We read with great interest the article by Ferner [1], which focused on the methodological difficulties of detecting medication errors. The author has acknowledged the limited reliability of epidemiological data collected by the self-report method. There is a high possibility that spontaneous error report leads to significant underestimation of true error frequency, since personnel may omit to report errors. On the other hand, direct observation, especially by trained, disguised observers, is considered the most efficient and accurate method for detecting medication errors [2].
We believe that the self-report method may further be associated with underreporting of specific medication error types, especially with dose errors. In a recent multinational study, Valentin et al. [3] used self report to assess the types of parenteral drug administration errors in Intensive Care Units (ICUs). They found that the frequency of dose errors was much lower compared with the frequency of administration time errors and dose omission errors. In contrast, in five ICU studies [4–8] based on error direct observation, wrong dose was observed and included among the three more frequent error types in all five studies, while administration time and dose omission errors were observed in four and three of these studies, respectively, and were included among the three more frequent error types in only two of these five studies each.
There are two reasons why different medication error types may not be equally reported. First, personnel must become aware of an error in order to report it and, compared with administration time and dose omission errors (which can be detected through ICU daily charts), dose errors are much less likely to be noticed. Second, well-described self-report bias includes social desirability bias and self-esteem bias; according to them, respondents tend to reply in a manner either viewed favourably by important others or consistent with their self-esteem perceptions, respectively [9]. Dose errors have been primarily attributed to individual deficiencies, such as inadequate mathematical skills or medication knowledge, limited experience and failure to follow policies (i.e. properly checking drugs) [10]. In contrast, administration time and dose omission errors have generally been associated with organizational deficiencies, mainly increased workload. Due to the subconscious tendency to be socially agreeable and protect self-esteem, it seems plausible to expect that self-reporting individuals would more likely underreport medication error types associated with personal deficiencies.
If dose error underestimation is really a problem in self-report studies, this could lead to erroneous conclusions about medication error epidemiology and contributing factors, and, more importantly, to ineffective preventive interventions. The hypothesis that dose errors are underreported during self-report could be tested by studies designed specifically for comparing the frequencies of different error types detected by direct observation vs. self-report. However, simultaneous medication error detection by these two methods during a study could lead to increased error self report, because even if disguised observation is used, personnel may suspect the true reasons for the observers' presence (since they are concurrently asked to self-report errors).
We would therefore suggest that a more appropriate way to compare error types between direct observation and self report would be by conducting the study sequentially, in two phases. During phase 1, only self report would be used for medication error detection, whereas during phase 2 self report would be combined with disguised observation. This study design would allow comparison of self report error frequency between phases 1 and 2 (to test whether even disguised observation can affect error self report), and comparison of self report error frequency during phase 1 vs. direct observation error frequency (to test whether certain error types are underestimated during self report). To minimize bias, individuals asked to self-report errors and being observed in the two study phases should be the same, while factors contributing to errors (e.g. nursing workload) should not differ much. Finally, employing a large sample and a multicentre study design could minimize random variation in error incidence between the two study phases.
Competing interests
None declared.
REFERENCES
- 1.Ferner RE. The epidemiology of medication errors: the methodological difficulties. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2009;67:614–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2125.2009.03417.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Flynn EA, Barker KN, Pepper GA, Bates DW, Mikeal RL. Comparison of methods for detecting medication errors in 36 hospitals and skilled-nursing facilities. Am J Health Syst Pharm. 2002;59:436–46. doi: 10.1093/ajhp/59.5.436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Valentin A, Capuzzo M, Guidet B, Moreno R, Metnitz B, Bauer P, Metnitz P. Errors in administration of parenteral drugs in intensive care units: multinational prospective study. BMJ. 2009;338:b814. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fahimi F, Ariapanah P, Faizi M, Shafaghi B, Namdar R, Tavakoli Ardakani M. Errors in preparation and administration if intravenous medications in the intensive care unit of a teaching hospital: an observational study. Aust Crit Care. 2008;21:110–6. doi: 10.1016/j.aucc.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kopp BJ, Erstad BL, Allen ME, Theodorou AA, Priestley G. Medication errors and adverse drug events in an intensive care unit: direct observation approach for detection. Crit Care Med. 2006;34:415–25. doi: 10.1097/01.ccm.0000198106.54306.d7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tissot E, Cornette C, Demoly P, Jacquet M, Barale F, Capellier G. Medication errors at the administration stage in an intensive care unit. Intensive Care Med. 1999;25:353–9. doi: 10.1007/s001340050857. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van den Bemt PM, Fijn R, van der Voort PHJ, Gossen AA, Egberts TCG, Brouwers JRBJ. Frequency and determinants of drug administration errors in the intensive care unit. Crit Care Med. 2002;30:846–50. doi: 10.1097/00003246-200204000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Calabrese AD, Erstad BL, Brandl K, Barletta JF, Kane SL, Sherman DS. Medication administration errors in adult patients in the ICU. Intensive Care Med. 2001;27:1592–8. doi: 10.1007/s001340101065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Waltz CF, Strickland OL, Lenz ER. Measurement in Nursing and Health Research. 3rd edn. New York: Springer Publishing Co., Inc; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- 10.O'Shea E. Factors contributing to medication errors: a literature review. J Clin Nurs. 1999;8:496–504. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2702.1999.00284.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]