Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Mar 5.
Published in final edited form as: Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009 Mar 5;150B(2):271–281. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30808

5-HTTLPR Genotype and Anxiety-Related Personality Traits: A meta-analysis and new data

Marcus R Munafò 1, Nelson B Freimer 2, Whitney Ng 1, Roel Ophoff 2,3, Juha Veijola 4, Jouko Miettunen 4, Marjo-Riitta Järvelin 5,6, Anja Taanila 6,7, Jonathan Flint 8
PMCID: PMC2819421  NIHMSID: NIHMS64455  PMID: 18546120

Abstract

We investigated the strength of evidence for association of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and the personality trait of Harm Avoidance. We used new primary data from a large sample of adults drawn from the Finnish population. We also applied meta-analytic techniques to synthesize existing published data. The large number studies of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism allowed us to apply a formal test of publication bias, as well as formally investigate the impact of potential moderating factors such as measurement instrument.

Univariate ANOVA of primary data (n = 3,872), with 5-HTTLPR genotype as a between-groups factor, indicated no evidence of association with Harm Avoidance (p = 0.99). Meta-analysis indicated no evidence of significant association of 5-HTTLPR with Harm Avoidance (d = 0.02, p = 0.37), or EPQ Neuroticism (d = 0.01, p = 0.71), although there was evidence of association with NEO Neuroticism (d = 0.18, p < 0.001).

Our analyses indicate that the 5-HTTLPR variant is not associated with Harm Avoidance. Together with our previous analyses of a large sample of participants with extreme Neuroticism scores (defined by the EPQ), we have data that excludes a meaningful genetic effect of the 5-HTTLPR on two measures of anxiety-related personality traits. There remains the possibility that the variant influences the NEO personality questionnaire measure of Neuroticism. However, a large, well-powered primary study is required to test this hypothesis directly and adequately.

Keywords: 5-HTTLPR, Genotype, Meta-Analysis, Neuroticism, Harm Avoidance

Introduction

While twin studies have shown that individual differences in personality traits are substantially influenced by genetic factors (Loehlin 1993), identifying the specific genetic variants that are responsible for the variation in personality has proved to be difficult (Munafo et al. 2003). Theories of personality propose that individual differences in personality traits are associated with individual differences in activity in specific neurotransmitter pathways (Depue and Collins 1999), and as a result various candidate genes with known functional effects on these pathways have been investigated (Munafo et al. 2003). Amongst these, the serotonin transporter gene is one of the most widely investigated, particularly in relation to anxiety-related personality traits (Munafo et al. 2003). However, despite a very large number of published studies, there remains uncertainty regarding the strength and nature of any association (Munafo et al. 2005a).

Anxiety-related traits are continuously distributed dimensions of normal human personality, and are of particular interest in psychiatric genetics as they are predictive of risk of psychiatric disease (Ormel et al. 2004), in particular major depression (Willis-Owen et al. 2005), but may be assessed in the healthy population. These traits can be measured using a variety of questionnaires, of which the most frequently used are derived from Costa and McCrae’s (Costa and McCrae 1997) fivefactor model (FFM) of personality (NEO-PI, NEO-PI-R, NEO-FFI), Eysenck’s (Eysenck and Eysenck 1991) model of personality (EPQ, EPQ-R) and Cloninger’s (Cloninger 1986) tri-dimensional theory of personality (TPQ, TCI). The construct within each of these that corresponds most closely to an anxiety-related trait differs, with Costa and McCrae’s FFM and Eysenck’s model reflecting a Neuroticism (N) scale, and Cloninger’s tri-dimensional theory reflecting a Harm Avoidance (HA) scale. The N construct reflects a tendency to experience negative emotions and interpret situations as threatening (Costa and McCrae 1997), whereas the HA construct is defined as avoidance of aversive stimuli, anxiety proneness and an aversion to risk-taking (Cloninger 1986; Zohar et al. 2003).

There is some evidence that N and HA may not be equivalent. Previous studies have shown that the variance shared by N and HA is between 26% and 55% (De Fruyt et al. 2000). While Costa and McCrae’s N correlates highly with Eysenck’s N (0.77) (Aluja et al. 2002), it correlates only modestly with Cloninger’s HA (0.30) (Curtin et al. 1995), suggesting that measures of N may differ substantially from those of HA. Despite this, studies of the molecular genetic basis of anxiety-related traits have used both measures of N and HA almost equally.

The serotoninergic system has been the principal focus in molecular genetic investigations of emotional behaviour in general, and anxiety-related traits in particular (Lesch et al. 2003). The serotonin transporter (5-HTT) regulates the reuptake of 5-HT at synapses (Heils et al. 1996; Heinz et al. 2000; Lesch et al. 1996), and molecular genetic investigations of this pathway have focused in large part on the role of the serotonin transporter gene. The 5-HTTLPR is a polymorphism in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (Heils et al. 1996), with two common alleles: a long allele with 16 repeats (L) and a short allele with 14 repeats (S). The dominant S allele (Lesch et al. 1996) has been reported to result in lower transcriptional activity than the L allele (Heils et al. 1996), leading to a relative reduction in mRNA levels, serotonin binding, and re-uptake (Little et al. 1998).

Following the first published report of association between the S allele of 5-HTTLPR and increased anxiety-related personality trait score (Lesch et al. 1996), which suggested that this polymorphism may account for up to 4% of phenotypic variance in anxiety-related personality (using both the NEO measure of N and the TPQ / TCI measure of HA), a large number of studies have attempted to replicate this association. As this literature has grown, and in particular following several failures to replicate, a number of meta-analytic studies have attempted to determine with greater certainty the strength of the evidence for this association. Meta-analysis is an increasingly popular tool for assessing the strength of evidence for gene-phenotype associations, given the likely small effect size of single gene effects, and the resulting probability than most individual studies are likely to lack statistical power (Munafo and Flint 2004). In addition, meta-analytic techniques have the potential to identify unexpected sources of heterogeneity and test for possible publication bias.

We initially reported little evidence of an association between the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and anxiety-related personality (Munafo et al. 2003). However, two subsequent meta-analyses suggested an association with N (as measured by NEO instruments) but not with HA (Schinka et al. 2004; Sen et al. 2004a). These meta-analyses did not include measures of N derived using EPQ instruments. We subsequently concluded that while the magnitude of the effect appeared to differ by measurement instrument, there was no formal evidence that measurement instrument moderated any association (Munafo et al. 2005a), although various re-analyses indicated that caution is necessary in the use of meta-analytic techniques as methods used to create standardised scores and weight individual studies may bias results (Munafo et al. 2005b; Schinka 2005; Sen et al. 2005).

Given that the genetic effect of the 5-HTTLPR on N is likely to be small, we analysed extreme scoring individuals from two large English populations (n = 88,142, and 20,921) (Willis-Owen et al. 2005). Although the combined sample had sufficient power to detect an effect contributing 0.5% of phenotypic variance at a significance threshold of p = 0.01, we were unable to find statistically significant evidence of genetic association. In that study N was measured using the EPQ, leaving open the possibility that there is an effect of 5-HTTLPR on anxiety-related personality traits defined by a different instrument.

In the present study, we investigated the strength of evidence for association of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and the TCI measure of HA. We used a large sample of adults drawn from the Finnish population. We also applied established meta-analytic techniques to synthesize existing published data. The large number studies of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism allowed us to apply a formal test of publication bias, as well as investigate the impact of potential moderating factors such as measurement instrument.

Method

Participants

Participants in this study were part of the Northern Finland Birth Cohort (NFBC 1966), which consists of n = 12,058 live births to mothers in the two northern-most provinces of Finland in 1966 (Rantakallio 1988). DNA samples were collected from n = 5,923 participants within the NFBC 1966 birth cohort. Of these, data were available on a total of n = 3,872 participants who provided a DNA sample, were successfully genotyped for the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism, and completed the TCI measure of HA.

Measures

Participants completed a Finnish version of the TCI (Miettunen et al. 2004). Briefly, the original English version was translated into Finnish, and back-translated blindly to the original English scale, by a professional English translator. The original version and the back-translation were compared and corrections were made accordingly. Full details, including psychometric properties, are reported elsewhere (Miettunen et al. 2004). The TCI defines four personality dimensions: harm avoidance, novelty seeking, reward dependence and persistence. Harm avoidance is defined as the relative balance of anxiety proneness versus risk taking (Cloninger 1986).

Genotyping

Participants were genotyped for the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism as follows: Amplification of 5-HTTLPR was carried out in a 20 ul reaction containing 40–80 ng genomic DNA, 0.2 mM dATP, 0.2 mM dCTP, 0.2 mM dTTP, 0.1 mM dGTP, 0.1 mM 7-deaza-dGTP (Roche), 0.25 mM of each primer (Heils et al. 1996), 1.5 mM MgCl2, 1X Buffer II (Applied Biosystems), 2% DMSO, and 3.5 U AmpliTaq Gold Polymerase (Applied Biosystems). Thermal cycling conditions included an initial activation at 94 °C for 5 minutes, followed by two cycles of 94 (30s), 66 (30s) and 72°C (1 min), subsequent cycles with decreasing annealing temperature (three cycles of 65 °C [30 s], three cycles of 64 °C [30 s], 35 cycles of 63 °C [30 s]), and a final extension of 72 °C for 10 minutes. Data was collected on an ABI Prism 3700 DNA Analyzer, and analyzed using GeneScan v3.7 and Genotyper v3.6 NT. Scoring the LPR required an exceptionally rigorous procedure. Efforts to score this polymorphism using procedures similar to those employed for other length polymorphisms resulted in genotypes that were not in HWE. Each 5-HTTLPR genotype was independently scored by 3 readers. A fourth reader rescored all data using two filters to resolve the observed HWE discrepancies. The first filter removed scores with peak heights < 200. The second filter removed erroneous scores based on a background peak that was always present at a certain level but not always visible with the eye because of variable peak height of the second peak (allele 2); this filter compared: (peak height1 - peak height2) / peak height 1. Fidelity checks on a sub-set of samples genotyped in duplicate runs indicated a genotyping error rate of < 1%.

Selection of Studies for Inclusion in Meta-Analysis

Association studies of the serotonin transporter gene 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and anxiety-related traits in healthy adults (aged 16 and over) were included in the meta-analysis. Studies included those reporting data on male and female participants of any ethnic origin. Only studies that used validated, standardized, self-report questionnaire measures of HA or N were included. Data collected from clinical populations (including chronic headache, anorexia, alcoholism and organic disease) were excluded. In the case of studies that reported data previously reported elsewhere, the largest sample available was included only.

Search Strategy

The search was performed on two bibliographic databases: PubMed and PsychInfo. The search strategy included keywords of “personality”, “serotonin transporter gene”, “harm avoidance”, “anxiety”, “neuroticism” and “5-HTTLPR”. These databases were searched for appropriate studies from the earliest available data to 31st October 2007. Bibliographies of studies identified by the search strategy were also hand-searched for further relevant studies.

Data Extraction

The primary outcome measure was the mean score of the anxiety-related personality measure grouped by 5-HTTLPR genotype, with genotype grouped as presence (SS or SL) or absence (LL) of the 5-HTTLPR S allele. The data that were extracted included: number of participants in each genotype group, mean anxiety-related trait score and standard deviation by genotype, personality instrument, ancestry, and statement of Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (HWE). Data from healthy controls were used in studies that assessed both control and clinical populations. If multiple questionnaires were administered to the same participants, results were extracted from all questionnaires, and data were identified as having been extracted from a duplicate sample. In cases where data were not reported in an appropriate format for inclusion, authors were contacted directly and appropriate summary statistics requested.

Data Analysis

Primary data from the NFBC 1966 were analysed using linear regression models, for both the effect of number of S alleles, and genotype grouped as presence (SS or SL) or absence (LL) of the 5-HTTLPR S allele. This grouping assumes a dominant model of genetic action of the S allele, consistent with existing functional data and the majority of studies to date. We repeated all analyses adjusting for the effects of sex. Unadjusted and adjusted results are reported in the text. Data from all participants (i.e., unselected) were included in these analyses. Power analysis indicated 95% power to detect an effect size of d = 0.12, and 80% power to detect an effect size of d = 0.09 (in both cases equivalent to less than one point difference on the HA scale of the TCI).

Secondary data from the published literature were analyzed within both a fixed effects and a random effects framework, using Cohen’s d for each individual study sample, with genotype grouped as presence (SS or SL) or absence (LL) of the 5-HTTLPR S allele. Data from both selected (i.e., extreme-scoring) and unselected study samples were included in these analyses. First we tested for an effect of construct, grouping individual study samples according to whether they measured HA or N. In the case of studies which reported measures of N using both EPQ and NEO instruments, data from the NEO instrument only were used. Where there was evidence of a significant difference between the sub-group estimates, we conducted further analyses stratified by construct and, in the case of measures of N, grouped individual study samples according to whether they measured N using questionnaires derived from the EPQ or NEO family of instruments.

Fixed effects analysis was used to combine individual study effect sizes within sub-groups, using inverse variance methods to generate a summary effect size and 95% confidence interval (CI). A fixed effects framework assumes that the effect of genotype is constant across studies, and between-study variation is considered to be due to chance or random variation. For the stratified analyses, random effects analyses were used in the presence of significant between-study heterogeneity, with effects sizes pooled using DerSimonian and Laird methods. A random effects framework assumes that between-study variation is due to both chance or random variation and an individual study effect. Random effects models are more conservative than fixed effects models and generate a wider confidence interval. The significance of the pooled effect sizes was determined using a Z test. We used a χ2 test and the I2 statistic to assess between-study heterogeneity, with the latter providing a measure of the proportion of variation that is explained by between-study variation (Higgins et al. 2003).

For the stratified analyses, where there was evidence of significant association, the effect size of the first published study was compared to the pooled effect size of the remaining studies using a Z test, as there is evidence for a substantially greater estimate of effect size in the first published study (Trikalinos et al. 2004). Meta-regression of individual study effect size against year of publication was also conducted, and potential ascertainment bias (e.g., due to publication bias) assessed using the Egger test (Egger et al. 1997).

Statistical power for the primary analysis of NFBC 1966 data was derived by simulation and implemented though a Perl (http://www.perl.com) interface designed for quantitative traits (i.e., HA). This derivation included several basic parameters including the size of sampling population, proportion selected for genotyping, alpha threshold, effect size, and allele frequency. In order to reduce complexity, power was only calculated for those samples lacking any family structure.

Data were analysed using SPSS (v.12) and Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (v.2) statistical software packages. Exact p-values are given throughout.

Results

Association Analysis of NFBC 1966 Data

We obtained genotypes for n = 3,872 individuals (44% male) born in 1966, from whom questionnaire data were collected in 1997. The mean HA score was 14 (range 0 – 34, SD = 6). Genotype frequencies did not deviate significantly from Hardy-Weinberg Equilibrium (SS: 17%, SL: 48%, LL: 35%; p = 0.61).

Univariate ANOVA of HA score across all participants, with 5-HTTLPR genotype as a between-groups factor (SS, SL, LL), indicated no evidence of association (F [2, 3869] = 0.01, p = 0.99). Grouping individuals with one or more S alleles and controlling for the effect of sex did not alter these results substantially. HA scores grouped by genotype are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Harm Avoidance Score by 5-HTTLPR Genotype.

SS
(n = 661)
SL
(n = 1859)
LL
(n = 1352)

Male M 13 13 13
SD (6) (6) (6)
Female M 14 15 15
SD (6) (6) (6)
Total M 14 14 14
SD (6) (6) (6)

We estimated power by simulation and found that at significance thresholds of p = 0.05 and 0.01 we had 100% power to detect effects of 1% and 0.5% of phenotypic variance. The sample has 63% power to detect an effect of just 0.1% of phenotypic variance at a significance threshold of p = 0.05.

Characteristics of Studies Included in Meta-Analysis

A total of 51 studies, including the primary data from the present study, consisting of k = 64 study samples, were eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis. Of these, data were unavailable on k = 9 study samples (Bachner-Melman et al. 2005; Cohen et al. 2002; Kim et al. 2005; Kim et al. 2006a; Kremer et al. 2005; Lerman et al. 2000; Park et al. 2004; Schmidt et al. 2000; Thierry et al. 2004), so that a total of 42 studies, comprising k = 55 study samples, contributed to the meta-analysis (Ball et al. 1997; Benjamin et al. 2000a; Brummett et al. 2003; Comings et al. 2000; Cruz et al. 1995; Deary et al. 1999; Dragan and Oniszczenko 2006; Du et al. 2000; Ebstein et al. 1997; Flory et al. 1999; Greenberg et al. 2000; Ham et al. 2004; Hamer et al. 1999; Hariri et al. 2005; Herbst et al. 2000; Hu et al. 2000; Jacob et al. 2004; Joo et al. 2007; Jorm et al. 1998; Katsuragi et al. 1999; Kumakiri et al. 1999; Lang et al. 2004; Lanzagorta et al. 2006; Lesch et al. 1996; Mazzanti et al. 1998; Middeldorp et al. 2007; Monteleone et al. 2006; Munafo et al. 2006; Nakamura et al. 1997; Nilsson et al. 2007; Osher et al. 2000; Ricketts et al. 1998; Samochowiec et al. 2001; Samochowiec et al. 2004; Schmitz et al. 2007; Sen et al. 2004b; Stein et al. 2007; Strobel et al. 2000; Szekely et al. 2004; Tsai et al. 2002; Umekage et al. 2003; Vormfelde et al. 2006; Willis-Owen et al. 2005). This included 7 studies, comprising k = 15 study samples, that reported data on multiple personality instruments from the same sample (Jacob et al. 2004; Kumakiri et al. 1999; Nakamura et al. 1997; Osher et al. 2000; Samochowiec et al. 2004; Vormfelde et al. 2006; Willis-Owen et al. 2005), and 3 studies, comprising k = 5 study samples, that recruited participants based on a selected personality score (Ball et al. 1997; Deary et al. 1999; Willis-Owen et al. 2005). In addition, 1 study comprised unpublished data (Cruz et al. 1995), and 2 studies recruited from both patient and healthy control populations, so that only data from healthy controls were included (Brummett et al. 2003; Monteleone et al. 2006). The characteristics of these studies are described in Table 2.

Table 2.

Characteristics of Included Studies.

5-HTTLPR

Author Year SS SL LL Measure Phenotype Ancestry Remarks

(mean) (SD) (n) (mean) (SD) (n) (mean) (SD) (n)
Cruz 1995 1995 13.50 5.70 20 10.30 4.60 26 12.00 5.40 26 EPQ Neuroticism European Unpublished data
Lesch 1996 1996 56.30 11.20 95 56.60 12.00 247 53.10 11.50 163 NEO Neuroticism European
Ebstein 1997 1997 13.15 4.58 32 12.15 6.17 66 12.86 5.90 23 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Nakamura 1997 (NEO) 1997 142.99 9.55 128 143.29 10.88 55 137.33 9.45 3 NEO Neuroticism East Asian Duplicate sample
Nakamura 1997 (TPQ/TCI) 1997 18.14 5.98 118 17.21 7.20 52 11.67 3.06 3 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian Duplicate sample
Jorm 1998 1998 4.36 3.50 155 4.49 3.27 350 4.42 3.33 254 EPQ Neuroticism European
Mazzanti 1998 1998 11.70 4.90 41 10.60 5.50 106 11.20 5.80 68 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Ricketts 1998 1998 16.80 6.36 10 12.71 6.96 14 10.85 5.05 13 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Flory 1999 1999 47.50 9.40 37 51.40 10.10 112 50.50 10.80 76 NEO Neuroticism European
Hamer 1999 1999 13.44 7.07 108 13.75 7.30 336 12.25 7.48 190 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Katsuragi 1999 1999 17.53 5.77 66 13.42 6.29 31 17.00 2.94 4 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian
Kumakiri 1999 (NEO) 1999 104.00 22.00 85 104.50 22.00 48 103.60 25.90 11 NEO Neuroticism East Asian Duplicate sample
Kumakiri 1999 (TPQ/TCI) 1999 18.90 6.30 85 18.60 7.20 48 18.40 6.20 11 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian Duplicate sample
Benjamin 2000 2000 13.27 6.04 346 Combined with s/s 12.51 6.01 108 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Comings 2000 2000 48.61 11.89 23 48.63 11.97 39 47.03 11.34 19 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Du 2000 2000 41.98 10.32 40 39.93 8.49 86 40.42 9.24 60 NEO Neuroticism European
Greenberg 2000 2000 59.02 12.25 66 60.17 11.21 217 56.29 11.65 114 NEO Neuroticism European
Herbst 2000 2000 10.20 6.30 79 10.40 6.10 198 11.20 6.50 148 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Hu 2000 2000 57.67 11.46 135 58.55 11.95 390 54.65 11.73 234 NEO Neuroticism European
Lerman 2000 2000 Data Not Available EPQ Neuroticism European Not included
Osher 2000 (NEO) 2000 91.50 25.60 39 92.80 23.80 70 92.30 24.30 35 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Osher 2000 (TPQ/TCI) 2000 14.62 6.93 39 14.37 6.37 73 11.81 6.80 36 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
Schmidt 2000 2000 Data Not Available NEO Neuroticism European Not included
Strobel 2000 2000 15.58 7.34 26 13.55 6.61 69 13.54 6.65 39 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Samochowiec 2001 2001 16.10 5.90 18 15.40 6.40 67 17.50 6.10 41 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Cohen 2002 2002 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Not included
Tsai 2002 2002 14.90 6.40 100 15.00 7.50 71 14.70 7.10 21 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian
Brummett 2003 2003 39.80 9.20 13 40.20 8.80 51 44.80 9.00 35 NEO Neuroticism European Control sample
Umekage 2003 2003 103.90 21.10 103 99.10 15.30 46 101.50 17.10 10 NEO Neuroticism East Asian
Ham 2004 2004 17.94 6.15 93 16.91 6.67 47 16.40 9.10 6 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian
Jacob 2004 (NEO) 2004 95.89 31.49 40 94.18 25.65 140 98.18 23.17 101 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Jacob 2004 (TPQ/TCI) 2004 14.80 6.97 40 15.54 6.50 140 15.07 6.38 101 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
Lang 2004 2004 31.60 7.20 41 29.90 7.60 102 28.80 5.74 85 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Park 2004 2004 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian Not included
Samochowiec 2004 (NEO) 2004 5.00 2.34 56 Combined with s/s 5.11 20.60 44 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Samochowiec 2004 (TPQ/TCI) 2004 14.18 4.93 56 Combined with s/s 15.23 5.67 44 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
Sen 2004 2004 87.80 18.86 83 84.50 20.83 183 79.90 19.41 149 NEO Neuroticism European
Szekely 2004 2004 14.55 5.80 29 14.52 6.22 69 14.62 6.66 53 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Thierry 2004 2004 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Not included
Bachner-Melman 2005 2005 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance Other Not included
Hariri 2005 2005 10.22 5.48 65 Combined with s/s 9.60 5.92 27 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Kim 2005 2005 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian Not included
Kremer 2005 2005 Data Not Available TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance Other Not included
Willis-Owen 2005 (singletons) 2005 14.15 9.98 129 13.65 10.09 327 13.22 10.19 274 EPQ Neuroticism European Selected sample
Willis-Owen 2005 (siblings) 2005 12.39 9.03 97 13.98 8.74 254 11.90 9.27 177 EPQ Neuroticism European Selected sample
Willis-Owen 2005 (EPIC) 2005 4.73 4.38 755 4.74 3.84 2029 4.88 4.37 1391 EPQ Neuroticism European Selected sample
Dragan 2006 2006 24.23 7.14 22 24.85 9.30 74 19.73 9.00 100 NEO Neuroticism European
Kim 2006a 2006 16.15 6.58 128 17.82 8.18 81 Combined with s/l TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian Not included
Lanzagorta 2006 2006 13.60 5.60 21 12.70 5.60 21 13.30 5.00 15 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Monteleone 2006 2006 95.50 13.20 33 90.50 11.50 39 91.00 9.60 22 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Control sample
Munafo 2006 2006 14.59 5.94 46 11.71 5.77 129 12.59 5.78 76 EPQ Neuroticism European
Nilsson 2006 (females) 2006 14.58 7.91 26 12.50 7.36 60 14.61 7.06 31 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Nilsson 2006 (males) 2006 11.44 6.45 18 10.73 6.59 30 11.45 5.67 31 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European
Joo 2007 2007 17.14 7.11 95 16.59 7.71 54 18.78 4.02 9 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance East Asian
Middeldorp 2007 2007 32.62 10.13 203 32.30 10.05 283 32.64 10.77 86 EPQ Neuroticism European
Schmitz 2007 (NEO) 2007 2.96 0.82 143 2.89 0.75 196 2.75 0.68 71 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Schmitz 2007 (TPQ/TCI) 2007 16.28 7.58 143 15.49 7.35 196 15.01 6.98 71 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
Schmitz 2007 (EPQ) 2007 13.49 5.99 143 12.55 5.81 196 11.23 5.53 71 EPQ Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Stein 2007 2007 48.62 9.87 68 47.97 9.98 108 47.30 10.38 71 NEO Neuroticism European
Vormfelde 2007 (females) (NEO) 2007 92.00 22.00 38 98.00 21.00 40 92.00 26.00 20 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Vormfelde 2007 (females) (TPQ/TCI) 2007 14.50 5.80 38 14.90 5.90 40 15.10 5.50 20 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
Vormfelde 2007 (males) (NEO) 2007 91.00 21.00 31 80.00 23.00 50 81.00 26.00 16 NEO Neuroticism European Duplicate sample
Vormfelde 2007 (males) (TPQ/TCI) 2007 13.60 6.20 31 12.00 7.00 50 11.10 7.50 16 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European Duplicate sample
NFBC 2007 13.99 6.18 2520 Combined with s/s 13.99 6.21 1352 TPQ/TCI Harm Avoidance European


5-HTTLPR

Author Year SS SL LL Measure Phenotype Ancestry Remarks

High Low % High High Low % High High Low % High

Ball 1997 1997 4 10 7.7 30 21 57.7 18 23 34.6 NEO Neuroticism European Selected sample
Deary 1999 1999 24 19 23.5 44 57 44.1 32 28 32.4 NEO Neuroticism European Selected sample

A further 17 studies were identified by the search strategy but were ineligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis, either because they reported data that were reported elsewhere, or because they reported data on measures of personality other than HA or N (Benjamin et al. 2000b; Golimbet et al. 2006; Gonda and Bagdy 2006; Gonda et al. 2007; Gonda et al. 2006; Gursoy 2002; Gustavsson et al. 1999; Jang et al. 2001; Kim et al. 2006b; Melke et al. 2001; Mizuno et al. 2006; Munafo et al. 2005c; Murakami et al. 1999; Park et al. 2006; Park et al. 2005; Sher et al. 2000; Sirota et al. 1999).

Meta-Analysis

The initial moderator analysis, excluding one study sample which reported EPQ N data where NEO N data were available for the same sample (Schmitz et al. 2007), did not indicate a moderating effect of construct on the association between 5-HTTLPR genotype and anxiety-related personality. For HA, there was no evidence of significant association (d = 0.02, 95% CI −0.03, 0.07, Z = 0.90, p = 0.37), and no evidence of between-study heterogeneity (χ2 [27] = 25.41, p = 0.55, I2 = 0.00). For N, there was evidence of significant association (d = 0.07, 95% CI 0.03, 0.11, Z = 3.44, p = 0.001), indicating higher N score in individuals with one or more copies of the S allele, and evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity (χ2 [27] = 65.30, p < 0.001, I2 = 58.65). However, these two sub-group effect size estimates did not differ significantly (χ2 [1] = 2.24, p = 0.14). When the analysis was conducted within a mixed-effects framework, using random effects methods to combine individual study sample effect sizes within sub-groups, these results were not altered substantially, although the evidence for a moderating effect of construct constituted a trend (χ2 [1] = 3.18, p = 0.08).

Given the trend for a moderating effect of construct in the mixed effects analysis, and the substantial between-study heterogeneity among the study samples which measured N, further exploratory analyses were conducted on study samples that used measures of N. For EPQ instruments, there was no evidence of significant association (d = 0.01, 95% CI −0.04, 0.06, Z = 0.37, p = 0.71), and no evidence of between-study heterogeneity (χ2 [7] = 10.57, p = 0.16, I2 = 0.00). For NEO instruments, there was evidence of significant association (d = 0.18, 95% CI 0.12, 0.24, Z = 5.57, p < 0.001), indicating higher N score in individuals with one or more copies of the S allele, and evidence of modest between-study heterogeneity (χ2 [20] = 40.50, p = 0.004, I2 = 50.62). These two sub-group effect size estimates differed significantly (χ2 [1] = 17.14, p < 0.001). When the analysis was conducted within a mixed-effects framework, using random effects methods to combine individual study sample effect sizes within sub-groups, the evidence for a moderating effect of instrument was non-significant (χ2 [1] = 2.60, p = 0.11).

On the basis of evidence of a significant moderating effect of instrument in the fixed effects analysis, and the reduction in between-study heterogeneity observed when study samples that used NEO instruments were considered alone, we conducted further analyses on study samples that used measures of NEO N. When the effect size estimate from the first published study (d = 0.29) (Lesch et al. 1996) was compared to the pooled effect size estimate from the remaining study samples (d = 0.16) there was no evidence that these differed (p = 0.20), and meta-regression did not indicate any evidence of association between year of publication and effect size estimate (p = 0.68). When the pooled effect size estimate from European study samples (d = 0.18) was compared to the pooled estimate from East Asian study samples (d = 0.11) there was no evidence that these differed (p = 0.74). Egger’s test indicated evidence of possible ascertainment bias (t [19] = 1.85, p = 0.08), but this was in the opposite direction to that which would be predicted if publication bias were operating.

Discussion

Our analyses, both of new primary data drawn from a large, population-based sample and of secondary data analyzed using meta-analytic techniques, indicate that the 5-HTTLPR variant is not associated with Harm Avoidance. These results are broadly consistent with the results of previous meta-analyses (Schinka et al. 2004; Sen et al. 2004a), but extend on these by including studies that used EPQ instruments, and those employing extreme-score designs, as well as incorporating data from approximately twenty additional studies compared with previous meta-analyses. Together with our previous report (Willis-Owen et al. 2005) of a large sample of participants with extreme Neuroticism scores (measured using the EPQ), we have data that excludes a genetic effect of the 5-HTTLPR on two measures of anxiety-related personality traits. There remains the possibility that the variant influences the NEO personality questionnaire measure of Neuroticism, and our meta-analysis provides support for this.

To address this question we currently are reliant upon relatively small sample sizes in the primary studies that used NEO instruments (albeit a relatively large number of individual studies). The 19 such studies included in our meta-analysis had a median sample size of n = 196, with a maximum of n = 759 subjects (Hu et al. 2000). Our meta-analysis found evidence of association with the 5-HTTLPR, modest between-study heterogeneity, no evidence of publication bias, and no association between individual study effect size and year of publication. This result leaves open the possibility that variation in an anxiety-related personality trait defined by the NEO instrument is influenced by the 5-HTTLPR, although we suggest that a large, well-powered primary study is needed to confirm or refute the observation. Based on the effect size estimate derived from our meta-analysis, this would require a sample size in excess of n = 1,000 to have 80% power at a significance level of p = 0.05. However, although a measurement-specific genetic effect is possible (and indeed plausible, given the imperfect correlation between even the two measures of Neuroticism), it is not clear how useful a genetic effect that is specific to one questionnaire will prove to be.

There are certain limitations to our primary sample which should be considered. First, we did not explicitly control for potential population stratification (for example, by using genomic controls). However, our sample was drawn from a relatively homogeneous population within a geographically-limited region. We therefore regard the risk of population stratification as low. Second, a functional SNP within the 5-HTTLPR variant has been identified which appears to modify the functional consequences of carrying the L allele (Hu et al. 2006). It would have been valuable to have genotyped this variant, although unfortunately resources were not available to allow this. Nevertheless, given the marked absence of an effect of the 5-HTTLPR, we regard it as unlikely that genotyping this additional SNP would have altered our results substantially.

It is worth commenting here on other possible confounds that may have hidden the genetic effect and those that may have contributed to false positive results. Interactions may be to blame here – if the effect attributable to the S and L alleles at the 5-HTTLPR susceptibility depend on the co-occurrence of alleles at other loci in the gene (Parsey et al. 2006) (or elsewhere in the genome), and if those alleles occur at different frequencies in the populations sampled, then inconsistent findings of positive and negative associations may be reported. Similarly, if the effect is only apparent when those carrying the susceptibility allele are exposed to the relevant environment, differences in the prevalence of environmental stressors between study populations is another cause of inconsistent findings. However even if interactions occur, they do not exclude the simplest hypothesis, that there is no main effect attributable to the 5-HTTLPR locus.

A final issue, rarely considered in the literature, is technical. The DNA in which the 5-HTTLPR is embedded has a high GC content (about 70%), and PCR-based assays require modifications to obtain reliable results. We have often observed that, under these conditions, additional, artifactual bands may be produced. We therefore routinely carry out the PCR in duplicate, with case and control participants randomly ordered on the same PCR analysis plate, and have the genotypes called blind to the status of the participant. We have found that these controls are essential to avoid bias in the genotyping, but we do not know to what extent similar controls are used in other laboratories. It is possible that technical artifacts may explain some of the inconsistencies in the literature.

In conclusion, our results indicate that the 5-HTTLPR variant is not associated with Harm Avoidance, or Neuroticism as measured by the EPQ. There remains the possibility that the variant influences the NEO personality questionnaire measure of Neuroticism. Meta-analysis, while valuable and offering the potential to generate new hypotheses, is not a replacement for adequately-powered primary studies. Therefore, a large, well-powered primary study is required to test this hypothesis directly and adequately.

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Wojciech Dragan, Vera Golimbet, Nuria Lanzagorta, Min-Soo Lee, Kent Nilsson, Filip Rybakowski, Jerzy Samochowiec, Anja Schmitz, Alexander Strobel, and Anna Szekely who kindly provided in a format that enabled their inclusion in the meta-analysis, and to Thuy Vu, Joe De Young and Michael Dupree for their assistance with genotyping. NF is supported by NIH grant 5RL1MH083268-02. JF is supported by the Wellcome Trust.

References

  1. Aluja A, García O, García L. A comparative study of Zuckerman’s three structural models for personality through the NEO-PI-R, ZKPQ-III-R, EPQ-RS and Goldberg’s 50-bipolar adjectives. Pers Indiv Diff. 2002;33:713–725. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bachner-Melman R, Dina C, Zohar AH, Constantini N, Lerer E, Hoch S, Sella S, Nemanov L, Gritsenko I, Lichtenberg P, et al. AVPR1a and SLC6A4 gene polymorphisms are associated with creative dance performance. PLoS Genet. 2005;1(3):e42. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0010042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ball D, Hill L, Freeman B, Eley TC, Strelau J, Riemann R, Spinath FM, Angleitner A, Plomin R. The serotonin transporter gene and peer-rated neuroticism. Neuroreport. 1997;8(5):1301–1304. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199703240-00048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benjamin J, Osher Y, Kotler M, Gritsenko I, Nemanov L, Belmaker RH, Ebstein RP. Association between tridimensional personality questionnaire (TPQ) traits and three functional polymorphisms: dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4), serotonin transporter promoter region (5-HTTLPR) and catechol O-methyltransferase (COMT) Mol Psychiatry. 2000a;5(1):96–100. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000640. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Benjamin J, Osher Y, Lichtenberg P, Bachner-Melman R, Gritsenko I, Kotler M, Belmaker RH, Valsky V, Drendel M, Ebstein RP. An interaction between the catechol O-methyltransferase and serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphisms contributes to tridimensional personality questionnaire persistence scores in normal subjects. Neuropsychobiology. 2000b;41(1):48–53. doi: 10.1159/000026632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brummett BH, Siegler IC, McQuoid DR, Svenson IK, Marchuk DA, Steffens DC. Associations among the NEO Personality Inventory, Revised and the serotonin transporter gene-linked polymorphic region in elders: effects of depression and gender. Psychiatr Genet. 2003;13(1):13–18. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200303000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cloninger CR. A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatr Dev. 1986;4(3):167–226. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cohen H, Buskila D, Neumann L, Ebstein RP. Confirmation of an association between fibromyalgia and serotonin transporter promoter region (5- HTTLPR) polymorphism, and relationship to anxiety-related personality traits. Arthritis Rheum. 2002;46(3):845–847. doi: 10.1002/art.10103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Comings DE, Gade-Andavolu R, Gonzalez N, Wu S, Muhleman D, Blake H, Mann MB, Dietz G, Saucier G, MacMurray JP. A multivariate analysis of 59 candidate genes in personality traits: the temperament and character inventory. Clin Genet. 2000;58(5):375–385. doi: 10.1034/j.1399-0004.2000.580508.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Costa PT, Jr, McCrae RR. Stability and change in personality assessment: the revised NEO Personality Inventory in the year 2000. J Pers Assess. 1997;68(1):86–94. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6801_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cruz C, Camarena B, Mejia JM, Paez F, Eroza V, Ramon De La Fuente J, Kershenobich D, Nicolini H. The dopamine D2 receptor gene TaqI A1 polymorphism and alcoholism in a Mexican population. Arch Med Res. 1995;26(4):421–426. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Curtin F, Walker JP, Badan M, Schulz P. The relations between scores on analogous scales from four personality questionnaires in 50 normal men. Pers Indiv Diff. 1995;19:705–711. [Google Scholar]
  13. De Fruyt F, Van De Wiele L, Van Heeringen C. Cloninger’s psychobiological model of temperament and character and the five-factor model of personality. Pers Indiv Diff. 2000;29:441–452. [Google Scholar]
  14. Deary IJ, Battersby S, Whiteman MC, Connor JM, Fowkes FG, Harmar A. Neuroticism and polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene. Psychol Med. 1999;29(3):735–739. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Depue RA, Collins PF. Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behav Brain Sci. 1999;22(3):491–517. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002046. discussion 518-69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dragan WL, Oniszczenko W. Association of a functional polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene with personality traits in females in a Polish population. Neuropsychobiology. 2006;54(1):45–50. doi: 10.1159/000095741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Du L, Bakish D, Hrdina PD. Gender differences in association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits. Psychiatr Genet. 2000;10(4):159–164. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200010040-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ebstein RP, Gritsenko I, Nemanov L, Frisch A, Osher Y, Belmaker RH. No association between the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and the Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) temperament of harm avoidance. Mol Psychiatry. 1997;2(3):224–226. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br Med J. 1997;315(7109):629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eysenck HJ, Eysenck SBG. The Eysenck Personality Questionnaire - Revised. London: Hodder and Stoughton; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  21. Flory JD, Manuck SB, Ferrell RE, Dent KM, Peters DG, Muldoon MF. Neuroticism is not associated with the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. Mol Psychiatry. 1999;4(1):93–96. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Golimbet VE, Lebedeva IS, Alfimova MV, Korovaitseva GI, Lezheiko TV, Abramova LI, Kaleda VG. Acoustic evoked potentials, serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and some psychopathological and psychological features in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. Zh Nevrol Psikhiatr Im S S Korsakova. 2006;106(2):44–49. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gonda X, Bagdy G. Relationship between serotonin transporter gene 5HTTLPR polymorphism and the symptoms of neuroticism in a healthy population. Psychiatr Hung. 2006;21(5):379–385. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gonda X, Rihmer Z, Juhasz G, Zsombok T, Bagdy G. High anxiety and migraine are associated with the s allele of the 5HTTLPR gene polymorphism. Psychiatry Res. 2007;149(1–3):261–266. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gonda X, Rihmer Z, Zsombok T, Bagdy G, Akiskal KK, Akiskal HS. The 5HTTLPR polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene is associated with affective temperaments as measured by TEMPS-A. J Affect Disord. 2006;91(2–3):125–131. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.12.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Greenberg BD, Li Q, Lucas FR, Hu S, Sirota LA, Benjamin J, Lesch KP, Hamer D, Murphy DL. Association between the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and personality traits in a primarily female population sample. Am J Med Genet. 2000;96(2):202–216. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(20000403)96:2<202::aid-ajmg16>3.0.co;2-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gursoy S. Absence of association of the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism with the mentally healthy subset of fibromyalgia patients. Clin Rheumatol. 2002;21(3):194–197. doi: 10.1007/s10067-002-8284-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gustavsson JP, Nothen MM, Jonsson EG, Neidt H, Forslund K, Rylander G, Mattila-Evenden M, Sedvall GC, Propping P, Asberg M. No association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms and personality traits. Am J Med Genet. 1999;88(4):430–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Ham BJ, Kim YH, Choi MJ, Cha JH, Choi YK, Lee MS. Serotonergic genes and personality traits in the Korean population. Neurosci Lett. 2004;354(1):2–5. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(03)00753-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hamer DH, Greenberg BD, Sabol SZ, Murphy DL. Role of the serotonin transporter gene in temperament and character. J Personal Disord. 1999;13(4):312–327. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1999.13.4.312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, Weinberger DR. A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(2):146–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.2.146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Heils A, Teufel A, Petri S, Stober G, Riederer P, Bengel D, Lesch KP. Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene expression. J Neurochem. 1996;66(6):2621–2624. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1996.66062621.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Heinz A, Jones DW, Mazzanti C, Goldman D, Ragan P, Hommer D, Linnoila M, Weinberger DR. A relationship between serotonin transporter genotype and in vivo protein expression and alcohol neurotoxicity. Biol Psychiatry. 2000;47(7):643–649. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00171-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Herbst JH, Zonderman AB, McCrae RR, Costa PT., Jr Do the dimensions of the temperament and character inventory map a simple genetic architecture? Evidence from molecular genetics and factor analysis. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(8):1285–1290. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br Med J. 2003;327(7414):557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hu S, Brody CL, Fisher C, Gunzerath L, Nelson ML, Sabol SZ, Sirota LA, Marcus SE, Greenberg BD, Murphy DL, et al. Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene and neuroticism in cigarette smoking behavior. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5(2):181–188. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar LA, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, Xu K, Arnold PD, Richter MA, Kennedy JL, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2006;78(5):815–826. doi: 10.1086/503850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Jacob CP, Strobel A, Hohenberger K, Ringel T, Gutknecht L, Reif A, Brocke B, Lesch KP. Association between allelic variation of serotonin transporter function and neuroticism in anxious cluster C personality disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):569–572. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.3.569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Jang KL, Hu S, Livesley WJ, Angleitner A, Riemann R, Ando J, Ono Y, Vernon PA, Hamer DH. Covariance structure of neuroticism and agreeableness: a twin and molecular genetic analysis of the role of the serotonin transporter gene. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(2):295–304. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.81.2.295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Joo YH, Oh HB, Kim B, Jung SH, Chung JK, Hong JP, Kim CY. No association between 5-HTTLPR and harm avoidance in Korean college students. J Korean Med Sci. 2007;22(1):138–141. doi: 10.3346/jkms.2007.22.1.138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jorm AF, Henderson AS, Jacomb PA, Christensen H, Korten AE, Rodgers B, Tan X, Easteal S. An association study of a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene with personality and psychiatric symptoms. Mol Psychiatry. 1998;3(5):449–451. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Katsuragi S, Kunugi H, Sano A, Tsutsumi T, Isogawa K, Nanko S, Akiyoshi J. Association between serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and anxiety-related traits. Biol Psychiatry. 1999;45(3):368–370. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00090-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Kim SJ, Kim YS, Choi NK, Hong HJ, Lee HS, Kim CH. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and personality traits in a Korean population. Neuropsychobiology. 2005;51(4):243–247. doi: 10.1159/000085819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim SJ, Kim YS, Lee HS, Kim SY, Kim CH. An interaction between the serotonin transporter promoter region and dopamine transporter polymorphisms contributes to harm avoidance and reward dependence traits in normal healthy subjects. J Neural Transm. 2006a;113(7):877–886. doi: 10.1007/s00702-006-0444-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Kim W, Choi YH, Yoon KS, Cho DY, Pae CU, Woo JM. Tryptophan hydroxylase and serotonin transporter gene polymorphism does not affect the diagnosis, clinical features and treatment outcome of panic disorder in the Korean population. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2006b;30(8):1413–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Kremer I, Bachner-Melman R, Reshef A, Broude L, Nemanov L, Gritsenko I, Heresco-Levy U, Elizur Y, Ebstein RP. Association of the serotonin transporter gene with smoking behavior. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(5):924–930. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Kumakiri C, Kodama K, Shimizu E, Yamanouchi N, Okada S, Noda S, Okamoto H, Sato T, Shirasawa H. Study of the association between the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and personality traits in a Japanese population. Neurosci Lett. 1999;263(2–3):205–207. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(99)00098-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lang UE, Bajbouj M, Wernicke C, Rommelspacher H, Danker-Hopfe H, Gallinat J. No association of a functional polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene promoter and anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;49(4):182–184. doi: 10.1159/000077363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lanzagorta N, Alexanderson-Rosas E, Ricalde A, Gomez Caudillo L, Sosa A, Camarena B, Aguilar A, Fresan A, Apiquian R, Nicolini H. Effect to the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTT) on personality dimensions in individuals without psychopathology. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2006;34(5):303–308. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lerman C, Caporaso NE, Audrain J, Main D, Boyd NR, Shields PG. Interacting effects of the serotonin transporter gene and neuroticism in smoking practices and nicotine dependence. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5(2):189–192. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Muller CR, Hamer DH, Murphy DL. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science. 1996;274(5292):1527–1531. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lesch KP, Zeng Y, Reif A, Gutknecht L. Anxiety-related traits in mice with modified genes of the serotonergic pathway. Eur J Pharmacol. 2003;480(1–3):185–204. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2003.08.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Little KY, McLaughlin DP, Zhang L, Livermore CS, Dalack GW, McFinton PR, DelProposto ZS, Hill E, Cassin BJ, Watson SJ, et al. Cocaine, ethanol, and genotype effects on human midbrain serotonin transporter binding sites and mRNA levels. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155(2):207–213. doi: 10.1176/ajp.155.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Loehlin JC. Nature, nurture, and conservatism in the Australian Twin Study. Behav Genet. 1993;23(3):287–290. doi: 10.1007/BF01082468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mazzanti CM, Lappalainen J, Long JC, Bengel D, Naukkarinen H, Eggert M, Virkkunen M, Linnoila M, Goldman D. Role of the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism in anxiety-related traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55(10):936–940. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.10.936. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Melke J, Landen M, Baghei F, Rosmond R, Holm G, Bjorntorp P, Westberg L, Hellstrand M, Eriksson E. Serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms are associated with anxiety-related personality traits in women. Am J Med Genet. 2001;105(5):458–463. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Middeldorp CM, de Geus EJ, Beem AL, Lakenberg N, Hottenga JJ, Slagboom PE, Boomsma DI. Family based association analyses between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and neuroticism, anxiety and depression. Behav Genet. 2007;37(2):294–301. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9139-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Miettunen J, Kantojarvi L, Ekelund J, Veijola J, Karvonen JT, Peltonen L, Jarvelin MR, Freimer N, Lichtermann D, Joukamaa M. A large population cohort provides normative data for investigation of temperament. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2004;110(2):150–157. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0047.2004.00344.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mizuno T, Aoki M, Shimada Y, Inoue M, Nakaya K, Takahashi T, Itoyama Y, Kanazawa M, Utsumi A, Endo Y, et al. Gender difference in association between polymorphism of serotonin transporter gene regulatory region and anxiety. J Psychosom Res. 2006;60(1):91–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Monteleone P, Santonastaso P, Mauri M, Bellodi L, Erzegovesi S, Fuschino A, Favaro A, Rotondo A, Castaldo E, Maj M. Investigation of the serotonin transporter regulatory region polymorphism in bulimia nervosa: relationships to harm avoidance, nutritional parameters, and psychiatric comorbidity. Psychosom Med. 2006;68(1):99–103. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000195746.52074.63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Munafo MR, Clark T, Flint J. Does measurement instrument moderate the association between the serotonin transporter gene and anxiety-related personality traits? A meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2005a;10(4):415–419. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Munafo MR, Clark T, Flint J. Promise and pitfalls in the meta-analysis of genetic association studies: a response to Sen and Schinka. Mol Psychiatry. 2005b;10:895–897. [Google Scholar]
  63. Munafo MR, Clark TG, Moore LR, Payne E, Walton R, Flint J. Genetic polymorphisms and personality in healthy adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry. 2003;8(5):471–484. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Munafo MR, Clark TG, Roberts KH, Johnstone EC. Neuroticism mediates the association of the serotonin transporter gene with lifetime major depression. Neuropsychobiology. 2006;53(1):1–8. doi: 10.1159/000089915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Munafo MR, Flint J. Meta-analysis of genetic association studies. Trends Genet. 2004;20(9):439–444. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2004.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Munafo MR, Roberts KH, Johnstone EC, Walton R, Yudkin PL. Association of serotonin transporter gene polymorphism with nicotine dependence: no evidence for an interaction with trait neuroticism. Pers Indiv Diff. 2005c;38:843–850. [Google Scholar]
  67. Murakami F, Shimomura T, Kotani K, Ikawa S, Nanba E, Adachi K. Anxiety traits associated with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in the Japanese. J Hum Genet. 1999;44(1):15–17. doi: 10.1007/s100380050098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Nakamura T, Muramatsu T, Ono Y, Matsushita S, Higuchi S, Mizushima H, Yoshimura K, Kanba S, Asai M. Serotonin transporter gene regulatory region polymorphism and anxiety-related traits in the Japanese. Am J Med Genet. 1997;74(5):544–545. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-8628(19970919)74:5<544::aid-ajmg18>3.0.co;2-c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Nilsson KW, Damberg M, Ohrvik J, Leppert J, Lindstrom L, Anckarsater H, Oreland L. Genes encoding for AP-2beta and the Serotonin Transporter are associated with the Personality Character Spiritual Acceptance. Neurosci Lett. 2007;411(3):233–237. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.10.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Ormel J, Rosmalen J, Farmer A. Neuroticism: a non-informative marker of vulnerability to psychopathology. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2004;39(11):906–912. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0873-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Osher Y, Hamer D, Benjamin J. Association and linkage of anxiety-related traits with a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region in Israeli sibling pairs. Mol Psychiatry. 2000;5(2):216–219. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Park JW, Han SR, Yang DW, Kim YI, Lee KS. Serotonin transporter protein polymorphism and harm avoidance personality in migraine without aura. Headache. 2006;46(6):991–996. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2006.00439.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Park JW, Kim JS, Kim YI, Lee KS. Serotonergic activity contributes to analgesic overuse in chronic tension-type headache. Headache. 2005;45(9):1229–1235. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2005.00247.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Park JW, Kim JS, Lee HK, Kim YI, Lee KS. Serotonin transporter polymorphism and harm avoidance personality in chronic tension-type headache. Headache. 2004;44(10):1005–1009. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4610.2004.04194.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Parsey RV, Hastings RS, Oquendo MA, Hu X, Goldman D, Huang YY, Simpson N, Arcement J, Huang Y, Ogden RT, et al. Effect of a triallelic functional polymorphism of the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region on expression of serotonin transporter in the human brain. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163(1):48–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.1.48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rantakallio P. The longitudinal study of the northern Finland birth cohort of 1966. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 1988;2(1):59–88. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3016.1988.tb00180.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ricketts MH, Hamer RM, Sage JI, Manowitz P, Feng F, Menza MA. Association of a serotonin transporter gene promoter polymorphism with harm avoidance behaviour in an elderly population. Psychiatr Genet. 1998;8(2):41–44. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199800820-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Samochowiec J, Rybakowski F, Czerski P, Zakrzewska M, Stepien G, Pelka-Wysiecka J, Horodnicki J, Rybakowski JK, Hauser J. Polymorphisms in the dopamine, serotonin, and norepinephrine transporter genes and their relationship to temperamental dimensions measured by the Temperament and Character Inventory in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiology. 2001;43(4):248–253. doi: 10.1159/000054898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Samochowiec J, Syrek S, Michal P, Ryzewska-Wodecka A, Samochowiec A, Horodnicki J, Zakrzewska M, Kucharska-Mazur J. Polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter and monoamine oxidase A genes and their relationship to personality traits measured by the Temperament and Character Inventory and NEO Five-Factor Inventory in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychobiology. 2004;50(2):174–181. doi: 10.1159/000079111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Schinka JA. Measurement scale does moderate the association between the serotonin transporter gene and trait anxiety: comments on Munafo et al. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:892–893. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Schinka JA, Busch RM, Robichaux-Keene N. A meta-analysis of the association between the serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and trait anxiety. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9(2):197–202. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Schmidt NB, Storey J, Greenberg BD, Santiago HT, Li Q, Murphy DL. Evaluating gene x psychological risk factor effects in the pathogenesis of anxiety: a new model approach. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(2):308–320. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Schmitz A, Hennig J, Kuepper Y, Reuter M. The association between neuroticism and the serotonin transporter polymorphism depends on structural differences between personality measures. Pers Indiv Diff. 2007;42:789–799. [Google Scholar]
  84. Sen S, Burmeister M, Ghosh D. Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004a;127(1):85–89. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sen S, Burmeister M, Ghosh D. 5-HTTLPR and anxiety-related personality traits meta-analysis revisited: response to Munafo and colleagues. Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10:893–895. [Google Scholar]
  86. Sen S, Villafuerte S, Nesse R, Stoltenberg SF, Hopcian J, Gleiberman L, Weder A, Burmeister M. Serotonin transporter and GABAA alpha 6 receptor variants are associated with neuroticism. Biol Psychiatry. 2004b;55(3):244–249. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Sher L, Greenberg BD, Murphy DL, Rosenthal NE, Sirota LA, Hamer DH. Pleiotropy of the serotonin transporter gene for seasonality and neuroticism. Psychiatr Genet. 2000;10(3):125–130. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200010030-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Sirota LA, Greenberg BD, Murphy DL, Hamer DH. Non-linear association between the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism and neuroticism: a caution against using extreme samples to identify quantitative trait loci. Psychiatr Genet. 1999;9(1):35–38. doi: 10.1097/00041444-199903000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Stein MB, Schork NJ, Gelernter J. Gene-by-environment (serotonin transporter and childhood maltreatment) interaction for anxiety sensitivity, an intermediate phenotype for anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007 doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301422. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Strobel A, Brocke B, Ebstein RP. Interaktionseffekte monoamin-relevanter genetischer polymorphismen mit traits des TPQ [Interaction effects of monoamine-relevant genetics polymorphisms with traits of the TPQ] Zeitschrift fuer Differentielle und Diagnostiche Psychologie. 2000;21:194–199. [Google Scholar]
  91. Szekely A, Ronai Z, Nemoda Z, Kolmann G, Gervai J, Sasvari-Szekely M. Human personality dimensions of persistence and harm avoidance associated with DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;126(1):106–110. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.20134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thierry N, Willeit M, Praschak-Rieder N, Zill P, Hornik K, Neumeister A, Lenzinger E, Stastny J, Hilger E, Konstantinidis A, et al. Serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) and personality in female patients with seasonal affective disorder and in healthy controls. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004;14(1):53–58. doi: 10.1016/s0924-977x(03)00064-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Trikalinos TA, Ntzani EE, Contopoulos-Ioannidis DG, Ioannidis JP. Establishment of genetic associations for complex diseases is independent of early study findings. Eur J Hum Genet. 2004;12(9):762–769. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tsai SJ, Hong CJ, Cheng CY. Serotonin transporter genetic polymorphisms and harm avoidance in the Chinese. Psychiatr Genet. 2002;12(3):165–168. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200209000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Umekage T, Tochigi M, Marui T, Kato C, Hibino H, Otani T, Kohda K, Kato N, Sasaki T. Serotonin transporter-linked promoter region polymorphism and personality traits in a Japanese population. Neurosci Lett. 2003;337(1):13–16. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(02)01291-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vormfelde SV, Hoell I, Tzvetkov M, Jamrozinski K, Sehrt D, Brockmoller J, Leibing E. Anxiety- and novelty seeking-related personality traits and serotonin transporter gene polymorphisms. J Psychiatr Res. 2006;40(6):568–576. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.10.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Willis-Owen SA, Turri MG, Munafo MR, Surtees PG, Wainwright NW, Brixey RD, Flint J. The serotonin transporter length polymorphism, neuroticism, and depression: a comprehensive assessment of association. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(6):451–456. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Zohar AH, Dina C, Rosolio N, Osher Y, Gritsenko I, Bachner-Melman R, Benjamin J, Belmaker RH, Ebstein RP. Tridimensional personality questionnaire trait of harm avoidance (anxiety proneness) is linked to a locus on chromosome 8p21. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;117(1):66–69. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.10029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES