Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Jan 22.
Published in final edited form as: Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007 Nov;22(11):1095–1100. doi: 10.1002/gps.1791

THE ROLE OF PATIENT PERSONALITY IN THE IDENTIFICATION OF DEPRESSION IN OLDER PRIMARY CARE PATIENTS

Laura W McCray 1,2, Hillary R Bogner 1,2, Mary D Sammel 2,3, Joseph J Gallo 1,2
PMCID: PMC2810143  NIHMSID: NIHMS168826  PMID: 17407104

SUMMARY

Background

Our aim was to evaluate whether personality factors significantly contribute to the identification of depression in older primary care patients, even after controlling for depressive symptoms.

Methods

We examined the association between personality factors and the identification of depression among 318 older adults who participated in the Spectrum study.

Results

High neuroticism (unadjusted odds ratio (OR) 2.36, 95% confidence interval (CI) [1.42, 3.93]) and low extraversion (adjusted OR 2.24, CI [1.26, 4.00]) were associated with physician identification of depression. Persons with high conscientiousness were less likely to be identified as depressed by the doctor (adjusted OR 0.45, CI [0.22, 0.91]).

Conclusion

Personality factors influence the identification of depression among older persons in primary care over and above the relationship of depressive symptoms with physician identification. Knowledge of personality may influence the diagnosis and treatment of depression in primary care.

Keywords: aged, depression, diagnosis, personality, primary health care

KEY POINTS

  • Older adults with high neuroticism and those with low extraversion were more likely to be identified as depressed by the primary care doctor. Persons with high conscientiousness were less likely to be identified as depressed.

  • Awareness of how stable personality characteristics may influence the identification of depression may inform the accurate diagnosis and treatment of depression among older adults in primary care.

INTRODUCTION

Research has established that personality traits are associated with the diagnosis and course of depression. Persons with depression and anxiety are more likely to have high neuroticism, or the tendency to have negative emotions or a pessimistic outlook on the world, compared to persons who are not depressed or anxious (Bienvenu et al., 2004; Chopra et al., 2005; Cuijpers, van Straten, & Donker, 2005). Persons with high neuroticism are at greater risk then those with low neuroticism for the development of depression (Boyce, Parker, Barnett, Cooney, & Smith, 1991) and endure a more severe course of depressive illness (Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994). Little is known about the influence of personality factors on physician decision making and the diagnosis of mental illness in the primary care setting.

Depression and other mood disorders have long been associated with personality, defined as an “enduring emotional, interpersonal, experiential, attitudinal, and motivational style that determines the person’s reaction to his or her environment” (Allport, 1937; McCrae, 1991). The current study utilizes the five factor model of personality, which includes the personality traits neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness (Table 1) (McCrae & Costa, 1987). Costa and McCrae’s model posits that each of the five major personality factors are personal attributes that may be expressed along a continuum. Individuals may have different levels of each of the five personality traits, influencing the way in which they interact with their environment. The five factor model has been extensively studied in multiple settings, across multiple instruments and observers (Costa & McCrae, 1997; McCrae, 1991; McCrae & Costa, 1987).

Table 1. The Five Factor Model of Personality.

Examples of adjectives which define each personality factor along its continuum (McCrae, 1991; Wiggins, 2003).

PERSONALITY FACTOR HIGH (+) LOW (−)
Neuroticism Negative affect, pessimistic outlook towards the world, self-blame Unemotional, overly calm, lack of concern for potential problems in health
Extraversion Sociable, friendly, talkative Isolated, passive, unfeeling, quiet
Openness to Experience Daring, imaginative, original, broad interests Down to earth, conventional, conservative, dislikes change
Agreeableness Cooperative, sympathetic, trustful Ruthless, inability to trust, stingy, manipulative, irritable
Conscientiousness Thorough, well-organized, careful, hard-working Disorganized, lazy, negligent, lack of self-discipline

No known studies have specifically looked at the influence of personality on the identification of depression by the primary care doctor. Previous studies have examined the influence of patient characteristics, such as age, race, and sex, on physician’s decision making and clinical care (Schulman et al., 1999; Sheifer, Escarce, & Schulman, 2000). These studies suggest that diagnosis is not a completely objective process and may be influenced by stable patient characteristics above and beyond the clinical presentation of depression. The current study is an attempt to examine the influence of patient personality on depression identification in primary care. Our aim was to evaluate whether personality factors significantly contribute to the identification of depression, even after controlling for potentially influential covariates, including depressive symptoms.

We hypothesized that older patients with high neuroticism and those with low extroversion would be more likely to be identified as depressed by their primary care physician. The negative outlook on life associated with high neuroticism and the isolation or introversion associated with low extraversion may be interpreted as symptoms of depression, leading to identification of depression by the primary care doctor beyond what might be expected due to depressive symptoms alone.

METHODS

The Spectrum study

The objective of the Spectrum survey was to describe the correlates of depression in older adults. Non-academic physician practices were recruited from the membership list of the Maryland Academy of Family Physicians (MAFP) and included practices from Baltimore City, Maryland, and the four surrounding counties. In all, 47 physicians (28 family physicians and 19 internists) from 13 practices participated in the study. Persons aged 65 and older were approached by trained interviewers and asked several screening questions to determine eligibility for the study.

Details of the study design, including physician and patient recruitment, can be found elsewhere (H.R. Bogner et al., 2004; Gallo et al., 2005). Experienced lay interviewers were trained in screening and interviewing by the study investigators in conjunction with the Battelle Memorial Institute’s Center for Public Health Research and Evaluation in Baltimore, Maryland. Older adults were invited to participate in the study based on sampling probabilities based on a standard screening instrument. This sampling technique was employed to enrich the sample with patients who had significant depressive symptoms and with persons who may be at risk for the development of depression (Gallo et al., 2005).

Participants who agreed to be part of the study were scheduled for an in-home interview which consisted of a 90 minute survey questionnaire. In-home interviews were obtained for 357 people, but two persons broke off the interview before it was completed, leaving a sample of 355 persons. Complete information on personality factors was available for 318 participants, constituting the final sample. The research protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine.

Measurement strategy

We used standard questions to obtain information from the respondents on age, gender, self-reported ethnicity, and education. Physicians were asked to provide their assessment of patient’s depression at the index visit as “none, mild, moderate, or severe.” For this investigation, physician rating of depression was coded ‘0’ for no depression and ‘1’ for mild, moderate, or severe depression.

Personality was assessed using the NEO-FFI (Five Factor Inventory), a 60-item scale which has been validated in multiple settings (Costa & McCrae, 1997; McCrae, 1991; McCrae & Costa, 1987). Performance on this scale has been shown to be independent of physical and cognitive change and is relatively stable over time (Costa & McCrae, 1997; Hogan, Johnson, & Briggs, 1997; Weiss et al., 2005). The scale is designed to produce a score for each of the five personality factors (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and conscientiousness).

Scores for each personality factor are standardized according to population norms (mean=50, standard deviation=10) and then grouped into five categories (very low, low, average, high, and very high) according to standard protocol (Costa, 2003). For the purposes of this study, we grouped the scores into three levels: low, average, and high, for each of the personality factors. Our rationale for the use of three categories was that very few participants fell within the “very low” and “very high” categories for each of the personality factors. We therefore combined “very low” and “low” into one “low” category, and similarly, “very high” and “high” into one “high” category. We retained standard thresholds according to established population norms (Costa, 2003).

The Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale is a 20-item scale designed to measure depressive symptoms in the general population (Radloff, 1977). The CES-D has been employed in multiple samples including older persons (Gatz, Johansson, Pedersen, Berg, & Reynolds, 1993; Newmann, Engel, & Jensen, 1991). The total number of medical conditions, as self-reported by participants, was used as a measure of medical co-morbidity, as in other work (H. R. Bogner, Ford, & Gallo, 2006; Perkins et al., 2004).

Analytic strategy

After descriptive analysis, sociodemographic characteristics, number of medical conditions, CES-D score, and NEO-FFI personality scores were compared between persons who were rated as “not at all” depressed with persons who were rated as depressed, employing χ2 or t tests as appropriate to the form of the data. Next, multivariable logistic regression was performed to estimate associations between each personality factor and our dependent variable, physician identification of depression. Based on bivariate associations with our main outcome, we included covariates in multivariate models that we found were associated at the P < .20 level (“model 1”). We added CES-D scores separately in “model 2,” to assess the relationship of identification of depression to personality factors independently from the association of identification with depressive symptoms. Stratified logistic regression provided a sensitivity analysis to account for the sampling strategy employed (Gallo et al., 2005).

RESULTS

Sample Characteristics

Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are presented in Table 2. Significant differences were found for age, ethnicity, and number of medical conditions between those with and without identification of depression by the primary care doctor. Respondents with and without depression, as identified by the primary care doctor, were similar in the proportion of women and persons with less than a high school education. Using a threshold value of p <0.20, younger patients, and those with a greater number of medical conditions, were more likely to be identified as depressed. African Americans were less likely to be identified as depressed (p <.001) as previously reported (Gallo et al., 2005).

Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics.

Comparison of patients identified by physicians as “not at all” depressed with patients identified as depressed, among patients with complete information (n = 318). Data from the Spectrum Study (2001–2003). Unless otherwise indicated, data are expressed as mean (SD). CES-D, Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.

Variable “Not at all” depressed (n = 141) Depressed (n = 177) p value
Sociodemographic
Age in years 75.7 (6.4) 74.7 (5.6) 0.12
Women, n (%) 109 (34.3) 137 (43.1) 0.98
African American, n (%) 63 (19.8) 44 (13.8) <0.001
White, n (%) 78 (24.5) 130 (40.9) <0.001
Education less than high school, n (%) 58 (18.2) 67 (21.1) 0.55
Physical health
Number of medical conditions 6.57 (3.16) 7.91 (3.75) <0.001
Depressive Symptoms
CES-D score 11.06 (9.56) 17.74 (11.41) <0.001
Personality traits
Neuroticism 51.04 (6.80) 54.34 (7.81) <0.001
Extraversion 49.71 (8.63) 44.80 (10.37) <0.001
Openness to experience 44.92 (7.69) 44.56 (9.21) 0.71
Agreeableness 50.85 (10.70) 49.64 (11.06) 0.33
Conscientiousness 47.90 (9.04) 45.36 (9.30) 0.02
*

p- values generated for comparison of groups with χ2 or t test, as appropriate.

Multivariable analysis

Results from logistic regression analyses for the individual personality traits are presented in Table 3. Sensitivity analyses that adjust for the probability of selection showed no substantial change from the point estimates reported here. Compared to those with average neuroticism, persons with high neuroticism were significantly more likely to be identified as depressed by their primary care doctor (unadjusted odds ratio (OR) = 2.36, 95% confidence interval (CI) [1.42, 3.93]). Persons with low extraversion were more likely to be identified as depressed by their primary care doctor compared to those with average extraversion (unadjusted OR = 2.47, CI [1.47, 4.16]). After adjustment, persons with high conscientiousness, compared to persons with average conscientiousness, were less likely to be identified as depressed (adjusted OR = 0.45, CI [0.22, 0.91].

Table 3. Association between personality and identification of depression by the primary care doctor.

OR, odds ratio. 95% confidence interval shown in brackets. Model 1 includes terms for self-identified ethnicity, number of medical conditions, and age; Model 2 includes all terms from Model 1 plus the Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale score.

Unadjusted OR Model 1 Model 2
NEUROTICISM
Low (n=32) 0.80 [0.37, 1.70] 0.70 [0.31, 1.56] 0.81 [0.35, 1.87]
Average (n=180) 1.00 1.00 1.00
High (n=106) 2.36 [1.42, 3.93] 2.02 [1.19, 3.45] 1.46 [0.82, 2.58]
p value 0.002 0.012 0.327
EXTRAVERSION
Low (n=129) 2.47 [1.47, 4.16] 2.42 [1.39, 4.22] 2.24 [1.26, 4.00]
Average (n=122) 1.00 1.00 1.00
High (n=67) 0.66 [0.36, 1.20] 0.59 [0.31, 1.11] 0.65 [0.34, 1.26]
p value <0.001 <0.001 0.001
OPENNESS TO EXPERIENCE
Low (n=157) 1.49 [0.94, 2.40] 1.57 [0.95, 2.58] 1.58 [0.94, 2.65]
Average (n=128) 1.00 1.00 1.00
High (n=33) 1.81 [ 0.82, 3.98] 1.62 [0.70, 3.72] 1.74 [0.73, 4.16]
p value 0.150 0.172 0.174
AGREEABLENESS
Low (n=100) 1.31 [0.77, 2.22] 1.41 [0.79, 2.49] 1.00 [0.54, 1.83]
Average (n=130) 1.00 1.00 1.00
High (n=88) 1.20 [0.70, 2.07] 1.14 [0.64, 2.03] 1.11 [0.61, 2.02]
p value 0.580 0.505 0.935
CONSCIENTIOUSNESS
Low (n=119) 1.46 [0.89, 2.39] 1.24 [0.74, 2.11] 1.00 [0.57, 1.74]
Average (n=146) 1.00 1.00 1.00
High (n=53) 0.54 [0.29, 1.03] 0.50 [0.25, 0.98] 0.45 [0.22, 0.91]
p value 0.014 0.037 0.062

DISCUSSION

The principle finding of this study was that persons with high neuroticism and those with low extraversion were more likely to be identified as depressed by the primary care physician, than persons with average levels of these personality factors. Persons with high conscientiousness were less likely to be identified as depressed, even while controlling for severity of depressive symptoms.

We acknowledge that there are several limitations to this study. There may have been sample bias in physician and patient recruitment, limiting the generalizability of our findings. For instance, patients with higher agreeableness or openness to experience may have been more likely to engage in the study. We tried to mitigate information bias by allowing patients to complete the NEO-FFI personality instrument in private, prior to the face-to-face interview. Sensitivity analyses were performed using stratified logistic regression to adjust for the probability of selection. Despite limitations, our results deserve attention because this is the first known attempt to examine the relationship between individual personality factors and the identification of depression by the primary care doctor.

We found that persons with high neuroticism were more likely to be identified as depressed by the primary care doctor. Because persons who have high neuroticism are more likely to endure a chronic and more severe course of depression (Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994), it might be important to target such persons for more intensive treatments, such as combined medication and psychotherapy. Depressed persons with high neuroticism may also benefit from interventions such as cognitive behavioral therapy which could address their negative cognitions (Beck, 2005).

Participants with low extraversion were more likely to be identified as depressed by the primary care physician, even while controlling for depressive symptoms. Primary care physicians should pay close attention to persons who appear to be socially isolated, introverted, or less outgoing, when screening for depression. Physicians may want to employ more rigorous diagnostic tools to avoid over-diagnosis of depression in persons who appear to have low extraversion. If persons with low extraversion are diagnosed as depressed, treatments may be tailored to address behavioral activation, which may be particularly important to their recovery (Barbour & Blumenthal, 2005; Thompson, 1996).

We found that high conscientiousness was associated with decreased odds of identification of depression by the primary care doctor. These patients may appear to be well-organized, and to “have it all together,” while they may indeed be experiencing significant levels of depressive symptoms (the “good patient” (Wittink, Barg, & Gallo, 2006)). Primary care physicians should be aware of these findings, as to not miss the diagnosis of depression in patients who appear to have high conscientiousness.

Our findings may have important implications for the diagnosis and management of depression in older persons in primary care. Basic knowledge of personality factors and their association with the identification of depression may influence the accurate diagnosis of depression in older patients with certain personality characteristics and may also influence the physician’s choice of therapy. Future, longitudinally based studies can address the implications of personality on treatment decisions and outcomes in primary care.

Acknowledgments

Dr. McCray was supported by a Health Resources and Services Administration Faculty Development Program in Primary Care training grant. Dr. Bogner was supported by an NIMH mentored Patient-Oriented Research Career Development Award (MH 67671-01) and is a Robert Wood Johnson Generalist Physician Faculty Scholar (2004-2008). The Spectrum Study was supported by grants MH62210-01, MH62210-01S1, and MH67077 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

References

  1. Allport GW. Personality; a psychological interpretation. New York: H. Holt and Company; 1937. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barbour KA, Blumenthal JA. Exercise training and depression in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging. 2005;26(Suppl 1):119–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beck AT. The current state of cognitive therapy: a 40-year retrospective. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62(9):953–959. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.9.953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bienvenu OJ, Samuels JF, Costa PT, Reti IM, Eaton WW, Nestadt G. Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality: a higher- and lower-order personality trait investigation in a community sample. Depression & Anxiety. 2004;20(2):92–97. doi: 10.1002/da.20026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bogner HR, Ford DE, Gallo JJ. The role of cardiovascular disease in the identification and management of depression by primary care physicians. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2006;14(1):71–78. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000192479.82189.e1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bogner HR, Wittink M, Merz JF, Straton JB, Cronholm PF, Rabins PV, et al. Personal characteristics of older primary care patients who provide a buccal swab for APOE testing and banking of genetic material: The Spectrum Study. Community Genetics. 2004;7(4):202–210. doi: 10.1159/000082263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boyce P, Parker G, Barnett B, Cooney M, Smith F. Personality as a vulnerability factor to depression. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1991;159:106–114. doi: 10.1192/bjp.159.1.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chopra KK, Bagby RM, Dickens S, Kennedy SH, Ravindran A, Levitan RD. A dimensional approach to personality in atypical depression. Psychiatry Research. 2005;134(2):161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Clark LA, Watson D, Mineka S. Temperament, personality, and the mood and anxiety disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103(1):103–116. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Costa P. In: NEO Five Factor Inventory Test Booklet-Form S (Adult) McCrae RR, editor. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  11. Costa P, McCrae RR. Stability and change in personality assessment: the revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1997;68(1):86–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6801_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cuijpers P, van Straten A, Donker M. Personality traits of patients with mood and anxiety disorders. Psychiatry Research. 2005;133(2–3):229–237. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Gallo JJ, Bogner HR, Straton JB, Margo K, Lesho P, Rabins PV, et al. Patient characteristics associated with participation in a practice-based study of depression in late life: the Spectrum study. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine. 2005;35(1):41–57. doi: 10.2190/K5B6-DD8E-TH1R-8GPT. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Gatz M, Johansson B, Pedersen N, Berg S, Reynolds C. A cross-national self-report measure of depressive symptomatology. International Psychogeriatrics. 1993;5(2):147–156. doi: 10.1017/s1041610293001486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hogan R, Johnson J, Briggs SR. Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego: Academic Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  16. McCrae RR. The five-factor model and its assessment in clinical settings. Journal of Personality Assessment. 1991;57(3):399–314. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5703_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1987;52(1):81–90. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.1.81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Newmann JP, Engel RJ, Jensen JE. Age differences in depressive symptom experiences. Journal of Gerontology. 1991;46(5):P224–235. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.5.p224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Perkins AJ, Kroenke K, Unutzer J, Katon W, Williams JW, Hope C, et al. Common comorbidity scales were similar in their ability to predict health care costs and mortality. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2004;57(10):1040–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2004.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Radloff L. The CES-D Scale: A Self-Report Depression Scale for Research in the General Population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
  21. Schulman KA, Berlin JA, Harless W, Kerner JF, Sistrunk S, Gersh BJ, et al. The effect of race and sex on physicians’ recommendations for cardiac catheterization.[see comment][erratum appears in N Engl J Med 1999 Apr 8;340(14):1130] New England Journal of Medicine. 1999;340(8):618–626. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199902253400806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Sheifer SE, Escarce JJ, Schulman KA. Race and sex differences in the management of coronary artery disease. American Heart Journal. 2000;139(5):848–857. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8703(00)90017-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Thompson LW. Cognitive-behavioral therapy and treatment for late-life depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1996;57(Suppl 5):29–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Weiss A, Costa PT, Jr, Karuza J, Duberstein PR, Friedman B, McCrae RR. Cross-sectional age differences in personality among medicare patients aged 65 to 100. Psychology & Aging. 2005;20(1):182–185. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Wiggins JS. Paradigms of personality assessment. New York: Guilford Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  26. Wittink MN, Barg FK, Gallo JJ. Unwritten rules of talking to doctors about depression: integrating qualitative and quantitative methods. Annals of Family Medicine. 2006;4(4):302–309. doi: 10.1370/afm.558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES