Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Addict Behav. 2009 Sep 9;35(2):95. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.09.008

Cognitive Attributions for Smoking Among Adolescents in China

Qian Guo a,*, Jennifer B Unger b, Stanley P Azen c, Chaoyang Li d, Donna Spruijt-Metz c, Paula H Palmer c, Chih-Ping Chou c, Liming Lee e, Ping Sun c, C Anderson Johnson b
PMCID: PMC2821664  NIHMSID: NIHMS144815  PMID: 19800741

Abstract

To design more effective health communication messages for smoking cessation and prevention, it is important to understand people’s own perceptions of the factors that influence their decisions to smoke. Studies have examined cognitive attributions for smoking in Western countries but not in the Chinese cultural context. In a study of 14,434 Chinese adolescents, exploratory factor analysis grouped 17 cognitive attributions into 8 factors: curiosity, coping, social image, social belonging, engagement, autonomy, mental enhancement, and weight control. The factors were ranked based on the participants’ self-reports of importance and by the strength of their associations with smoking behavior. Among all smokers, curiosity was the most frequently-ranked attribution factor at the early stages of smoking but not for daily smoking. Coping was highly-ranked across smoking stages. Social image and social belonging were more highly-ranked at earlier stages, whereas engagement and mental enhancement were ranked more highly at later stages of smoking. More attributions were associated with smoking among males than among females. This information could be useful for the development of evidence-based anti-smoking programs in China.

Keywords: Attributions, Smoking, Attribution Theory, Adolescents, China

1. Introduction

Despite ubiquitous information about the adverse health consequences of smoking, many current smokers continue to smoke and many adolescents begin to experiment with smoking each day (Edwards, 2004). Empirical studies have identified numerous personal, social, and environmental factors associated with smoking (Moolchan, Ernst, & Henningfield, 2000; Schepis & Rao, 2005; Turner, Mermelstein, & Flay, 2004; Tyas & Pederson, 1998). However, it remains unclear how individuals actually perceive the causes of their smoking behaviors. To develop effective smoking cessation and prevention programs, it is important to understand individuals’ perceptions of the factors that motivate them to smoke and prevent them from quitting, so that these issues can be addressed explicitly in curricula and self-help materials.

Attribution theories describe how people explain the causes of their behaviors and the behaviors of others. Attribution theorists posit that people are motivated to explain the causes of personal behaviors to make the social environment seem more manageable (Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967). People’s explanations of behaviors, or attributions, can be classified as either personal or situational. Personal attributions imply volitional intention on the part of the actor, whereas situational attributions imply that contextual or environmental factors influence the behavior. Similar to other cognitive perceptions, attributions can be inaccurate or biased. For example, people tend to attribute the behavior of others to personal factors and attribute their own behaviors to situational factors (Jones & Nisbett, 1971; Monson & Snyder, 1977). However, attribution theorists argue that the task is not to determine the true causes of events, but to discern people’s perceptions of the causes because these perceptions influence people’s subsequent actions regardless of their accuracy. In other words, it might be more effective to design health communication messages that counter-argue the reasons why smokers perceive that they smoke, in addition to altering the personal, social, and environmental variables that are empirically associated with their smoking behaviors.

Several studies have identified cognitive attributions for smoking by asking people directly why they and other people initiate, maintain, or become addicted to smoking (Allbutt, Amos, & Cunningham-Burley, 1995; Rugkasa et al., 2001; Sarason, Mankowski, Peterson, & Dinh, 1992; Treacy et al., 2007). However, most of these studies have not assessed the relative importance of attributions to the individual, and most have not linked cognitive attributions with actual smoking behaviors to determine which are most influential (Kleinke, Staneski, & Meeker, 1983). A comprehensive investigation about cognitive attributions, their relative importance, and their associations with actual smoking behaviors could provide a better understanding about how people rationalize their decisions about smoking, which could be helpful in the design of more effective health communications for smoking cessation and prevention.

Most previous studies of attributions for smoking have been conducted in Western cultures including European countries (Allbutt et al., 1995; Berlin et al., 2003; Eiser, Sutton, & Wober, 1977, 1978; Palmqvist & Martikainen, 2005; Rugkasa et al., 2001; Treacy et al., 2007), the United States (Cronan, Conway, & Kaszas, 1991; Jenks, 1994a, 1994b; Kleinke et al., 1983; Sarason et al., 1992), and a few other countries (McGee & Stanton, 1993; Zoller & Maymon, 1983). Little is known about the attributions for smoking in China, where one-third of the world’s smokers reside (Yang et al., 1999; Yang et al., 2004). Attributions for smoking found in previous studies might not generalize to China, where social and cultural norms about smoking are quite different from those in Western countries. For example, smoking is often portrayed in U.S. culture as a way of expressing one’s individuality and autonomy (Davis, Gilpin, Loken, Viswanath, & Wakefield, 2008). It is not clear whether these individualistic motivations would operate similarly in a collectivistic culture such as China. Another difference between most Western cultures and China is that in China smoking is primarily a male behavior (Yang et al., 1999; Yang et al., 2004), whereas most Western countries have small gender gaps in smoking prevalence. Previous studies have shown that attributions for smoking vary across cultures. In one study, although the smoking behaviors among high school students in Israel and the United States were quite similar, the reasons for smoking and not smoking perceived by the students themselves were qualitatively and quantitatively different in the two countries (Zoller & Maymon, 1983).

Most studies of cognitive attributions for smoking have focused on adults. More research is needed among adolescents, because adolescence is a critical period for smoking initiation and formation of attitudes and beliefs about the consequences and functional meanings of one’s behavioral choices. Most previous studies also have focused on current smokers, with the goal of understanding why they continue to smoke. However, information about which factors might convince nonsmokers to smoke in the future is equally important, and information about how reasons for smoking may vary at different points on smoking trajectories also could be informative. Health communication messages about smoking cessation and prevention might be more effective if they address and counter the most common self-reported reasons for different points on smoking trajectories, as well as nonsmokers’ perceptions of possible future risk factors for smoking.

Therefore, the present study was conducted to investigate cognitive attributions for smoking, their relative importance, and their associations with actual smoking behaviors among Chinese adolescents. It differentiated the total sample into four subgroups: never smokers, lifetime smokers, past 30-day smokers, and daily smokers, which are equivalent to never smokers, tried/experimental smokers, regular smokers, and established smokers classified by Mayhew et al (Mayhew, Flay, & Mott, 2000).

2. Methods

Data for this analysis were obtained from a larger study being conducted in China to assess the effects of changing economic and social factors on health behaviors (Johnson et al., 2006). The project included seven large cities located in the northeastern, central, southwestern, and coastal regions of China.

2.1. Participants

Participants were recruited from schools in each of the seven cities. All schools in the metropolitan area of each city were stratified by median income in the district (high, medium, low) and by school academic performance (high, medium, low), resulting in nine clusters of schools. One middle school and one high school were randomly selected from each of the nine clusters. One classroom from the 7th and 8th grades in the selected middle schools and one classroom from the 10th and 11th grades in the selected high schools were recruited. In addition, one professional high school was selected from each district, major courses of study within each professional school were randomly selected, and students in these majors were recruited from the 10th and 11th grades. As a result, 147 schools were selected across the seven cities, 15,516 students were invited, and 14,434 students (93.0%) participated.

2.2. Procedures

The students completed the surveys in their schools. Students were eligible to participate if they provided parental informed consent. The informed consent and data collection procedures were reviewed and approved by both the University of Southern California and Chinese Institutional Review Boards. More details about the methodology of this study were reported elsewhere (Grenard et al., 2006; Guo et al., 2007; Johnson et al., 2006).

2.3. Measures

Demographic characteristics included age, gender, ethnicity, and geographic region. Cognitive attributions for smoking were assessed by the question, “I smoke, (or might smoke), because: (circle all that apply)”, with seventeen possible reasons for smoking listed as response options. The list of 17 possible attributions was generated based on previous qualitative and quantitative research on attributions for smoking among Chinese American and Taiwanese American college students (Hsia & Spruijt-Metz, 2003) and U.S. adolescents (Spruijt-Metz et al., 2005), along with consultation with educational and medical experts in China. Smoking behaviors were assessed at three levels: lifetime smoking, past 30-day smoking, and daily smoking.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Frequencies were calculated to describe the demographic characteristics and smoking status of the sample, and gender differences were evaluated with chi-square tests. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was used to group the 17 attributions into common factors. Because these variables were dichotomous, a tetrachoric correlation matrix of these variables was generated for the EFA. The principal components method was used to extract factors. An oblique rotation was used to allow correlations among the factors. The number of factors was determined based on the theoretical knowledge and the statistical rule of “eigenvalue greater than 1”. Based on the EFA, variables that loaded on the same factors were summed to create new variables representing the themes of attributions for smoking. Each of the new variables was dichotomously recoded as “0” if the value of the sum was 0, and as “1” if the value of the sum was equal to or greater than 1.

The relative importance of each attribution for smoking was ranked in two ways. First, the rankings were determined by the magnitudes of frequencies of attributions, first in the entire sample and then stratified by stages of smoking and gender. Second, the rankings were determined by the strength of their associations with smoking behavior, obtained from polychotomous logistic regression models. The polychotomous logistic regression models were computed first in the full sample and then separately by gender, adjusting for age, geographic region, district economy rank, and school academic rank. The outcome was a new variable created to represent four stages of smoking and coded as “0” if a student had never smoked, “1” if he/she had ever smoked but didn’t smoke during the past 30 days, “2” if he/she had smoked during the past 30 days but didn’t smoke daily, and “3” if he/she had smoked daily during the past 30 days. This analysis identified the attribution factors that were most strongly associated with each smoking status.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic characteristics and smoking status of the sample

As shown in Table 1, the sample contained slightly more females (51.4%) than males (48.6%). The distribution of ethnicity was not significantly different among males and females (p=0.30), but the distributions of age groups (p<0.001) and geographic regions (p=0.03) were significantly different. The prevalence of smoking was higher among males than among females at all stages of smoking (p<0.001).

Table 1.

Demographic Characteristics and Smoking Status of the Sample (N=14,434)

All n (%) Male n (%) Female n (%) Gender Difference
6993 (48.6) 7388 (51.4)
Age
12 years or younger 1265 (8.8) 549 (7.8) 696 (9.4) χ2(5)=46.8
13 2695 (18.6) 1360 (19.5) 1332 (18.0) p<0.0001
14 1902 (13.2) 1009 (14.4) 890 (12.1)
15 1505 (10.4) 660 (9.4) 842 (11.4)
16 3720 (25.8) 1759 (25.2) 1955 (26.5)
17 years or older 3347 (23.2) 1656 (23.7) 1673 (22.6)
Ethnicity
Han 13766 (95.8) 6665 (95.7) 7071 (96.0) χ2(1)=1.1
Others 600 (4.2) 302 (4.3) 294 (4.0) p=0.30
City
Chengdu 2198 (15.2) 1047 (15.0) 1141 (15.4) χ2(6)=14.2
Hangzhou 1902 (13.2) 929 (13.2) 970 (13.1) p=0.03
Shenyang 2327 (16.1) 1117 (16.0) 1188 (16.1)
Wuhan 2106 (14.6) 995 (14.2) 1111 (15.0)
Harbin 1852 (12.8) 893 (12.8) 950 (12.9)
Kunming 1992 (13.8) 1040 (14.9) 949 (12.9)
Qingdao 2057 (14.3) 972 (13.9) 1079 (14.6)
Smoking Status
Never smoker 8760 (61.2) 3517 (50.8) 5215 (71.1) χ2(3)=811.4
Lifetime smoker 3524 (24.6) 1947 (28.0) 1562 (21.3) p<0.0001
Past 30-day smoker 1544 (10.8) 1053 (15.2) 489 (6.6)
Daily smoker 484 (3.4) 413 (6.0) 70 (1.0)

3.2. Themes of cognitive attributions for smoking

As shown in Table 2, the EFA suggested that the 17 attributions could be grouped into 8 factors. We labeled these 8 factors as curiosity, coping, social image, social belonging, engagement, autonomy, mental enhancement, and weight control. These themes were similar to, but not exactly the same as, the factors obtained in a previous study that used a similar measure among a multiethnic sample of 8th graders in great Los Angeles (Spruijt-Metz, Gallaher, Unger, & Johnson, 2005). That study identified 4 factors from 19 attributions: personal (including mental enhancement, engagement, and social belonging in this study), functional (equivalent to coping in this study), social image, and weight control. In this study, curiosity and autonomy did not load on any factors and were therefore retained as single items. The decision was made to retain these items because even though they did not load on factors with other attributions, curiosity was endorsed quite frequently by the respondents and therefore represents an important attribution for smoking.

Table 2.

Cognitive Attributions for Smoking - Factor Pattern Matrix

Factor
I smoke (or might smoke), because: Coping Weight Control Mental Enhancement Social Image Engagement Social Belonging Autonomy Curiosity
It helps me forget my problems 0.93 0.07 −0.10 −0.03 −0.06 0.12 0.05 −0.04
It helps me deal with anger 0.88 0.01 −0.06 0.03 0.07 0.04 0.00 0.07
It helps me deal with stress 0.84 0.04 0.23 −0.05 0.00 −0.05 −0.02 −0.03
It helps me to relax 0.66 −0.05 0.25 0.10 0.13 −0.08 0.00 −0.05
It helps me keep my weight down 0.08 0.92 0.08 0.07 −0.09 −0.05 −0.10 0.08
It keeps me from eating too much −0.01 0.85 0.04 −0.04 0.12 −0.03 0.07 0.05
It helps me concentrate 0.12 0.10 0.79 −0.07 0.04 0.03 0.02 0.02
It gives me more energy 0.16 −0.08 0.77 0.07 −0.01 0.03 0.12 0.00
It helps me study −0.02 0.39 0.64 0.01 −0.01 0.08 −0.03 −0.07
It makes me look good 0.05 0.02 −0.02 0.92 0.00 −0.08 0.00 0.11
I would have more friends −0.03 0.05 0.06 0.66 0.04 0.30 0.00 −0.07
It keeps me from being bored 0.16 −0.02 −0.05 0.02 0.89 0.01 −0.06 0.05
It gives me something to do 0.02 0.06 0.15 0.00 0.75 0.04 0.04 −0.01
I don’t like to refuse when someone gives me a cigarette 0.06 −0.10 0.14 −0.01 −0.01 0.91 −0.12 0.18
I don’t want to make another person smoke alone −0.01 0.26 −0.13 0.05 0.11 0.51 0.32 −0.17
I feel like I’m making my own decisions 0.04 −0.05 0.10 0.00 −0.03 −0.06 0.95 0.10
I’m curious what it’s like −0.03 0.08 −0.04 0.05 0.03 0.08 0.08 0.90

Factor loadings greater than 0.50 are shown in bold

3.3. Relative importance of cognitive attributions, ranked by self-reports

Table 3 shows that, based on self-reports of ever smokers, curiosity was ranked 1st and 2nd for lifetime smoking and past 30-day smoking respectively, but only 4th for daily smoking. Other than curiosity, coping was ranked 1st across all stages, including lifetime smoking, past 30-day smoking, and daily smoking. Social image and social belonging were ranked as more important at earlier stages of smoking, whereas engagement and mental enhancement were ranked as more important at later stages of smoking. Autonomy and weight control were ranked as the least important among all ever smokers.

Table 3.

Relative Importance of Cognitive Attributions for Smoking, Ranked by Self-reports

Never Smoker Lifetime Smoker Past 30-day Smoker Daily Smoker

n (%) Rank n (%) Rank n (%) Rank n (%) Rank
All
Curiosity 462 (5.3) 2 1212 (34.6) 1 510 (33.3) 2 134 (27.9) 4
Coping 494 (5.7) 1 695 (19.9) 2 639 (41.8) 1 331 (68.8) 1
Social Image 117 (1.3) 6 288 (8.2) 3 246 (16.1) 5 127 (26.4) 6
Social Belonging 125 (1.4) 5 264 (7.5) 4 272 (17.8) 4 133 (27.7) 5
Engagement 107 (1.2) 7 250 (7.1) 5 332 (21.7) 3 192 (39.9) 2
Autonomy 176 (2.0) 3 202 (5.8) 6 187 (12.2) 6 81 (16.8) 7
Mental Enhancement 152 (1.7) 4 131 (3.7) 7 173 (11.3) 7 160 (33.3) 3
Weight Control 64 (0.7) 8 62 (1.8) 8 64 (4.2) 8 34 (7.1) 8
Male
Curiosity 137 (3.9) 2 626 (32.4) 1 370 (35.5) 2 120 (29.3) 5
Coping 156 (4.5) 1 344 (17.8) 2 462 (44.3) 1 298 (72.7) 1
Social Image 53 (1.5) 6 201 (10.4) 3 205 (19.7) 5 121 (29.5) 4
Social Belonging 54 (1.5) 5 174 (9.0) 4 215 (20.6) 4 118 (28.8) 6
Engagement 50 (1.4) 7 160 (8.3) 5 259 (24.9) 3 178 (43.4) 2
Autonomy 81 (2.3) 3 128 (6.6) 6 143 (13.7) 7 76 (18.5) 7
Mental Enhancement 69 (2.0) 4 85 (4.4) 7 146 (14.0) 6 148 (36.1) 3
Weight Control 13 (0.4) 8 29 (1.5) 8 48 (4.6) 8 29 (7.1) 8
Female
Curiosity 322 (6.2) 2 583 (37.5) 1 140 (28.8) 2 14 (20.0) 3
Coping 337 (6.5) 1 349 (22.5) 2 176 (36.2) 1 32 (45.7) 1
Social Image 63 (1.2) 6 86 (5.5) 5 41 (8.4) 6 5 (7.1) 6
Social Belonging 70 (1.4) 5 89 (5.7) 4 57 (11.7) 4 15 (21.4) 2
Engagement 57 (1.1) 7 90 (5.8) 3 72 (14.8) 3 13 (18.6) 4
Autonomy 93 (1.8) 3 73 (4.7) 6 44 (9.1) 5 5 (7.1) 6
Mental Enhancement 81 (1.6) 4 45 (2.9) 7 27 (5.6) 7 12 (17.1) 5
Weight Control 51 (1.0) 8 33 (2.1) 8 16 (3.3) 8 5 (7.1) 6

Based on self-reports of never smokers, coping was ranked 1st and curiosity was ranked 2nd, which is similar to that of past 30-day smokers. Mental enhancement, social belonging, and social image were ranked 4th, 5th and 6th, which is similar to that of daily smokers. Weight control was ranked as the least important, which is similar to that of all ever smokers. However, autonomy was ranked as the 3rd most important and engagement was ranked as the 2nd least important, which is different from that of all ever smokers.

3.4. Relative importance of cognitive attributions, ranked by strength of associations

Table 4 shows that, based on strength of association with smoking, curiosity remained ranked 1st and 2nd for lifetime smoking and past 30-day smoking respectively, but only 4th for daily smoking. Other than curiosity, coping was consistently ranked 1st across all stages of smoking, followed by engagement, social belonging, and social image which were about equally important across all stages of smoking. Autonomy and mental enhancement were significantly associated with smoking, but less important compared with the others. Weight control was not significantly associated with adolescent smoking.

Table 4.

Relative Importance of Cognitive Attributions for Smoking, Ranked by Strength of Associations with Smoking

All
Male
Female
β(se) OR(95% CI) LS PS DS β(se) OR(95% CI) LS PS DS β(se) OR(95% CI) LS PS DS
Curiositya
 Lifetime smoking 2.05(0.06) 7.78(6.86,8.82)* 1 2.23(0.10) 9.31(7.59,11.43)* 1 1.99(0.08) 7.34(6.22,8.65)* 1
 Past 30-day smoking 1.64(0.08) 5.13(4.36,6.05)* 2 2.00(0.12) 7.41(5.84,9.40)* 2 1.30(0.13) 3.67(2.84,4.73)* 3
 Daily smoking 1.24(0.13) 3.46(2.66,4.50)* 4 1.64(0.17) 5.16(3.72,7.15)* 3 0.60(0.33) 1.82(0.95,3.47) --
Copinga
 Lifetime smoking 0.87(0.08) 2.38(2.05,2.76)* 2 0.98(0.12) 2.67(2.12,3.36)* 2 0.86(0.10) 2.37(1.94,2.90)* 2
 Past 30-day smoking 1.73(0.09) 5.61(4.73, 6.67)* 1 2.01(0.13) 7.48(5.85,9.57)* 1 1.50(0.14) 4.47(3.43,5.83)* 1
 Daily smoking 2.30(0.13) 10.00(7.70,12.99)* 1 2.64(0.16) 14.07(10.19,19.43)* 1 1.70(0.32) 5.48(2.95,10.16)* 2
Social Imagea
 Lifetime smoking 0.49(0.13) 1.64(1.27,2.12)* 5 0.81(0.18) 2.24(1.58,3.18)* 4 0.06(0.20) 1.06(0.71,1.57) --
 Past 30-day smoking 0.65(0.14) 1.91(1.44,2.52)* 6 0.93(0.19) 2.53(1.75,3.67)* 5 0.15(0.25) 1.16(0.71,1.90) --
 Daily smoking 0.84(0.18) 2.31(1.63,3.29)* 5 1.20(0.22) 3.32(2.15,5.11)* 5 −0.54 (0.57) 0.58(0.19,1.78) --
Social Belonginga
 Lifetime smoking 0.67(0.13) 1.96(1.52,2.51)* 4 0.88(0.18) 2.41(1.70,3.42)* 3 0.39(0.19) 1.47(1.01,2.14)& 4
 Past 30-day smoking 1.21(0.14) 3.35(2.57,4.38)* 4 1.37(0.18) 3.95(2.75,5.67)* 4 0.96(0.22) 2.61(1.69,4.04)* 4
 Daily smoking 1.30(0.17) 3.66(2.61,5.15)* 3 1.41(0.22) 4.10(2.67,6.29)* 4 1.74(0.38) 5.67(2.70,11.92)* 1
Engagementa
 Lifetime smoking 0.77(0.13) 2.16(1.66,2.81)* 3 0.80(0.18) 2.24(1.56,3.21)* 5 0.79(0.20) 2.21(1.49,3.28)* 3
 Past 30-day smoking 1.47(0.14) 4.33(3.31,5.66)* 3 1.53(0.19) 4.62(3.21,6.65)* 3 1.42(0.22) 4.14(2.69,6.36)* 2
 Daily smoking 1.69(0.17) 5.40(3.91,7.47)* 2 1.81(0.21) 6.13(4.07,9.21)* 2 1.27(0.41) 3.57(1.61,7.92)# 3
Autonomya
 Lifetime smoking 0.41(0.12) 1.51(1.19,1.92)* 6 0.55(0.17) 1.73(1.25,2.40)* 6 0.28(0.19) 1.33(0.92,1.91) --
 Past 30-day smoking 0.73(0.14) 2.07(1.59,2.70)* 5 0.87(0.18) 2.38(1.68,3.38)* 6 0.65(0.22) 1.91(1.24,2.95)# 5
 Daily smoking 0.60(0.19) 1.83(1.27,2.64)# 7 0.85(0.22) 2.33(1.50,3.62)* 6 0.08(0.53) 1.09(0.39,3.06) --
Mental Enhancementa
 Lifetime smoking −0.54(0.15) 0.58(0.44,0.77)* 7 −0.51(0.20) 0.60(0.41,0.89)& 7 −0.72(0.22) 0.49(0.31,0.76)# 5
 Past 30-day smoking −0.12(0.15) 0.88(0.66,1.19) -- −0.06(0.20) 0.94(0.64,1.39) -- −0.51(0.28) 0.60(0.35,1.03) --
 Daily smoking 0.75(0.17) 2.12(1.51,2.97)* 6 0.79(0.22) 2.20(1.44,3.36)* 7 0.74(0.41) 2.09(0.93,4.69) --
Weight Controla
 Lifetime smoking −0.30(0.21) 0.74(0.49,1.12) -- 0.33(0.39) 1.39(0.64,3.01) -- −0.53(0.26) 0.59(0.35,1.00) --
 Past 30-day smoking −0.36(0.24) 0.70(0.44,1.12) -- 0.31(0.41) 1.36(0.61,3.01) -- −0.60(0.35) 0.55(0.28,1.08) --
 Daily smoking −0.48(0.30) 0.62(0.34,1.12) -- 0.07(0.46) 1.08(0.44,2.64) -- 0.00(0.57) 1.00(0.33,3.07) --
a

reference group is non-smoking.

*

p<0.001;

#

p<0.01;

&

p<0.05.

LS: lifetime smoking; PS: past 30-day smoking; DS: daily smoking.

As shown in Table 4, among both males and females, curiosity was ranked 1st for lifetime smoking and coping was ranked 1st for past 30-day smoking. However, the relative importance of most other attributions differed between genders. More attributions were significantly associated with male smoking than female smoking. The numbers of significant attributions among males and females were 7 versus 5 for lifetime smoking, 6 versus 5 for past 30-day smoking, and 7 versus 3 for daily smoking. Among attributions that were associated with both male smoking and female smoking, the associations were almost all stronger among males than among females.

4. Discussion

4.1. Cognitive attributions for smoking and their relative importance

This study indicates that Chinese adolescents attributed their smoking behavior to curiosity about smoking, coping, social image, social belonging, engagement, autonomy, mental enhancement, and weight control. These findings are consistent with some previous studies and inconsistent with others, as described in detail below.

Curiosity was found to be the most important reason for lifetime smoking, which is consistent with previous findings (Sarason et al., 1992). Cronan et al found that, even in late adolescence, curiosity was still an important reason for starting to smoke, reported by more than half of the adolescents who have smoked (Cronan et al., 1991). The present study indicates that curiosity became less important at more advanced stages of smoking.

Previous studies have shown that social image factors such as appearing cool, tough, mature, and attractive to the opposite sex can motivate adolescents to smoke (Allbutt et al., 1995; Rugkasa et al., 2001; Treacy et al., 2007). One study demonstrated that many of the reasons given by Navy recruits about why they started smoking during their first year in the service were related to social image: “to show I wasn’t afraid,” “to want to be cool,” and “to look like an adult” (Cronan et al., 1991). Other studies have found that image-related attributions were consistently important for smoking across ages (Stanton & Silva, 1993), were more important among females than among males (Barton, Chassin, Presson, & Sherman, 1982), and predicted future cigarette smoking (Aloise-Young, Hennigan, & Graham, 1996). Consistent with previous findings in Western cultural contexts, this study indicates that Chinese adolescents perceived social image as an important reason for their smoking as well. It was ranked as more important by males than by females and was more important at earlier stages of ever smoking. The positive images that adolescents associate with smoking might be derived from western media, where cigarette smoking has been presented as fashionable, elegant, cool, and sophisticated (Davis et al., 2008). Chinese adolescents have numerous opportunities to be exposed to such images because Western media and tobacco advertising are becoming increasingly popular in China. However, the rankings derived by strength of association with smoking indicated that social image was equally important across all stages of smoking, especially among males. In this study, the attribution about projecting a social image of autonomy did not load with the other social image attributions and was not strongly associated with smoking behavior. Autonomy is not necessarily perceived as a positive trait in a collectivist cultural context such as China; therefore it is not surprising that smokers did not report smoking to project a social image of autonomy. However, never smokers reported they might smoke in the future to project a social image of autonomy. Smoking prevention messages for never-smokers might be more effective if they address and counter this expectation.

Adolescents in previous studies have reported that they smoke to facilitate social interactions and strengthen social relationships (Allbutt et al., 1995; Cronan et al., 1991; Rugkasa et al., 2001; Sarason et al., 1992; Stanton & Silva, 1993; Treacy et al., 2007). Adolescents were motivated to smoke to make friends, maintain friendship, and achieve group membership and identity. One study reported this motivation as “social pressure” (Sarason et al., 1992). However, another study found that adolescents were not passively victimized by peer pressure to smoke, but actively sought out friendships with smokers and experimented with smoking as a way of gaining entry into peer groups of smokers (Rugkasa et al., 2001). “Friendship” as a reason for smoking has been found to be relatively consistent across age (Stanton, Mahalski, McGee, & Silva, 1993). Our study indicates that Chinese adolescents also perceived social belonging as an important reason for smoking across all smoking stages.

The role of coping in adolescent smoking has been inconsistent across studies. In some studies, adolescents did express that smoking helped them to calm down when they were stressed (Cronan et al., 1991). However, in another study, adolescents felt that coping with life was a reason for adult smoking and was not relevant to adolescents (Rugkasa et al., 2001). In their views, depression, anxiety, and stress were all adult issues that were irrelevant to them. Those adolescents disagreed that they were motivated to smoke by personal reasons; instead, they explained that they were motivated to smoke for social reasons to gain social status image and group membership. In the present study, coping was almost the most important reason for all stages of smoking. The reasons why Chinese adolescents should report this are not clear but could be explored in future research.

Engagement was found to be a reason for smokers’ smoking in this study, which is consistent with previous findings (Cronan et al., 1991). Engagement was more important at later stages of smoking among males, according to both self-reports and strength of associations with smoking. However, never smokers did not perceive that they might smoke to engage. Because they had never smoked, they might not have formed opinions about whether smoking is useful for engagement.

Mental enhancement was less important statistically as a predictor of smoking status. However, male daily smokers reported mental enhancement as an important reason for smoking, which is worrisome. Based on attribution theory, if people perceive mental enhancement as a reason for smoking, they might smoke when they need to refresh mind or increase concentration. Therefore, among Chinese male smokers, smoking cessation programs might be more effective if they teach adolescents more effective methods of mental enhancement such as good study habits, good sleep hygiene, and other related behaviors such as diet and physical activity.

Weight control was reported as a reason for smoking infrequently in this study, which is consistent with previous findings. Although in a qualitative focus group interview, several individual adolescents expressed weight concern as a motive for both their own smoking and their parent smoking (Allbutt et al., 1995), another quantitative study found that the percentage of adolescents who smoked for the purpose of losing weight was not high, compared with percentages of adolescents who smoked due to other reasons, such as the curiosity, friend smoking, images, and boredom (Cronan et al., 1991).

4.2. Relative importance of cognitive attributions for smoking, ranked by self-reports and by strength of associations with smoking

One of the few studies that examined associations between cognitive attributions and smoking behaviors found that, while smokers gave significantly more agreement to relaxation as a smoking motive, the most important motives associated with cigarette consumption were actually addiction and affective Smoking (Kleinke et al., 1983). In this study, curiosity and coping were consistently ranked by both self-reports and strength of associations with smoking as the most important attributions, and weight control were consistently ranked by the two means as the least important attributions of smoking. This implies that the most obvious reasons that Chinese adolescents give for their smoking are reliable and should not be overlooked. However, the relative importance of other reasons is less consistent. For example, compared with social image and social belonging, engagement was detected as more important by strength of associations but ranked as less important by self-reports. This implies that there is some discrepancy between Chinese adolescents’ self-reported reasons for smoking and the factors that are actually correlated with their smoking behavior. To be most effective, smoking prevention and cessation programs could address both types of attributions – those that are actually associated with smoking behavior and those that adolescents believe are influencing their smoking behavior.

4.3. Cognitive attributions for different stages of smoking

Several studies have demonstrated that reasons for smoking vary across stages of smoking trojectory (Baade & Stanton, 2006; Mayhew et al., 2000). For example, one study found that adolescents frequently attributed curiosity, social norms, and social pressure as reasons for beginning smoking, and pleasure and addiction as reasons for current smoking (Sarason et al., 1992). Findings of the present study are consistent with this finding; curiosity was important at the earlier stages, engagement was important at later stages, and coping was important across all stages of smoking. This suggests that health communications for smoking prevention and cessation should be stage-matched.

4.4. Cognitive attributions for male smoking and female smoking

Smoking is much more prevalent among Chinese male adolescents than among female adolescents. Some Western studies have found that male and female adolescents make similar attributions for smoking (Grube, Rokeach, & Getzlaf, 1990; Jenks, 1994b; Palmqvist & Martikainen, 2005; Stanton et al., 1993), but others have found gender differences in attributions (Anderson & Anderson, 1990; Sarason et al., 1992). In this study, attributions for male smoking and female smoking were not exactly the same. In addition, more attributions were significantly associated with male smoking than with female smoking. This might make smoking prevention interventions difficult to implement in the classroom setting, where boys and girls sit together and receive the same information. More intensive health communications via personal counseling, internet, phone calls or mail might be needed to prevent smoking or encourage cessation among Chinese boys.

4.5. Summary

This study extends the results of previous attribution studies in that it not only identified cognitive attributions for smoking, but also examined the relative importance of each cognitive attribution at different smoking stages and between genders. Therefore, the findings provide more complete information about the causes of adolescent smoking and are instructive for development of evidence-based anti-smoking programs.

4.6. Limitations and future directions

We should acknowledge several limitations in this study. First, data used were from an ongoing study and not specifically collected for the present study. Therefore, the measures for cognitive attributions for smoking were less than ideal. Previous studies have indicated that peer smoking (Chen, Fang, Li, Stanton, & Lin, 2006; Hesketh, Ding, & Tomkins, 2001; Zhang, Wang, & Zhou, 2005) and familial smoking (Chen, Stanton et al., 2006; Xiang et al., 1999; Yang et al., 2004; Zhang et al., 2005) influence Chinese adolescents to smoke. Unfortunately, these important reasons for smoking were not included in present investigation. Future studies should investigate broader attributions for smoking at the intrapersonal, interpersonal, institutional, community, and policy levels. Qualitative approach (such as focus groups) conducted among Chinese adolescents could help to generate more attributions for smoking that are unique to Chinese adolescent culture. Guided by attribution theory, future studies could also better categorize attributions in terms of their locus (personal and situational), stability (stable and unstable), and controllability (controllable and uncontrollable), which may have different implications for self-esteem, expectations, and social emotions (Weiner, 1986). Second, we were unable to account for the potential intraclass correlations among participants within each school due to limited capability of the statistical program we used. However, because the main purpose of the EFA was to estimate the factor loadings rather than to make statistical inferences, the impact of using conventional EFA on our results is likely minimal. Third, although this retrospective study has successfully identified cognitive attributions for Chinese adolescent smoking, their relative importance, and their associations with actual smoking behaviors, longitudinal studies are needed to reveal the roles of these cognitive attributions in subsequent smoking behaviors.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the University of Southern California Transdisciplinary Tobacco Use Research Center, funded by the National Institutes of Health (grant #1 P50 CA84735-01) and the Sidney R. Garfield Endowment. The authors thank the project directors and staff at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention in the cities of Chengdu, Hangzhou, Harbin, Qingdao, Shenyang, and Wuhan and the Institute for Health Education in Kunming, China, for assistance with project coordination and data collection. We also thank the principals, physicians, and teachers in the participating schools for their cooperation. Finally, we express our gratitude to the national Chinese Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Peking University School of Public Health, and the municipal government, Health Bureau, and Education Committee in each participating city for their support. The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Allbutt H, Amos A, Cunningham-Burley S. The social image of smoking among young people in Scotland. Health Education Research. 1995;10:443–454. [Google Scholar]
  2. Aloise-Young PA, Hennigan KM, Graham JW. Role of the self-image and smoker stereotype in smoking onset during early adolescence: a longitudinal study. Health Psychology. 1996;15(6):494–497. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.15.6.494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson RC, Anderson KE. Success and failure attributions in smoking cessation among men and women. Journal of the American Association of Occupational Health Nurses. 1990;38(4):180–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baade PD, Stanton WR. Determinants of stages of smoking uptake among secondary school students. Addictive Behaviors. 2006;31(1):143–148. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2005.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barton J, Chassin L, Presson CC, Sherman SJ. Social image factors as motivators of smoking initiation in early and middle adolescence. Child Development. 1982;53(6):1499–1511. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Berlin I, Singleton EG, Pedarriosse AM, Lancrenon S, Rames A, Aubin HJ, et al. The Modified Reasons for Smoking Scale: factorial structure, gender effects and relationship with nicotine dependence and smoking cessation in French smokers. Addiction. 2003;98(11):1575–1583. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00523.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chen X, Fang X, Li X, Stanton B, Lin D. Stay away from tobacco: a pilot trial of a school-based adolescent smoking prevention program in Beijing, China. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2006;8(2):227–237. doi: 10.1080/14622200600576479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen X, Stanton B, Fang X, Li X, Lin D, Zhang J, et al. Perceived smoking norms, socioenvironmental factors, personal attitudes and adolescent smoking in China: a mediation analysis with longitudinal data. Journal of Adolescence Health. 2006;38(4):359–368. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cronan TA, Conway TL, Kaszas SL. Starting to smoke in the Navy: when, where and why. Social Science and Medicine. 1991;33(12):1349–1353. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(91)90278-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Davis KE, Gilpin EA, Loken B, Viswanath K, Wakefield MA. Themes and Targets of Tobacco Advertising and Promotion. Tobacco Control Monograph Series 19: The Role of the Media in Promoting and Reducing Use. 2008:141–178. [Google Scholar]
  11. Edwards R. The problem of tobacco smoking. Bmj (Clinical Research Ed) 2004;328(7433):217–219. doi: 10.1136/bmj.328.7433.217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eiser JR, Sutton SR, Wober M. Smokers, non-smokers and the attribution of addiction. British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 1977;16(4):329–336. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1977.tb00239.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Eiser JR, Sutton SR, Wober M. Smokers’ and non-smokers’ attributions about smoking: a case of actor-observer differences? British Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology. 1978;17(2):189–190. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1978.tb00263.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Grenard JL, Guo Q, Jasuja GK, Unger JB, Chou CP, Gallaher PE, et al. Influences affecting adolescent smoking behavior in China. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2006;8(2):245–255. doi: 10.1080/14622200600576610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Grube JW, Rokeach M, Getzlaf SB. Adolescents’ value images of smokers, ex-smokers, and nonsmokers. Addictive Behaviors. 1990;15(1):81–88. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90010-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Guo Q, Johnson CA, Unger JB, Lee L, Xie B, Chou CP, et al. Utility of the theory of reasoned action and theory of planned behavior for predicting Chinese adolescent smoking. Addictive Behaviors. 2007;32(5):1066–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Heider F. The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley; 1958. [Google Scholar]
  18. Hesketh T, Ding QJ, Tomkins A. Smoking among youths in China. American Journal of Public Health. 2001;91(10):1653–1655. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.10.1653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hsia FN, Spruijt-Metz D. The meanings of smoking among Chinese American and Taiwanese American college students. Nicotine and Tobacco Research. 2003;5(6):837–850. doi: 10.1080/14622200310001615259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jenks RJ. Attitudes and perceptions toward smoking: smokers’ views of themselves and other smokers. Journal of Social Psychology. 1994a;134(3):355–361. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1994.9711740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jenks RJ. Smoking and satisfaction and motivations: a comparison of men and women. Journal of Social Psychology. 1994b;134(6):847–849. doi: 10.1080/00224545.1994.9923019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Johnson CA, Palmer PH, Chou CP, Pang Z, Zhou D, Dong L, et al. Tobacco use among youth and adults in Mainland China: the China Seven Cities Study. Public Health. 2006;120(12):1156–1169. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2006.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Jones EE, Davis KE. From acts to dispositions: the attribution process in person perception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 1965;2:219–266. [Google Scholar]
  24. Jones EE, Nisbett RE. The actor and the observer: divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior. Morristown: New Jersey: General Learning Press; 1971. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kelley HH. Attribution theory in social psychology. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 1967;15:192–238. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kleinke CL, Staneski RA, Meeker FB. Attributions for smoking behavior: comparing smokers with nonsmokers and predicting smokers’ cigarette consumption. Journal of Research in Personality. 1983;17:242–255. [Google Scholar]
  27. Mayhew KP, Flay BR, Mott JA. Stages in the development of adolescent smoking. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2000;59(Supplement 1):S61–81. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00165-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. McGee R, Stanton WR. A longitudinal study of reasons for smoking in adolescence. Addiction. 1993;88(2):265–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb00810.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Monson TC, Snyder M. Actors, observers, and the attribution process. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1977;13:89–111. [Google Scholar]
  30. Moolchan ET, Ernst M, Henningfield JE. A review of tobacco smoking in adolescents: treatment implications. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescence Psychiatry. 2000;39(6):682–693. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200006000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Palmqvist RA, Martikainen LK. Changes in reasons given for adolescent smoking, 1984–1999. Substance Use and Misuse. 2005;40(5):645–656. doi: 10.1081/ja-200055363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Rugkasa J, Knox B, Sittlington J, Kennedy O, Treacy MP, Abaunza PS. Anxious adults vs. cool children: children’s views on smoking and addiction. Social Science and Medicine. 2001;53(5):593–602. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00367-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sarason IG, Mankowski ES, Peterson AV, Jr, Dinh KT. Adolescents’ reasons for smoking. Journal of School Health. 1992;62(5):185–190. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1992.tb06039.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Schepis TS, Rao U. Epidemiology and etiology of adolescent smoking. Current Opinion in Pediatrics. 2005;17(5):607–612. doi: 10.1097/01.mop.0000176442.49743.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Spruijt-Metz D, Gallaher P, Unger JB, Johnson CA. Unique contributions of meanings of smoking and outcome expectancies to understanding smoking initiation in middle school. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2005;30(2):104–111. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3002_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Stanton WR, Mahalski PA, McGee R, Silva PA. Reasons for smoking or not smoking in early adolescence. Addictive Behaviors. 1993;18(3):321–329. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(93)90033-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Stanton WR, Silva PA. Consistency in children’s recall of age of initiating smoking. International Journal of Epidemiology. 1993;22(6):1064–1069. doi: 10.1093/ije/22.6.1064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Treacy MP, Hyde A, Boland J, Whitaker T, Abaunza PS, Stewart-Knox BJ. Children talking: emerging perspectives and experiences of cigarette smoking. Qualitative Health Research. 2007;17(2):238–249. doi: 10.1177/1049732306297679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Turner L, Mermelstein R, Flay B. Individual and contextual influences on adolescent smoking. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1021:175–197. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Tyas SL, Pederson LL. Psychosocial factors related to adolescent smoking: a critical review of the literature. Tobacco Control. 1998;7(4):409–420. doi: 10.1136/tc.7.4.409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Weiner B. An attributional theory of motivation and emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  42. Xiang H, Wang Z, Stallones L, Yu S, Gimbel HW, Yang P. Cigarette smoking among medical college students in Wuhan, People’s Republic of China. Preventive Medecine. 1999;29(3):210–215. doi: 10.1006/pmed.1999.0525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Yang G, Fan L, Tan J, Qi G, Zhang Y, Samet JM, et al. Smoking in China: findings of the 1996 National Prevalence Survey. The Journal of the American Medicine Association. 1999;282(13):1247–1253. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.13.1247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Yang G, Ma J, Chen AP, Brown S, Taylor CE, Samet JM. Smoking among adolescents in China: 1998 survey findings. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2004;33(5):1103–1110. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Zhang L, Wang WF, Zhou G. A cross-sectional study of smoking risk factors in junior high school students in Henan, China. Southeast Asian Journal of Tropical Medicine and Public Health. 2005;36(6):1580–1584. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Zoller U, Maymon T. Smoking behavior of high school students in Israel. Journal of School Health. 1983;53(10):613–617. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1983.tb01122.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES