of the 54 tasks, it is unclear how the participant would respond to similar words read in connected text.
O’Conner and Klein (2004) sought to determine the effects of procedural facilitation on the reading comprehension of participants with ASD. All participants achieved high scores on word identification and low scores on reading comprehension measures. Participants were asked to read five stories aloud in four conditions. In the prereading condition, researchers asked students questions before reading to access prior knowledge. On anaphoric cuing passages, pronouns were underlined and participants identified the referent from a choice of three words written under the corresponding pronoun. Similarly, in the cloze condition, students read a passage with 12 blanks, and selected a word to fill in the blank from a choice of three words (other than a pronoun) that could be deduced from information in the previous three sentences. Students wrote the chosen word in each blank. In control conditions, the participants read the passages without any additional prompts or cues. Following each condition, a researcher-made test consisting of 12 items (e.g., free retell, identification of main idea, title generation, answering fact-based and inference questions) was administered.
Results showed that more than half of the participants with ASD increased performance on the comprehension test under the anaphoric cueing condition but not the prereading questions and cloze conditions. The authors noted that in the activating prior knowledge condition, participants responded to questions with information inconsistent with the present reading, which possibly not only failed to enhance comprehension, but actually impeded comprehension. Also, in both the anaphoric cuing and cloze conditions the participants completed the task with approximately 80% accuracy, but gains were greater following anaphoric cuing.
These findings suggest that anaphoric cueing is a promising reading comprehension intervention, and the authors note that students with ASD can potentially be taught to use the strategy independently. Based on the findings of this study, it may be necessary to initially provide greater structure when teaching a comprehension strategy to children with ASD. That is, in the anaphoric cueing condition, students were briefly taught that texts contain shorter words that represent longer words. Students were given a choice of words representing the pronoun and were asked to circle the one that corresponded to the “shortcut” word (O’Connor & Klein, 2004, p. 120); therefore, providing more structure by presenting options rather than an open ended prompt (i.e, cloze procedure, prior knowledge questions). Additional research is needed to understand the degree of support needed for children with ASD to learn a comprehension strategy as well as a method for fading such supports.
Summary
Despite some individual variability, following participation in meaning-focused interventions, students with ASD made gains on vocabulary and/or comprehension quizzes (Dugan et al., 1995; Kamps et al., 1995), question generation and responding during reading (Whalon & Hanline, 2008), ability to follow written directions (Rosenbaum & Breiling, 1976), and retelling important events of a story (O’Connor & Klein, 2004). Additional benefits were noted for general education peers in peer-mediated studies (Dugan et al.; Kamps et al.; Whalon & Hanline). Although only measured in three studies, social validity reports from teachers (Dugan et al.; Kamps et al.), participants with ASD, their peers, as well parents (Whalon & Hanline) indicated some perceived benefit after children with ASD participated in meaning-focused interventions. Further, results showed potential for interactive reading interventions such as cooperative learning to target additional, important skills for children with ASD such as social communication (Kamps et al.), and academic engagement (Dugan et al.; Kamps et al.).
Although results suggest that students with ASD can develop meaning-focused skills, some variability was reported, and attributed to individual characteristics. For example, in one study, participants with autism and a moderate cognitive disability did not receive the same benefits from interventions as their peer identified with HFA (Kamps et al., 1995). Also, the majority of meaning-focused studies included participants identified with high functioning forms of autism (n = 23 of 29). Whalon and Hanline (2008) reported that gains made following an interactive reading comprehension intervention reflected the degree of participant language development prior to intervention. Specifically, participants achieving higher scores on language assessments prior to intervention experienced more immediate gains.
Multi-component Interventions
Two studies incorporated multi-component interventions to increase code-focused and meaning-focused skills of children with ASD. Kamps et al. (1989) measured the effectiveness of peer tutors for increasing the academic skills of two children with autism described as having good decoding but poor comprehension skills. When targeting reading, participants with autism read aloud to their general education peers and answered comprehension questions based on what was read. Results indicated the benefits of the intervention.
In a similar study, Kamps et al. (1994) evaluated the impact of classwide peer tutoring on the fluency and reading comprehension as well as social interaction skills of children with HFA and average academic skills. Students were assigned to a different peer tutor weekly, and tutoring roles were reciprocal. Students took turns repeatedly reading, providing corrective and positive feedback, and asking 3 min of “wh” comprehension questions. The number of words read correctly per min were recorded and posted in the classroom. Bonus points were given for appropriate tutor-learner behaviors, and free time followed tutoring to provide participants with an opportunity for social interaction. Following classwide peer tutoring, participants with HFA increased the mean number of words read correctly per min. A return to baseline resulted in a decrease for 2 participants, but rates increased once classwide peer tutoring was reintroduced and exceeded baseline levels.
Like the cooperative learning study conducted by Kamps et al. (1995) targeting comprehension skills, the peer tutoring studies addressing both fluency and comprehension varied their outcome measure based on the characteristics of participants with ASD. When participants with ASD were described as high functioning, outcome measures included both fact-based and inferential questions (Kamps et al., 1994), but when participants with autism were described as having a cognitive disability measures were comprised of fact-based items only (Kamps et al., 1989). Also, consistent with the cooperative learning study, the number of inferential and fact-based questions administered was not specified making it difficult to interpret the extent to which participants made gains on fact versus inference based questions (Kamps et al., 1994).
Summary
Peer-mediated studies addressing the oral reading fluency and comprehension of children with ASD resulted in increased number of words read correctly per min and comprehension questions answered correctly (Kamps et al., 1989; 1994). Also, the classwide peer tutoring intervention revealed gains in duration of social communication of both children with ASD and their general education peers (Kamps et al., 1994). Results from these studies suggest that children with ASD can benefit from peer-mediated interventions to increase their oral reading fluency as well as ability to respond to comprehension questions in a relatively short amount of time (i.e., duration of interventions 5–17 hours).
Discussion
The Simple View consists of two distinct components, decoding and language comprehension, which together achieve reading comprehension. Children with ASD are often considered to be good rule-based or rote learners (Frith, 2003; Sigman, Dissanayake, Arbelle & Ruskin, 1997), and as a result phonics (i.e., decoding) may be a strength relative to meaning-focused skills. Research investigating the reading development of children with ASD indicates that similar to language development (Minshew, Goldstein & Siegel, 1995), when children with ASD develop reading skills, they can demonstrate good mechanics of reading or code-focused skills (i.e., word identification; Frith, 2003; Lord & Paul, 1997; Minshew, Goldstein, Taylor, & Seigel, 1994). Yet, consistent reports that children with ASD experience reading comprehension difficulties (Mayes & Calhoun, 2003a; 2003b; Nation et al., 2006; Minshew et al.; Wahlberg & Magliano, 2004) make it probable that despite good word recognition ability, many children with ASD will experience some difficulties with reading comprehension.
Because of the unstable reading profile associated with ASD (Nation et al., 2006), some learners will have difficulty developing both word reading and comprehension skills. Therefore, it is important that reading instruction emphasize both code- and meaning-focused skills. Although limited in number and variable in quality, the reviewed studies indicate that children with ASD can benefit from instruction in the five areas of reading recommended by the NRP as well as NRP advocated strategies. In combination these studies yield support for comprehensive reading instruction to include the five areas of reading with a focus on reading/language comprehension in the early grades.
Children with ASD may benefit from phonics instruction consistent with the NRP and offered through general education curriculum. For example, the NRP suggested teaching students how to identify sounds in words (i.e., phonemic awareness), map those sounds to each corresponding letter (i.e., phonics), and blend those sounds together to form words (NICHD, 2000). This approach is used in many comprehensive commercial reading programs (e.g., Reading Mastery, Open Court), and is similar to the nonverbal reading approach utilized in the Coleman et al. study (2005). Such approaches are direct and should be taught systematically by introducing all primary sound-letter relationships in a logical and sequential manner (e.g., teaching individual consonant and vowel letter sound relationships prior to blends and digraphs; Ehri, 2004).
Further, the NRP recommended authentic practice with developing code-focused skills in a number of different contexts that include connected text so that decoding is not interpreted as an isolated skill but rather part of the reading process (Ehri, 2004). One possible method or context for practicing code-focused skills is to supplement general education instruction with computer-assisted instruction. All four reviewed studies addressing code-focused skills utilized computer-assisted instruction, and results were promising suggesting this method a viable instructional modality. Evidence is insufficient to advocate using computer-assisted instruction as a sole instructional mode, but rather suggests this method can support and enhance the learning of children with ASD.
Also, providing children with ASD the opportunity to practice reading connected text aloud can increase reading fluency. In two of the included studies, children with ASD and their general education peers increased their oral reading fluency performance by repeatedly reading passages aloud and charting their progress (Kamps et al., 1994; 1989), a strategy advocated by the NRP (NICHD, 2000). In their review, the NRP found that oral reading fluency practice increased proficiency/fluency with code-focused skills permitting the reader to focus on constructing meaning.
Because children with ASD may develop decoding skills but continue to have trouble comprehending text, emphasis on meaning-focused skills from the early grades is an instructional priority. Five studies sought to enhance the meaning-focused reading skills of children with ASD by supplementing reading instruction with peer mediated activities (Dugan et al., 1995; Kamps et al., 1989; 1994; 1995; Whalon & Hanline, 2008). Overall, results suggest that peer mediated strategies, also recommended by the NRP, can be an effective instructional grouping to increase meaning-focused (i.e., vocabulary and/or comprehension) skills of some children with ASD as well as their general education peers. Some variability was noted in reading comprehension gains achieved by participants with autism and a moderate cognitive disability (Kamps et al., 1995), but a comprehension strategy was not directly taught. Only one study included direct instruction of a reading comprehension strategy (Whalon & Hanline). The authors utilized reciprocal questioning and children with ASD and their typically developing peers were able to learn a reading comprehension strategy simultaneously.
When considering the instructional methods used to increase meaning-focused skills, specifically, direct comprehension instruction, the lack of such interventions targeting individuals with ASD is surprising. That is, when teaching multi-method strategy instruction, it is recommended to initially simplify the task, provide scaffolding through modeling and think aloud, and implement procedural prompts (i.e., self-monitoring checklist, visual cue cards, generic questions; Rosenshine, Meister, & Chapman, 1996). Such strategies have been incorporated in a number of intervention studies targeting learners with ASD to teach a variety of skills. For instance, children with ASD have increased language development through storybook reading interventions that included modeling and scaffolding procedures (Bellon, Ogletree & Harn, 2000; Koppenhaver, Erickson, Harris et al., 2001; Koppenhaver, Erickson & Skotko, 2001). Also, to increase independence, social communication, and/or prosocial behaviors, researchers have incorporated the use of visuals (Krantz, MacDuff, & McClannahan, 1993; MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 1993), scripts (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993; Sarokoff, Taylor, & Poulson, 2001; Stevenson, Krantz, & McClannahan, 2000), and self-monitoring (Koegel & Frea, 1993; Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, & Frea, 1992; Mancina, Tankersley, Kamps, Kravits, & Parrett, 2000) during instruction. Consequently, it is possible that children with ASD can benefit from reading comprehension instruction recommended by the NRP.
For example, the NRP found question generation the single most effective reading comprehension strategy. A child with ASD can be taught to generate questions with a visual cue card paired with a script. This initial structure can be systematically faded to a visual cue paired with a signal word, a visual cue alone, and so forth until the child with ASD generates questions independently. Also, a self-monitoring checklist can be created to prompt the child with ASD when to stop and ask questions during reading. As when teaching a comprehension strategy to any child, the teacher will have to explain the purpose of the strategy, and model using the strategy with the intended prompts (i.e., visual cue cards, scripts, signal words, and/or self-monitoring checklist) as he/she thinks through the process aloud.
Another potential intervention to increase reading comprehension of children with ASD is the development of a cueing system during reading such as anaphoric cueing (O’Connor & Klein, 2004). Participants with ASD increased their ability to determine important elements of a story following cues built into text that helped the reader with ASD identify pronouns and corresponding referents. Cueing systems that help clarify abstract and decontextualized language such as anaphoric cueing are promising and need further study. Because of the importance of learning to monitor one’s own reading comprehension, future research should investigate the impact of teaching students with ASD to independently use the anaphoric cueing strategy, and compare the anaphoric cueing strategy with comprehension strategy instruction methods identified by the NRP. It is possible that to increase independence during reading, more instruction teaching identification of pronouns/referents in text as well as language may be needed.
Future Research
NCLB requires that all students have access to reading instruction consistent with reading research (NCLB, 2001). Yet, this review clearly demonstrates that research evaluating reading interventions for children with ASD is preliminary and therefore insufficient to guide practice. Consequently, it is critical for researchers to design and evaluate reading interventions based on the science of reading and responsive to the instructional needs of learners with ASD. The following five recommendations are suggested:
Evaluate the impact of comprehensive reading instruction on the reading development of children with ASD. Although the included studies investigated one or two components of reading instruction (i.e., phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, vocabulary, or comprehension), in practice instruction should be comprehensive to include all five. Future research should investigate how children with ASD respond to a comprehensive program in which each of the reading components is emphasized and explicitly linked making connections among the five components and the reading process more apparent (e.g., purposefully selecting text that contain sound-letter relationships being taught in phonemic awareness/phonics lessons to build reading fluency; providing opportunities to practice all five components in a number of different contexts and using a variety of texts, selecting vocabulary based on class readings). In addition, researchers should investigate what if any additional supports (e.g., visual cues, self-monitoring checklists, peer mediated instruction) children with ASD need to participate in and benefit from a comprehensive reading program.
To ensure quality and rigor, researchers should adhere to the Quality Indicators for Research Methodology and Evidence Based Practices established by the Council for Exceptional Children (CEC) Division for Research (Odom et al., 2004). For example, to realize the extent gains are made by individual participants, researchers should include repeated, social validly, and treatment fidelity measures. Future single subject research studies should include measures in the context of the reading activity as well as on more distant weekly pre and post-test measures. Continual measures are important in single subject research because such measures establish a pattern of behavior or instances of deviations from a pattern thereby producing more detailed information for evaluation (McCormick, 1995). It is also important that generalization is adequately addressed.
To gain a better understanding of how individuals with various characteristics spanning the autism spectrum benefit from reading instruction, researchers must include detailed descriptions of participant characteristics (i.e., reading development at the onset of the study, language ability, academic achievement, IQ scores), any individualization of instruction, and duration and intensity of instruction needed to facilitate progress.
Future research should extend the literature base reviewed here. For example, how can computer assisted instruction supplement a comprehensive reading program? How can different instructional arrangements (e.g., one to one, whole group, small group, peer-mediated) be combined to facilitate reading acquisition?
Future research should investigate not only the effects comprehension strategy interventions on reading comprehension but also language, social communication, and engagement levels of children with ASD.
Conclusion
Research addressing the reading skills of children with ASD is just beginning. Researchers should continue this line of inquiry with an emphasis on interventions that address the five areas of reading. In the meantime, teachers should provide reading instruction from the early grades that is consistent with recommendations made by the NRP. Preliminary evidence suggests that learners with ASD can make gains in reading skills when intervention is provided.
Contributor Information
Kelly J. Whalon, The College of William and Mary
Stephanie Al Otaiba, Florida State University.
Monica E. Delano, University of Louisville
References
- *.Basil C, Reyes S. Acquisition of literacy skills by children with severe disability. Child Language Teaching and Therapy. 2003;19:27–48. [Google Scholar]
- Bellon ML, Ogletree BT, Harn WE. Repeated storybook reading as a language intervention for children with autism: A case study on the application of scaffolding. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2000;15:52–58. [Google Scholar]
- Browder D, Courtade-Little G, Wakeman S, Rickelman R. From sight words to emergent literacy. In: Browder D, Spooner F, editors. Teaching reading, math, and science to students with significant cognitive disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes; 2006. pp. 63–91. [Google Scholar]
- Browder DM, Wakeman SY, Spooner F, Ahlgrim-Delzell L, Algozzine B. Research on reading instruction for individuals with significant cognitive disabilities. Council for Exceptional Children. 2006;72:391–408. [Google Scholar]
- Calhoon JA. Factors affecting the reading of rimes in words and nonwords in beginning readers with cognitive disabilities and typically developing readers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2001;31:491–504. doi: 10.1023/a:1012268909286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Catts HW, Kamhi AG. Classification of reading disabilities. In: Catts HW, Kamhi AG, editors. Language and reading disabilities. 2. Boston: Pearson; 2005. pp. 72–93. [Google Scholar]
- Colasent R, Griffith PL. Autism and literacy: Looking into the classroom with rabbit stories. The Reading Teacher. 1998;51:414–420. [Google Scholar]
- Colby KM. The rationale for computer-based treatment of language difficulties in nonspeaking autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia. 1973;3:261–273. doi: 10.1007/BF01538283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Coleman-Martin MB, Heller KW, Cihak DF, Irvine KL. Using computer assisted instruction and the nonverbal reading approach to teach word identification. Focus on autism and other developmental disabilities. 2005;20:80–90. [Google Scholar]
- Conners FA. Reading instruction for students with moderate mental retardation: Review and analysis of research. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 1992;96:577–597. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Dugan E, Kamps D, Leonard B, Watkins N, Rheinberger A, Stackhaus J. Effects of cooperative learning groups during social studies for students with autism and fourth-grade peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1995;28:175–188. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1995.28-175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ehri LC. Teaching phonemic awareness and phonics: An explanation of the National Reading Panel meta-analyses. In: McCardle P, Chhabra V, editors. The voice of evidence in reading research. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes; 2004. pp. 153–186. [Google Scholar]
- Frith U. Autism: Explaining the enigma. 2. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Gough PB, Hoover WA, Peterson CL. Some observations on a simple view of reading. In: Cornoldi C, Oakhill J, editors. Reading comprehension difficulties: Process and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1996. pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]
- Griswold E, Barnhill GP, Myles BS, Hagiwara T, Simpson RI. Asperger syndrome and academic achievement. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2002;17:94–103. [Google Scholar]
- *.Heimann M, Nelson KE, Tjus T, Gillberg C. Increasing reading and communication skills in children with autism through an interactive multimedia computer program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1995;25:459–480. doi: 10.1007/BF02178294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hewett FM. Teaching reading to an autistic boy through operant conditioning. The Reading Teacher. 1964;17:613–618. [Google Scholar]
- Houston D, Al Otaiba S, Torgesen JK. Learning to read: Phonics and fluency. In: Browder D, Spooner F, editors. Teaching reading, math, and science to students with significant cognitive disabilities. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes; 2006. pp. 93–123. [Google Scholar]
- Kamhi AG. Finding beauty in the ugly facts about reading comprehension. In: Catts HW, Kamhi AG, editors. The connections between language and reading disabilities. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2005. pp. 201–212. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kamps DM, Barbetta PM, Leonard BR, Delquadri J. Classwide peer tutoring: An integration strategy to improve reading skills and promote peer interactions among students with autism and general education peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1994;27:49–61. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-49. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- *.Kamps DM, Leonard B, Potucek J, Garrison-Harrell LG. Cooperative learning groups in reading: An integration strategy for students with autism and general classroom peers. Behavioral Disorders. 1995;21:89–109. [Google Scholar]
- *.Kamps D, Locke P, Delquadri J, Hall RV. Increasing academic skills of students with autism using fifth grade peers as tutors. Education and Treatment of Children. 1989;12:38–51. [Google Scholar]
- Koegel RL, Frea WD. Treatment of social behavior in autism through the modification of pivotal social skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1993;26:369–377. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koegel LK, Koegel RL, Hurley C, Frea WD. Improving social skills and disruptive behavior in children with autism through self-management. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1992;25:341–353. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1992.25-341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koppenhaver DA, Erickson KA, Harris B, McLellan J, Skotko BG, Newton RA. Storybook-based communication intervention for girls with Rett syndrome and their mothers. Disability and Rehabilitation. 2001;23:149–159. doi: 10.1080/09638280150504225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koppenhaver DA, Erickson KA, Skotko BG. Supporting communication of girls with Rett Syndrome and their mothers in storybook reading. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education. 2001;48:395–410. [Google Scholar]
- Krantz PJ, MacDuff MT, McClannahan LE. Programming participation in family activities for children with autism: Parents’ use of photographic activity schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1993;26:137–139. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Teaching children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1993;26:121–132. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lanquetot R. Autistic children and reading. The Reading Teacher. 1984;38:182–186. [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Paul R. Language and communication in ASD. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, editors. Handbook of ASD and pervasive developmental disorders. 2. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1997. pp. 195–225. [Google Scholar]
- Ludlow AK, Wilkins AJ, Heaton P. The effect of coloured overlays on reading ability in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36:507–516. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0090-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDuff GS, Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Teaching children with autism to use photographic activity schedules: Maintenance and generalization of complex response chains. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1993;26:86–97. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mancina C, Tankersley M, Kamps D, Kravits T, Parrett J. Brief report: Reduction of inappropriate vocalizations for a child with autism using a self-management treatment program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2000;30:599–606. doi: 10.1023/a:1005695512163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Ability profiles in children with autism: Influence of age and IQ. Autism. 2003a;6:65–80. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007001006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayes SD, Calhoun SL. Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-Binet: IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2003b;33:329–341. doi: 10.1023/a:1024462719081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCormick S. What is single-subject experimental research? In: Neuman SB, McCormick S, editors. Single-subject experimental research: Applications for Literacy. Newark, DE: International Reading Association; 1995. pp. 1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Speech and language in high-functioning autistic individuals. Neuropsychology. 1995;9:255–261. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Taylor HG, Siegel DJ. Academic achievement in high functioning autistic individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 1994;16:261–270. doi: 10.1080/01688639408402637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myles BS, Hilgenfeld TD, Barnhill G, Griswold D, Hagiwara T, Simpson RL. Analysis of reading skills in individuals with Asperger’s syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. 2002;17:44–47. [Google Scholar]
- Nation K, Clarke P, Wright B, Williams C. Patterns of reading ability in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2006;36:911–919. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0130-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction (NIH Publication No. 00-4769) Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office; 2000. Report of the National Reading Panel. [Google Scholar]
- No Child Left Behind (NCLB) Act, 20 U. S. C. § 6301 et seq. 2001).
- Oakhill JV, Cain K, Bryant PE. The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and cognitive processes. 2003;18:443–468. [Google Scholar]
- *.O’Connor IM, Klein PD. Exploration of strategies for facilitating the reading comprehension of high-functioning students with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2004;14:115–127. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000022603.44077.6b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odom SL, Brantlinger E, Gersten R, Horner RD, Thompson B, Harris K. Quality indicators for research in special education and guidelines for evidence-based practices: Executive Summary. Division for Research: Council for Exceptional Children. 2004. Fall. Retrieved July 1, 2007, from http://www.cecdr.org/news.cfm?id=D9775CBD-C09F-1D6F-F990DE584CD46125.
- Perfetti CA, Landi N, Oakhill J. The acquisition of reading comprehension skill. In: Snowling MJ, Hulme C, editors. The science of reading: A handbook. Malden, MA: Blackwell; 2005. pp. 227–247. [Google Scholar]
- *.Rosenbaum MS, Breiling J. The development and functional control of reading comprehension behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1976;9:323–333. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1976.9-323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenshine B, Meister C, Chapman S. Teaching students to generate questions: A review of the intervention studies. Review of Educational Research. 1996;66:181–211. [Google Scholar]
- Sarakoff RA, Taylor BA, Poulson CL. Teaching children with autism to engage in conversational exchanges: Script fading with embedded textual stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2001;34:81–84. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2001.34-81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sigman M, Dissanayake C, Arbelle S, Ruskin E. Cognition and emotion in children and adolescents with autism. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, editors. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. 2. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1997. pp. 248–265. [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson CL, Krantz PJ, McClannahan LE. Social interaction skills for children with autism: A script-fading procedure for nonreaders. Behavioral Interventions. 2000;15:1–20. [Google Scholar]
- *.Tjus T, Heimann M, Nelson KE. Gains in literacy through the use of a specially developed multimedia computer strategy. Autism. 1998;2:139–156. [Google Scholar]
- Wahlberg T, Magliano JP. The ability of high function individuals with autism to comprehend written discourse. Discourse Processes. 2004;38:119–144. [Google Scholar]
- *.Whalon KJ, Hanline MF. Effects of a reciprocal questioning intervention on the question generation and responding of children with autism spectrum disorder. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities. 2008;432:367–387. [Google Scholar]