Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jan 20.
Published in final edited form as: Psychol Aging. 2005 Sep;20(3):493–506. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.493

Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analyses of NEO-PI-R Scales In the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging

Antonio Terracciano 1, Robert R McCrae 2, Larry J Brant 3, Paul T Costa Jr 4
PMCID: PMC2808690  NIHMSID: NIHMS110335  PMID: 16248708

Abstract

We examined age trends in the five factors and 30 facets assessed by the Revised NEO Personality Inventory in Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging data (N = 1,944; 5,027 assessments) collected between 1989 and 2004. Consistent with cross-sectional results, Hierarchical Linear Modeling analyses showed gradual personality changes in adulthood: a decline up to age 80 in Neuroticism, stability and then decline in Extraversion, decline in Openness, increase in Agreeableness, and increase up to age 70 in Conscientiousness. Some facets showed different curves from the factor they define. Birth cohort effects were modest, and there were no consistent Gender × Age interactions. Significant non-normative changes were found for all five factors; they were not explained by attrition but might be due to genetic factors, disease, or life experience.

Keywords: Five-Factor Model, personality change, aging, longitudinal study, HLM, gender


For the better part of a century, psychologists have investigated stability and change in adult personality (Helson, 1999; Kelly, 1955; Soldz & Vaillant, 1999). In the early 1980s, findings of consistent rank-order stability in personality traits emerged as some of the strongest evidence for the existence of traits themselves. Cross-sectional, longitudinal, and cross-sequential analyses of mean level personality change in adulthood suggested that personality changed little, and Costa and McCrae (1988) concluded that "personality is stable after age 30" (p. 853).

Many researchers (Helson, Jones, & Kwan, 2002; Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Piedmont, 2001; Srivastava, John, Gosling, & Potter, 2003) have questioned that position, and, based on subsequent research, McCrae and Costa (2003) extended and qualified their position. There are notable normative changes between late adolescence and age 30: Neuroticism (N) and Extraversion (E) decline, Agreeableness (A) and Conscientiousness (C) increase, and Openness (O) first increases, then declines. After age 30, both longitudinal (Costa, Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000) and cross-sectional (McCrae et al., 1999) analyses suggest that there are continuing normative declines in N, E, and O, albeit at a modest rate of 1 to 2 T-score points (0.1 to 0.2 SD) per decade.

Despite decades of research, many uncertainties remain in the description of age changes. Developmental trends for A and C are unclear, because these factors appear to increase with age in cross-sectional analyses, but did not increase in a large-scale longitudinal analysis (Costa et al., 2000). Even less is known about longitudinal trends for the specific traits or facets of the five factors; facets of A and C have been studied longitudinally only over a six-year period in middle-aged men and women (Costa et al., 2000). Little is known about age trends in extreme old age: The few studies that focus on this segment of the lifespan have found inconsistent results (see, Weiss et al., 2005).

The present study analyzes new longitudinal data to refine the description of age changes in the broad dimensions and specific facets of the Five-Factor Model (FFM; McCrae & John, 1992) of personality. By using a comprehensive model of personality and employing a multilevel modeling approach on a large sample with over 5000 assessments, this study attempts to clarify and refine the description of the longitudinal course of personality trait development in adulthood.

Mean Level Changes and Developmental Curves

Cross-sectional studies are a convenient way to estimate age changes, provided that birth cohort and sampling biases can be ruled out. Longitudinal studies, which use subjects as their own controls, are the most effective way of ruling out such biases, and are thus a natural complement to cross-sectional studies (although they are themselves susceptible to time-of-measurement and attrition biases). Since 1989, Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging (BLSA) participants have completed the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992) by computer administration.1 Older participants have been tested repeatedly, and new participants have joined the study continuously. As a result, considerable data have been collected, but they vary in retest interval and number of administrations per individual. To maximize information from this dataset, we will use Hierarchical Linear Modeling (HLM; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) analyses to supplement cross-sectional analyses and describe developmental curves for personality traits in adulthood.

HLM and other multilevel modeling approaches are increasingly being used to examine intra-individual age trajectories, to define normative trends, and to investigate individual variations around those normative trends (Hertzog & Nesselroade, 2003). Mroczek and Spiro (2003), in a sample of 1600 men from the Normative Aging Study, found curvilinear slopes for N, which declined up to age 80, and for E, which increased in the younger cohort and declined in the older cohort. They also found significant individual variation around these normative trends. Using HLM analysis on data spanning over 40 years of adulthood from two relatively small samples, Helson et al. (2002) found small to moderate increases with age on some measures of norm-adherence, a curvilinear trend on measures of social assurance, and declines on social vitality (see also Jones, Livson, & Peskin, 2003). They found evidence of individual differences around normative trends and argued that life events and cultural climate shape the growth curves of personality traits. Steunenberg, Twisk, Beekman, Deeg, and Kerkhof (2005) in a sample of 2,117 respondents aged 55–85 from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam found a U-shaped curve for N. We will use HLM analyses to describe normative age changes and to investigate two variables (birth cohort and sex) that may moderate them. This is the first study that uses HLM to analyze trends for the five broad dimensions and the 30 lower-order facets of the NEO-PI-R.

Although measures related to N and E have been included in most studies of aging and personality, there is reason to continue research on these dimensions. Meta-analyses of birth cohort (or time-of-measurement) effects in children and college students suggest that Americans have shifted toward substantially higher levels of N and E during recent decades (Twenge, 2000, 2001). Successive generations of college students have shown an increase in N scores of nearly one full SD over the interval from 1952 to 1993, and a similar increase in E from 1966 to 1993. Most of those students were born between 1932 and 1973, as were members of the BLSA who were 20 to 60 at the time of their first computer-administered NEO-PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992). If Twenge’s findings represent enduring birth cohort effects, cross-sectional studies should show lower scores for older generations in N and E, because older participants were born in an earlier era of lower N and E. If Twenge’s findings represent transient time-of-measurement effects (as if high N and E had become more “fashionable” over the years), and if that trend has continued since 1993, longitudinal studies should show increases in N and E. We test both these hypotheses.

Developmental changes in adult personality may differ for men and women. Some theorists have hypothesized that gender differences in personality traits are most pronounced in young adulthood and tend to diminish or even reverse during later life, towards a "normal androgyny" (Gutmann, 1987). Although such changes could be biologically based, they are usually seen as the consequence of an age-related shift in sex-role demands. Wink and Helson (1993) found women were less competent, more emotionally dependent, and higher in succorance than men in young adulthood, but those differences were attenuated or reversed in middle adulthood. Srivastava, John, Gosling, and Potter (2003) also reported Age × Gender interactions but chiefly on N, with men stable and women declining cross-sectionally (see also, Small, Hertzog, Hultsch, & Dixon, 2003; Viken, Rose, Kaprio, & Koskenvuo, 1994). By contrast, most other studies have found similar age curves for men and women (e.g., McCrae & Costa, in press). We examine Age × Gender interactions in BLSA data.

Method

Participants and Procedure

The sample consisted of 1,944 community-dwelling volunteers from the BLSA, an ongoing multidisciplinary study of normal aging administered by the National Institute on Aging. The BLSA begun in 1958 with a convenience sample of men, mostly college educated and predominantly white individuals. Later recruitment has helped balance the sample by including minorities and, since 1978, women. Participants are generally healthy and highly educated (M = 16.2 years of education, SD = 2.6); the present sample is 70.3% Caucasian, 22.3% African American, and 7.4% other or unknown. There were 977 men and 967 women. Data were collected between September, 1989, and July, 2004, during regularly scheduled visits. Mean age at first NEO-PI-R administration was 58.8 years (SD = 16.8) for men, and 54.5 years (SD = 16.9) for women. Mean age at most recent NEO-PI-R administration was 63.7 years (SD = 18.1) for men, and 58.5 years (SD = 18.4) for women. Across the span of the study, age ranged from 20 to 96 years (M = 65.5, SD = 15.8), but most data were from participants aged over 60 (see Figure 1). Older individuals were assessed more frequently than younger; the mean interval between administrations was 2.8 years (SD = 2.2). Participants completed from 1 to 11 administrations (M = 2.6; SD = 1.9); the 1,944 participants provided a total of 5,027 assessments (Appendix Table A shows the frequency of administrations). Individuals with only one administration were younger [t(1,942) = 16.5; p < .01, d = .72], more likely to be women (56%, χ2(1) = 18.47, p < .01), and slightly less educated [t(1,688) = 2.4; p < .05, d = .13] than individuals with multiple administrations. After correcting for age and sex, there were no significant differences between these two groups on any of the five factors.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Number of personality assessment points obtained by age.

The BLSA attempts to maintain maximum participation from each individual with a lifelong commitment. Most participants with at least one NEO-PI-R administration are still in the study (72%); some are deceased (11%). Drop-outs consist of 4% who have formally withdrawn (although they are willing to participate by phone, mail, and/or home visits), 1% lost to follow-up, and 12% at least one year past their due date. At their first NEO-PI-R administration, individuals that later dropped out of the study (17%) were slightly older [t(1,1718) = −2.4; p < .05, d = .15], less educated [t(1,1467) = 2.8; p < .01, d = .17], and higher on N [t(1,1718) = −3.4; p < .01, d = .20] than active participants (72%). There were no differences on the other factors.

Measure

The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992), designed to measure the Five-Factor Model of personality, was used to assess self-reported personality traits. The NEO-PI-R consists of 240 items answered on a five-point Likert format ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The NEO-PI-R assesses 30 facets, six for each dimension of the FFM (see Table 1 for a listing of the 30 facet scales). Raw scores were standardized as T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10) using combined-sex norms reported in the Manual; factor scores combining information from each of the 30 facets are also expressed as T-scores (Costa & McCrae, 1992, Table 2). In the current sample (on the first assessment for each participant) the internal consistencies for the five domains were .91, .87, .87, .88, and .92 respectively for N, E, O, A, and C. Factor structure in this sample shows high congruence with the normative structure (Tucker's phis = .97 to .99). The NEO-PI-R has been translated into several languages and used in more than 50 cultures (McCrae et al., 2005). Additional evidence on convergent and discriminant validity is presented in the Manual (Costa & McCrae, 1992), including cross-observer agreement and prediction of external criteria such as psychological well-being, needs and motivation, creativity, educational and occupational achievements, and coping mechanisms.

Table 1.

Cross-Sectional Regression Coefficients Predicting NEO-PI-R Factors and Facets from Age in Decades.

Factor/Facet R2 β0 β1
N: Neuroticisma 0.019* 52.3 (.77) −0.79 (.13)
E: Extraversiona 0.045* 56.7 (.77) −1.24 (.13)
O: Openness 0.035* 60.2 (.82) −1.16 (.14)
A: Agreeableness 0.039* 43.5 (.76) 1.15 (.13)
C: Conscientiousnessa 0.002* 51.8 (.84) 0.29 (.14)
N1: Anxietya 0.010* 51.5 (.77) −0.57 (.13)
N2: Angry Hostilitya 0.014* 52.5 (.76) −0.68 (.13)
N3: Depressiona 0.004* 50.0 (.77) −0.39 (.13)
N4: Self-Consciousnessa 0.003* 50.2 (.79) −0.32 (.13)
N5: Impulsiveness 0.029* 54.5 (.74) −0.95 (.13)
N6: Vulnerabilitya 0.003* 46.1 (.78) 0.32 (.13)
E1: Warmtha 0.000 50.8 (.79) −0.05 (.13)
E2: Gregariousness 0.000 51.4 (.83) −0.10 (.14)
E3: Assertivenessa 0.008* 55.9 (.79) −0.52 (.13)
E4: Activitya 0.085* 61.9 (.78) −1.78 (.13)
E5: Excitement-Seekinga 0.081* 57.4 (.72) −1.59 (.12)
E6: Positive Emotionsa 0.063* 59.2 (.81) −1.56 (.14)
O1: Fantasy 0.095* 62.4 (.80) −1.92 (.13)
O2: Aesthetics 0.001 54.3 (.81) −0.14 (.14)
O3: Feelings 0.064* 59.8 (.79) −1.54 (.13)
O4: Actions 0.027* 57.1 (.84) −1.03 (.14)
O5: Ideasa 0.015* 56.1 (.80) −0.74 (.14)
O6: Values 0.042* 58.1 (.80) −1.24 (.13)
A1: Trusta 0.042* 45.0 (.74) 1.16 (.13)
A2: Straightforwardness 0.020* 45.6 (.76) 0.82 (.13)
A3: Altruisma 0.001 51.2 (.78) −0.18 (.13)
A4: Compliance 0.038* 43.7 (.79) 1.18 (.13)
A5: Modesty 0.000 47.0 (.78) 0.13 (.14)
A6: Tender-Mindednessa 0.000 49.9 (.78) 0.06 (.13)
C1: Competencea 0.016* 57.0 (.81) −0.77 (.14)
C2: Ordera 0.001 49.0 (.82) −0.14 (.14)
C3: Dutifulnessa 0.019* 46.2 (.74) 0.77 (.12)
C4: Achievement Strivinga 0.004* 53.1 (.86) −0.41 (.15)
C5: Self-Disciplinea 0.000 48.5 (.86) −0.10 (.15)
C6: Deliberationa 0.002 50.0 (.79) 0.26 (.13)

Note. N = 1,942. β0 = intercept; β1 = linear slope. Standard errors are given in parenthesis.

*

p < .05.

a

Significant quadratic components. Quadratic coefficients are available in Appendix Table C.

Table 2.

HLM Coefficients and Variance Estimates of Intercept, Linear, and Quadratic Equations Predicting NEO-PI-R Factors and Facets from Age in Decades

σ2 Residual
Within-
Subject Intercept Linear Quadratic



NEO-PI-R scale Variance γ00: M γ01: Gender γ02: Cohort u0: Variance γ10: M u1: Variance γ20: M u2: Variance
N: Neuroticism 12.45 40.8 (.67)** 3.43 (.40)** −0.75 (.22)** 70.42** −0.795 (.19)** 7.88** .338 (.05)** .37*
E: Extraversion 9.55 47.1 (.67)** 1.57 (.42)** 81.07** −1.489 (.10)** 6.45* −.271 (.05)** .32
O: Openness 12.92 47.7 (.70)** 3.39 (.45)** 82.27** −1.047 (.11)** 2.48**
A: Agreeableness 12.47 41.8 (.64)** 6.28 (.40)** 72.68** 1.122 (.11)** 5.75** −.115 (.05)* .29*
C: Conscientiousness 13.04 51.6 (.31)** 1.13 (.23)** 96.16** 0.145 (.19) 7.10* −.362 (.05)** .44*
N1: Anxiety 20.14 42.2 (.67)** 2.91 (.41)** −0.76 (.24)** 67.83** −0.648 (.21)**a 6.01* .321 (.05)** .23
N2: Angry Hostility 21.03 46.7 (.29)** −0.99 (.24)** 66.36** −1.186 (.22)** 5.74** .268 (.05)** .31*
N3: Depression 21.17 42.8 (.65)** 1.72 (.39)** −1.39 (.24)** 56.79** −0.804 (.22)** 7.00* .507 (.05)** .26
N4: Self-Consciousness 23.45 44.8 (.67)** 1.35 (.40)** −0.84 (.25)** 67.45** −0.566 (.23)* 6.42 .299 (.05)** .41
N5: Impulsiveness 25.76 48.3 (.20)** 52.91** −0.975 (.11)** 2.27**
N6: Vulnerability 21.03 41.9 (.67)** 2.63 (.40)** −1.69 (.25)** 68.00** −0.271 (.22) 7.21 .530 (.05)** .45
E1: Warmth 20.43 46.4 (.66)** 3.21 (.42)** 73.35** −0.445 (.12)** 5.40 −.307 (.05)** .24
E2: Gregariousness 18.21 49.0 (.71)** 1.46 (.45)** 85.91** −0.211 (.12) 7.22 −.154 (.05)** .23
E3: Assertiveness 15.78 56.2 (.72)** −1.41 (.44)** 1.09 (.24)** 87.98** −0.107 (.20) 6.20 −.335 (.05)** .35
E4: Activity 20.10 49.4 (.68)** 1.29 (.42)** 83.01** −2.340 (.12)** 5.81 −.378 (.05)** .38
E5: Excitement-Seeking 16.93 52.7 (.62)** −4.08 (.39)** 66.49** −1.361 (.11)** 5.26 .193 (.05)** .29
E6: Positive Emotions 21.14 46.0 (.70)** 3.17 (.43)** 1.07 (.25)** 76.98** −0.876 (.21)** 4.63* −.302 (.05)** .23*
O1: Fantasy 22.38 50.9 (.31)** 1.21 (.25)** 78.34** −0.477 (.22)* 6.26* .096 (.05) .36
O2: Aesthetics 16.59 44.9 (.67)** 5.69 (.43)** 73.13** −0.088 (.11) 2.55**
O3: Feelings 25.14 43.8 (.65)** 4.44 (.41)** 67.14** −1.430 (.12)*b 5.00** −.116 (.05)* .18
O4: Actions 25.48 43.1 (.71)** 5.10 (.44)** 89.17** −1.520 (.12)** 5.46 −.261 (.05)** .38
O5: Ideas 18.57 53.8 (.70)** −1.31 (.44)** 0.72 (.22)** 77.01** −0.107 (.19) 1.22**
O6: Values 24.19 47.3 (.68)** 1.42 (.43)** −0.69 (.24)** 70.20** −1.933 (.21)** 0.57**
A1: Trust 19.04 48.6 (.64)** 2.52 (.38)** −1.01 (.24)** 62.15** −0.081 (.21) 7.26 −.233 (.05)** .47
A2: Straightforwardness 26.21 45.3 (.61)** 3.70 (.39)** 56.60** 0.799 (.10)** 0.74**
A3: Altruism 25.41 45.7 (.67)** 3.86 (.40)** 1.01 (.25)** 61.71** 0.454 (.23)* 4.36 −.276 (.05)** .18
A4: Compliance 25.30 45.3 (.66)** 4.05 (.42)** 69.70** 1.324 (.11) 0.28**
A5: Modesty 22.77 42.8 (.65)** 3.31 (.42)** 66.47** 0.194 (.11)b 1.38**
A6: Tender-Mindedness 29.11 45.0 (.69)** 4.14 (.41)** 0.78 (.27)** 68.23** 0.621 (.25)* 6.35 −.173 (.06)** .39
C1: Competence 28.21 55.1 (.31)** 2.30 (.27)** 68.37** 0.503 (.24)* 6.36 −.577 (.06)** .37
C2: Order 20.82 48.6 (.28)** 85.45** −0.362 (.12)** 6.79** −.184 (.06)** .39*
C3: Dutifulness 24.01 52.1 (.24)** 58.01** 0.239 (.11)*a 5.23 −.347 (.05)** .38
C4: Achievement Striving 22.15 51.2 (.32)** 0.74 (.26)** 86.83** −0.125 (.23) 8.83 −.177 (.06) .44
C5: Self-Discipline 21.51 49.0 (.28)** 89.79** −0.799 (.13)** 11.35 −.471 (.06)** .73
C6: Deliberation 20.78 53.6 (.30)** 1.91 (.25)** 70.77** 1.697 (.23)** 8.85 −.219 (.06)** .56

Note. N = 1,944. Standard errors are shown in parenthesis. Significance of coefficients is tested by t ratio (coefficients/Standard error). Significance of variance terms is tested with chi-square. Coefficients are applied to (Age – Mean Age) in decades. Gender: 1 = Male, 2 = Female. Cohort is year of birth centered around the grand mean (1938).

a

Significant cohort effect on linear change.

b

Significant gender effect on linear change.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01.

Some of the data on which the present study is based have been used for other purposes, including correlations at one time point with other variables (see Author Note) and two-year retest correlations (N = 338) reported in McCrae, Yik, Trapnell, Bond, & Paulhus (1998) as an estimate of retest reliability (rtts = .83 to .91 for domains, .64 to .86 for facets). However, none of these data have been used to investigate longitudinal trends in mean trait levels.

Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analyses

HLM is a flexible approach that can be applied to evaluate individuals’ growth trajectories. In HLM analyses the number and spacing of measurement observations may vary across persons, given that the time-series observations in each individual are viewed as nested within a person. Even data from individuals who were tested only on a single occasion can be used to stabilize estimates of mean and variance. In this way, all available data can be included in analyses. This is a major advantage of conducting analysis within the HLM framework; by contrast, missing data and varying timing pose major problems in conventional repeated measures analysis of variance.

Our data fit a so-called accelerated longitudinal design, in which longitudinal data are collected on members of different birth cohorts. With this design we can estimate age-trajectories over a broad age span using data collected in a shorter time interval. Conceptually, HLM describes the growth curve of each individual and pieces these curves together to estimate an overall age trajectory. Statistically, it uses sophisticated methods to reduce error of measurement and to make fullest possible use of the information in the data (Brant & Verbeke, 1997; Verbeke & Molenberghs, 2000).

HLM is hierarchical because it allows modeling both within- and between-individual variations. At level-1, each individual’s data are fitted to a regression line (linear, quadratic, or other higher-order models). Level-1 coefficients are empirical Bayesian estimates, which are optimal estimates based on data from the individual and the entire population. Individual data are weighted by the number of data points and the reliability of the regression. This approach tends to shrink the individuals’ coefficients toward the population means; the degree of shrinkage is inversely proportional to the reliability of the individual data. Variance and covariance components are estimated through maximum likelihood procedures (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).

In the level-2 equations, the outcome/dependent variables are the level-1 regression coefficients and the independent variables are the characteristics of level-2 units, the individuals. At level-2, individual difference variables, such as cohort and sex, are used to explain between-subjects variation in the intercept and the linear slope.

To evaluate the longitudinal trajectories, we first defined the level-1 model, and then tested possible level-2 predictors. At level-1 linear and then quadratic models were tested. The quadratic model was chosen when it provided better fit according to the chi-square test of deviance at p < .01. After the level-1 model was determined, year of birth in decades (as a continuous variable) and sex were entered in the model as level-2 variables. Level-2 variables were retained if they improved the fit of the model, or in other words, explained a significant amount of the variance in mean intercept or slope. Age was centered on the grand mean (M = 64.5 years) to minimize the correlation between the linear and quadratic terms. At level-2, year of birth was centered on its grand mean (1938).

Results

Cross-Sectional Analyses of Age Differences in Mean Levels

Cross-sectional analyses were performed on the first administration available for each subject. Descriptive statistics for six age groups (20 to 39.9, 40 to 49.9, 50 to 59.9, 60 to 69.9, 70 to 79.9, and 80 to 100) are shown in Appendix Table B. The results of linear regressions for the five factors are reported in Table 1, and results of significant linear (for O and A) or curvilinear effects (for N, E, and C) are depicted with dashed lines in Figures 2a, 3a, 4a, 5a, and 6a. The linear slopes suggest that age differences amount to about one T-score point per decade. The Figures show that the N curve was concave, whereas the E curve was convex, with older individuals scoring increasingly lower. Older individuals were lower on O and higher on A. The C curve was convex. Age and age-squared together accounted for from 1.3% of the variance in C to 5.3% in E.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

a–b. Estimated T-scores for Neuroticism and its facets from 30 to 90 years.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

a–b. Estimated T-scores for Extraversion and its facets from 30 to 90 years.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

a–b. Estimated T-scores for Openness and its facets from 30 to 90 years.

Figure 5.

Figure 5

a–b. Estimated T-scores for Agreeableness and its facets from 30 to 90 years.

Figure 6.

Figure 6

a–b. Estimated T-scores for Conscientiousness and its facets from 30 to 90 years.

These cross-sectional results do not support the hypothesis that Twenge’s (2000; 2001) findings reflect enduring birth cohort effects. Her college student samples were born in the same years as BLSA participants aged 20–60. As Figure 2a shows, within that portion of the curve, later-born cohorts do have higher scores on N, but the effect is only about one-third as large as the effect Twenge reported. Furthermore, later-born cohorts are younger at time of testing, so their higher N score is probably due to their younger age. Figure 3a shows that there is an even smaller difference in E across these cohorts. (Srivastava et al., 2003, also failed to find large cross-sectional age differences in these factors.)

Linear regression results for the 30 facet scales are reported in Table 1. The slopes of the facets generally correspond to the trajectory of the factors they define. However, the strength of association with age varied across facets, and some showed a distinct trajectory. The largest effects were found for for E4: Activity, E5: Excitement Seeking, E6: Positive Emotions, O1: Fantasy, and O3: Feelings, which all are lower in older individuals. The largest positive slopes are found for A1: Trust, A2: Straightforwardness, and A4: Compliance. The pattern of age differences resembles that found in other cultures: The rank-order correlation of the slopes in Table 1 with the cross-sectional age trends found in 12 other cultures (see McCrae & Costa, in press) across the 30 facets is .73 (p < .001).

In this large sample, quadratic terms were significant for 20 of the 30 facets. But most accounted for less than 1% of additional variance: Curvilinear effects accounting for an additional 1% to 3% of the variance were found only for N3: Depression and N6: Vulnerability, which showed a nadir around age 50–60, and for C1: Competence and C5: Self-Discipline, which peaked at age 40–50.

Finally, we conducted regression analyses to determine whether there were Age × Gender interactions. Significant effects were found for overall N, N2: Angry Hostility, and O3: Feelings. In all three, the cross-sectional decline was more marked for women than for men. The effects, however, accounted for less than one-quarter of 1% of the variance in personality scores.

HLM Analyses of Change in Mean Levels

We used between- and within-individual variance estimates from the basic one-way ANOVA with random effects model (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002, p. 24) to estimate the proportion of the stable variance in personality traits. The proportion of stable variance can be computed as the ratio of between-subjects variance (u0: intercept variance) to the total variance (u0: intercept variance + σ2: within-subject variance). Of the total variability across persons and occasions, 84%, 88%, 88%, 85%, and 87% for N, E, O, A, and C respectively, was between-persons (individual differences), and the remaining portion was within-persons (intra-individual variability).

For the five factors and the 30 facets, Table 2 provides HLM estimates for the final model. Fixed and random effects for the intercept, linear, and quadratic terms are reported, along with standard errors. Cohort (date of birth) was a significant predictor of the intercept of N and C, with later-born cohorts having lower N and higher C. Note that these effects are net of the estimated effects of age (the level-1 predictor). Gender was a significant predictor of the intercept for all factors except C, with women scoring higher than men (Costa, Terracciano, & McCrae, 2001). The random effects (u0: variance) associated with the intercepts of the five factors, which reflect individual differences in personality traits, were all statistically significant. These between-individuals’ variances are substantially higher than residual within-individuals’ variances (σ2). The proportions of between-subjects variance for the five factors are also higher than comparable proportions found in other multilevel modeling studies of personality (Mroczek and Spiro, 2003; Steunberg et al., 2005), which suggests that personality traits appear to be more stable when assessed with the longer and more reliable NEO-PI-R factors.

The quadratic terms are presented only when the quadratic model fit the data better than the linear model. In those cases, the linear terms are the coefficients from the quadratic equations. With the exception of O, a quadratic model fit the data significantly better than a linear model for all factors. At level 2, neither birth cohort nor gender was a significant predictor of variability in the slopes, so the curves of the five factors were unrelated to cohort and do not depend on gender.2 Solid lines in Figure 2a to Figure 6a depict the estimated slopes of the five factors, setting cohort as equal to the sample’s mean date of birth of 1938, and sex as intermediate between men and women (1.5). N appears to decline at decelerating rate up to age 80, and then is stable or increases slightly in the very old. E is stable in young adulthood and then declines at an accelerating rate in the very old. O has a substantial linear decline; A has a substantial increase. C follows a convex curve, with an increase up to age 70 and then a small decline. The changes in adulthood on the five factors amount to about one T-score point, or .1 SD, per decade.3

With the exception of the quadratic coefficient of E, all variances associated with the slopes were significant, indicating that there are individual differences in the slopes. These might be attributed to life events, genetic variability, or the onset of a dementing disorder. A more methodological explanation of the slope variability is attrition. Using a dummy variable that contrasted dropouts and active subjects, we tested whether the trajectories of subjects lost to follow-up differ (cf. Sliwinski, Hofer, Hall, Buschke, & Lipton, 2003). HLM analyses indicated that attrition was not a significant predictor of the slope variability, or in other words, the linear and quadratic coefficients were not significantly different for dropouts and active participants on any of the five factors. As would be expected from attrition analyses reported in the Method section, the only significant effect was found on the intercept of N, with dropouts on average scoring 1.6 T-scores higher than active participants (p < .01).

HLM slopes for the facets are depicted in Figure 2b to Figure 6b. Five N facets share the trajectory of the factor they define, which declined and then was stable or increased between ages 70 and 90. N5: Impulsiveness has a linear decline of about one T-score point per decade, without the upturn in old age seen in the other facets. Among the facets of E, E4: Activity has a convex curve with the most rapid decline in old age, whereas E5: Excitement-Seeking has a concave curve with the largest decline in early adulthood. E1: Warmth, E2: Gregariousness, E3: Assertiveness, and E6: Positive Emotions have curvilinear slopes with peaks around age 60. The facet of O showing the largest effect was O6: Values, which declined by two T-score points per decade. Openness to Feelings and Actions declined at an accelerating rate in old age. Other facets of O were substantially stable. The facets of A with the largest increase were A4: Compliance and A2: Straightforwardness. A1: Trust increased up to age 60 and then declined. The other facets of A were substantially stable or increased slightly with linear or curvilinear slopes. C6: Deliberation was the facet of C with the largest increase. The other C facets increased up to age 60–70 but then declined.

The effects of gender on facets' intercepts were predictable and consistent with cross-cultural gender-differences (Costa et al., 2001). Cohort effects on facets' intercepts indicate that later-born cohorts are lower on most facets of N, particularly N3: Depression and N6: Vulnerability, lower on O6: Values and A1: Trust, and higher on other facets, including E3: Assertiveness, E6: Positive Emotions, C6: Deliberation, and particularly C1: Competence. Gender had a significant effect on slope only for two of the 30 facet scales: O3: Feelings, for which gender differences become smaller with age, and A5: Modesty, for which the differences become larger. These gender effects explained 1% and 4% of the slopes' variances, respectively. Significant cohort effects on the linear slopes were found only for N1: Anxiety and C3: Dutifulness, accounting for less than 1% of the variance.

Within-subjects variability (σ2) is larger for the 30 facets than for the five factors. This is in part explained by the fact that the facets are less reliable, which in turn suggests that most of the within-subjects variability is due to measurement error. The estimated stable components of variance for the 30 facets range from 69% to 83% (M = 76%). Across facets, these stability values are highly correlated with two-year test-retest stability coefficients (r = .86; p < .001; McCrae et al., 1998).

Discussion

In this article we have presented longitudinal personality data from a large sample of adults. The strengths of the study include the analysis of multiple data points for most participants, the use of a well-validated instrument that provides a comprehensive assessment of personality traits, and sophisticated statistical analyses. Among the limitations are the use of a single, self-report method of measurement; the under-representation of young adults; and attrition: It is possible that individuals who dropped out of the study would show different developmental paths. But analyses conducted in the present study and in a large meta-analysis of longitudinal studies (Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, in press) showed that attrition had no effect on mean-level changes in any domain of personality. Another limitation is the fact that the BLSA is not representative of the U.S. population in terms of education and socio-economic status. In general, however, findings appear to be consistent with a much broader body of literature. For example, similar trajectories for N are found in the Normative Aging Study (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003) and in a large Dutch random sample (Steunenberg et al., 2005).

How does one describe personality changes in adulthood? HLM analyses indicated that about 85% of the variance in personality trait scores over the course of this study was due to stable individual differences. This estimate is comparable to the rank-order correlations found in the literature (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988), and it is reassuring that results from these sophisticated HLM analyses are consistent with conclusions from decades of research using less optimal statistical methods. About 15% of the variance is due to intraindividual variability, and this includes measurement error as well as normative and non-normative changes. However, those relatively small changes are of both theoretical and practical interest.

Data on normative changes from the BLSA are summarized in Figure 2a–b through Figure 6a–b, and merit several comments. First, the curves fall almost entirely within the average range, and changes from age 30 to age 90 are modest in magnitude, even for those few individuals who live to that exceptional age.

Second, the direction of the observed changes is, in general, consistent with previous longitudinal and cross-sectional studies showing declines in N, E, and O, and increases in A and C during adulthood (McCrae & Costa, 2003). However, McCrae and Costa (2003) did not anticipate curvilinear trends in later adulthood, in part because so little research has been conducted on participants over age 70. The upturn of N toward the end of the lifespan seen in the present study was also reported in a cross-sectional analysis by Costa and McCrae (1988) and in more recent longitudinal analyses (Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Small et al., 2003; Steunenberg et al., 2005). Among those studies there are no consistent curvilinear effects for E, which might be due to the weight that different E scales give to the factors’ facets: As Figure 3b shows, the developmental course of E varies considerably by facet. Roberts et al. (in press) found continuing longitudinal increases in A and C after age 30, but there is little information on the developmental curves of A and C in very old samples. A cross-sectional study of patients aged 65 to 100 (Weiss et al., 2005) found evidence for higher levels of A among older individuals, but Small et al. found that neither initial level of A or C nor change across six years was related to age. Further studies are needed to evaluate the decline in C in late life reported here.

A third feature of Figure 2a through Figure 6a is the general similarity of results from different analytic designs. Cross-sectional curves mirror HLM results. At the level of the facets, the rank-order correlation across the 30 facets between the cross-sectional linear slopes in Table 1 and the longitudinal linear slopes from HLM analyses was r = .72, p < .001. Substantively, when cross-sectional and longitudinal results agree, the most parsimonious interpretation is that effects are due to aging itself, rather than to some combination of practice, birth cohort, and time-of-measurement effects (Costa & McCrae, 1982).

There are some differences between cross-sectional and HLM results for N and C that suggest cohort or sampling bias effects. HLM analyses indicate that later-born cohorts have lower levels of N (e.g., individuals born in 1940 are predicted to score about one T-score point lower on N than individuals born in 1930), a finding opposite to the trend reported by Twenge (2000). The dramatic effects that Twenge (2000; 2001) observed in college-age samples do not seem to apply to these adults, who appear to have been fortunate enough to escape the “Age of Anxiety” (Twenge, 2000). Perhaps culture has stronger effects on personality in childhood and adolescence, but in adulthood such influences appear to be diminished.

Some studies (e.g., Wink & Helson, 1993) have reported significant Age × Gender interactions, especially for N, and interpreted this as evidence of differential development in men and women. In the present study we replicated this finding in cross-sectional analyses (although the effect accounted for less than 1% of the variance), but not in HLM analyses. A general absence of interactions is also found in cross-cultural studies (McCrae & Costa, in press), in other longitudinal studies (e.g., Helson et al., 2002), and in a recent meta-analysis of age changes (Roberts et al., in press).

Facet-level Findings

Most of the facets showed age trends that resemble that of the factor they define, but the variations within domain are worth noting. N5: Impulsiveness shows a linear decline across the lifespan, whereas the other N facets show curvilinear effects. N6: Vulnerability has the largest increase after age 70, which might be related to the fact that Vulnerability and Conscientiousness are the traits most affected by the onset of Alzheimer's disease (Siegler et al., 1991).

The facets of E have distinct patterns. E3: Assertiveness increases from age 30 to age 60. E4: Activity is stable in young adulthood and then declines at an accelerating rate in older age. Given the physiological changes that occur in the later half of life, it is not surprising that older individuals have a slower pace and feel less energetic and vigorous than young adults. Also notable in the E domain is the decline in E5: Excitement Seeking, which by age 30 has already declined substantially from its peak in adolescence (Costa & McCrae, 1992). The trajectory of E6: Positive Emotions is similar to the curve in life satisfaction found in a recent study by Mroczek and Spiro (2005).

It is perhaps not surprising that Openness to Values declines with age, because it is widely believed that older individuals are less willing to re-examine social, political and religious values (Krosnick & Alwin, 1989). A similar decline in Openness to Feelings is consistent with the hypothesis of affective blunting in old age (Lane, Sechrest, & Riedel, 1998). More remarkable is the fact that Openness to Aesthetics and Ideas show almost no decline from age 30 to 90.

The steepest increase among A facets is for A4: Compliance; the least change is found for A5: Modesty. The convex curve of C1: Competence echoes recent findings (Trzesniewski, Robins, Roberts, & Caspi, 2004) of a peak at age 70 for self-esteem, a variable closely related to Competence (Costa, McCrae, & Dye, 1991); whereas the monotonic increase in C6: Deliberation is consistent with other indicators of a lifelong drop in variables related to impulsiveness.

The longitudinal trends of the 30 facets depicted in this study provide a detailed picture of changes in personality. The differences among facets may explain some of the disagreements in the literature about the longitudinal course of personality traits. There is a large body of research on some of the broad dimensions of personality; the results of this study indicate that it will be informative to extend the analysis to a more fine-grained level to better understand changes in personality.

Interpreting Normative Age Changes

On the one hand, personality changes seen in adulthood are rather modest. Considering that most traits change at a rate of less than one T-score point per decade, it is perhaps not surprising that the clear majority of adults report that they have "stayed pretty much the same in personality" over the past few years (Herbst, McCrae, Costa, Feaganes, & Siegler, 2000, p. 381). On the other hand, analyses reported in this study and in the broader literature consistently show that there are mean level changes that can be detected with large samples and long time intervals. What remains is to understand the origins of these maturational changes. The present data are merely descriptive; here we can mention possible explanations and some supporting evidence.

There are two basic classes of explanations, environmental and biological, and psychologists have usually preferred the former. Srivastava et al. (2003) argued that Age × Gender interactions on N might reflect gender-based social experiences. Helson et al. (2002) predicted that period of life and social climate influence personality change. Roberts, Caspi, and Moffitt (2001) linked personality change to work experience. Zucker, Ostrove, and Stewart (2002) discussed "ways in which women's adult development may have been shaped by experiences particular to both gender and birth cohort" (p. 236). Roberts, Wood, and Smith (2005) held that a driving mechanism of personality development is psychological investment in age-graded social roles. The social pressures associated with getting married, creating a family, entering the workforce or advancing in one's career are thought to promote changes in the direction of greater maturity. Roberts et al. (2005) argued that those social roles tend to be shared across cultures and cohorts, and these universal tasks of social living are portrayed as a plausible explanation of the similarity of personality changes across cultures and cohorts.

Five-Factor Theory (FFT; McCrae & Costa, 1999) postulates that the traits encompassed by the FFM “are endogenous dispositions that follow intrinsic paths of development essentially independent of environmental influences” (McCrae et al., 2000, p. 173). That would explain why cross-sectional and longitudinal results converge; why the differing social experiences of men and women do not result in reliable differences in developmental curves for personality traits; and why cultures with dramatically different recent histories do not show marked divergences in the pattern of age differences across the lifespan (Yang, McCrae, & Costa, 1998). According to FFT, birth cohort, gender experience, culture, and history have little impact on trait development because traits are governed by biological processes common to the human species.

Indirect evidence supporting FFT comes from cross-cultural and cross-cohort studies that call into question environmental explanations, but there are also more direct sources of evidence, including comparative studies (e.g., King, Weiss, & Farmer, 2005) showing similar age trends in non-human primates and twin studies (Viken et al., 1994) showing strong genetic effects on the stability of personality traits.

FFT attributes personality change to intrinsic maturation, but it does not attempt to predict what changes will be observed. It is possible that the pattern of changes seen in Figure 2Figure 6 are results of evolution; for example, higher levels of C and A may be important for parenting and other adult responsibilities, and responsible adults may have successfully raised more offspring. Less clear from an evolutionary (as well as social role) perspective are the increases in N and declines in C in very old age; they may be due to losses of function in the aging brain (Resnick, Pham, Kraut, Zonderman, & Davatzikos, 2003) or to diminishing social support (Jackson, Antonucci, & Brown, 2004).

Sources of Non-Normative Change

HLM analyses showed that significant variance in individual trajectories remained unaccounted for by age and age-squared. This finding is consistent with previous multilevel modeling studies (Helson et al., 2002; Mroczek & Spiro, 2003; Small et al., 2003) and indicates that personality change itself is an individual difference variable. There is a growing interest in relating these non-normative changes to biological and environmental factors, and multilevel modeling analyses of large longitudinal datasets can be a powerful method to address these issues.

Personality changes that deviate from the normative trajectories shown in Figure 2Figure 6 might be due to genetic factors (McGue, Bacon, & Lykken, 1993), and genetically-informative research designs would greatly benefit this inquiry. Genetic association studies could examine whether polymorphisms in candidate genes are associated with non-normative changes in personality traits. FFT also suggests other biological influences on traits, such as addictions (Terracciano & Costa, 2004), pharmacological interventions (Maki, Resnick, & McCrae, 2004, July), and clinical depression (Costa, Bagby, & McCrae, 2004).

Of particular interest are studies that have examined change in personality traits among Alzheimer's patients using ratings from caregivers and other well-informed observers (e.g., Dawson, Welsh-Bohmer, & Siegler, 2000; Siegler, Dawson, & Welsh, 1994; Siegler et al., 1991; Strauss, Pasupathi, & Chaterjee, 1993). These studies, which compared concurrent with retrospective premorbid ratings, consistently found that after diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease, patients were rated as lower on C and E, and higher on N, particularly N6: Vulnerability. Although they provide a very consistent and plausible picture, these studies are retrospective and are not informative as to whether there are changes years before the diagnosis that could be used as early signs of the onset of the disorder. A study by Balsis, Carpenter, and Storandt (2005) on personality changes indicated that “observable personality changes may occur in the very earliest stages of the disease and precede measurable cognitive loss” (p. 100). Future studies should attempt to detect these possible early changes in personality using long-term prospective designs.

Researchers have examined the role of relationships and gender-related and work experience in shaping the developmental course of personality traits. For example, Roberts Helson, and Klohnen (2002) found that Dominance was correlated positively with the number of years in the labor force and negatively with the experience of divorce in women aged 27–43, but not in women 21–27 or 43–52. Changes in motherhood status and the impact of women’s movement were found to be unrelated to changes in Dominance. Costa et al. (2000) tested whether events like marriage, divorce, widowhood, death of parent or child, and retirement had an effect on personality traits. From among the 30 events assessed, they found effects of divorce and job change on personality traits, though it was not clear how enduring the effects might be.

Acute events thus appear to have limited effects on personality traits, but chronic stressors may. In particular, caregiver burden in known to have effects on well-being and depressive mood (Jang, Clay, Roth, Haley, & Mittelman, 2004; Russo Vitaliano, Brewer, Katon, & Becker, 1995). Whether these stressful situations would have an impact on general personality traits remains to be seen.

Although there are a number of findings suggesting that life events can affect personality traits, none has been clearly replicated, and future research should explicitly address this. Further explorations of the possible effects of life events (Costa et al., 2000; Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003; Roberts et al., 2002), psychotherapy (Piedmont, 2001), and acculturation experiences (Leininger, 2002) should continue, along with a vigorous program of research on possible biological causes of personality change. Perhaps normative trait development is biologically-based, whereas deviations from the normal developmental trajectory are due to life experiences.

Appendix

Appendix Table A.

Frequency of NEO-PI-R Administrations.

Administration N Mean Age Age Range
1 730 48.97 20–96
2 530 57.20 21–95
3 212 66.32 20–94
4 133 71.95 30–96
5 123 72.46 50–94
6 119 72.38 51–93
7 62 73.75 54–94
8 17 75.37 56–92
9 11 82.38 68–92
10 6 82.65 72–93
11 1 83.57 75–90

Note. N refers to the number of individuals having exactly the specified number of administrations. Age range is from youngest age at first administration to oldest age at last administration.

Appendix Table B.

Mean NEO-PI-R Factors and Facets from the First Administration for Six Age Groups.

Age groups 20–40 40–50 50–60 60–70 70–80 80–100
Mean Age 30.9 45.6 54.5 65.1 74.6 84.1
N 333 407 340 370 304 190
Factor/Facet
N: Neuroticism 50.7 48.3 47.3 46.8 45.8 48.1
E: Extraversion 51.2 52.1 51.1 48.8 47.5 44.7
O: Openness 56.5 54.4 54.6 52.0 52.0 50.4
A: Agreeableness 46.2 49.5 50.2 50.4 52.3 52.9
C: Conscientiousness 49.5 51.7 50.8 50.8 49.3 47.7
N1: Anxiety 50.6 48.4 47.7 47.6 46.6 48.9
N2: Angry Hostility 51.7 48.6 48.0 47.9 47.9 47.9
N3: Depression 50.2 47.6 46.8 47.1 46.2 50.0
N4: Self-Consciousness 50.4 47.9 47.3 48.6 47.2 49.7
N5: Impulsiveness 51.2 50.0 50.3 48.6 47.0 46.4
N6: Vulnerability 49.4 46.3 46.3 47.4 48.4 52.3
E1: Warmth 49.2 51.2 51.7 50.4 51.0 48.7
E2: Gregariousness 50.6 51.1 50.4 51.4 51.1 49.1
E3: Assertiveness 53.1 54.0 54.3 53.5 51.4 50.2
E4: Activity 54.8 55.4 52.9 49.9 49.1 45.3
E5: Excitement-Seeking 53.5 48.9 48.3 47.3 46.0 44.3
E6: Positive Emotions 52.6 53.2 52.4 49.1 47.3 44.7
O1: Fantasy 56.6 53.2 52.7 49.1 48.2 47.2
O2: Aesthetics 53.6 53.3 54.6 53.1 53.2 53.1
O3: Feelings 53.4 53.8 52.4 49.2 48.4 46.5
O4: Actions 53.1 52.5 52.1 51.0 49.3 47.4
O5: Ideas 55.0 51.1 51.9 51.1 51.9 49.6
O6: Values 53.6 53.0 51.6 49.4 49.6 46.9
A1: Trust 47.0 51.4 52.4 52.5 53.8 53.4
A2: Straightforwardness 48.0 49.6 49.6 50.9 52.1 51.9
A3: Altruism 49.1 51.4 51.1 50.0 50.1 48.3
A4: Compliance 47.5 49.2 50.1 50.4 52.8 54.4
A5: Modesty 46.8 48.1 47.8 47.7 48.2 47.5
A6: Tender-Mindedness 48.5 51.5 51.3 49.8 50.1 49.7
C1: Competence 52.0 55.1 54.5 52.5 50.8 47.9
C2: Order 47.7 49.2 48.5 48.7 47.6 46.8
C3: Dutifulness 47.8 50.4 50.4 51.6 52.5 50.7
C4: Achievement Striving 50.8 52.2 51.1 51.0 49.4 49.0
C5: Self-Discipline 45.6 49.7 49.2 49.0 47.6 44.1
C6: Deliberation 50.1 51.6 51.8 52.5 51.5 50.9

Note. SDs for the factors ranged from 8.4 to 11.9; for the facets, SDs ranged from 8.1 to 12.3.

Appendix Table C.

Cross-Sectional Regression Coefficients Predicting NEO-PI-R Factors and Facets from Age in Decades and longitudinal (HLM) Estimates of Gender Effect on linear slope.

Cross-sectional Longitudinal


NEO-PI-R scale R2 β0 β1 β2 γ11: Gender
N: Neuroticism 0.027** 60.7 −4.07 0.29 −0.33 (.20)
E: Extraversion 0.053** 48.6 1.96 −0.29 −0.28 (.20)
O: Openness 0.036** 62.2 −1.98 0.07 −0.06 (.22)
A: Agreeableness 0.040** 40.9 2.15 −0.09 0.04 (.20)
C: Conscientiousness 0.013** 41.6 3.73 −0.36 −0.29 (.22)
N1: Anxiety 0.017** 19.1 −1.88 0.14 −0.23 (.21)
N2: Angry Hostility 0.021** 17.0 −1.66 0.12 −0.16 (.21)
N3: Depression 0.021** 18.6 −2.69 0.22 −0.17 (.22)
N4: Self-Consciousness 0.014** 18.6 −1.81 0.15 −0.05 (.21)
N5: Impulsiveness 0.029** 16.7 0.00 −0.04 −0.12 (.21)
N6: Vulnerability 0.032** 14.4 −2.22 0.21 −0.29 (.21)
E1: Warmth 0.006** 20.5 1.06 −0.10 −0.29 (.21)
E2: Gregariousness 0.002 15.5 0.59 −0.06 −0.03 (.23)
E3: Assertiveness 0.016** 14.6 1.32 −0.14 −0.35 (.21)
E4: Activity 0.093** 19.2 0.66 −0.13 −0.18 (.21)
E5: Excitement-Seeking 0.086** 23.2 −2.04 0.11 0.06 (.20)
E6: Positive Emotions 0.071** 20.5 0.81 −0.13 −0.46 (.22)
O1: Fantasy 0.097** 24.7 −1.73 0.07 −0.19 (.22)
O2: Aesthetics 0.001 19.8 −0.04 0.00 −0.24 (.22)
O3: Feelings 0.066** 22.7 0.00 −0.05 −0.55 (.21)**
O4: Actions 0.028** 17.5 0.21 −0.05 0.42 (.22)
O5: Ideas 0.019** 25.1 −1.59 0.11 0.03 (.22)
O6: Values 0.042** 23.3 −0.24 −0.02 −0.08 (.22)
A1: Trust 0.050** 16.0 1.75 −0.11 −0.03 (.21)
A2: Straightforwardness 0.020** 19.3 0.36 0.00 −0.38 (.21)
A3: Altruism 0.007** 21.5 0.91 −0.09 −0.34 (.21)
A4: Compliance 0.040** 17.9 −0.12 0.05 0.25 (.22)
A5: Modesty 0.002 16.2 0.60 −0.05 0.67 (.22)**
A6: Tender-Mindedness 0.006** 17.9 1.02 −0.09 −0.03 (.22)
C1: Competence 0.042** 19.3 1.83 −0.19 −0.41 (.22)
C2: Order 0.004* 16.2 0.90 −0.09 −0.31 (.23)
C3: Dutifulness 0.027** 18.8 1.44 −0.10 0.15 (.20)
C4: Achievement Striving 0.008** 18.4 0.77 −0.08 0.22 (.24)
C5: Self-Discipline 0.029** 13.9 2.83 −0.26 −0.26 (.24)
C6: Deliberation 0.009** 14.3 1.36 −0.11 0.01 (.22)

Note. N = 1,944. Standard errors are shown in parenthesis. 1 = Male, 2 = Female.

*

p < .05

**

p < .01

Footnotes

1

About 5% of these administrations were actually collected by paper-and-pencil during participants’ visits. The computer administration was preferred by most participants, including the very old.

2

The non-significant gender interaction terms are available in Appendix Table C.

3

A reviewer pointed out that because some BLSA participants are married, their data might not be fully independent observations. We therefore repeated the analyses of the five factors within two subsamples with independent observations: Men and women. In women, the curvilinear effect for A was not significant; otherwise, the pattern of significant results in both subsamples replicated the overall findings.

Portions of these data were presented at the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, August, 2004, Honolulu, HI, and at the Annual Convention of the Gerontological Society of American, November, 2004, Washington, DC. Two year retest stability data (N =338) were reported in McCrae, Yik, Trapnell, Bond, & Paulhus (1998). As part (about 15%) of the NEO-PI-R normative sample, some of these data were reported in Costa & McCrae (1992). As correlates of other measures and in psychometric analyses, some of these data were reported in Costa & McCrae (1992); Jefferson, Herbst, & McCrae (1998); Costa, Herbst, McCrae, Samuels, & Ozer (2002); McCrae & Costa (2004); Terracciano, Merritt, Zonderman, & Evans, (2003); and Terracciano & Costa (2004). Portions of these data were also reported in McCrae, Stone, Fagan, & Costa, (1998) and McCrae, Jang, Livesley, Riemann, & Angleitner (2001).

Contributor Information

Antonio Terracciano, Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Baltimore, MD.

Robert R. McCrae, Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Baltimore, MD

Larry J. Brant, Research Resources Branch, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Baltimore, MD

Paul T. Costa, Jr., Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Department of Health and Human Services, Baltimore, MD

References

  1. Balsis S, Carpenter BD, Storandt M. Personality change precedes clinical diagnosis of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60:P98–P101. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.2.p98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brant LJ, Verbeke G. Describing the natural heterogeneity of aging using multilevel regression models. International Journal of Sports Medicine. 1997;18:S225–S231. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-972719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Costa PT, Jr, Bagby RM, McCrae RR. Personality self-reports are accurate during acute depressive episodes. Manuscript submitted for publication. 2004 [Google Scholar]
  4. Costa PT, Jr., Herbst JH, McCrae RR, Samuels J, Ozer DJ. The replicability and utility of three personality types. European Journal of Personality. 2002;16:S73–S87. [Google Scholar]
  5. Costa PT, Jr., Herbst JH, McCrae RR, Siegler IC. Personality at midlife: Stability, intrinsic maturation, and response to life events. Assessment. 2000;7:365–378. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR. An approach to the attribution of age, period, and cohort effects. Psychological Bulletin. 1982;92:238–250. [Google Scholar]
  7. Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR. Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54:853–863. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.5.853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  9. Costa PT, Jr., McCrae RR, Dye DA. Facet scales for Agreeableness and Conscientiousness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences. 1991;12:887–898. [Google Scholar]
  10. Costa PT, Jr., Terracciano A, McCrae RR. Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: Robust and surprising findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:322–331. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dawson DV, Welsh-Bohmer KA, Siegler IC. Premorbid personality predicts level of rated personality change in patients with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders. 2000;14:11–19. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200001000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Gutmann D. Reclaimed powers: Toward a new psychology of men and women in later life. New York: Basic Books, Inc; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  13. Helson R. A longitudinal study of creative personality in women. Creativity Research Journal. 1999;12:89–101. [Google Scholar]
  14. Helson R, Jones C, Kwan VSY. Personality change over 40 years of adulthood: Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of two longitudinal samples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83:752–766. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Herbst JH, McCrae RR, Costa PT, Jr, Feaganes JR, Siegler IC. Self-perceptions of stability and change in personality at midlife: The UNC Alumni Heart Study. Assessment. 2000;7:379–388. doi: 10.1177/107319110000700406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hertzog C, Nesselroade JR. Assessing psychological change in adulthood: An overview of methodological issues. Psychology and Aging. 2003;18:639–657. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Jackson JS, Antonucci TC, Brown E. A cultural lens on biopsychosocial models of aging. In: Costa PT Jr, Siegler IC, editors. Recent advance in psychology and aging: Advances in cell aging and gerontology. Vol. 15. Ansterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Publishing; 2004. pp. 221–241. [Google Scholar]
  18. Jang Y, Clay OJ, Roth DL, Haley WE, Mittelman MS. Neuroticism and longitudinal change in caregiver depression: Impact of a spouse-caregiver intervention program. Gerontologist. 2004;44:311–317. doi: 10.1093/geront/44.3.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Jefferson T, Herbst JH, McCrae RR. Associations between birth order and personality traits: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings. Journal of Research in Personality. 1998;32:498–509. [Google Scholar]
  20. Jones CJ, Livson N, Peskin H. Longitudinal hierarchical linear modeling analyses of California psychological inventory data from age 33 to 75: An examination of stability and change in adult personality. Journal of Personality Assessment. 2003;80:294–308. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA8003_07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Kelly EL. Consistency of the adult personality. American Psychologist. 1955;10:659–681. [Google Scholar]
  22. King JE, Weiss A, Farmer KH. A chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) analogue of cross-national generalization of personality structure: Zoological parks and an African sanctuary. Journal of Personality. 2005;73:389–410. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00313.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Krosnick JA, Alwin DF. Aging and susceptibility to attitude-change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1989;57:416–425. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.3.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Lane RD, Sechrest L, Riedel R. Sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1998;39:377–385. doi: 10.1016/s0010-440x(98)90051-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Leininger A. Vietnamese-American personality and acculturation: An exploration of relationships between personality traits and cultural goals. In: McCrae RR, Allik J, editors. The Five-Factor Model of personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers; 2002. pp. 189–218. [Google Scholar]
  26. Maki PM, Resnick SM, McCrae RR. Changes in self-reported personality coincident with declines in cognition: Results from parallel hormone therapy trials in elderly men and women; Paper presented at the Ninth International Conference on Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders; Philadelphia. 2004. Jul, [Google Scholar]
  27. McCrae RR, et al. Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:547–561. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . A Five-Factor Theory of personality. In: Pervin LA, John OP, editors. Handbook of personality: Theory and research. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford; 1999. pp. 139–153. [Google Scholar]
  29. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . Personality in adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory perspective. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  30. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr A contemplated revision of the NEO Five-Factor Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences. 2004;36:587–596. [Google Scholar]
  31. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr . Cross-cultural perspectives on adult personality trait development. In: Mroczek D, Little T, editors. Handbook of personality development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; (in press) [Google Scholar]
  32. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Jr, Lima MP, Simoes A, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, et al. Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures. Developmental Psychology. 1999;35:466–477. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.2.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. McCrae RR, Costa PT, Jr, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, Hrebrickova M, Avia MD, et al. Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality, and lifespan development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;78:173–186. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. McCrae RR, Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Riemann R, Angleitner A. Sources of structure: Genetic, environmental, and artifactual influences on the covariation of personality traits. Journal of Personality. 2001;69:511–535. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.694154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. McCrae RR, John OP. An introduction to the Five-Factor Model and its applications. Journal of Personality. 1992;60:175–215. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McCrae RR, Stone SV, Fagan PJ, Costa PT., Jr Identifying causes of disagreement between self-reports and spouse ratings of personality. Journal of Personality. 1998;66:285–313. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. McCrae RR, Yik MSM, Trapnell PD, Bond MH, Paulhus DL. Interpreting personality profiles across cultures: Bilingual, acculturation, and peer rating studies of Chinese undergraduates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1998;74:1041–1055. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.74.4.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. McGue M, Bacon S, Lykken DT. Personality Stability and Change in Early Adulthood - a Behavioral Genetic-Analysis. Developmental Psychology. 1993;29:96–109. [Google Scholar]
  39. Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Modeling intraindividual change in personality traits: Findings from the normative aging study. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2003;58B:P153–P165. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.3.p153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Mroczek DK, Spiro A. Change in life satisfaction during adulthood: Findings from the veterans affairs normative aging study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2005;88:189–202. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Piedmont RL. Cracking the plaster cast: Big Five personality change during intensive outpatient counseling. Journal of Research in Personality. 2001;35:500–520. [Google Scholar]
  42. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  43. Resnick SM, Pham DL, Kraut MA, Zonderman AB, Davatzikos C. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: A shrinking brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3295–3301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03295.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. The kids are alright: Growth and stability in personality development from adolescence to adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2001;81:670–683. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Roberts BW, Caspi A, Moffitt TE. Work experiences and personality development in young adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:582–593. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Roberts BW, Helson R, Klohnen EC. Persoanlity development and growth in women across 30 years: Three perspectives. Journal of Personality. 2002;70:79–102. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Roberts BW, Wood D, Smith JL. Evaluating Five Factor Theory and social investment perspectives on personality trait development. Journal of Research in Personality. 2005;39:166–184. [Google Scholar]
  48. Roberts RE, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W. Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. (in press) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Russo J, Vitaliano PP, Brewer DD, Katon W, Becker J. Psychiatric-Disorders in Spouse Caregivers of Care Recipients with Alzheimers-Disease and Matched Controls - a Diathesis Stress Model of Psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:197–204. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.104.1.197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Siegler IC, Dawson DV, Welsh KA. Caregiver Ratings of Personality-Change in Alzheimers-Disease Patients - a Replication. Psychology and Aging. 1994;9:464–466. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.9.3.464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Siegler IC, Welsh KA, Dawson DV, Fillenbaum GG, Earl NL, Kaplan EB, et al. Ratings of personality change in patients being evaluated for memory disorders. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders. 1991;5:240–250. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199100540-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Sliwinski MJ, Hofer SM, Hall C, Buschke H, Lipton RB. Modeling memory decline in older adults: The importance of preclinical dementia, attrition, and chronological age. Psychology and Aging. 2003;18:658–671. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.18.4.658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Small BJ, Hertzog C, Hultsch DF, Dixon RL. Stability and change in adult personality over 6 years: Findings from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 2003;58B:P166–P176. doi: 10.1093/geronb/58.3.p166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Soldz S, Vaillant GE. The Big Five personality traits and the life course: A 45-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality. 1999;33:208–232. [Google Scholar]
  55. Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J. Development of personality in early and middle age: Set like plaster or persistent change? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2003;84:1041–1053. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Steunenberg B, Twisk JWR, Beekman ATF, Deeg DJH, Kerkhof A. Stability and change of neuroticism in aging. Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2005;60:P27–P33. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.1.p27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Strauss ME, Pasupathi M, Chaterjee A. Concordance between observers in descriptions of personality change in Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging. 1993;8:475–480. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.8.4.475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Terracciano A, Costa PT., Jr Smoking and the Five-Factor Model of personality. Addiction. 2004;99:472–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00687.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Terracciano A, Merritt M, Zonderman AB, Evans MK. Personality traits and sex differences in emotion recognition among African Americans and Caucasians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2003;1000:309–312. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Trzesniewski KH, Robins RW, Roberts BW, Caspi A. Personality and self-esteem development across the life span. In: Costa PTJ, Siegler IC, editors. Recent Advances in Psychology and Aging. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  61. Twenge JM. The Age of Anxiety? Birth cohort change in anxiety and neuroticism, 1952–1993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2000;79:1007–1021. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.6.1007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Twenge JM. Birth cohort changes in extraversion: A cross-temporal meta-analysis, 1966–1993. Personality and Individual Differences. 2001;30:735–748. [Google Scholar]
  63. Verbeke G, Molenberghs G. Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York: Springer; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  64. Viken RJ, Rose RJ, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. A developmental genetic-analysis of adult personality - Extroversion and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1994;66:722–730. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.66.4.722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Weiss A, Costa PT, Jr, Karuza J, Duberstein PR, Friedman B, McCrae RR. Cross-sectional age differences in personality among medicare recipients aged 65 to 100. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20:182–185. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wink P, Helson R. Personality change in women and their partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1993;65:597–605. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.3.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yang J, McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Adult age differences in personality traits in the United States and the People’s Republic of China. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. 1998;53B:P375–P383. doi: 10.1093/geronb/53b.6.p375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zucker AN, Ostrove JM, Stewart AJ. College-educated women’s personality development in adulthood: Perceptions and age differences. Psychology and Aging. 2002;17:236–244. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES