Skip to main content
Pharmacy and Therapeutics logoLink to Pharmacy and Therapeutics
. 2009 Dec;34(12):678-683, 694.

Using Meta-analysis to Compare the Efficacy of Medications for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Youths

Stephen V Faraone
PMCID: PMC2810184  PMID: 20140141

Abstract

Medications used to treat attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have been well researched, but comparisons among agents are hindered by the absence of head-to-head clinical trials. By using meta-analysis, we sought to compare the efficacy of these medications for the symptoms of ADHD. We analyzed published literature on the pharmacotherapy of ADHD to describe the variability of drug–placebo effect sizes and conducted a literature search to identify double-blind, placebo-controlled studies of youths with ADHD that were published after 1979. Meta-analysis regression was used to assess the influence of the medication type on drug effects. We also assessed for publication bias.

Thirty-two trials met our criteria and were included in this meta-analysis. These trials involved 16 drugs using 20 different outcome measures of ADHD behaviors. The effect sizes for immediate-release stimulants and long-acting stimulants were similar and were greater than the effect sizes for non-stimulants. There was no evidence of publication bias.

Although nearly all of the ADHD medications had significant effects, we found substantial variability. When translated into the costs of treating large numbers of patients, these effect sizes have implications for formulary medication choices.

Keywords: ADHD, medications, efficacy

INTRODUCTION

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), which affects 8% to 10% of youths and 4% to 5% of adults,13 is a source of chronic disability that can lead to academic impairment, social dysfunction, substance-use disorders, and an increased risk of accidents and criminal behavior.4 The symptoms and associated impairments of ADHD result in high costs for clinical services and to society.510

Fortunately, ADHD is a treatable disorder, and several classes of medications are available on many formularies. The stimulant medications comprise methylphenidate (MPH), dextroamphetamine (d-Amph), and mixed amphetamine salts (MAS). Stimulants have been used for decades to treat ADHD. They increase the availability of synaptic dopamine11,12 and reduce the overactivity, impulsivity, and inattention that are characteristic of patients with ADHD. Stimulants also improve associated behaviors, including on-task activities, academic performance, and social functioning.13 Compared with short-acting formulations, which require dosing up to three times per day, long-acting (LA) formulations extend the action of these medications over 8 to 12 hours to allow once-daily dosing.1416 Although stimulants are the most widely used ADHD medications, several nonstimulant medications are also effective for treating the disorder: tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs),1719 bupropion (Wellbutrin, GlaxoSmithKline),2022 modafinil (Provigil, Cephalon),23,24 monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitors,25,26 extended-release guanfacine (Intuniv, Shire),27,28 and atomoxetine (Strattera, Lilly).2931

ADHD is a costly disorder because (1) its onset is early in life, (2) it causes a wide range of disabilities, and (3) it is a chronic condition, with most cases persisting into adulthood.32 The medical utilization costs of ADHD have been quantified by several investigators.

Leibson et al.7 conducted a population-based cohort study of 4,119 children. Among these children, the 309 who met criteria for ADHD were more likely to also have nonpsychiatric medical diagnoses, including major injuries. Children with ADHD were more likely to use inpatient, outpatient, and emergency care services. The nine-year median costs for youths with ADHD, compared with those without ADHD, were more than double ($4,306 vs. $1,944). Similar results were seen by Chan et al.33 In their study of 5,439 children, 165 had ADHD and 322 had asthma. Children with ADHD had higher mean total health care costs ($1,151) than those with asthma ($1,091) and children with neither disorder ($712).

Another cohort study of 100,000 youths reported that the annual average medical expenditure was $1,574 per ADHD patient compared with $541 among matched controls.9 In a cohort study of 2,252 adults, Secnik et al.8 found that individuals with ADHD had significantly higher outpatient costs ($3,009 vs. $1,492 for matched controls), inpatient costs ($1,259 vs. $514), prescription drug costs ($1,673 vs. $1,008), and total medical costs ($5,651 vs. $2,771). These studies were consistent in concluding that people with ADHD, compared with persons without ADHD, showed substantially greater use of medical care in multiple delivery settings.

Given the high human and financial costs of ADHD, treatment is clearly warranted. But how should physicians, managed care professionals, and P&T committees choose among the many medications known to be safe and effective?

Ideally, physicians would evaluate different treatments by referring to randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled studies that directly compared the treatments. Although individual medications are typically investigated thoroughly during placebo-controlled studies, studies comparing different treatments are less common and often involve smaller sample sizes. In the absence of direct comparative head-to-head trials, the best evidence comes from comparing the individual randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled studies of each treatment using the method of meta-analysis, which provides a systematic quantitative framework for assessing the effects of medications reported in different studies.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

A literature search was conducted to identify double-blind, placebo-controlled studies of ADHD in youths (6 to 18 years of age) that were published after 1979. We included both parallel and crossover designs. We searched for articles using the following search engines: PubMed, Ovid, ERIC, CINAHL, MEDLINE, PREMEDLINE, the Cochrane database, e-psyche, and social sciences abstracts. We also reviewed presentations from meetings of the American Psychiatric Association (APA) and the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (AACAP). We included only studies that used randomized, double-blind methodology with placebo controls; that defined ADHD using diagnostic criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 3rd revised edition (DSM-III-R) or 4th edition (DSM-IV); and that followed subjects for two weeks or more.

To be included, studies had to have presented the means and standard deviations (SDs) of either change in scores or endpoint scores for the drug and placebo groups. For studies presenting data for more than one fixed dose, we used the highest dose because it seemed likely that the inclusion of lower doses would bias the study toward less optimal outcomes compared with studies using a design that used titration to optimal doses. Ideally, we would have included an analysis of dose effects, but such data were not available in enough studies to make this feasible.

We excluded studies that rated behavior in laboratory environments, involved fewer than 20 subjects in either the drug or placebo groups, explored appropriate doses for future work, or selected ADHD samples for the presence of a comorbid condition (e.g., studies of ADHD among children with mental retardation).

Effect sizes for dependent measures in each study were expressed as standardized mean differences (SMDs). The SMD is computed by taking the mean of the active drug group minus the mean of the placebo group and dividing the result by the pooled SD of the groups. Studies reporting change scores provided endpoint minus baseline scores for drug and placebo groups. In this case, the SMD was computed as the difference between change scores. For studies reporting endpoint scores, the SMD was computed as the difference between endpoint scores.

Studies were weighted according to the number of participants included. Our meta-analysis used the random effects model of DerSimonian and Laird.34 We used the I2 index to assess the heterogeneity of effect sizes.35 Its value lies between 0 and 100 and estimates the percentage of variation among effect sizes that can be attributed to heterogeneity. A significant I2 suggests that the effect sizes analyzed are not estimating the same population effect size. We used metaanalytic regression to assess the degree to which the effect sizes varied with class of medication. Egger’s method was used to assess for publication biases.36

For each study, we treated all dependent outcome measures reported as a separate data point for entry into the analysis. Several studies provided data on more than one measure. Because measures reported from the same study are not statistically independent, standard statistical procedures produce inaccurate P values. To address this intrastudy clustering, we adjusted variance estimates using Huber’s formula,37 as implemented in Stata software.38 This formula is a “theoretical bootstrap” that produces robust statistical tests. The cluster scores (i.e., the sum of scores within studies) are entered into the formula to estimate variance. The resulting P values are valid even when observations are not statistically independent.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the 32 articles that met criteria for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Studies are listed more than once if more than one drug was assessed or if independent studies of the same drug were reported. The studies in the table evaluated 15 drugs using 20 different measures of ADHD symptoms to assess efficacy. These measures were the Inattention/Overactivity with Aggression (IOWA) Conners Index; the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham (SNAP-IV) total score; inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive scales; the Clinical Global Impressions (CGI) scale (both overall ratings, ADHD specific ratings and improvement ratings); the Global Efficacy Scale; the ADHD Rating Scale total score; inattentive and hyperactive–impulsive scales; the Conners ADHD Rating Scale total score; inattentive and hyperactive–impulsive scales; the Children’s Scale; the Conners Abbreviated Symptom Questionnaire for Teachers; the ADHD Clinical Composite Rating Scale; and the Conners–Wells ADHD Adolescent Self-Report Scale.

TABLE 1.

Features of the Meta-analysis

Study Drug No. in Drug Group No. in Placebo Group Mean Age % Male DSM Edition
Conners, 1980 MPH 20 20 12 95
Taylor, 1987 MPH 37 37 9 100 3
Klorman, 1994 MPH 44 44 9 84 3R
Conners, 1996 Bupropion 61 31 9 90 3R
Schachar, 1997 MPH 37 29 8 77 3R
Manos, 1999 MAS 42 42 10 79 4
Manos, 1999 MPH 42 42 10 79 4
Zeiner, 1999 MPH 36 36 9 100 3R
Pliszka, 2000 MAS 20 18 8 NA NA
Pliszka, 2000 MPH 20 18 8 NA NA
James, 2001 MAS 35 35 9 60 4
James, 2001 d-Amph 35 35 9 60 4
James, 2001 d-Amph ER 35 35 9 60 4
Wolraich, 2001 MPH 97 90 9 87 4
Wolraich, 2001 OROS MPH 95 90 9 78 4
Michelson, 2001 Atomoxetine 82 83 11 72 4
Michelson, 2002 Atomoxetine 84 83 10 71 4
Biederman, 2002 Atomoxetine 31 21 10 0 4
Biederman, 2002 MAS-XR 120 203 9 80 4
Greenhill, 2002 MPH-MR 155 159 9 83 4
Spencer, 2002 Atomoxetine 49 47 10 81 4
Spencer, 2002 Atomoxetine 53 45 10 81 4
Biederman, 2003 MPH-LA 63 71 9 77 4
Wigal, 2004 MPH 41 41 10 88 4
Wigal, 2004 d-MPH 42 41 10 88 4
Kelsey, 2004 Atomoxetine 126 60 10 71 4
Biederman, 2005 Modafinil 163 81 10 71 4
Findling, 2005 OROS MPH 89 85 9 70 4
Weiss, 2005 Atomoxetine 99 51 NA 80 4
Swanson, 2006 Modafinil 120 63 10 74 4
Wilens, 2006 OROS MPH 87 90 15 80 4
Greenhill, 2006 Modafinil 127 65 10 73 4
Biederman, 2006 Modafinil 48 51 11 74 4
Spencer, 2006 MAS-XR 26 28 14 64 4
Spencer, 2006 MAS-XR 27 24 14 64 4
Findling, 2006 MPH 120 39 10 79 4
Findling, 2006 MPH-MR 120 39 10 81 4
Biederman, 2007 LDX 73 72 9 71 4
Palumbo, 2008 MPH 29 30 9 83 4
Palumbo, 2008 Clonidine 31 30 9 87 4
Biederman, 2008 GXR 54 61 10 66 4

Note: Studies are listed once for each drug evaluated.

d-Amph = dextroamphetamine; d-MPH = dexmethylphenidate; DSM = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; ER = extended-release; GXR = guanfacine extended release; LA = long-acting; LDX = lisdexamfetamine dimesylate; MAS = mixed amphetamine salts; MPH = methylphenidate; MR = modified release; NA = not available; OROS = osmotic-release oral system; XR = extended release.

Each drug–placebo comparison provided information on more than one outcome score, which allowed us to compute 150 effect sizes. The drugs studied fell into three classes: non-stimulants, immediate-release (IR) stimulants, and LA stimulants.

The nonstimulants studied were atomoxetine, bupropion, modafinil, clonidine (Catapres, Boehringer Ingelheim) and extended-release guanfacine (GXR). The IR stimulants were methylphenidate (MPH), dextroamphetamine (d-Amph), mixed amphetamine salts (MAS), and dexmethylphenidate (d-MPH). The LA stimulants were MAS-extended release (MAS-XR), d-Amph extended release (d-Amph ER), MPH-modified release (MPH-MR), osmotic-release oral system MPH (OROS-MPH), MPH-long acting (MPH-LA), and lisdexamfetamine dimesylate (LDX).

A pooled analysis of all studies computed an effect size of 0.78 (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.72, 0.83). As Figure 1 shows (with diamonds representing each subclass of medication), the effect sizes for IR stimulants (0.99; 95% CI, 0.88, 1.1) and LA stimulants (0.95; 95% CI, 0.85, 1.1) were similar and greater than the effect sizes for nonstimulants (0.57; 95% CI, 0.53, 0.62). There was no evidence of publication bias (t[146] = 1.1; P = 0.27) in the pooled analysis of all studies.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Standardized mean differences and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) stratified by type of drug. The point indicates the effect size for each study. The horizontal line through each box gives the 95% CI. The diamonds give CIs for each type of drug. d-Amph = dextroamphetamine; ER = extended release; ES = effect size; GXR = guanfacine extended-release; IR = immediate release; LA = long-acting; LCI = lower 95% confidence interval; LDX = lisdexamfetamine dimesylate; MAS = mixed amphetamine salts; MPH = methylphenidate; MR = modified release; No. = number of observations; OROS = osmotic release oral system; UCI = upper 95% confidence interval; XR = extended release.

I2 was significant (P < 0.0001) and indicated that 68% of the variability among effect sizes was a result of heterogeneity. One source of that heterogeneity was revealed by meta-analysis regression, which found a significant effect of drug type (F(2, 31) = 17; P < 0.0001). The effect sizes for nonstimulant medications were significantly less than those for IR stimulants (F[1, 31] = 25; P < 0.0001) or LA stimulants (F[1, 31] = 15; P = 0.001). The two classes of stimulant medications did not differ significantly (F[1, 31] = 0.3; P = 0.62).

We ran the heterogeneity analyses stratified by type of medication. For the LA stimulants, I2 was significant (P < 0.0001) and indicated that 66% of the variability among effect sizes was a result of heterogeneity. For nonstimulants and short-acting stimulants, I2 was low (0 and 2.7%, respectively) and was not significant (P = 0.8 and 0.5, respectively).

Figure 1 presents our results stratified by type of medication. The effect size for each medication was highly significant (P < 0.001) with the exception of two non-stimulants: bupropion (P = 0.2) and clonidine (P = 0.9). As the figure shows, both drugs had small effect sizes, but because the number of observations for these was small, we had low power to detect such small effects.

DISCUSSION

Our meta-analysis found significant differences between stimulants and nonstimulants. We found no evidence of publication bias. Although head-to-head trials are needed to make definitive statements about differences in efficacy, our results comparing stimulants and nonstimulants are compatible with the efficacy differences between atomoxetine and methylphenidate3941 and between atomoxetine and MAS42,43 and the conclusions of a prior review limited to a smaller subset of studies that excluded short-acting stimulants.44

The robust effects of most ADHD medications can be seen in Figure 1, with most of them having statistically significant effects on ADHD outcomes. It is also notable that for LA and IR stimulants, variability within each class was statistically significant. As described by Faraone,45 the SMD effect size statistic can be translated into treatment costs of wasted treatment using simulations. In the absence of real-world comparative effectiveness trials, this is often the only way to model treatment costs.

For illustration, we can assume that placebo outcomes are distributed as a standard normal distribution and treatment outcomes are similarly distributed with three times the variability plus a constant that is equal to the SMD. When we simulate treatment of 100,000 patients, the SMD for nonstimulants (0.57) would yield 40,000 patients who do not respond (i.e., 40,000 wasted treatments). Each wasted treatment incurs unnecessary costs. These include the cost of the medication and the cost of the visits needed to titrate the medication. The SMD for both types of stimulants (1.0) would yield only 25,000 wasted treatments. If we consider the largest SMD in Figure 1 (1.52 for LDX), the number of wasted treatments would be only 12,000. This outlying observation might explain why we found significant heterogeneity for LA stimulants but not for the other classes of medication.

These results have implications for how physicians, other health care professionals, and P&T committees should choose among the many medications known to be safe and effective for ADHD. Currently, there are no strong criteria for deciding which medication should be tried first. Physicians must rely on clinical experience, the patient’s history of pharmacotherapy, the need for once-a-day dosing, and other clinical features that might influence their choice. For example, for patients with a history of substance abuse, prescribers would likely consider nonstimulants or stimulants that are more resistant to abuse.

Although reducing the costs of treatment should be secondary to finding the optimal treatment for each patient, the cost data reviewed previously suggest that improved efficacy and reduced costs are not necessarily at odds with one another. Cost does become an issue for P&T committees when IR stimulants (which are available as generic brands and are thus less expensive) are being compared with LA stimulants (most of which are not available as generics and hence are more expensive). Although the meta-analysis shows no differences in efficacy between these two types of stimulants, LA stimulants are associated with greater adherence46 and a lower likelihood of being abused or diverted.47 The strongest conclusion from the meta-analysis is that the nonstimulants are less efficacious than either type of stimulant. This suggests that a stimulant medication should be the treatment of choice in the absence of any clinical indicator that a nonstimulant is preferable.

It is difficult to compare the efficacy of ADHD agents with the efficacy of the most common nonmedical treatment for ADHD—behavior therapy; whereas there are many double-blind placebo-controlled studies of medication, behavior therapy studies tend to be uncontrolled. The meta-analysis of behavior therapy studies by Fabiano et al.48 described 154 uncontrolled studies and only 20 controlled studies, and none of the latter studies were reported to have used double-blind methodology. The mean SMD for all of these studies was 0.67 (95% CI, 0.54–0.80). Thus, the SMD for behavior therapy is comparable to the SMD for nonstimulants and less than the SMD for stimulants.

STUDY LIMITATIONS

This work had several limitations. We relied on simulations to model costs of wasted treatment instead of using comparative-effectiveness trials, which were not available. Further, the interpretation of effect sizes from clinical trials might not be generalizable to the real-world clinical experience of patients, especially if trials use specially selected patients, such as those without other concurrent psychiatric disorders or concomitant medications. Effect sizes are sometimes derived from only single trials of a specific medication. Multiple trials of each medication yield more confidence in the estimates of effect size, as indicated by the CIs in Figure 1. Because we relied on data presented by authors, we were restricted by what investigators chose to present. For example, we could not compute the effect sizes at specific time points, because such data were rarely provided.

Future work should review time-course studies such as the analogue school laboratory paradigm. Although that work does not assess outcomes in the patient’s environment, it would provide useful data about efficacy peak or trough effect. Similarly, we did not assess differential duration of action between medication classes because this effect is rarely presented.

Although meta-analyses for indirect comparisons of treatments are useful, there are some disadvantages. All meta-analyses are limited by the quality of the studies analyzed. For that reason, we confined our review to double-blind, placebo-controlled studies.

The results of a meta-analysis may also differ from those of direct comparisons. For example, Bucher et al.49 compared sulfamethoxazole–trimethoprim (e.g., Bactrim) with dapsone–pyrimethamine for the prevention of Pneumocystis carinii in HIV-infected patients. The meta-analysis suggested that the former treatment was much better.

In contrast, direct comparisons from randomized trials found a much smaller, nonsignificant difference. Song et al.50 examined 44 published metaanalyses that used measure of effect magnitude to compare treatments indirectly. In most cases, the metaanalyses did not differ from the results of direct comparisons. However, for three of the 44 comparisons, there were significant differences between the direct and the meta-analysis estimates.

Chou et al.51 compared initial highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) plus a protease inhibitor (PI) against a non-nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitor (NNRTI). They conducted a direct meta-analysis of 12 head-to-head comparisons and an indirect meta-analysis of six trials of NNRTI-based HAART and eight trials of PI-based HAART. In the direct meta-analysis, NNRTI-based regimens were better than PI-based regimens for virological suppression. In contrast, the indirect meta-analyses showed that NNRTI-based HAART was worse than PI-based HAART for virological suppression.

Thus, although results of meta-analyses usually agree with those of head-to-head direct comparisons, the results of indirect comparisons using measures of effect magnitude should be viewed cautiously. Many variables can affect the apparent efficacy of treatments such as study quality, the nature of the population studied, the setting for the intervention, and the nature of the outcome measure. If these factors differ between studies, indirect comparisons can be misleading. For example, if all the stimulant studies used outcome measures that were more reliable than those used in the nonstimulant studies, the apparent difference between stimulants and nonstimulants would be an artifact.

CONCLUSION

Despite the limitations encountered, the findings highlight the variability of effect of ADHD medications. The efficacy of IR and LA stimulants was similar and significantly greater than that of nonstimulants. Moreover, for LA and IR stimulants, variability within each class was statistically significant. Because medication effect size differences translate into large cost differences when one is treating many patients, these data should be taken into account when P&T committees are making formulary decisions.

Footnotes

Disclosure. This work was supported, in part, by a grant from Shire Development, Inc., in Wayne, Pennsylvania, to Dr. Faraone. The data analysis and manuscript writing were completed by Dr. Faraone; Health Learning Systems, part of CommonHealth®, provided assistance with proofreading, editing, and submission of the manuscript. Dr. Faraone is currently receiving research support from Shire Development and the National Institutes of Health. In previous years, he received consulting fees or research support and was on the advisory board or a speaker for Shire Laboratories, Inc.; Eli Lilly & Company; Pfizer; Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceutical, Inc.; and the National Institutes of Health. Portions of this article have been updated from earlier research published online by Faraone SV, Biederman J, and Spencer TJ, in Medscape General Medicine 2006;8(4)4.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Faraone SV, Sergeant J, Gillberg C, Biederman J. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: Is it an American condition? World Psychiatr. 2003;2(2):104–113. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Faraone SV, Biederman J. What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. J Atten Disord. 2005;9(2):384–391. doi: 10.1177/1087054705281478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kessler RC, Adler L, Barkley R, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(4):716–723. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Biederman J, Faraone SV. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2005;366(9481):237–248. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)66915-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Chan E, Zhan C, Homer C.Health care use and costs for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: National estimates from the Medical Expenditure Panel Survey Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 20021565(5):504–511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Birnbaum HG, Kessler RC, Lowe SW, et al. Costs of attention-deficit–hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in the U.S: Excess costs of persons with ADHD and their family members in 2000. Curr Med Res Opin. 2005;21(2):195–206. doi: 10.1185/030079904X20303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leibson CL, Katusic SK, Barbaresi WJ, et al. Use and costs of medical care for children and adolescents with and without attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. 2001;285(1):60–66. doi: 10.1001/jama.285.1.60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Secnik K, Swensen A, Lage MJ. Comorbidities and costs of adult patients diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pharmacoeconomics. 2005;23(1):93–102. doi: 10.2165/00019053-200523010-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Swensen AR, Birnbaum HG, Secnik K, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Increased costs for patients and their families. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2003;42(12):1415–1423. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200312000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wu EQ, Birnbaum HG, Zhang HF. Health care costs of adults treated for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder who received alternative drug therapies. J Manag Care Pharm. 2007;13(7):561–569. doi: 10.18553/jmcp.2007.13.7.561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang G, et al. Mechanism of action of methylphenidate: Insights from PET imaging studies. J Atten Disord. 2002;6(Supp. 1):S31–S43. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601s05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Volkow ND, Wang G, Fowler JS, et al. Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular dopamine in the human brain. J Neurosci. 2001;21(2):RC121. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-02-j0001.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Greenhill LL, Pliszka S, Dulcan MK, et al. Summary of the practice parameter for the use of stimulant medications in the treatment of children, adolescents, and adults. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(11):1352–1355. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Greenhill LL, Findling RL, Swanson JM. A double-blind, placebo-controlled study of modified-release methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2002;109(3):E39. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.3.e39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wolraich M, Greenhill LL, Pelham W, et al. Randomized controlled trial of OROS methylphenidate q.d. in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2001;108(4):883–892. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.4.883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Biederman J, Lopez FA, Boellner SW, Chandler MC. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group study of SLI381 in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2002;110(2):258–266. doi: 10.1542/peds.110.2.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Spencer T, Biederman J, Coffey B, et al. A double-blind comparison of desipramine and placebo in children and adolescents with chronic tic disorder and comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59(7):649–656. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.7.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wilens T, Biederman J, Baldessarini R, et al. Cardiovascular effects of therapeutic doses of tricyclic antidepressants in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(11):1491–1501. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199611000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Biederman J, Baldessarini RJ, Wright V, et al. A double-blind placebo controlled study of desipramine in the treatment of ADD: I. Efficacy. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28(5):777–784. doi: 10.1097/00004583-198909000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Conners K, Casat C, Gualtieri T, et al. Bupropion hydrochloride in attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. J Am Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35(10):1314–1321. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199610000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Casat CD, Pleasants DZ, Schroeder DH, Parler DW. Bupropion in children with attention deficit disorder. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1989;25(2):198–201. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Casat CD, Pleasants DZ, Van Wyck Fleet J. A double-blind trial of bupropion in children with attention deficit disorder. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1987;23(1):120–122. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Taylor FB, Russo J. Efficacy of modafinil compared to dextroamphetamine for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2000;10(4):311–320. doi: 10.1089/cap.2000.10.311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rugino TA, Copley TC. Effects of modafinil in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An open-label study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(2):230–235. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ernst M. MAOI treatment of Adult ADHD. Paper presented at the National Institute of Mental Health Conference on Alternative Pharmacology of ADHD; Washington, DC. 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Shekim WO, Davis LG, Bylund DB, et al. MAO in children with attention deficit disorder and hypreractivity: A pilot study. Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139(7):936–938. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.7.936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Scahill L, Chappell PB, Kim YS, et al. A placebo-controlled study of guanfacine in the treatment of children with tic disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(7):1067–1074. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Biederman J, Melmed RD, Patel A, et al. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of guanfacine extended release in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2008;121(1):e73–e84. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Spencer T, Biederman J. Non-stimulant treatment for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. 2002;6(Supp. 1):S109–S119. doi: 10.1177/070674370200601s13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Michelson D, Faries D, Wernicke J, et al. Atomoxetine in the treatment of children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A randomized, placebo-controlled, dose–response study. Pediatrics. 2001;108(5):E83. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.5.e83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Michelson D, Adler L, Spencer T, et al. Atomoxetine in adults with ADHD: Two randomized, placebo-controlled studies. Biol Psychiatry. 2003;53(2):112–120. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01671-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Faraone S, Biederman J, Mick E. The age-dependent decline of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychol Med. 2006;36(2):159–165. doi: 10.1017/S003329170500471X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chan E, Zhan C, Homer CJ. Health care use and costs for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: National estimates from the medical expenditure panel survey. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(5):504–511. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.156.5.504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Controlled Clin Trials. 1986;7:177–188. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in metaanalyses. BMJ. 2003;327(7414):557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Egger M, Davey Smith G, et al. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Huber PJ. The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under non-standard conditions. Proc Fifth Berkeley Sympos Math Statist Probab. 1967;1:221–233. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Stata User’s Guide: Release 70. College Station, TX: Stata Corp.; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Kemner JE, Starr HL, Ciccone PE, et al. Outcomes of OROS methylphenidate compared with atomoxetine in children with AHD: A multicenter, randomized prospective study. Adv Ther. 2005;22(5):498–512. doi: 10.1007/BF02849870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Newcorn JH, Kratochvil CJ, Allen AJ, et al. Atomoxetine and osmotically released methylphenidate for the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Acute comparison and differential response. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165:721–730. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05091676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Starr HL, Kemner J. Multicenter, randomized, open-label study of OROS methylphenidate versus atomoxetine: Treatment outcomes in African-American children with ADHD. J Natl Med Assoc. 2005;97(10 Suppl):11S–16S. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wigal SB, Wigal TL, McGough JJ, et al. A laboratory school comparison of mixed amphetamine salts extended release (Adderall XR) and atomoxetine (Strattera) in schoolaged children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. 2005;9(1):275–289. doi: 10.1177/1087054705281121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Faraone SV, Wigal S, Hodgkins P. Forecasting three-month outcomes in a laboratory school comparison of mixed amphetamine salts extended release (Adderall XR) and atomoxetine (Strattera) in schoolaged children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Atten Disord. 2007;11(1):74–82. doi: 10.1177/1087054706292196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Santosh P, et al. Long-acting medications for the hyperkinetic disorders: A systematic review and European treatment guideline. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;15(8):476–495. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0549-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Faraone SV. Understanding the effect size of ADHD medications: Implications for clinical care. Medscape Psychiatry Mental Health. 2003;8(2) [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gau SS, Shen HY, Chou MC, et al. Determinants of adherence to methylphenidate and the impact of poor adherence on maternal and family measures. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2006;16(3):286–297. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.16.286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wilens T, Gignac M, Swezey A, et al. Characteristics of adolescents and young adults with ADHD who divert or misuse their prescribed medications. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006;45(4):408–414. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000199027.68828.b3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Fabiano GA, Pelham WE, Jr, Coles EK, et al. A meta-analysis of behavioral treatments for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009;29(2):129–140. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bucher HC, Guyatt GH, Griffith LE, Walter SD. The results of direct and indirect treatment comparisons in meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J Clin Epidemiol. 1997;50(6):683–691. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(97)00049-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Song F, Altman DG, Glenny AM, Deeks JJ. Validity of indirect comparison for estimating efficacy of competing interventions: Empirical evidence from published metaanalyses. BMJ. 2003;326(7387):472. doi: 10.1136/bmj.326.7387.472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Chou R, Fu R, Huffman LH, Korthuis PT. Initial highly-active antiretroviral therapy with a protease inhibitor versus a non-nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitor: Discrepancies between direct and indirect metaanalyses. Lancet. 2006;368(9546):1503–1515. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69638-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pharmacy and Therapeutics are provided here courtesy of MediMedia, USA

RESOURCES