Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Feb 14.
Published in final edited form as: J Behav Med. 2008 Nov 22;32(1):20. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0

Discrimination and racial disparities in health: evidence and needed research

David R Williams 1,2,, Selina A Mohammed 3
PMCID: PMC2821669  NIHMSID: NIHMS168906  PMID: 19030981

Abstract

This paper provides a review and critique of empirical research on perceived discrimination and health. The patterns of racial disparities in health suggest that there are multiple ways by which racism can affect health. Perceived discrimination is one such pathway and the paper reviews the published research on discrimination and health that appeared in PubMed between 2005 and 2007. This recent research continues to document an inverse association between discrimination and health. This pattern is now evident in a wider range of contexts and for a broader array of outcomes. Advancing our understanding of the relationship between perceived discrimination and health will require more attention to situating discrimination within the context of other health-relevant aspects of racism, measuring it comprehensively and accurately, assessing its stressful dimensions, and identifying the mechanisms that link discrimination to health.

Keywords: Racism, Discrimination, Stress, Health disparities, Race, Ethnicity


This paper will provide an overview of the current evidence for and needed research on the role of perceived discrimination in health. It seeks to situate the research on personal experiences of discrimination within the larger literature on racism and health. It begins by describing some salient patterns in the large and persistent racial/ethnic variations in health that have provided an impetus to better understand the role of racism in health. It centrally focuses on recent research on perceived discrimination and health. It critiques the existing literature with an eye toward highlighting the needed improvements in the conceptualization and measurement of perceived discrimination that would advance our understanding of the potential role of race-related stressors in health.

Disparities and the added burden of race

Racial disparities in health in the U.S. are large and pervasive. For most of the 15 leading causes of death including heart disease, cancer, stroke, diabetes, kidney disease, hypertension, liver cirrhosis and homicide, African Americans (or blacks) have higher death rates than whites (Kung et al. 2008). These elevated death rates exist across the life-course with African Americans and American Indians having higher age-specific mortality rates than whites from birth through the retirement years (Williams 2005). Other data indicate that almost 100,000 black persons die prematurely each year who would not die if there were no racial disparities in health (Levine et al. 2001). Another noteworthy characteristic of racial disparities is their persistence over time. Despite gains in life expectancy for both blacks and whites, the 7 year racial gap in life expectancy in 1960 was still 5.1 years in 2005 (National Center for Health Statistics (2007). Similarly, although infant mortality has declined over time for both blacks and whites, the relative gap between the races is much wider today than it was in 1950 (Williams and Jackson 2005; NCHS 2007). For some health outcomes, the disparities are worsening. Trend data for heart disease and cancer—the two leading causes of death in the United States—indicate that blacks and whites had comparable death rates for these conditions in 1950, but African Americans now have higher mortality rates than whites (Williams and Jackson 2005; NCHS 2007).

Research also reveals that pathogenic factors linked to race continue to affect health even when socioeconomic status (SES) is controlled. In national data there are residual racial differences in health at every level of SES for multiple indicators of health status, including self-rated health, heart disease mortality, hypertension and obesity (Pamuk et al. 1998). This pattern exists for a broad range of other outcomes. A striking example comes from national data on infant mortality by mothers’ education for all women age 20 years and older. African American women with a college degree or more education have a higher rate of infant mortality than white, Hispanic (or Latino), and Asian and Pacific Islander women who have not completed high school (Pamuk et al. 1998). Further evidence of the markedly elevated disease risk for African Americans comes from national data on chronic disease risk factors for blacks, whites and Hispanics age 40 and over (Crimmins et al. 2007). This study assessed indicators of blood pressure risk (systolic, diastolic, and pulse rate), inflammation risk (C-reactive protein, fibrinogen, albumin) and metabolic risk (total cholesterol, HDL cholesterol, BMI and glycated hemoglobin). A summary indicator of total risk counted how many of these 10 risk factors were outside of the normal range. This study found that even after adjustment for income, education, gender and age, blacks had higher scores on blood pressure, inflammation, and total risk. Importantly, blacks maintained a higher risk profile even after adjusting for health behaviors (smoking, poor diet, physical activity and access to care).

These data suggest that there are added factors linked to racial status that adversely affect the health of disadvantaged minority populations in the United States. In seeking to understand these striking burdens of race, researchers are pursuing three lines of inquiry. First, the measures of SES are not equivalent across race. For example, compared to whites, college-educated blacks are more likely to experience unemployment, employed blacks are more likely to be exposed to occupational hazards and carcinogens even after adjusting for job experience and education, blacks have lower wealth at every level of income, and have less purchasing power because the costs of a broad range of goods and services are higher in Black communities (Kaufman et al. 1997; Williams and Collins 1995). Second, there is increasing attention to the need to capture exposure to health risks over the life course. Racial/ethnic differences in childhood SES and early life psychosocial and economic adversity are likely to be important contributors to racial disparities in adult health. Third, researchers are exploring the multiple ways by which racism can adversely affect health (Jones 2000; Williams 2004; Ahmed et al. 2007).

Perceived discrimination as a stressor

The term, racism, refers to an organized system that categorizes population groups into ‘races’, and uses this ranking to preferentially allocate societal goods and resources to groups regarded as superior (Bonilla-Silva 1996). Fundamental to racism is cultural racism that undergirds an ideology of inferiority that ranks some racial groups as inherently or culturally superior to others and supports the social norms and institutions that implement this ideology (Jones 1997). Racism often leads to the development of negative attitudes and beliefs toward racial outgroups (prejudice), and differential treatment of members of these groups by both individuals and social institutions (discrimination). Importantly, because racism is deeply embedded in the culture and institutions of society, discrimination can persist in institutional structures and policies even in the context of marked declines in individual level racial prejudice and discrimination. Moreover, negative racial stereotypes that are deeply rooted in mainstream culture can serve as an additional source of discriminatory behavior even among persons who may not be prejudiced. Considerable scientific evidence indicates that discrimination persists in multiple contexts of American society including housing, labor markets, criminal justice and education (Blank et al. 2004; Fix and Struyk 1993). Targets of discrimination are aware of some of the discriminatory behavior directed at them and these perceptions of unfair treatment can generate stress (Clark et al. 1999).

Perceived racial or ethnic discrimination is one aspect of racism that is increasingly receiving empirical attention as a class of stressors that could have consequences for health and for understanding disparities in health. This is consistent with broader interest in the role of stress as a determinant of social disparities in health (Pearlin et al. 2005). Stress appears to accelerate cellular aging (Epel et al. 2006) and the chronic stressors triggered by multiple environmental assaults can lead to wear and tear on the body that can dysregulate multiple biological systems and lead to premature illness and mortality (Seeman et al. 2004). A word about terminology—in this paper, we use the terms discrimination, perceived discrimination, interpersonal discrimination and self reported discrimination interchangeably. We also use the term racial discrimination to refer to both racial and ethnic discrimination.

Prior reviews of the early research on discrimination and health have found an inverse association between discrimination and morbidity (Williams et al. 2003; Krieger 1999). Mental health status was the most frequently used health outcome and other self-reported indicators of health were widely used. Most early studies were U.S.-based and cross-sectional in design. To assess the current state of the empirical evidence on discrimination and health, we conducted a systematic review of the PubMed database to update the recent review by Paradies (2006) which covered the period of 2000–2004. We used the search terms racism, racial discrimination, race and discrimination, perceived discrimination, prejudice, racial prejudice, racialized prejudice, race discrimination, racialized discrimination, ethnic discrimination, social discrimination, racialized and racialization to identify relevant studies published between 2005 and 2007. Our search initially yielded 5,107 articles. These articles or their abstracts were individually reviewed to select only those published during the three year period 2005–2007 (epubs for 2008 publications were excluded), that empirically examined the association between a measure of perceived discrimination and an indicator of health status or healthcare utilization. One hundred and fifteen papers met our criteria (Table 1). We acknowledge that focusing only on PubMed does not provide a comprehensive picture of all of the published research on this topic since the work of some social scientists is not included in this database. It nonetheless serves to illustrate the growing scientific interest in this subject.

Table 1.

Published research on discrimination and health in PubMed, 2005–2007a

Study Sample size Design Outcome variables Findings
1. Mental health
Banks et al. (2006) 570 Blacks Cross-sectional Anxiety & depressive symptoms Positive association
Beiser and Hou (2006) 647 SE Asian refugees Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Positive association
Bhui et al. (2005) 2054 UK workers Cross-sectional Anxiety & depression Positive association
Birman et al. (2005) 269 Soviet immigrant teens in US Cross-sectional Russian identity
American identity
Positive association
Inverse association
Brody et al. (2006) 714 Black adolescents Longitudinal Conduct problems & depressive symptoms Positive association
Brook et al. (2006a) 210 Black and Puerto Rican elementary kids Cross-sectional Rebellious behavior Positive association
Brondolo et al. (2005) 420 Blacks & Latinos Cross-sectional Emotions of threat, harm, & anger Positive association
Broudy et al. (2007) 113 Multiethnic adults Cross-sectional Daily moods (anger, sadness & nervousness) Positive association
Bynum et al. (2007) 247 Black freshmen Cross-sectional Psychological distress Positive association
Crouter et al. (2006) 218 Mexican American families Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Positive association
Dyrbye et al. (2007) 3,080 U.S. medical students Cross-sectional Burnout, depressive symptoms, low QOL Positive association
Gee et al. (2006b) 666 U.S. blacks, black & Latino immigrants Cross-sectional Mental Health Inverse association
Gee et al. (2007b) 2,047 Asian American adults Cross-sectional Past year mental disorders Positive association
Greene et al. (2006) 136 High school students Longitudinal Depressive symptoms self- esteem
Self-esteem
Positive association
Inverse association
Karlsen et al. (2005) 3,446 from 5 UK ethnic immigrants Cross-sectional Psychosis
Anxiety or depressive disorder
Positive association
Positive association
Khaylis et al. (2007) 91 Undergrad students Cross-sectional PTSD symptoms Positive association
Lam (2007) 122 Vietnamese college students Cross-sectional Depression & anxiety symptoms Positive association
Lam et al. (2005) 1,451 Teens in Hong Kong Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms
Life satisfaction & purpose
Positive association
Inverse association
Lincoln et al. (2007) 4,915 African Americans & Caribbean Blacks Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Conditional association
Major et al. (2007) 3 Samples of Latino students (n = 191) Cross-sectional Self-esteem Inverse association
Miller and Travers (2005) 208 UK minority ethnic teachers Cross-sectional Psychological symptoms (GHQ)
Job satisfaction
Positive association
Inverse association
Montgomery and Foldspang (2007) 131 Middle Eastern refugees in Denmark Cross-sectional Internalizing symptoms
Externalizing symptoms
Positive association
No association
Noh et al. (2007) 180 Adult Korean immigrants, Toronto Cross-sectional Positive affect
Depressive symptoms
Inverse association
Positive association
Oppedal et al. (2005) 1,275 Immigrant 10th graders in Norway Cross-sectional Psychiatric problems Positive association
Pantzer et al. (2006) 1,246 Native and immigrant adolescents in Spain Cross-sectional Health related quality of life Inverse association
Pole et al. (2005) 668 Hispanic, white and black police officers Cross-sectional PTSD symptoms Positive association
Poyrazli and Lopez (2007) 439 International & U.S. college students Cross-sectional Homesickness Positive association
Romero et al. (2007) 519 Latino & non-Latino teens Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms, drug use & violence Positive association
Salvatore and Shelton (2007) 250 University students Laboratory Cognitive functioning Inverse association
Schulz et al. (2006b) 700 Black women Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Positive association
Sheridan (2006) 222 British Muslims Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Positive association
Siefert et al. (2007) 824 Black mothers Cross-sectional Probable depression Positive association
Simons et al. (2006) 332 Black teen males Longitudinal Violent delinquency Positive association
Smokowski and Bacallao (2007) 323 Latino teens Cross-sectional Internalizing problems and low self-esteem Positive association
Steffen and Bowden (2006) 168 Hispanic-American immigrants Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms
Sleep disturbance
Positive association
Positive association
Stevens et al. (2005a) 1,127 Moroccan immigrant teens in Netherlands Cross-sectional Externalizing behaviors Positive association
Stevens et al. (2005b) 1,127 Moroccan immigrant teens in Netherlands Cross-sectional Internalizing problems Positive association
Umaña-Taylor and Updegraff (2007) 273 Latino adolescents Cross-sectional Depressive symptoms Positive association
Utsey and Hook (2007) 215 Black college students Cross-sectional Psychological distress Positive association
Utsey et al. (2006) 323 U.S. black adults Cross-sectional Quality of life Conditional association
Veling et al. (2007) 459 Ethnic minorities in The Netherlands Cross-sectional Incidence of psychotic disorders Positive association
Vines et al. (2006) 476 Black women Cross-sectional Negative emotions Positive association
Wadsworth et al. (2007) 626 Blacks, whites, & Bangladeshis Cross-sectional Psychological distress Conditional association
Wagner and Abbott (2007) 120 Diabetic blacks Cross-sectional Depression Positive association
Waltermaurer et al. (2006) 88 Black women Cross-sectional Intimate partner violence Positive association
Wamala et al. (2007b) 33,328 Swedish adults Cross-sectional Psychological distress Positive association
Yoder et al. (2006) 212 American Indian youth Cross-sectional Suicidal ideation Positive association
2. Blood pressure/hypertension
Brown et al. (2006) 3,300 Multi-ethnic women Cross-sectional Systolic & diastolic BP No association
Clark and Gochett (2006) 217 Black youth Cross-sectional Blood pressure Conditional association
Clark (2006a) 234 Black high schoolers Cross-sectional Systolic & diastolic BP Conditional association
Cozier et al. (2006) 2316 Black women Longitudinal Hypertension incidence Conditional association
Davis et al. (2005) 356 Black adults Cross-sectional Hypertension Conditional association
Matthews et al. (2005) 207 Black & white teens Cross-sectional Ambulatory blood pressure Conditional association
Peters (2006) 162 Blacks Cross-sectional Chronic stress emotions
Blood pressure
Positive association
No association
Peters et al. (2007) 145 Black hypertensive patients Cross-sectional Blood pressure
Satisfaction with care
Positive association
Inverse association
Roberts et al. (2007) 1,110 Middle-aged blacks Cross-sectional Hypertension Conditional association
Ryan et al. (2006) 666 U.S. blacks, blacks & Latino immigrants Cross-sectional Physical health
Systolic pressure
Diastolic pressure
Inverse association
U-shaped association
No association
3. Reactivity
Clark (2006b) 110 Black college women Lab experiment Vascular reactivity Positive association
Clark et al. (2006) 72 black men Lab experiment Cardiovascular reactivity Positive association
Clark et al. (2006a) 153 Black youth Lab experiment Large arterial elasticity Conditional association
King (2005) 115 Black college women Lab experiment Affective stress reactions Positive association
Lepore et al. (2006) 80 black & white women Lab experiment Cardiovascular reactivity Positive association
Merritt et al. (2006) 73 Normotensive black men Lab experiment Cardiovascular reactivity Positive association
Richman et al. (2007) 165 Normotensive black & white adults Lab experiment Cardiovascular reactivity Positive association
Thomas et al. (2006) 122 Employed blacks & whites Lab experiment Reactivity to phenylephrine Positive association
4. Other physical health
Borrell et al. (2006) 1,722 Blacks Cross-sectional Self-reported health (SF- 12)
Depressive symptoms (CES-D)
Inverse association
Positive association
Gee et al. (2007a) 2,095 Asian Americans Cross-sectional Chronic health conditions Positive association
Gee et al. (2006a) 2,241 Filipino Americans Cross-sectional Chronic health conditions Positive association
Harris et al. (2006a) 4,108 Maori & 6,269 Europeans in New Zealand Cross-sectional 4 self-reported measures of ill health Positive association
Harris et al. (2006b) 12,500 Adults in New Zealand Cross-sectional 5 measures of self-reported ill health Positive association
Larson et al. (2007) 639 Aboriginal & other Australians Cross-sectional Self-reported poor physical & mental health Positive association
Lauderdale (2006) 15,064 Arab women Cross-sectional Low birth weight and prematurity Positive association
Lewis et al. (2006) 181 Black women Longitudinal Coronary artery calcification Positive association
Locher et al. (2005) 1,000 Elderly adults Cross-sectional Nutritional risk Conditional association
Moody-Ayers et al. (2005) 42 Diabetic blacks Cross-sectional Glycosylated hemoglobin
Self-rated health
No association
No association
Piette et al. (2006) 810 Adult diabetes patients Cross-sectional Hemoglobin A1c
Physical symptom burden
Poor physical functioning (SF-12)
Positive association
Positive association
Positive association
Schulz et al. (2006a) 343 Black women Longitudinal Symptoms of depression
Self-rated health
Positive association
Inverse association
Sellers et al. (2006) 399 Well-educated black men Cross-sectional Physical health
Mental health
No association
Inverse association
Stetler et al. (2006) 48 Black participants Longitudinal Antibody response to flu vaccine Inverse association
Sujoldzic et al. (2006) 1,282 Immigrant teens from Bosnia and Herzegovina residing in Bosnia, Croatia or Austria Cross-sectional Health problems
Psychological well-being
Positive association
Inverse association
Taylor et al. (2007) 49,161 Black women Longitudinal Breast cancer incidence Positive association
Thomas et al. (2006) 93 Employed men & women Cross-sectional Sleep stage 4
Physical fatigue
Inverse association
Positive association
Vines et al. (2007) 447 Black women (35– 49 years) Cross-sectional Waist-hip ratio Inverse association
Wise et al. (2007) 22,002 U.S. black women Longitudinal Incidence of uterine leiomyomata (fibroids, myomas) Positive association
Young et al. (2005) 1,008 Illicit drug users Cross-sectional Mental health
Chronic conditions
Inverse association
Positive association
Zamboni and Crawford (2007) 174 Black gay/bisexual men Cross-sectional Sexual problems Positive association
5. Health care utilization
Adegbembo et al. (2006) 924 Low-income blacks & whites Cross-sectional Health care trust Positive association
Banks and Dracup (2006) 61 Blacks with MI Cross-sectional Delay in seeking treatment No association
Bazargan et al. (2005) 287 Black and Latino adults Cross-sectional Alternative care use as substitute for conventional care Positive association
Benkert et al. (2006) 145 Low-income blacks Cross-sectional Satisfaction with care Inverse association
Casagrande et al. (2007) 1,408 Blacks & whites Cross-sectional Delays in seeking medical care
Non-adherence to medical care
Positive association
Positive association
Chen et al. (2005) 3.884 adults Cross-sectional Preferences for same race physician Positive association
Dailey et al. (2007) 1,229 Black and white women Cross-sectional Mammography screening No association
Etowa et al. (2007) 237 Black Canadian women Cross-sectional Access to healthcare Inverse association
Facione and Facione (2007) 817 Latino, black & white women Cross-sectional Non-adherence to cancer screening & fewer provider visits Positive association
Fowler-Brown et al. (2006) 3,694 Black & white rural southern adults Cross-sectional Satisfaction with care
Preventative services use
Inverse association
No association
Hoyo et al. (2005) 144 Adult black women Cross-sectional Adherence to pap smear screening No association
Jang et al. (2005) 230 Korean elderly Cross-sectional Satisfaction with care Inverse association
Malat and Hamilton (2006) 1,189 Blacks Cross-sectional Preference for same race physician Positive association
Malat and van Ryn (2005) 1,189 Blacks Cross-sectional Preference for same race physician Positive association
Nápoles-Springer et al. (2005) 163 Blacks, Latinos & whites Cross-sectional Perceptions of the quality of medical encounters Inverse association
Sohler et al. (2007) 523 Adults Cross-sectional Poor quality of healthcare Positive association
Trivedi and Ayanian (2006) 54,968 Respondents Cross-sectional Received: cholesterol test, eye exam, hemoglobin A1c & flu shots
Received: aspirin, PSA test
Inverse association
No association
Van Houtven et al. (2005) 545 Black, Latino & white adults Cross-sectional Delay of filling prescriptions
Delay of treatments
Positive association
Positive association
Wamala et al. (2007a) 31,851 Swedish adults Cross-sectional Refraining from seeking treatment Positive association
6. Substance use & health behaviors
Bennett et al. (2005) 2,129 Black college students Cross-sectional Tobacco use Positive association
Borrell et al. (2007) 1,507 Blacks & 1,813 whites Cross-sectional Lifetime crack, speed, heroin
Alcohol, tobacco use
Lifetime cocaine & marijuana
No association
Positive association
Positive association
Brook et al. (2006b) 731 South African teens Cross-sectional Cigarette smoking Positive association
Choi et al. (2006) 2,082 Middle school (multi & monoracial) Cross-sectional Ever substance use & frequency
Violent behavior
Positive association
Positive association
Gee et al. (2007a) 2,217 Filipino Americans Cross-sectional Prescription medication use
Illicit drug use
Alcohol dependence
Positive association
Positive association
Positive association
Gibbons et al. (2007) 889 Black families Longitudinal Subsequent drug use Positive association
Kalichman et al. (2006) 2,122 Black and Coloured South Africans Cross-sectional HIV risk behavior Positive association
Krieger et al. (2005) 159 Blacks, 249 Latinos & 208 whites Cross-sectional Cigarette smoking
Psychological distress
Positive association
Positive association
Landrine et al. (2006) 1,569 Black, Latino, Asian, and White adults Cross-sectional Cigarette smoking & psychiatric symptoms Positive association
Terrell et al. (2006) 134 Black adolescents Cross-sectional Alcohol consumption Positive association
a

Conditional association = discrimination is unrelated to health in the overall sample, but an association exits only for some sub-group

Trends in the recent literature

Studies of mental health continue to dominate the discrimination and health literature. Forty-seven studies are listed under the mental health category in Table 1 but several other articles that included a mental health measure are listed under the other summary categories because multiple indicators of health were utilized. A broad range of mental health outcomes has been examined in recent papers. These include studies that have examined the relationship between discrimination and schizophrenia among ethnic minorities in the Netherlands (Veling et al. 2007), burn-out in U.S. medical students (Dyrbye et al. 2007), daily moods among multi-ethnic U.S. adults (Broudy et al. 2007), cognitive impairment among black and white university students (Salvatore and Shelton 2007), and current rates of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of Asian Americans (Gee et al. 2007b). Discrimination has also been associated with homesickness among college students (Poyrazli and Lopez 2007) and conduct problems among adolescents (Brody et al. 2006). Other recent research has related perceived discrimination to multiple forms of violence (Choi et al. 2006). These include intimate partner violence (Waltermaurer et al. 2006) and violence among adolescents (Simons et al. 2006). Almost without exception, studies of discrimination and mental health find that higher levels of discrimination are associated with poorer mental health status. At the same time, almost all studies are cross-sectional leaving open the possibility that perceptions of discrimination are a consequence of mental health status. It is therefore noteworthy that the few published prospective studies (Brody et al. 2006; Greene et al. 2006; Simons et al. 2006; Schulz et al. 2006b), have found that there is a positive association between perceived discrimination and changes in mental health symptoms. This pattern is consistent with one earlier national study of African Americans which found that baseline depression and depressive symptoms were not associated with subsequent reports of discrimination (Brown et al. 2000).

There has long been interest in the relationship between discrimination and blood pressure (Williams and Neighbors 2001; Brondolo et al. 2003), as well as, cardiovascular disease more broadly (Wyatt et al. 2003). Table 1 lists eight recent studies that use laboratory experiments to expose individuals to analogues of racist events. The studies of cardiovascular reactivity find that acute experiences of stress continue to be related to increases in blood pressure reactivity in the laboratory setting. However, our understanding of the relationship between exposure to discrimination and the sustained elevation of blood pressure remains elusive. In recent studies, the patterns remain complex and unclear. While one study found a U-shaped association between discrimination and systolic blood pressure among Latinos and African Americans in New Hampshire (Ryan et al. 2006), some other studies have not found an association between perceived discrimination and blood pressure. This includes analyses of 3,300 middle-aged women in the MESA study (Brown et al. 2006). In a study of black and white adolescents, unfair treatment attributed to race was unrelated to ambulatory blood pressure, but unfair treatment due to physical appearance was (Matthews et al. 2005). Table 1 uses the term “conditional association” to indicate the absence of an association between discrimination and health in the overall sample, but with the existence of an association only for some sub-group. This pattern dominates the recent studies of discrimination and blood pressure. In analyses of 2,316 cases of incident hypertension in the Black Women’s Health Study (BWHS, Cozier et al. 2006), although discrimination was unrelated to incident hypertension in the total sample, it was positively related among women born outside of the U.S. In the Metro Atlanta Heart Disease Study, although discrimination was unrelated to blood pressure, high levels of stress due to discrimination were predictive of increased hypertension risk (Davis et al. 2005). Moreover, some of the conditional findings are counterintuitive. For example, in studies of black adolescents, discrimination was inversely related to blood pressure only among those who responded to discrimination with a passive coping style (Clark and Gochett 2006) or among those who were low on trait anger (Clark 2006a).

A broad range of physical health outcomes have been considered in the 21 recent studies listed in Table 1. Several large cross-sectional studies have found a positive association between discrimination and chronic health conditions or other self-reported indicators of ill-health. These include a national study of Asian Americans (Gee et al. 2007a), a study of Filipino Americans in San Francisco and Hawaii (Gee et al. 2006a), an African American sample in the CARDIA study (Borrell et al. 2006), and a national study in New Zealand (Harris et al. 2006b). In the New Zealand study a dose–response relationship was observed between perceived discrimination and each of the five indicators of health: self-rated health, physical functioning, mental health, cigarette smoking, and self-reported cardiovascular disease (Harris et al. 2006b) Other cross-sectional studies have found self-reported discrimination related to abdominal fat (Vines et al. 2007), hemoglobin A1c (Piette et al. 2006), poorer sexual functioning (Zamboni and Crawford 2007), nutritional risk among Black men (Locher et al. 2005) less stage 4 sleep (i.e., “deep,” or slow-wave sleep) and physical fatigue (Thomas et al. 2006). Longitudinal analyses of the large cohort of the BWHS have found a positive association between discrimination and the incidence of uterine myomas (fibroids) (Wise et al. 2007) and the incidence of breast cancer (Taylor et al. 2007). Other prospective analyses indicate that perceived discrimination predicts coronary artery calcification (Lewis et al. 2006) and changes in self-rated health (Schulz et al. 2006a).

Table 1 list 19 studies that have examined the extent to which reported experiences of discrimination can shape health care seeking and adherence behaviors. Some studies have focused on perceived discrimination in general while others have attended to perceptions of bias within the health care context. While perceptions of racism were unrelated to delayed seeking of treatment in a small study of African Americans with an acute myocardial infarction (Banks and Dracup 2006), it has been associated with delays or failure to seek treatment for less severe conditions in the U.S. (Facione and Facione 2007; Van Houtven et al. 2005; Wagner and Abbott 2007; Casagrande et al. 2007) and Sweden (Wamala et al. 2007a). However, the findings have not been uniform with discrimination unrelated to cancer screening (Dailey et al. 2007; Hoyo et al. 2005) and the use of preventive care (Fowler-Brown et al. 2006) in some studies. Perceived discrimination has also been associated with the failure to seek preventative services such as cholesterol testing, hemoglobin A1c testing and eye exams for diabetes and flu shots (Trivedi and Ayanian 2006) and the use of alternative care instead of conventional care (Bazargan et al. 2005). Several studies have also found an inverse association between discrimination and satisfaction with care among African Americans and Whites (Benkert et al. 2006; Fowler-Brown et al. 2006) and Korean Americans (Jang et al. 2005).

Earlier research had also indicated an association between discrimination and cigarette smoking and alcohol use. Recent studies reveal that perceived discrimination is associated with an increased risk of multiple substances, such as marijuana, inhalants and cocaine among middle school students (Choi et al. 2006). In the CARDIA study of young adults, discrimination was associated with marijuana, tobacco and alcohol use, but not cocaine use, among black but not white participants (Borrell et al. 2006). Similarly, both chronic and acute racial discrimination were associated with prescription drug use, illicit drug use and alcohol dependence among Filipino adults in San Francisco and Honolulu (Gee et al. 2007a). Other U.S. studies continue to find positive associations between discrimination and tobacco (Landrine et al. 2006; Krieger et al. 2005; Bennett et al. 2005) and alcohol use (Terrell et al. 2006). Studies from South Africa also find that perceived discrimination is positively associated with cigarette smoking (Brook et al. 2006b) and HIV risk behavior (Kalichman et al. 2006).

Another striking pattern in the current research is the broader range of contexts that have been considered. The earliest studies of discrimination and health disproportionately focused on the African American population. Recent studies have included all of the other racial/ethnic populations in the U.S. with several studies focusing on Asian American populations (Gee et al. 2006a, 2007a, b, c; Lam 2007; Jang et al. 2005). In recent years, studies have also utilized national samples in New Zealand (Harris et al. 2006a, b) and Sweden (Wamala et al. 2007a). Studies from Australia continue to examine the association between discrimination and Aboriginal health (Larson et al. 2007) and studies from the U.K. have examined discrimination and health among African-Caribbean, Bangladeshi, and White adults (Wadsworth et al. 2007), British Muslims (Sheridan 2006), minority ethnic teachers (Miller and Travers 2005); and multiple ethnic immigrant adults (Bhui et al. 2005; Karlsen et al. 2005). Two studies from South Africa have also examined discrimination in relation to adolescent risk behaviors (Brook et al. 2006b; Kalichman et al. 2006). Studies in Norway (Oppedal et al. 2005), Denmark (Montgomery and Foldspang 2007), the Netherlands (Veling et al. 2007; Stevens et al. 2005a), Spain (Pantzer et al. 2006), Bosnia, Croatia and Austria (Sujoldzic et al. 2006), Hong Kong (Lam et al. 2005) and Canada (Beiser and Hou 2006; Noh et al. 2007; Etowa et al. 2007) have examined the association between perceived discrimination and health for multiple immigrant groups.

It is also noteworthy that very few studies explicitly examine the role of discrimination in accounting for racial disparities in health. Some early studies provided evidence that discrimination makes an incremental contribution to SES in explaining disparities (Williams et al. 2003). A few recent studies find that perceived discrimination accounts for some of the racial disparities in health. This is evident for Maori-European disparities on four indicators of self-reported health in a national study of New Zealand (Harris et al. 2006a), Aboriginal–non Aboriginal variations in self-reported physical and mental health in Australia (Larson et al. 2007), and in U.S. studies for black-white differences in health care trust (Adegbembo et al. 2006), sleep quality and physical fatigue (Thomas et al. 2006) and Hispanic-white differences in PTSD symptoms (Pole et al. 2005).

There has also been concern regarding the extent to which subjective reports of discrimination are independent of other psychological characteristics. Three recent studies found that the association between discrimination and health remained robust after adjustment for social desirability bias (Gee et al. 2007b; Krieger et al. 2005; Pole et al. 2005). In addition, in a study of Latino and African American adults, Brondolo et al. (2005) found that the association between discrimination and negative emotions was independent of cynical hostility and positive and negative affect, while a study of multiethnic adults found that the relationship between perceived discrimination and mood was independent of trait anxiety, social desirability and cynical hostility (Broudy et al. 2007). A study of black, white and Bangladeshi adults in the UK found an association between discrimination and psychological distress after adjustment for negative affect (Wadsworth et al. 2007).

Research challenges

As evidence continues to mount suggesting that perceived discrimination is a risk factor for multiple health outcomes, there is increasing scientific interest in this area of research. For some, this has led to the routine, mechanical and a-theoretical addition of a discrimination scale to health studies without adequate thought regarding either the assessment of discrimination or the underlying mechanisms and processes by which discrimination would be presumed to affect health. We believe that the time has come for careful re-assessment of our current approaches to the study of discrimination and health with an eye toward investing in what is needed to improve our scientific understanding of this phenomenon and its health consequences. Perceived discrimination is a psychosocial stressor and there is much that can be learned from the larger literature on stress to advance the study of discrimination and health (Williams et al. 2003). Enhancing our understanding of discrimination and health will require more systematic attention to comprehensively and accurately measuring discrimination, assessing how it combines with other aspects of racism and other stressors to affect health and paying greater attention to the underlying pathways by which discrimination can affect health.

Measuring perceived discrimination comprehensively

There is no consensus on an optimal measure of perceived discrimination. A recent review evaluated 34 different measures of perceived discrimination (Kressin et al. 2008), but the current conceptualization and assessment of discrimination is limited. Recent studies of perceived discrimination and health tend to capture two domains of stressors, daily hassles and life events. The most widely used measure in recent studies reviewed is the Everyday Discrimination Scale (Williams et al. 1997). The scale has several attractive features, including its brevity, good psychometric properties (Krieger et al. 2005), and its use in multiple racial/ethnic populations in the U.S. and its increasing use in international contexts such as South Africa (Williams et al. 2008). The scale attempts to capture aspects of interpersonal discrimination that are chronic or episodic but generally minor, somewhat analogous to the assessment of daily hassles in the stress literature. A few studies use it in conjunction with other indicators of discrimination but it is often used as the only measure of discrimination. Another very useful approach to capturing ongoing discrimination is the dairy approach of Brondolo and her colleagues (Brondolo et al. 2005, 2008a, b; Broudy et al. 2007). However, there is a need for greater awareness that measures like the Everyday Discrimination Scale capture an important, but limited aspect of the phenomenon of self-reported discrimination.

Discriminatory experiences that are acute and observable and are analogous to life events in the stress literature are captured by many of the other commonly used measures of discrimination in recent studies. These include the Experiences of Discrimination scale (Krieger 1990), the Schedule of Racist Events (Landrine and Klonoff 1996), the Major Experiences of Discrimination scale (Williams et al. 1997; Kessler et al. 1999); the Racism and Life Experiences Scale (Harrell 1997), and the Index of Race-Related Stress (Utsey and Ponterotto 1996). Prior research has shown that failure to assess stress comprehensively leads to an underestimation of the effects of stress on health (Turner et al. 1995). The current assessment of discrimination tends to neglect the measurement of chronic stressors in major domains of life such as work. Chronic stressors refers to experiences that provide persistent negative exposure to threat or excessive demand (Baum et al. 1993). The stress literature indicates that although chronic stressors are difficult to measure, they are stronger predictors of the onset and course of the disease than acute life events (Cohen et al. 1995). It is noteworthy that capturing chronic exposure to discrimination over time, as assessed by the Everyday Discrimination Scale, was associated with subclinical cardiovascular disease (Lewis et al. 2006). The Perceived Racism Scale (McNeilly et al. 1996) captures some aspects of chronic discrimination but more effort is needed to comprehensively characterize chronic and ongoing experiences of discrimination, especially its multiple dimensions in the domain of work, a major site of discriminatory experiences in contemporary society.

Traumas are a class of stressful experiences that can have long-lasting negative effects on health (Stam 2007; Pearlin et al. 2005), that have been neglected in the discrimination literature. Traumas are distinct from life events in that they capture events that are extreme, overwhelming and horrific in impact. It is likely that traumatic experiences are reported on some of the life events-like measures of discrimination but inadequate detail is ascertained to distinguish them from less severe experiences. Carter (2007) has emphasized the importance of assessing race-based traumatic experiences as distinct from major experiences of discrimination. He argues that because there is likely to be intense emotional reaction to these severe, life threatening or dangerous experiences, they have great potential for psychological injury.

The stress literature has also recognized macro-stressors as one component of stressor exposure. These are large-scale system-related stressors such as economic recessions or natural disasters. They provide indirect exposure to stressors, that can have “collateral effects” on individuals (Yehuda et al. 2005). Research reveals that events such as earthquakes, terrorist attacks and the onset of war can trigger increases in acute symptoms of heart disease, increased hospital admissions and heart disease mortality (Bhattacharyya and Steptoe 2007). Highly publicized race-related traumatic events, such as extreme examples of police brutality could have similar effects. Some of the items on the Index of Race-Related Stress (such as references to racial abuses of the Jim Crow era) capture aspects of macro-stressors (Utsey and Ponterotto 1996) but comprehensively assessing the potential contribution of large-scale, race-related traumatic events will require researchers to capitalize on emergent opportunities to assess the health consequences of macro-stressors. Such efforts could utilize some of the strategies used in the larger stress literature such as creatively using hospital admissions and vital statistics data. Future research needs to identify the extent to which major race-related stressors can initiate new disease processes or exacerbate existing disease. There is no agreement in the stress literature on the length of time from the emotional trigger from a macro-stressor to the onset of symptoms. However, some research suggests that cardiac mortality tends to be elevated for at least 6 months post-bereavement (Bhattacharyya and Steptoe 2007).

Researchers should also be alert to the possibility that macro-stressors that are ostensibly unrelated to race or ethnicity, can be racialized in ways that can generate increased discrimination for socially stigmatized groups. The terrorist attacks on 11 September 2001 are an example. These incidents had documented negative health consequences (DiMaggio et al. 2007), but they also triggered increased discrimination and harassment targeted to Arab Americans in the 6 months after the terrorist attacks (Lauderdale 2006). Lauderdale (2006) found that Arab American women in California had an increase in the rate of low birth weight and pre-term birth in the 6 months after September 11 compared to the 6 months before. Other women in California did not experience a change in birth outcome risk post September 11.

Assessing exposure to racial discrimination in its full complexity also requires attention to capturing the effects of discrimination on others and the potential intergenerational effects of racism. Few measures of discrimination ask questions about experiences of discrimination that occurred in the lives of family members and close friends of the respondent (Kressin et al. 2008). In contrast, the stress literature has long noted that vicarious experiences of stress can also adversely affect the individual (Cohen et al. 1995). One recent study found that the father’s experience of workplace discrimination was positively associated with his depressive symptoms, as well as, those of his family members (Crouter et al. 2006). Recent research on the effects of historical trauma on the health of American Indians illustrates the importance of assessing this dimension of racism (Whitbeck et al. 2004; Brave Heart 2003; Brave Heart and DeBruyn 1998). The term historical trauma refers to the cumulative psychological wounding of an individual and his/her group due to the history of genocide and other atrocities that American Indians and other indigenous people experienced from European colonizers. The explicit attempt is to capture such exposure over the life-span of the individual and across generations. Research highlights the importance of assessing not only the actual experiences, but also the role that traumatic reminders of these experiences can play (Stam 2007). Assessment instruments with good psychometric properties have been developed to assess historical trauma and have found, for example, that some 50% of American Indians think regularly about these historical losses (Whitbeck et al. 2004). Empirical studies have also linked exposure to historical trauma to multiple health outcomes. This research is similar to studies of other generational group traumas, including studies of the health consequences of the Jewish Holocaust on survivors and their descendants. The assessment of these traumas in future research should be expanded to include other race-specific experiences such as the historic brutal lynchings of blacks and the internment and re-location of Japanese Americans during the Second World War.

Measuring discrimination accurately

Enhancing our understanding of discrimination and health will also require greater attention to identifying strategies to address the limitations of the currently used measures. Problems that have been identified with the reliability and validity of traditional life event scales also apply to most measures of discrimination (Monroe 2008). These problems include unreliability of recall, recall bias, criterion validity and construct validity have (Dohrenwend 2006). It is important to capture exposure to discrimination over the life course and the most common time frame for the assessment of discrimination is lifetime exposure (Kressin et al. 2008). This is appropriate given the goal of capturing cumulative exposure over the life course. However, problems linked to recall are more severe when the recall period of stressors is longer and when the data are gathered from retrospective reports. Of particular concern are some studies of PTSD that suggest that recall bias can lead to an overestimation of a dose–responsive relationship between exposure and outcome (Dohrenwend 2006). Current mood affects memory retrieval and both depressed and PTSD patients have impaired retrieval in terms of memory (Harvey and Bryant 2002). A study of motor vehicle accident victims who provided symptoms one month after the incident and were asked to recall those symptoms two years later found that the severity of the stress and injury and the trajectory of recovery can lead to over reporting or under-reporting of symptoms (Harvey and Bryant 2000). While the majority correctly recalled most of the symptom clusters, persons with few symptoms two years later tended to omit symptoms that they had reported in the acute phase of the disease, and persons with high levels of symptoms at the two year interview were more likely to recall the presence of acute symptoms that they had not reported in the initial assessment.

Research reveals that severe events are recalled better than less severe events (Monroe 2008), so to the extent that the focus is on the assessment of severe discriminatory incidents the recall problem may be somewhat minimized. More importantly, the assessment tools for the assessment of discrimination need to be appropriate to the task. More research is needed on how experiences of racial discrimination are stored in memory and best accessed for recall. In the meantime, research studying lifetime exposure to discrimination should consider using an event history calendar. It is a procedure that has been developed by cognitive psychologists that capitalizes on current scientific understanding of the nature of autobiographical memory to assess experiences over the life course (Belli 1998). More generally, research by cognitive psychologists and survey methodologists provide many insights into how respondents understand, interpret, and respond to questions that would enhance the collection of data on discrimination (Schwarz 2007). We consider a few examples.

The sensitive nature of the topic of race and racial discrimination has implications for how the discrimination variable should be assessed and the accuracy of reports of discrimination (Williams et al. 2003). Recent research on race-of-interviewer effects indicates that blacks are reluctant to reveal their true racial beliefs on race sensitive questions when talking to white interviewers. Instructively, Krysan and Couper (2003) found that the strongest effect of blacks being deferent to a white interviewer was for perceptions of racial discrimination (compared to attitudes on four other categories of race-related questions regarding racial and race-associated policies, black politics and the pace of civil rights). This pattern held both for in-person interviews and for self-administered interviews with a digital video of the interviewer. Irrespective of the race of the interviewer, making race salient in the assessment of discrimination can lead to response bias compared to the use of neutral terminology (Gomez and Trierweiler 2001). Many questions used to assess discrimination explicitly ask respondents to report on “racial discrimination” or experiences of discrimination “because of your race.” In contrast, the approach of Williams and his colleagues (Williams et al. 1997; Kessler et al. 1999) frames the questions about discrimination in terms of unfair treatment and asks about attribution only after a behaviorally descriptive experience, without emotionally charged language, has been endorsed. This approach seeks to address not only the problem of the sensitivity of questions regarding discrimination but also concerns about the problem of attributional ambiguity. Respondents are often uncertain of the reason (or attribution) for a specific interpersonal incident. Thus, building attribution into the question is likely to underestimate discriminatory encounters for which the attribution is uncertain (Williams et al. 2003). Asking questions about both racial and non-racial discrimination may capture more of the potential pathogenic phenomenon of perceived unfairness, and also reduce some of the measurement error that can occur if questions are asked only of racial discrimination. However, there is debate regarding optimal measurement approaches for perceived racial discrimination (Kressin et al. 2008), and limited empirical evidence on the effects, if any, that building discrimination and racial terminology into the assessment of perceived discrimination has on respondents’ willingness and ability to recall and report these experiences.

An unresolved issue in the literature is the extent to which unfair treatment based on race may have effects that are distinctive from other forms of unfair treatment. Because of the centrality of race in American society, the salience of racial identity can affect the appraisal processes of some individuals in ways that could lead race-attributed experiences to be more impactful because they are especially threatening to an individual’s sense of rights and opportunities (Pearlin et al. 2005). Some studies have found that perceptions of racial and non-racial discrimination are similarly related to health (Williams et al. 1999; Kessler et al. 1999). Recent neuroimaging research indicates that the experience of unfairness is associated with negative emotional responses and can activate regions of the brain involved with emotional regulation, suggesting that seeking justice and fairness may be a basic human impulse and its violation can trigger physiological consequences (Tabibnia et al. 2008). Accordingly, irrespective of attribution, the perception of unfair treatment may generate distress. For example, in the Whitehall study, perceived unfairness has been related to incident coronary events (De Vogli et al. 2007a), incident psychiatric morbidity (Ferrie et al. 2006), and metabolic syndrome (De Vogli et al. 2007b). These studies were not framed within the context of discrimination but unfairness is operationalized with measures similar to those used in the discrimination literature.

Lewis et al. (2006) recently reported that although Everyday Discrimination attributed to race was unrelated to coronary calcification for black women, a combined measure capturing racial and non-racial discrimination was positively associated with coronary calcification. On the other hand, one study found that black women who attributed chronic discrimination to race demonstrated greater blood pressure reactivity than those who attributed them to other social status categories (Guyll et al. 2001). Future research needs to more systematically assess the extent to which racial discrimination differs in its causes and consequences from other types of discrimination and the extent to which the approach to capturing attribution affects our understanding of the levels and health consequences of racial discrimination. Some limited evidence suggests that racial discrimination as a stressor may differ from other stressors, possibly because its threat may be more systematic, insidious and constant than other stressors (Stetler et al. 2006).

More generally, research on asking sensitive questions in survey research reveals that to the extent that respondents perceive questions regarding discrimination to be socially unacceptable or undesirable, they are likely to underreport those experiences (Tourangeau and Yan 2007). Research reveals that the mode of administration of sensitive questions makes a difference in terms of the response. In particular, the presence of others in the interview context adversely affects accurately collecting complete data. Thus, eliminating the presence of an interviewer by using a self-administered instrument and using “forgiving” or normalizing wording for sensitive questions can be helpful in addressing this issue (Tourangeau and Yan 2007).

Measuring discrimination accurately will also require researchers to not only capture the multiple domains of discrimination but to ensure that adequate questions are asked in each domain. In the assessment of multiple phenomena, survey methodologists find that multiple questions about components of a phenomenon will provide a more accurate level than a few global questions (Schaeffer and Presser 2003). Multiple questions are more likely to clearly convey what is being asked and to yield a more thorough search of memory. For example, researchers have found that population-based reports of alcohol consumption markedly under-report alcohol use compared to data on taxable alcohol available for consumption. However, assessing alcohol use with detailed, within-location, beverage-specific questions (asking respondents whether they had consumed specific alcoholic beverages, at any of several specific locations and then about how much and how often they drank in each location) accounted for 94% of the taxable alcohol available for consumption compared to the 40–60% with standard alcohol use questions (Casswell et al. 2002). For each class of discriminatory experiences, similar attention should be given to ensuring that all relevant contexts and components are assessed.

A challenge to getting a valid measure of discrimination via self-report is that some individuals cope with stress by denying its occurrence. Studies reveal that among patients with cancer, four to 47% deny the diagnosis, 8–70% deny the impact and 18–42% deny affect (Vos and de Haes 2007). Related constructs include avoidance, distancing, suppression and repressive coping. Denial can reflect conscious or unconscious efforts to minimize the pain of negative experiences. It has been argued that the emotional pain of racism can render some individuals unable to recall specific events (Carter 2007). Vos and de Haes (2007) suggest that denial should be viewed not as a one-time event, but as a process in which there is a continuum from facts to ambiguous events. Accordingly, denial can be a passive escape strategy linked to poorer psychological function, or it can be part of a series of active distractive strategies that are adaptive and lead to lower levels of psychological distress. Getting a clearer sense of the levels and consequences of denial in research on discrimination should be a priority for future research.

One of the most impressive patterns in the recent literature on racial discrimination is the broad range of national contexts in which it has been studied. Undoubtedly, there are some commonalities to the manifestation of discrimination. However, as with other stressors, culture, history and social context can determine the kinds and categories of stressful experiences. As studies of discrimination continue to proliferate across racial, ethnic, cultural, national, and socioeconomic contexts, researchers should ensure that the assessments of discriminatory experiences are appropriate to the specific population group under study. National data for South Africa reveals that levels of chronic Everyday Discrimination and major acute experiences of discrimination are markedly lower than in the U.S. (Williams et al. 2008). It is currently unclear whether this variation reflects national or cultural differences in the levels of discrimination, the discourse about race, the willingness to report experiences of discrimination, the level of social interaction between dominant and non-dominant racial groups, or the applicability of measures of discrimination developed in the U.S. to a different context. Measuring discrimination comprehensively will require more explicit attention to assessing the relevant forms of its manifestation in specific contexts.

Assessing the stressful dimensions of discrimination

An important lesson from the stress literature for researchers studying discrimination is that exposure to most stressful experiences does not lead to illness. The overwhelming majority of persons exposed to even the most severe traumatic life experiences have transient symptoms in response to these problems (Yehuda et al. 2005; Baum et al. 1993; Cohen et al. 2007). While there are emotional reactions and symptoms in response to severe stressors, most of them are resolved in the ensuing weeks or months. For example, after a trauma most people recover from symptoms in a year and only 5–10% of those exposed to traumatic experiences go on to develop PTSD (Bryant 2003; Carter 2007). However, the relationship between stress and health status may vary by health outcome with an estimated 20–25% of people who experience major stressful life events developing major depression (Cohen et al. 2007).

The challenge then is to identify and assess those dimensions of discriminatory experiences that are likely to have long-term negative effects. The stress literature suggests that several characteristics of stressors are important determinants of the long-term negative impact of stress. Stressors that are ambiguous, negative, unpredictable, and uncontrollable are particularly pathogenic (Carter 2007; Dougall et al. 1999). The intensity of the stressor also matters including how disturbing and upsetting it is (Dougall et al. 1999). The frequency and duration of stressors are also key determinants of their impact with chronic or repeated stressors or prolonged exposure being predictive of adverse effects. Such stressors or difficulties are likely to make continuing demands or pose ongoing threats (Dohrenwend 2006). The impact that the stressor has on the environment and social functioning of the individual is also important. Stressors that occur in central role areas of an individual’s life, affect multiple areas, lead to loss of resources, have objective negative impact on the normal activities of the individual, and cause a lot of change, disruption or readjustment are likely to be very consequential (Dohrenwend 2006; Carter 2007). The presence of symptoms such as post traumatic or peritraumatic disassociation and of panic reactions appear to predict long term negative effects of exposure to traumas (Yehuda et al. 2005).

An important priority in the future assessment of perceived discrimination is to assess markers of the stressfulness of experiences. A central problem in the assessment of stress is what Dohrenwend (2006) calls “intracategory variability.” That is, the categories on life events scales are typically so broad (death of a loved, unemployment, or serious illness or injury) that they capture experiences that can vary in their stressfulness and impact. Knowing that such an event occurs provides no information on how negative, unexpected, and undesirable, the event was. The same is true for measures of discrimination in employment, housing or in interaction with the police. Researchers studying discrimination should devote more attention to assessing the severity of discriminatory incidents by capturing the number, intensity, and duration of these experiences (Carter 2007).

The stress literature offers three options for assessing severity. First, the narrative-rating methodology is a labor intensive interview that gathers details about each potentially stressful experience so that trained researchers can then rate them on multiple dimensions of stressfulness (Dohrenwend 2006). This approach has been shown to be superior to standard checklists of stressful events but because interviewing and rating can take as much as 16 hours per individual interview (Dohrenwend 2006), it is too time consuming and expensive to be a practical option for most researchers. A second approach is to clearly define what kinds of experiences should be included in a category and/or spell out the inclusion or exclusion criteria for the target stressful experience (Dohrenwend 2006). For example, in the Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire (Kubany et al. 2000), instead of asking the respondent if s/he had been in a motor vehicle accident, the respondent is asked “were you involved in a motor vehicle accident for which you received medical attention or that badly injured or killed someone.” Similarly, the question assessing the presence of childhood physical abuse is “were you physically punished in a way that resulted in bruises, burns, cuts or broken bones?” (Kubany et al. 2000). This strategy could be applied to questions about racial discrimination in which behaviorally descriptive language that avoided global and emotionally charged words could clearly specify the experiences that should be included. This approach can improve assessment but has the downside of potentially missing other important stressors that do not fall within the specified criteria (Dohrenwend 2006).

A third strategy involves subjective ratings by the respondent of the severity and negative impact of the stressful experience (Cohen et al. 2007). In the PTSD literature, respondents are often asked after reporting a particular event to indicate how much they had been affected by it and how upset they had been by it (Carter 2007). Similarly, a study relating caregiver stress to mortality had respondents indicate “how much of a mental or emotional strain” each reported caregiving task had been (Schulz and Beach 1999). A recent measure of hurricane-related stressors required respondents to provide a global rating of the 29 stressors ascertained by indicating “how stressful overall” the experiences had been on a 0–10 scale, “where 0 means not at all stressful and 10 means the most stressful thing you can imagine” (Kessler et al. 2008). The researchers then compared respondents rating their stress as severe (9–10), serious (7–8), moderate (5–6) or mild (3–4) to those who did not rate it as stressful (0–2). Such approaches lead to stronger associations between life events and mental health in the general stress literature (Dohrenwend 2006), but this approach has been used in only a few studies in research on discrimination and health.

Analysis of data from the Metro Atlanta Heart Disease Study illustrates the promise of this approach. This study found that although perceived discrimination was unrelated to hypertension, both discrimination at work (Din-Dzietharn et al. 2004) and discrimination more generally (Davis et al. 2005) were associated with increased risk of hypertension, but only among persons reporting high levels of stress because of discrimination. In this study, if respondents reported experiences of discrimination they were asked to rank their general level of stress when they had those experiences from 1 = none to 5 = high. Several states participating in the annual Behavioral Risk Factor Surveys in the U.S. have included the Reactions to Race module, developed by Camara Jones, which includes questions that assess emotional and physical symptoms as a result of exposure to discriminatory experiences. This is a fertile opportunity to explore the contribution of ratings of severity to the association between discrimination and health. One limitation of obtaining appraisals of stressfulness is that for studies of mental health, respondents’ reports of stressfulness can lead to confounding between the measure of stress and the self-reported outcome (Dohrenwend 2006).

Other evidence suggests that two aspects of a discriminatory experience that can affect its stressfulness are the degree of ambiguity of the situation and the identity of the perpetrator. Limited evidence from the discrimination literature indicates that characteristics of the perpetrator can predict the degree of adverse impact with the effects being more negative when the perpetrator belongs to the same racial group as the target (Mays et al. 2007). The ambiguity surrounding an experience of discrimination can also be a determinant of its stressfulness, and greater attention should be given to assessing attributional ambiguity (Carter 2007). Several recent studies have noted stronger, more negative effects from subtle or ambiguous racial encounters than from blatant ones (Bennett et al. 2005; Stetler et al. 2006; Merritt et al. 2006). There may also be racial differences in the effects of ambiguity. A recent study of university students found that blacks experienced greater impairment in cognitive functioning when faced with ambiguous evidence of prejudice than when exposed to blatant prejudice (Salvatore and Shelton 2007). The opposite pattern was evident for whites. It may be that the socialization of blacks may enable them to cope better with blatant than subtle prejudice while the socialization of whites may lead them to fail to perceive subtle prejudice. Routinely assessing the extent to which racial attribution is uncertain appears to be an important priority in the future measurement of discrimination.

Measuring vigilance and anticipatory stress

Vigilance regarding the threat of discrimination and the anticipation of future occurrences of discrimination could be as predictive of the adverse health impact as the actual effects of past discriminatory experiences. The negative physiological effects of exposure to stressors are often triggered by the initial perception of threat which can occur long before the actual exposure to the stressor. Several researchers have emphasized that anticipatory coping and anxiety, heightened vigilance, and intrusive thoughts or images can play an important role in determining the negative effects of stressors on health (Yehuda et al. 2005; Pearlin et al. 2005; Carter 2007; Baum et al. 1993; Dougall et al. 1999). This may be especially important for the stress linked to racial discrimination because one’s racial status is a characteristic that a respondent can do little about. Carter (2007) uses the term “cultural paranoia” to refer to a high level of vigilance that many minority group members maintain.

Brosschot and colleagues (2006) use the term “perseverative cognition” to capture worry, rumination and anticipatory stress. They review evidence from multiple sources that suggests that repeated or chronic activation of the cognitive imagery of one or more psychosocial stressors can serve to prolong the stress and exacerbate the negative effects of stress on health. These chronic cognitive processes can lead to “prolonged physiological activation.” The resulting anticipatory stress, reflected in chronic or sustained vigilance can lead to dysregulation of both emotional and physiological functioning. Considerable evidence suggests that prolonged physiological activation is a risk factor for multiple disease conditions (Brosschot et al. 2006). Thus, capturing the level of anticipatory stress, worry, rumination, regarding the threat of discrimination is an important priority for future research. Williams (1997) has suggested that the finding of elevated nocturnal blood pressure levels of African Americans during sleep could reflect a heightened vigilance and a failure to ever completely relax because of the constant threat of discrimination and other dangers linked to residence in hostile residential contexts. Recent research has shown that discrimination contributes to African Americans’ failure to display the expected nocturnal decline in blood pressure (Brondolo et al. 2008a). Despite its importance, issues of vigilance have seldom been addressed in the research on discrimination. Lindström (2008) recently reported that a single-item indicator of anticipatory ethnic discrimination was associated with lower levels of psychological health in a national sample of adults in Sweden. In a study of African American youth, Clark et al. (2006) found that a measure of racism-related vigilance was inversely related to large arterial elasticity (a preclinical index of cardiovascular function) for boys but not girls. The mean age of this sample was 12 years old suggesting that the processes may begin early in life and future research should attend to the potential gendered nature of these responses.

Perceived discrimination and the multiple mechanisms of racism

Perceived discrimination is a historically neglected race-related aspect of life that may adversely affect health. However, it does not comprehensively capture the effects of racism in society. Some researchers erroneously assume that a scale that assesses racial discrimination captures a respondent’s exposure to racism. Perceived discrimination, though important, is only one component of racism. We briefly consider some of the other pathways by which racism can affect health. Fully capturing the role of racism in health will require an understanding of how discrimination combines with other aspects of racism to increase the risk of disease.

Residential segregation

Arguably, the most decisive way in which racism can affect health is through institutional mechanisms of racism. The aspect of institutional discrimination most widely studied for its health implications has been residential segregation (Williams and Collins 2001; Acevedo-Garcia et al. 2003). This body of research indicates that residential segregation can shape SES and thus health by restricting access to education and employment opportunities, discounting the economic value of a given level of SES, and creating health-damaging conditions in residential environments. Historically, two pronounced patterns of residential segregation in the U.S. have been the geographic isolation of American Indians on reservations and the residential concentration of African Americans in poor urban areas. There is growing concern about the health consequences of the persisting segregation of blacks and American Indians and the increasing segregation of Latinos (Acevedo-Garcia et al. 2008). Although the majority of poor persons in the U.S. are white, poor white families are not concentrated in the ways that poor blacks and Latinos are. The critical determinant of the problems linked to segregation is not racial composition per se but the concentration of economic and social disadvantage and the absence of an infrastructure that promotes opportunity.

Research has linked residential segregation to an elevated risk of illness and death (Williams and Collins 2001; Acevedo-Garcia et al. 2003). There are multiple pathways through which the concentrated poverty created by segregation can adversely affect health (Williams and Collins 2001; Schulz et al. 2002). First, the conditions created by poverty and segregation make it more difficult for residents to adhere to good health practices. The higher cost, poorer quality, and lower availability of healthy foods in economically disadvantaged neighborhoods can lead to poor nutrition. The heavy targeting of disadvantaged minority communities with advertising for tobacco and alcohol can encourage the use of these products. The lack of recreation facilities and concerns about personal safety can discourage leisure time physical exercise. Second, the concentration of poverty can lead to exposure to elevated levels of economic hardship and other chronic and acute stressors at the individual, household and neighborhood level. Third, the weakened community and neighborhood infrastructure in segregated areas can also adversely affect interpersonal relationships and trust among neighbors. Fourth, the institutional neglect and disinvestment in poor, segregated communities contributes to increased exposure to environmental toxins, poor quality housing and criminal victimization. Thus, perceived discrimination must be understood and assessed within the larger context of institutional racism which has created differential exposure to a broad range of stressors.

Differential access to societal goods and resources

Institutional and individual discrimination can also reduce non-dominant groups’ access to a broad range of desirable goods and services. Medical care is one example. Discrimination can affect both access to care and the quality and intensity of medical treatment. U.S. research reveals that residential segregation can affect access to medical care by determining both the particular institutions where minorities access care and the type and quality of their health care providers. In segregated minority communities, health care facilities are more likely to close, pharmacies are less likely to be adequately stocked with medication, and residents are more likely to be treated by lower-quality physicians who are less able to refer to specialty care (Williams and Jackson 2005). In addition, there are large racial/ethnic differences in the quality and intensity of medical care with blacks and other minorities receiving fewer medical procedures and poorer quality medical care than whites (Smedley et al. 2003). This pattern persists even when differences in health insurance, SES, stage and severity of disease, co-morbidity, and the type of medical facility are taken into account. Cultural racism has led to pervasive negative racial stereotypes of racial groups regarded as inferior (Williams 2004). Some evidence suggests that unconscious discrimination based on these negative stereotypes of minorities is a likely determinant of this pervasive bias in the delivery of care (van Ryn 2002; Green et al. 2007).

Discrimination and other stressors

As noted, the experience of self-reported discrimination is not a magic bullet that captures all of the psychosocial stressors necessary to estimate the contribution of racism-generated stressors to disparities in health. Perceived discrimination is only one way by which racism generates stress. Institutional discrimination can generate multiple stressors that can adversely affect health. For example, institutional discrimination in employment and residential contexts can trigger elevated exposure to traditional stressors, especially those linked to social and economic deprivation, including unemployment and underemployment. A large federal survey found that even after adjustment for a broad range of demographic, SES, and health status factors blacks were more likely than whites to report six economic hardships (unable to meet essential expenses, pay full rent or mortgage, pay full utility bill, or had utilities or telephone shut off or was evicted from one’s apartment; Bauman 1998). Similarly, because of the distinctive residential environments created by segregation, racial minorities are also exposed to elevated levels of neighborhood stressors and violence. Taking institutional discrimination seriously will require renewed focus on racial differences in traditional stressors: violence, criminal victimization, neighborhood conditions, financial stress, and relationship stress. Relatedly, all racism-related stressors must be situated within the context of the total stress burden of respondents’ lives with the recognition that racism (interpersonal and institutional) is only one source of stress. As a practical matter, researchers typically lack adequate information to determine the extent to which other stressors are race-related or not but must seek to characterize all stressors. Thus, understanding the potential contribution of stressful life experiences to racial disparities in health necessitates the assessment of perceived discrimination and a systematic effort to assess race-related and other social, psychological and environmental (physical and chemical) stressors that respondents face.

Pearlin et al. (2005) emphasized that the stressors that are patterned by social disadvantage are the “serious stressors” that capture major hardships, conflicts, and disruptions in life. These include disorderly transitional events which are role changes that are non-normative, undesired, involuntary and sometimes irreversible, such as teenage parenthood or school dropout. These stressors are concentrated among low SES groups and in addition to their negative emotional impact, they are critical in transmitting social disadvantage from one generation to the next because they tend to place low SES persons on trajectories of low education, low job prospects, and low income that lead to the proliferation of other stressors. Financial stressors, especially those that are characterized by continuity and repetitiveness over the life course are among the most powerful of stressful life experiences (Pearlin et al. 2005). Other sources of social and economic adversity, especially those that are chronic and recurring and that occur in major social domains such as the role of breadwinning, work, and family should also be assessed.

Discrimination should be assessed within this larger social context of the multiple stressful exposures within which it is embedded. Research is needed to elucidate, in careful longitudinal analyses, how perceived discrimination relates to other types of stressors and combines with them to produce patterns of cumulative exposure to multiple adversities that can adversely affect health status. The processes may be complex. Experiences of discrimination such as the loss of one’s job can easily lead to “stress proliferation processes” in which the primary stressor of unemployment can give rise to multiple other types of secondary stressors, such as financial strain and family conflict (Pearlin et al. 2005). Traumas are among the most potent types of stressful experiences that can trigger stress proliferation with the secondary stressors that are triggered by traumas contributing more to later health than the primary trauma itself (Pearlin et al. 2005).

Understanding how discrimination relates to other stressors will require examining the ways in which multiple forms of psychosocial stress accumulate to adversely affect health. The stress literature suggests that one’s ability to manage a new stressor is reduced by the burden and demands of preexisting stressors (Cohen et al. 2007). Moreover, stressors may combine not only in additive ways but also in interactive ways. Future research on perceived discrimination should pay special attention to potential interactions among stressors. This includes attention to environmental stressors linked to the physical, chemical, and built environment. Some limited evidence indicates that psychosocial stressors and environmental factors may interact with each other. One study found that high levels of air pollution were associated with distress among people with recent life events, but unrelated to distress among persons with no social stressors (Evans et al. 1987). Another study of an Indian sample, and two U.S. college student samples, found that social stressors adversely affected mental health only among persons living under the environmental condition of crowding (Lepore et al. 1991).

Future research needs to sensitively and thoroughly categorize the stressors in the social environments within which vulnerable populations live and work. These residential and occupational environments can be distinctive in the types and quantity of stressors to which individuals are exposed. Efforts are needed to catalogue and quantify exposures linked to the physical, chemical and built environment and assess how they combine with psychosocial stressors (including discrimination) and accumulate over the life course. Special attention should be given to the potential of differential effects of chemical exposures. Prior research indicates for example, that smoking has more negative effects on some vulnerable minority populations than on whites. Research also needs to explore biological profile differences across groups with the recognition that not all biological profile differences are driven by underlying genetics, but some could reflect complex interactions between exposure to disadvantaged environments and biology.

Internalized racism

Internalized racism or self-stereotyping is another mechanism by which the larger negative stereotypes about race can adversely affect health. One response of stigmatized racial populations to the categorical societal beliefs about their biological and/or cultural inferiority is to accept as true the dominant society’s ideology about them (Pettigrew 1964). The internalization of negative cultural images by stigmatized groups appears to create expectations, anxieties and reactions that can adversely affect social and psychological functioning. Fischer et al. (1996) show that across multiple national contexts, such as Japan, India, the United Kingdom and Israel, groups that are socially regarded as inferior have poorer academic performance than their more highly regarded peers. U.S. research indicates that when a stigma of inferiority is activated under experimental conditions, performance on an examination was adversely affected (Steele 1997). African Americans who were told in advance that blacks perform more poorly on exams than whites, women who were told that they perform more poorly than men, and white men who were told that they usually do worse than Asians, all had lower scores on an examination than control groups who were not confronted with a stigma of inferiority (Steele 1997; Fischer et al. 1996).

A study of the elderly highlights the potential health consequences of positive and negative self-stereotyping for stigmatized groups (Levy et al. 2002). It found that positive self-perceptions of aging, assessed at baseline, were associated with a 7.5 year longer life expectancy in a 23 year follow-up. However, our current understanding is limited regarding the ways in which the acceptance of negative racial stereotypes can adversely affect health. Some evidence indicates that in addition to adversely affecting academic performance, the activation of the stigma of inferiority also leads to increases in blood pressure (Blascovitch et al. 2001). Similarly, Taylor and colleagues have found a positive association between internalized racism and alcohol consumption and psychological distress among African Americans (Taylor et al. 1991; Taylor and Jackson 1990, 1991). Other recent research indicates that high levels of internalized racism are associated with increased risk of overweight or abdominal obesity among black women in the Caribbean (Tull et al. 1999; Chambers et al. 2004; Butler et al. 2002), and with waist circumferences, diastolic blood pressure and fasting glucose among black women but not men in Africa (Tull et al. 2007). Racial stereotypes are only one source of self-stereotyping. Their influence needs to be combined with other sources of stigmatization, especially those that are more likely to be prevalent among socially disadvantaged populations. For example, there are elevated rates of obesity in many racial minority populations and evidence of both a stigma linked to obesity and discrimination targeted at overweight individuals (Carr and Friedman 2005). Future research needs to explore the extent to which elevated health risks are located at multiple intersections of stigmatization and discrimination. Understanding how experiences of racial discrimination relate to internalized racism and combine to affect health is also important (Carter 2007).

Understanding how discrimination might affect health

Much current research on discrimination and health gives scant attention to the mechanisms and processes by which this association might exist. The stress literature indicates that stress affects health through three principal pathways (Cohen et al. 1995). First, exposure to stress can give rise to negative emotional states. These responses can generate psychological distress that adversely affects health. Second, behavioral coping responses to manage stress can lead to the initiation of unhealthy behaviors such as tobacco use and alcohol abuse, the disengagement from health activities such as sleep and exercise and the neglect of adherence to medical regimens. Third, both the psychological and behavioral responses to acute and chronic stressors can lead to structural and functional changes in multiple physiological systems, including the neuroendocrine, autonomic, and immune systems. These changes in physiology and behavior can lead to changes in health. Importantly, stressors, including discrimination can play a role in the onset, progression, and severity of illness. Studies of discrimination and health need to devote greater thought to the conditions under which particular aspects of discrimination are more or less likely to affect specific points of the disease continuum.

Too often there is little consideration to identifying or thinking about the exposure and lag times that would be necessary for a relationship to exist between exposure to discrimination and subsequent illness. Most chronic illnesses such as cancer, CHD, diabetes, and hypertension develop over many years and are often diagnosed long after the disease process was initiated. Accordingly, recent exposure to stressors, including discrimination, should not be related to the onset of these conditions. At the same time, because stress can impact the course of disease, it is important to consider how stress might affect the trajectory of an illness. This is especially important given the large racial and SES disparities in the progression of disease. As noted earlier, some recent studies have begun to pay attention to the role of discrimination in the management of chronic illness, healthcare seeking behaviors and in adherence to medical treatment. This is a very promising area in the study of discrimination and chronic disease and is consistent with the literature on stress that indicates that one of the important ways in which stress affects health is by shaping behavioral management of disease and leading to particular health behaviors that can have negative health consequences (Cohen et al. 2007). In future research, increased attention should be given to outcomes that capture the management, course, progression, severity, and recurrence of illness.

The stress literature also suggests that the types of stressors and the aspects of stress that may be important in determining the risk of getting an illness may be different from those aspects that are determinants of the progression, course, and severity of the illness. That is, the factors that are linked to the onset of disease may be different from those that overwhelm the body’s defenses in coping with illness. The distinction between chronic and acute discrimination may be important for the underlying pathways by which discrimination-induced stress can lead to disease. As with the general literature on stress, acute experiences of discrimination may trigger acute episodes of illness while chronic discrimination may exacerbate existing disease processes (Bhattacharyya and Steptoe 2007). In the absence of coronary disease, for example, severe emotional distress from acute stress can trigger acute cardiovascular responses and events that can lead to cardiovascular dysfunction. On the other hand, chronic stressors can accelerate the atherosclerotic processes and lead to more rapid progression of disease (Brotman et al. 2007). Similarly, mental health research reveals that major stressful life events (that require a lot of change in usual activities) lead to the onset of psychopathology, while minor stressful experiences play a role in the recurrence and severity of psychopathology (Dohrenwend 2006). Future research on discrimination and health needs to pay greater attention to matching the measure of discriminatory experience with the presumed disease process.

Research is also needed to elucidate the linkages between experiences of discrimination and processes of illness. Ackerman et al. (2002) examined the role of life events in exacerbating episodes of multiple sclerosis (MS). They followed 23 women with MS for a year and conducted a weekly assessment of stress. They found that life events served to rapidly precipitate the relapse and the progression of disease, occurring an average of 14 days before the exacerbation of MS. The study also found that a broad range of stressors were equally associated with the processes of exacerbating the disease. This type of intensive study can serve as a useful model for future research that can shed light on the pathways from discrimination to health.

Future research on discrimination and health could also profitably focus its attention on those outcomes where prior research has documented that stress in general is linked to health. It is not reasonable to expect to find effects of discrimination on health in areas where the stress literature has not shown an effect of stress. Stam (2007) has identified five physiological categories where severe stress has been shown to affect intermediate physiological symptoms. These are: the neuroendocrine system, the cardiovascular system (increased heart rate and blood pressure responses), the gastrointestinal system (gastrointestinal ulcers, irritable bowel syndrome), pain sensitivity and chronic pain and immune function (suppressed immunity). Brotman et al. (2007) also indicate that components of the metabolic syndrome are promising places to look for the effects of stress on cardiovascular disease.

Personal and social factors and discrimination

Research on discrimination and health should also attend to the personal & situational factors that might affect the underlying processes. Recent studies suggest that social factors can affect the prevalence and the impact of the discrimination. Borrell et al. (2006) found a positive association between perceived discrimination and SES. However, the larger literature is mixed on this topic (Paradies 2006). Pearlin et al. (2005) emphasize that the consequences of a stressor may vary by SES even if the prevalence of the stressor does not. Inadequate attention has been given to examining interactions between discrimination and SES. The expected patterns are unclear. Discrimination could be more severe for low SES groups because it may be harsher, more easily legitimized and invisible, or it could be more impactful for high SES groups because it could be perceived as a threat to their status (Jackson et al. 2006). Some limited evidence is consistent with higher costs of discrimination among high SES blacks (Forman 2003), but this finding deserves replication.

Gender is another social variable that might affect the relationship between discrimination and health. Consistent with the view that black men are more likely to be targets of discrimination than black women because of gendered stereotypes that view them as more threatening, men tend to report higher levels of discrimination than women (Carter 2007). Gender differences in the level and consequences of a stressor can be evident in opposite directions. For example, men report higher levels of exposure to traumas but PTSD is more prevalent in women (Stam 2007). One recent study found that gender moderated the association between discrimination and anxiety but not depression (Banks and Dracup 2006) while another found that the association between discrimination and physical and mental health was stronger for women than for men (Borrell et al. 2006). It is also important to explore the intersections of race, class and gender. One study found that race-related stress had larger negative effects on middle class than working class men, suggesting that the additional resources of the middle class did not protect them from the negative effects of stressors (Pieterse and Carter 2007).

The shade of skin color (or skin tone) is another marker of social status and a potential predictor of exposure to discrimination that has received inadequate research scrutiny (Krieger 1999). Research has long indicated that skin tone is a marker of social status and an important predictor of access to socioeconomic opportunities and resources within the black population with the associations being stronger for women than for men (Keith and Herring 1991). Prior research has shown that skin tone is also a marker for discrimination, with darker skinned blacks reporting higher levels of discrimination than their lighter skinned peers (Keith and Herring 1991; Carter 2007). Similar patterns have been observed among Hispanics (Arce et al. 1987), and Jews (Kraus and Koresh 1992). It is not clear though, if this association exists in all contexts. For example, in the CARDIA study of African American young adults, skin color was unrelated to reports of discrimination (Borrell et al. 2007). Insufficient research attention has been given to the relationship of skin color to discrimination in recent research that would allow conclusions regarding either secular change in the association over time, or the identification of the conditions under which particular patterns are more or less likely to be evident. Some evidence indicates that skin tone continues to matter for the earnings of African Americans (Goldsmith et al. 2007), but it is unclear if there have been secular changes in its role as a marker for discrimination. Future research needs to examine the extent to which multiple social statuses combine to affect both the levels of exposure to discrimination and the effects that discrimination can have on health.

There is a broad range of personal and situational factors that are important in determining vulnerability and resilience to stressors. A full discussion of these is beyond the scope of this paper (see paper by Brondolo, this issue). However, understanding the complex ways in which psychosocial stressors get into the body to affect health will require future research to attend to and understand the large individual variation that exists in response to stressor exposure. There is broad recognition in the stress literature that the effects of the experience of a particular stressor combines with factors such as the presence of prior childhood and adult traumatic experiences, levels of social support, feelings of helplessness and perceived control, a family history of psychiatric disorders, psychological and behavioral coping responses, genetic vulnerability, and a range of other personal factors (Yehuda et al. 2005; Stam 2007; Baum et al. 1993; Carter 2007). These factors can directly mediate the negative effects of stressor exposure or interact with stress to buffer or exacerbate its effects on health. Research in the area of perceived discrimination and health needs to assess the role of these vulnerability and resilience factors.

Conclusions

The research on discrimination and health is continuing to grow rapidly. Although the discrimination variable has been operationalized in a variety of ways, the consistency of an inverse association between discrimination for an increasingly broad range of health outcomes, across multiple population groups in a wide range of cultural and national contexts is impressive, and lends credibility to the plausibility of perceived discrimination as an important emerging risk factor for disease. This paper has described many of the limitations of this work and outlined an ambitious research agenda so that the next generation of studies could provide more definitive evidence on the conditions under which exposure to discrimination can lead to changes in health status. It would also enhance our scientific understanding of the underlying mechanisms and processes that may be at work. Our review finds that failure to comprehensively and accurately characterize perceived discrimination and appropriately assess its association with health will lead to erroneous conclusions about the underlying relationship.

The research reviewed also raises larger questions for future research and policy regarding health and its determinants. The accumulating evidence emphasizes that health policy, across multiple societies needs to encompass the legacies of racial and ethnic inequality and the levels of intolerance, incivility and anti-immigrant sentiment. These factors appear to matter a lot for the health of targeted groups and as such require greater attention as a focus of health policy. Racism and ethnocentrism may not be inevitable. In the U.S., there have been marked declines in racial prejudice and discrimination in the last several decades that have resulted from a broad range of interventions (Schuman et al. 1997). However, there is still considerable opportunity to improve relations among population groups defined by race, ethnicity, migration history and other stigmatized social statuses. There is also the need to more seriously begin to provide targeted relief to individuals suffering from exposure to discrimination. One recent study provided the sobering finding that a written disclosure intervention that has been shown to be successful in improving health outcomes such as immune functioning among persons exposed to various stressors was not effective when applied to persons dealing with racial discrimination (Stetler et al. 2006). This raises the possibility that conventional stress management strategies may not be effective in relieving the personal suffering linked to racial discrimination.

As a strategy for health improvement, more systematic research attention needs to be given to expanding our knowledge of the individual and especially organizational interventions that can be effective in reducing the levels, determinants and consequences of interpersonal and institutional discrimination (Paradies 2005). Moreover, there is substantial progress yet to be made in dismantling the institutional structures, processes, and policies that undergird societal racism. As research continues to accumulate evidence that perceived discrimination can lead to adverse changes in health, such efforts are an increasingly necessary component of comprehensive approaches to improving the health of all in racialized societies.

Acknowledgments

Preparation of this paper was supported in part by Grant R01 MH 59575 from the National Institute of Mental Health (D.R.W), Grant 3 U-01 HL 087322-02S1, a Research Supplement to U-01 HL 87322-02 from the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (S.A.M.) and by the John D. and Catherine T. McArthur Foundation Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health (D.R.W). We wish to thank Steven Beeber and Gabriel Sirota for assistance with preparing the manuscript and Manuela Costa for assistance with the research.

References

  1. Acevedo-Garcia D, Lochner KA, Osypuk TL, Subramanian SV. Future directions in residential segregation and health research: A multilevel approach. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(2):215–221. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Acevedo-Garcia D, Osypuk TL, McArdle N, Williams DR. Toward a policy-relevant analysis of geographic and racial/ethnic disparities in child health. Health Affairs. 2008;27(2):321–333. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.27.2.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ackerman KD, Heyman R, Rabin BS, Anderson BP, Houck PR, Frank E, et al. Stressful life events precede exacerbations of multiple sclerosis. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2002;64:916–920. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000038941.33335.40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Adegbembo AO, Tomar SL, Logan HL. Perception of racism explains the difference between Blacks’ and Whites’ level of healthcare trust. Ethnicity & Disease. 2006;16(4):792–798. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Ahmed A, Mohammed S, Williams DR. Racial discrimination & health: Pathways & evidence. The Indian Journal of Medical Research. 2007;126:21–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Arce CH, Murguia E, Frisbie WP. Phenotype and life chances among Chicanos. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences. 1987;9:19–32. [Google Scholar]
  7. Banks AD, Dracup K. Factors associated with prolonged prehospital delay of African Americans with acute myocardial infarction. American Journal of Critical Care. 2006;15(2):149–157. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Banks KH, Kohn-Wood LP, Spencer M. An examination of the African American experience of everyday discrimination and symptoms of psychological distress. Community Mental Health. 2006;42(6):555–570. doi: 10.1007/s10597-006-9052-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Baum A, Cohen L, Hall M. Control and intrusive memories as possible determinants of chronic stress. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1993;55:274–286. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199305000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bauman K. Direct measures of poverty as indicators of economic need: Evidence from the survey of income and program participation. U.S. Census Bureau Population Division Technical Working Paper No. 30 1998 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bazargan M, Norris K, Bazaragan-Hejazi S, Akhanjee L, Calderon JL, Safvati SD, et al. Alternative healthcare use in the under-served population. Ethnicity & Disease. 2005;15:531–539. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beiser MN, Hou F. Ethnic identity, resettlement stress and depressive affect among Southeast Asian refugees in Canada. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;63(1):137–150. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Belli RF. The structure of autobiographical memory and the event hisory calendar: Potential improvements in the quality of retrospective reports in surveys. Memory (Hove, England) 1998;6(4):383–406. doi: 10.1080/741942610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Benkert R, Peters RM, Clark R, Keves-Foster K. Effects of perceived racism, cultural mistrust and trust in providers on satisfaction with care. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2006;98(9):1532–1540. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bennett GG, Wolin KY, Robinson EL, Fowler S, Edwards CL. Perceived racial/ethnic harassment and tobacco use among African American young adults. American Journal of Public Health. 2005;95(2):238–240. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.037812. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bhattacharyya MR, Steptoe A. Emotional triggers of acute coronary syndromes: Strength of evidence, biological processes, and clinical implications. Progress in Cardiovascular Diseases. 2007;49(5):353–365. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2006.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bhui K, Stansfeld S, McKenzie K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J, Weich S. Racial/ethnic discrimination and common mental disorders among workers: Findings from the EMPIRIC Study of Ethnic Minority Groups in the United Kingdom. American Journal of Public Health. 2005;95(3):496–501. doi: 10.2105/AJPH. 2003.033274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Birman D, Tricket E, Buchanan BM. A tale of two cities: Replication of a study on the acculturation and adaptation of immigrant adolescents from the former Soviet Union in a different community context. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2005;35(12):83–101. doi: 10.1007/s10464-005-1891-y.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Blank RM, Dabady M, Citro CF. Measuring racial discrimination. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  20. Blascovitch J, Spencer SJ, Quinn D, Steele C. African Americans and high blood pressure: The role of stereotype threat. Psychological Science. 2001;12(3):225–229. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bonilla-Silva E. Rethinking racism: Toward a structural interpretation. American Sociological Review. 1996;62:465–480. doi: 10.2307/2657316. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Borrell LN, Jacobs DRJ, Williams DR, Pletcher MJ, Houston TK. Self-reported racial discrimination and substance use in the Coronary Artery Risk Development in Adults Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;166(9):1068–1079. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Borrell LN, Kiefe CI, Williams DR, Diez-Roux AV, Gordon-Larsen P. Self-reported health, perceived racial discrimination, and skin color in African Americans in the CARDIA study. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;63(6):1415–1427. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Brave Heart M. The historical trauma response among natives and its relationship with substance abuse: A Lakota illustration. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. 2003;35(1):7–13. doi: 10.1080/02791072.2003.10399988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brave Heart M, DeBruyn L. The American Indian Holocaust: Healing historical unresolved grief. American Indian and Alaska Native Mental Health Research. 1998;8(2):56–78. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Brody GH, Chen YF, Murry VM, Ge X, Simons RL, Gibbons FX, Gerrard M, Cutrona C. Perceived discrimination and the adjustment of African American youths: A five-year longitudinal analysis with contextual moderation effects. Child Development. 2006;77(5) doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624. 2006.00927.x.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Brondolo E, Brady N, Thompson S, Tobin J, Cassells A, Sweeney M, et al. Perceived Racism and negative affect: Analyses of trait and state measures of affect in a community sample. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2008a;27(2):150–173. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2008.27.2.150.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Brondolo E, Libby D, Denton D, Thompson S, Beatty D, Schwartz J, et al. Racism and ambulatory blood pressure in a community sample. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2008b;70(1):49–56. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815ff3bd.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Brondolo E, Rieppi R, Kelly KP, Gerin W. Perceived racism and blood pressure: A review of the literature and conceptual and methodolgical critique. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2003;25(1):55–65. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2501_08. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Brondolo E, Thompson S, Brady N, Appel R, Cassells A, Tobin J, et al. The relationship of racism to appraisals and coping in a community sample. Ethnicity & Disease. 2005;15(4):14–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Brook D, Brook J, Balka E, Rosenberg G. Predictors of rebellious behavior in childhood parental drug use, peers, school environment, and child personality. Journal of addictive disorders. 2006a;25(2):77–87. doi: 10.1300/J069v25n02_11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Brook J, Morojele N, Brook D, Zhang C, Whiteman M. Personal, interpersonal and cultural predictors of stages of cigarette smoking among adolescents in Johannesburg, South Africa. Tobacco Control. 2006b;15(1):48–53. doi: 10.1136/tc.2005. 014878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Brosschot JF, Gerin W, Thayer JF. The perservative cognition hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006;60:113–124. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005. 06.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Brotman DJ, Golden SH, Wittstein IS. The cardiovascular toll of stress. Lancet. 2007;370:1089–1100. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61305-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Broudy R, Brondolo E, Coakley V, Brady N, Cassells A, Tobin JN, et al. Perceived ethnic discrimination in relation to daily moods and negative social interactions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2007;30(1):31–43. doi: 10.1007/s10865-006-9081-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Brown C, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Chang Y. The relation between perceived unfair treatment and blood pressure in a racially/ethnically diverse sample of women. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2006;164(3):257–262. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Brown TN, Williams DR, Jackson JS, Neighbors HW, Torres M, Sellers SL, et al. Being black and feeling blue: The mental health consequences of racial discrimination. Race and Society. 2000;2:117–131. doi: 10.1016/S1090-9524(00)00010-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Bryant RA. Early predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Society of Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53:789–795. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01895-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Butler C, Tull ES, Chambers EC, Taylor J. Internalized racism, body fat distribution, and abnormal fasting glucose among African-Caribbean women in Dominica, West Indies. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2002;94(3):143–148. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Bynum MS, Burton ET, Best C. Racism experiences and psychological functioning in African American college freshmen: Is racial socialization a buffer? Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2007;13(1):64–71. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.13.1.64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Carr D, Friedman MA. Is obesity stigmatizing? Body weight, perceived discrimination, and psychological well-being in the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2005;46:244–259. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Carter RT. Racism and psychological and emotional injury: recognizing and assessing race-based traumatic stress. The Counseling Psychiatrist. 2007;35(1):13–105. [Google Scholar]
  43. Casagrande SS, Gary TL, LaVeist TA, Gaskin DJ, Cooper LA. Perceived discrimination and adherence to medical care in a racially integrated community. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2007;22(3):389–395. doi: 10.1007/s11606-006-0057-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Casswell S, Huckle T, Pledger M. Survey data need not underestimate alcohol consumption. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research. 2002;26(10):1561–1567. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000034390.38886.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Chambers EC, Tull ES, Fraser HS, Mutunhu NR, Sobers N, Niles E. The relationship of internalized racism to body fat distribution and insulin resistance among African adolescent youth. Journal of the National Medical Association. 2004;96(12):1594–1598. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Chen FM, Fryer GE, Jr, Phillips RL, Jr, Wilson E, Pathman DE. Patients’ beliefs about racism, preferences for physician race, and satisfaction with care. Annals of Family Medicine. 2005;3(2):138–143. doi: 10.1370/afm.282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Choi Y, Harachi TW, Gillmore MR, Catalano RF. Are multiracial adolescents at greater risk? Comparisons of rates, patterns, and correlates of substance use and violence between monoracial and multiracial adolescents. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2006;76(1):86–97. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.1.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Clark R. Interactive but not direct effects of perceived racism and trait anger predict resting systolic and diastolic blood pressure in black adolescents. Health Psychology–Sep;(5). Related Articles. LinkOut. 2006a;25(5):580–585. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Clark R. Perceived racism and vascular reactivity in black college women: Moderating effects of seeking social support. Health Psychology. 2006b;25(1):20–25. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.1.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. The American Psychologist. 1999;54(10):805–816. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.10.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Clark R, Benkert RA, Flack JM. Large arterial elasticity varies as a function of gender and racism-related vigilance in black youth. The Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006a;39(4) doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Clark VR, Cobb RE, Hopkins R, Smith CE. Black racial identity as a mediator of cardiovascular reactivity to racism in African-American college students. Ethnicity & Disease. 2006b;16(1):108–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Clark R, Gochett P. Interactive effects of perceived racism and coping responses predict a school-based assessment of blood pressure in black youth. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2006;32(1):1–9. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3201_1.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Cohen S, Janicki-Deverts D, Miller GE. Psychological stress and disease. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2007;298(14):1685–1687. doi: 10.1001/jama.298.14.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Cohen S, Kessler RC, Gordon LU. Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psychiatric and physical disorders. In: Cohen S, Kessler RC, Lynn UG, editors. Measuring stress: A guide for health and social scientists. New York: Oxford University Press; 1995. pp. 3–26. [Google Scholar]
  56. Cozier Y, Palmer JR, Horton NJ, Fredman L, Wise LA, Rosenberg L. Racial discrimination and the incidence of hypertension in US black women. Annals of Epidemiology. 2006;16(9):681–687. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.11.008.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Crimmins EM, Kim JK, Alley DE, Karlamangla A, Seeman T. Hispanic paradox in biological risk profiles. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(7):1305–1310. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.091892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Crouter AC, Davis KD, Updegraff KA, Delgado M, Fortner M. Mexican American Fathers’ occupational conditions: Links to family members’ psychological adjustment. Journal of Marriage and the Family. 2006;68(4):843–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00299.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Dailey AB, Kasl SV, Holford TR, Jones BA. Perceived racial discrimination and nonadherence to screening mammography guidelines: results from the race differences in the screening mammography process study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;165(11):1287–1295. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Davis SK, Liu Y, Quarells RC, Din-Dzietharn R M.A.H.D.S. Group. Stress-related racial discrimination and hypertension likelihood in a population-based sample of African Americans: The Metro Atlanta Heart Disease Study. Ethnicity & Disease. 2005;15(4):585–593. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. De Vogli R, Brunner E, Marmot MG. Unfairness and the social gradient of metabolic symdrome in the Whitehall II study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2007a;63:413–419. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. De Vogli R, Ferrie J, Chandola T, Kivimaki M, Marmot M. Unfairness and health: Evidence from the Whitehall II Study. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2007b;61(6):513–518. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.052563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. DiMaggio C, Galea S, Richardson LD. Emergency department visits for behavioral and mental health care after a terrorist attack. Annals of Emergency Medicine. 2007;50(3):327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.annemergmed.2006.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Din-Dzietharn R, Nembhard WN, Collins R, Davis SD. Perceived stress following race-based discrimination at work is associated with hypertension in African-Americans. The metro Atlanta heart disease study 1999–2001. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58:449–461. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00211-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Dohrenwend BP. Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: Toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychological Bulletin. 2006;132(3):477–495. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.3.477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Dougall AL, Craig KJ, Baum A. Assessment of characteristics of intrusive thoughts and their impact on distress among victims of traumatic events. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1999;61:38–48. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199901000-00008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Dyrbye LN, Thomas MR, Eacker A, Harper W, Massie FSJ, Power DV, et al. Race, ethnicity, and medical student well-being in the United States. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2007;167(19):2103–2109. doi: 10.1001/archinte.167.19.2103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Epel E, Lin J, Wilhelm F, Wolkowitz O, Cawthon R, Adler N, et al. Cell aging in relation to stress arousal and cardiovascular diseases risk factors. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2006;31(3):277–287. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.08.011.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Etowa J, Weins J, Bernard WT, Clow B. Determinants of black women’s health in rural and remote countries. Canadian Journal of Nursing Research. 2007;39(3):56–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Evans GW, Jacobs SV, Dooley D, Catalano R. The interaction of stressful life events and chronic strains on community mental health. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1987;15(1):23–34. doi: 10.1007/BF00919755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Facione NC, Facione PA. Perceived prejudice in healthcare and women’s health protective behavior. Nursing Research. 2007;56(3):175–184. doi: 10.1097/01.NNR.0000270026.90359.4c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Ferrie MJ, Vahtera J, Marmot MG, Kivimaki M. Injustice at work and incidence of psychiatric morbidity: The Whitehall II study. Occupational and Environmental Medicine. 2006;63:443–450. doi: 10.1136/oem.2005.022269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Fischer CS, Hout M, Jankowski MS, Lucas SR, Swidler A, Voss K. Inequality by design: Cracking the bell curve myth. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  74. Fix M, Struyk RJ. Clear and convincing evidence: Measurement of discrimination in America. Washington, D.C: Urban Institute Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  75. Forman TA. The social psychological costs of racial segmentation in the workplace: A study of African Americans’ well-being. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2003;44(3):332–352. doi: 10.2307/1519783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Fowler-Brown A, Ashkin E, Corbie-Smith G, Thaker S, Pathman DE. Perception of racial barriers to health care in the rural South. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved. 2006;17(1):86–100. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2006.0022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Gee GC, Chen J, Spencer M, See S, Kuester O, Tran D, et al. Social support as a buffer for perceived unfair treatment among Filipino Americans: Differences between San Francisco and Hawaii. American Journal of Public Health. 2006a;96(4):677–684. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2004.060442. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Gee GC, Delva J, Takeuchi D. Relationships between self-reported unfair treatment and prescription medication use, illicit drug use, and alcohol dependence among Filipino Americans. American Journal of Public Health. 2007a;97(5):933–940. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.075739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Gee GC, Ryan A, Laflamme DJ, Holt J. Self-reported discrimination and mental health status among African descendents, Mexican Americans, and other Latinos in the New Hampshire REACH 2010 Initiative: The added dimension of immigration. American Journal of Public Health. 2006b;96(10):1821–1828. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.080085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Gee GC, Spencer MS, Chen J, Takeuchi D. A nationwide study of discrimination and chronic health conditions among Asian Americans. American Journal of Public Health. 2007c;97(7):1275–1282. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.091827.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Gee GC, Spencer M, Chen J, Yip T, Takeuchi D. The association between self-reported racial discrimination and 12-month DSM-IV mental disorders among Asian Americans nationwide. Social Science & Medicine. 2007b;64(10):1984–1996. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Gibbons FX, Yeh HC, Gerrard M, Cleveland MJ, Cutrona C, Simons RL, Brody GH. Early experience with racial discrimination and conduct disorder as predictors of subsequent drug use: A critical period hypothesis. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2007;88(Suppl 1):27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2006.12.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Goldsmith AH, Hamilton Darrick, Darity William., Jr From dark to light: Skin color and wages among African-Americans. The Journal of Human Resources. 2007;42(4):701–738. [Google Scholar]
  84. Gomez JP, Trierweiler S. Does discrimination terminology create response bias in questionnaire studies of discrimination? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2001;27(5):630–638. doi: 10.1177/0146167201275011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Green AR, Carney DR, Pallin DJ, Ngo LH, Raymond KL, Iezzoni LI, et al. Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for black and white patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2007;22(9):1231–1238. doi: 10.1007/s11606-007-0258-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Greene ML, Way N, Pahl K. Trajectories of perceived adult and peer discrimination among Black, Latino, and Asian American adolescents: Patterns and psychological correlates. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42(2):218–236. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Guyll M, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT. Discrimination and unfair treatment: Relationship to cardiovascular reactivity among African American and European American women. Health Psychology. 2001;20(5):315–325. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.20.5.315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Harrell SP. The Racism and Life Experience Scales (RaLES): Self-administration version. Los Angeles, CA: California School of Professional Psychology; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  89. Harris R, Tobias M, Jeffreys M, Waldegrave K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J. Effects of self-reported racial discrimination and deprivation on Māori health and inequalities in New Zealand: Cross-sectional study. Lancet. 2006a;367(9527):2005–2009. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68890-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Harris R, Tobias M, Jeffreys M, Waldegrave K, Karlsen S, Nazroo J. Racism and health: The relationship between experience of racial discrimination and health in New Zealand. Social Science & Medicine. 2006b;63(6):1428–1441. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Memory for acute stress disorder symptoms: a two-year prospective study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2000;188(9):602–607. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200009000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Harvey AG, Bryant RA. Acute stress disorder: A synthesis and critique. Psychological Bulletin. 2002;128(6):886–902. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Hoyo C, Yarnall SH, Skinner CS, Moorman PG, Sellers D, Reid L. Pain predicts non adherence to pap swear screening among middle-aged African American Women. Preventive Medicine. 2005;41:439–445. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004. 11.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Jackson B, Kubzansky LD, Wright RJ. Linking perceived unfairness to physical health: The perceived unfairness model. Review of General Psychology. 2006;10(1):21–40. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.1.21. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  95. Jang Y, Kim G, Chiriboga DA. Health, healthcare utilization, and satisfaction with service: Barriers and facilitators for older Korean Americans. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53(9):1613–1617. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53518.x.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Jones JM. Prejudice and racism. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Inc; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  97. Jones CP. Levels of racism: A theoretical framework and a gardener’s tale. American Journal of Public Health. 2000;90(8):1212–1215. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.8.1212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kalichman SC, Simbayi LC, Kagee A, Toefy Y, Jootse S, Cain D, et al. Associations of poverty, substance use and HIV transmission risk behaviors in three South African Communities. Social Science & Medicine. 2006;62:1641–1649. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.08.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Karlsen S, Nazroo JY, McKenzie K, Bhui K, Weich S. Racism, psychosis and common mental disorder among ethnic minority groups in England. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35(12):1795–1803. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005830.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Kaufman JS, Cooper RS, McGee DL. Socioeconomic status and health in blacks and whites: The problem of residual confounding and the resiliency of race. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass) 1997;8:621–628. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199710000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Keith VM, Herring C. Skin tone and stratification in the black community. American Journal of Sociology. 1991;97(3):760–778. doi: 10.1086/229819. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Kessler RC, Galea S, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Ursano RJ, Wessely S. Trends in mental illness and suicidality after Hurricane Katrina. Journal of Molecular Psychiatry. 2008;13:374–384. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Kessler RC, Mickelson KD, Williams DR. The prevalence, distribution, and mental health correlates of perceived discrimination in the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1999;40:208–230. doi: 10.2307/2676349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Khaylis A, Waelde LC, Bruce EJ. The role of ethnic identity in the relationship of race-related stress to PTSD symptoms among young adults. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation. 2007;8(4):91–105. doi: 10.1300/J229v08n04_06. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. King KR. Why is discrimination stressful? The mediating role of cognitive appraisal. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2005;11(3):202–212. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.11.3.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Kraus V, Koresh Y. The course of residential segregation: Ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and suburbanization in Israel. The Sociological Quarterly. 1992;33:303–319. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.1992.tb00377.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  107. Kressin NR, Kristal RL, Manze M. Perceptions of race/ethnicity based discrimination: A review of measures and evaluation of their usefullness for the health care setting. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved. 2008;19:697–730. doi: 10.1353/hpu.0.0041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Krieger N. Racial and gender discrimination: Risk Factors for high blood pressure? Social Science & Medicine. 1990;30(12):1273–1281. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)90307-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Krieger N. Embodying Inequality: A review of concepts, measures, and methods for studying health consequences of discrimination. International Journal of Health Services. 1999;29(2):295–352. doi: 10.2190/M11W-VWXE-KQM9-G97Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Krieger N, Smith K, Naishadham D, Hartman C, Barbeau EM. Experiences of discrimination: Validity and reliability of a self-report measure for population health research on racism and health. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;61(7):1576–1596. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Krysan M, Couper MP. Race in the live and virtual interview: Racial deference, social desirability, and activation effects in attitude surveys. Social Psychology Quarterly. 2003;66(4):364–383. doi: 10.2307/1519835. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Kubany ES, Haynes SN, Leisen MB, Owens JA, Kaplan AS, Watson SB, et al. Development and preliminary validation of a brief broad-spectrum measure of trauma exposure: The Traumatic Life Events Questionnaire. Psychological Assessment. 2000;12(2):210–224. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Kung HC, Hoyert DL, Xu J, Murphy SL. Deaths: Final data for 2005. National Vital Statistics Reports. 2008;56(10):4–26. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Lam B. Impact of perceived racial discrimination and collective self-esteem on psychological distress among vietnamese-american college students: Sense of coherence as mediator. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2007;77(3):370–376. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.3.370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Lam AM, Tsoi KW, Chan TS. Adolescent Chinese immigrants in Hong Kong: A comparison with locally born students and factors associated with their psychological well-being. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health. 2005;17(2):157–168. doi: 10.1515/ijamh.2005.17.2.157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Landrine H, Klonoff EA. The schedule of racist events: A measure of racial discrimination and a study of its negative physical and mental health consequences. The Journal of Black Psychology. 1996;22(2):144–168. doi: 10.1177/00957984960222002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Landrine H, Klonoff EA, Corral I, Fernandez S, Roesch S. Conceptualizing and measuring ethnic discrimination in health research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2006;29(1):79–94. doi: 10.1007/s10865-005-9029-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Larson A, Gillies M, Howard PJ, Coffin J. It’s enough to make you sick: The impact of racism on the health of Aboriginal Australians. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2007;31(4) doi: 10.1111/j.1753–6405.2007.00079.x.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Lauderdale DS. Birth outcomes for Arabic-named women in California before and after September 11. Demography. 2006;43(1):185–201. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Lepore SJ, Evans GW, Palsane MN. Social hassles and psychological health in the context of chronic crowding. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1991;32:357–367. doi: 10.2307/2137103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Lepore SJ, Revenson TA, Weinberger SL, Weston P, Frisina PG, Robertson R, et al. Effects of social stressors on cardiovascular reactivity in Black and White women. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2006;31(2):120–127. doi: 10.1207/s15324796abm3102_3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Levine RS, Foster JE, Fullilove RE, Briggs NC, Hull PC, Husaini BA, et al. Black-white inequalities in mortality and life expectancy, 1933–1999: Implications for Healthy People 2010. Public Health Reports. 2001;116:474–483. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.5.474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Levy BR, Slade MD, Kasl S, Kunker SR. Longevity increased by positive self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2002;83(2):261–270. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Lewis T, Everson-Rose S, Powell LH, Matthews KA, Brown C, Karavolos K, et al. Chronic exposure to everyday discrimination and coronary artery calcification in African-American women: The SWAN Heart Study. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2006;68:362–368. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221360.94700.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Lincoln KD, Chatters LM, Taylor RJ, Jackson JS. Profiles of depressive symptoms among African Americans and Caribbean Blacks. Social Science & Medicine. 2007;65:200–213. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.02.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Lindström M. Social capital, anticipated ethnic discrimination and self-reported psychological health: A population-based study. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;66(1):1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Locher JR, Roth CS, Sawyer-Baker P, Bodner E, Allman RM. Soclai isolation, support, and capital and nutirtional risk in an older sample: Ethnic and gender differences. Social Science & Medicine. 2005;60:747–761. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.06.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Major B, Kaiser CR, O’Brien LT, McCoy SK. Perceived discrimination as worldview threat or worldview confirmation: Implications for self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2007;92(6):1068–1086. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.1068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Malat J, Hamilton MA. Preference for same-race health care providers and perceptions of interpersonal discrimination in health care. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2006;47(2):173–187. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Malat J, van Ryn M. African-American preference for same-race healthcare providers: The role of healthcare discrimination. Ethnicity & Disease. 2005;15(4):740–747. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Matthews KA, Salomon K, Kenyon K, Zhou F. Unfair treatment, discrimination, and ambulatory blood pressure in black and white adolescents. Health Psychology. 2005;24(3):258–265. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.3.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Mays VM, Cochran SD, Barnes NW. Race, race-based discrimination, and health outcomes among African Americans. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:201–225. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. McNeilly MD, Anderson NB, Armstead CA, Clark R, Corbett M, Robinson EL, et al. The percieved racism scale: A multidimensional assessment of the experience of white racism among African Americans. Ethnicity and Disease. 1996;6(12):154–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Merritt MM, Bennett GG, Jr, Williams RB, Edwards CL, Sollers J., 3rd Perceived racism and cardiovascular reactivity and recovery to personally relevant stress. Health Psychology. 2006;25(3):364–369. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.3.364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Miller GV, Travers CJ. Ethnicity and the experience of work: Job stress and satisfaction of minority ethnic teachers in the UK. International Review of Psychiatry (Abingdon England) 2005;17(5):317–327. doi: 10.1080/09540260500238470.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Monroe SM. Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:33–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Montgomery E, Foldspang A. Discrimination, mental problems and social adaptation in young refugees. European Journal of Public Health. 2007;18(2):156–161. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckm073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Moody-Ayers SY, Stewart AL, Covinsky KE, Inouye SK. Prevalence and correlates of perceived societal racism in older African-American adults with type 2 diabetes mellitus. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53(12):2202–2208. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.00501.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Nápoles-Springer AM, Santoyo J, Houston K, Pérez-Stable EJ, Stewart AL. Patients’ perceptions of cultural factors affecting the quality of their medical encounters. Health Expectations. 2005;8(1):4–17. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-7625.2004.00298.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. National Center for Health Statistics. Health United States, 2007 with Chartbook Trends in the Health of Americans. Hyattsville, MD: 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Noh S, Kaspar V, Wickrama KA. Overt and subtle racial discrimination and mental health: Preliminary findings for Korean immigrants. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(7):1269–1274. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.085316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Oppedal B, Røysamb E, Heyerdahl S. Ethnic group, acculturation, and psychiatric problems in young immigrants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2005;46(6):646–660. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00381.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Pamuk E, Makuk D, Heck K, Reuben C. Socioeconomic status and health chartbook. Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  144. Pantzer K, Rajmi L, Tebe C, Codina F, Serra-Sutton V, Ferrer M, et al. Health related quality of life in immigrants and native school aged adolescents in Spain. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2006;60(8):694–698. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.044073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Paradies Y. Antiracism and indigenous Australians. Analysis of Social Issues aand Public Policy. 2005;5(1):1–28. [Google Scholar]
  146. Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2006;35(4):888–901. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Pearlin LI, Schieman S, Fazio EM, Meersman SC. Stress, health, and the life course: Some conceptual perspectives. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2005;46(June):205–219. doi: 10.1177/002214650504600206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Peters RM. The relationship of racism, chronic stress emotions, and blood pressure. Journal of Nursing Scholarship. 2006;38(3):234–240. doi: 10.1111/j.1547-5069.2006.00108.x.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Peters RM, Benkert R, Dinardo E, Templin T. Assessing quality of care for African Americans with hypertension. Journal for Healthcare Quality. 2007;29(3):10–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1945-1474.2007.tb00188.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Pettigrew TF. Profile of the Negro American. Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand; 1964. [Google Scholar]
  151. Pieterse AL, Carter RT. An examination of the relationship between general life stress, racism-related stress, and psychological health among black men. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2007;54(1):101–109. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.54.1.101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  152. Piette JD, Bibbins-Domingo K, Schillinger D. Health care discrimination, processes of care, and diabetes patients’ health status. Patient Education and Counseling. 2006;60(1):41–48. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2004.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Pole N, Best S, Metsler T, Marmar C. Why are Hispanics at greater risk for PTSD. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2005;11(2):144–161. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.11.2.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Poyrazli S, Lopez MD. An exploratory study of perceived discrimination and homesickness: A comparison of international students and American students. The Journal of Psychology. 2007;141(3):263–280. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.141.3.263-280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Richman LS, Bennett GG, Pek J, Siegler I, Williams RBJ. Discrimination, dispositions, and cardiovascular responses to stress. Health Psychology. 2007;26(6):675–683. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.26.6.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Roberts CB, Vines AI, Kaufman JS, James SA. Cross-sectional association between perceived discrimination and hypertension in African-American men and women: The Pitt County Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;167(5):624–632. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm334. EPub. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Romero AJ, Martinez D, Carvajal SC. Bicultural stress and adolescent risk behaviors in a community sample of Latinos and non-Latino European Americans. Ethnicity & Health. 2007;12(5):443–463. doi: 10.1080/13557850701616854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Ryan AM, Gee GC, Laflamme DF. The Association between self-reported discrimination, physical health and blood pressure: Findings from African Americans, Black immigrants, and Latino immigrants in New Hampshire. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved. 2006;17(2):116–132. doi: 10.1353/hpu.2006.0079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Salvatore J, Shelton JN. Cognitive costs of exposure to racial prejudice. Psychological Science. 2007;18(9):810–815. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01984.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Schaeffer NC, Presser S. The science of asking questions. Annual Review of Sociology. 2003;29:65–88. doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.29.110702.110112. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  161. Schulz R, Beach SR. Caregiving as a risk factor for mortality: The caregiver heal effects study. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1999;282(3):2215–2219. doi: 10.1001/jama.282.23.2215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Schulz AJ, Gravlee CC, Williams DR, Israel BA, Mentz G, Rowe Z. Discrimination, symptoms of depression, and self-rated health among african american women in detroit: Results from a longitudinal analysis. American Journal of Public Health. 2006a;96(7):1265–1270. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.064543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Schulz AJ, Israel BA, Zenk SN, Parker EA, Lichtenstein R, Shellman-Weir S, et al. Psychosocial stress and social support as mediators of relationships between income, length of residence and depressive symptoms among African American women on Detroit’s eastside. Social Science & Medicine. 2006b;62(2):510–522. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.06.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Schulz AJ, Williams DR, Israel BA, Lempert LB. Racial and spatial relations as fundamental determinants of health in Detroit. The Milbank Quarterly. 2002;80(4):677–707. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.00028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Schuman H, Charlotte S, Larwrence B, Maria K. Racial attitudes in America: Trends and interpretations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1997. Revised ed. [Google Scholar]
  166. Schwarz N. Cognitive aspects of survey methodology. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2007;21:277–287. doi: 10.1002/acp.1340. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  167. Seeman TE, Crimmins E, Huang MH, Singer B, Bucur A, Gruenewald T, et al. Cumulative biological risk and socio-economic differences in mortality: MacArthur studies of successful aging. Social Science & Medicine. 2004;58:1985–1997. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(03)00402-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Sellers SL, Bonham V, Neighbors HW, Amell JW. Effects of racial discrimination and health behaviors on mental and physical health of middle-class african American Men. Health Education & Behavior. 2006;27 doi: 10.1177/1090198106293526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Sheridan L. Islamophobia pre and post September 11th 2001. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2006;21(3):317–336. doi: 10.1177/0886260505282885. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Siefert K, Finlayson TL, Williams DR, Delva J, Ismail AI. Modifiable risk and protective factors for depressive symptoms in low-income African American mothers. The American Journal of Orthopsychiatry. 2007;77(1):113–123. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.77.1.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Simons RL, Simons LG, Burt CH, Drummund H, Stewart E, Brody GH, et al. Supportive parenting moderates the effect of discrimination upon anger, hostile view of relationships, and violence among African American boys. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2006;47(4):373–389. doi: 10.1177/002214650604700405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Smedley BD, Stith AY, Nelson AR. Unequal treatment: Confronting racial and ethnic disparities in health care. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2003. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Smokowski PR, Bacallao M. Acculturation, internalizing mental health symptoms, and self-esteem: Cultural experiences of Latino adolescents in North Carolina. Child Psychiatry and Human Development. 2007;37(3):273–292. doi: 10.1007/s10578-006-0035-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Sohler N, Li X, CC Perceived discrimination among severly disadvantaged people with HIV infection. Public Health Reports. 2007;122(3):347–355. doi: 10.1177/003335490712200308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Stam R. PTSD and stress sensitization: A tale of brain and body. Part 1: Human studies. Neuroscience. 2007;31:530–557. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Steele CM. A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. The American Psychologist. 1997;52(6):613–629. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Steffen PR, Bowden M. Sleep disturbance mediates the relationship between perceived racism and depressive symptoms. Ethnicity & Disease. 2006;16(1):16–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Stetler C, Chen E, Miller GE. Written disclosure of experiences with racial discrimination and antibody response to an influenza vaccine. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2006;13(1):60–68. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm1301_8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Stevens GW, Vollebergh WA, Pels TV, Crinjnen AA. Predicting externalizing problems in Moroccan immigrant adolescents in the Netherlands. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2005a;40(7):571–579. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0926-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Stevens GW, Vollebergh WA, Pels TV, Crinjnen AA. Predicting internalizing problems in Moroccan immigrant adolescents in the Netherlands. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2005b;40:1003–10011. doi: 10.1007/s00127-005-0988-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Sujoldzic A, Peternel L, Kulenovic T, Terzic R. Social determinants of health—a comparative study of Bosnian adolescents in different cultural contexts. Collegium Antropologicum. 2006;30(4):703–711. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Tabibnia G, Satpute AB, Lieberman MD. The sunny side of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry) Psychological Science. 2008;19:339–347. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  183. Taylor J, Henderson D, Jackson BB. A holistic model for understanding and predicting depression in African American women. Journal of Community Psychology. 1991;19:306–320. doi: 10.1002/1520-6629(199110)19:4<306::AID-JCOP2290190403>3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  184. Taylor J, Jackson B. Factors affecting alcohol consumption in black women, Part II. The International Journal of the Addictions. 1990;25(12):1415–1427. doi: 10.3109/10826089009056228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Taylor J, Jackson BB. Evaluation of a holistic model of mental health symptoms in African American women. The Journal of Black Psychology. 1991;18:19–45. doi: 10.1177/00957984910181003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  186. Taylor TR, Williams CD, Makambi KH, Mouton C, Harrell JP, Cozier Y, et al. Racial discrimination and breast cancer incidence in US Black women: The Black Women’s Health Study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;166(1):46–54. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Terrell F, Miller AR, Foster K, Watkins CE., Jr Racial discrimination-induced anger and alcohol use among black adolescents. Adolescence. 2006;41(163):485–492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Thomas KS, Bardwell WA, Ancoli-Israel S, Dimsdale JE. The toll of ethnic discrimination on sleep architecture and fatigue. Health Psychology. 2006;25(5):635–642. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.5.635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Tourangeau R, Yan T. Sensitive questions in surveys. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133(5):859–883. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Trivedi AN, Ayanian JZ. Perceived discrimination and use of preventive health services. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2006;21(6):553–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1525-1497.2006.00413.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Tull ES, Cort MA, Gwebu ET, Gwebu K. Internalized racism is associated with elevated fasting glucose in a sample of adult women but not men in Zimbabwe. Ethnicity & Disease. 2007;17(4):731–735. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Tull ES, Wickramasuriya T, Taylor J, Smith-Burns V, Brown M, Champaigne G, et al. Relationship of internalized racism to abdominal obesity and blood pressure in Afro-Caribbean women. Journal of the National Medical Association. 1999;91:447–452. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Turner RJ, Wheaton B, Lloyd DA. The epidemiology of social stress. American Sociological Review. 1995;60(1):104–125. doi: 10.2307/2096348. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  194. Umaña-Taylor AJ, Updegraff KA. Latino adolescents’ mental health: Exploring the interrelations among discrimination, ethnic identity, cultural orientation, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms. Journal of Adolescence. 2007;30(4):549–567. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Utsey SO, Hook JN. Heart rate variability as a physiological moderator of the relationship between race-related stress and psychological distress in African Americans. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2007;13(3):250–253. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.13.3.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Utsey SO, Lanier Y, Williams O, Bolden M, Lee A. Moderator effects of cognitive ability and social support on the relation between race-related stress and quality of life in a community sample of black Americans. Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2006;12(2):334–346. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.12.2.334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Utsey SO, Ponterotto JG. Development and validation of the index of race-related stress (IRRS) Journal of Counseling Psychology. 1996;43:490–501. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.43.4.490. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  198. Van Houtven CH, Voils CI, Oddone EZ, Weinfurt KP, Friedman JY, Schulman KA, et al. Perceived discrimination and reported delay of pharmacy prescriptions and medical tests. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2005;20(7):578–583. doi: 10.1007/s11606-005-0104-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. van Ryn M. Research on the provider contribution to race/ethnicity disparities in medical care. Medical Care. 2002;40(1):I140–I151. doi: 10.1097/00005650-200201001-00015.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Veling W, Selten JP, Susser E, Laan W, Mackenbach JP, Hoek HW. Discrimination and the incidence of psychotic disorders among ethnic minorities in The Netherlands. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;36(4):761–768. doi: 10.1093/ije/dym085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Vines AI, Baird DD, McNeilly M, Hertz-Piccioto I, Light KC, Stevens J. Social correlates of the chronic stress of perceived racism among black women. Ethnicity & Disease. 2006;16(1):101–107. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Vines AI, Baird DD, Stevens J, Hertz-Picciotto I, Light KC, McNeilly M. Associations of abdominal fat with perceived racism and passive emotional responses to racism in African American women. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(3):526–530. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.080663.. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Vos MS, de Haes JCJM. Denial in cancer patients, an explorative review. Psycho-Oncology. 2007;16:12–25. doi: 10.1002/pon.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Wadsworth E, Dillon K, Shaw C, Bhui K, Stansfeld S, Smith A. Racial discrimination ethnicity and work stress. Occupational Medicine (Oxford, England) 2007;57(1):18–24. doi: 10.1093/occmed/kql088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  205. Wagner J, Abbott G. Depression and depression care in diabetes: Relationship to perceived discrimination in African Americans. Diabetes Care. 2007;30(2):364–366. doi: 10.2337/dc06-1756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Waltermaurer E, Watson CA, McNutt LA. Black women’s health: The effect of perceived racism and intimate partner violence. Violence Against Women. 2006;12(12):1214–1222. doi: 10.1177/1077801206293545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Wamala S, Boström G, Nyqvist K. Perceived discrimination and psychological distress in Sweden. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2007b;190:75–76. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.021188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Wamala S, Merlo J, Boström G, Hogstedt C. Perceived discrimination, socioeconomic disadvantage and refraining from seeking medical treatment in Sweden. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2007a;61(5):409–415. doi: 10.1136/jech.2006.049999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Whitbeck LB, Adams GW, Hoyt DR, Chen X. Conceptualizing and measuring historical trauma among American Indian people. Journal of Community Psychology. 2004;33(34):119–130. doi: 10.1023/B:AJCP.0000027000.77357.31.. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  210. Williams DR. Race and health: Basic questions, emerging directions. Annals of Epidemiology. 1997;7(5):322–333. doi: 10.1016/S1047-2797(97)00051-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  211. Williams DR. Racism and Health. In: Whitfield KE, editor. Closing the Gap: Improving the Health of Minority Elders in the New Millenium. Washington, D.C: Gerontological Society of America; 2004. pp. 69–80. [Google Scholar]
  212. Williams DR. The health of U.S. racial and ethnic populations. Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2005;60B (Special Issue II):53–62. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.special_issue_2.s53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  213. Williams DR, Collins C. U.S. socioeconomic and racial differences in health: Patterns and explanations. Annual Review of Sociology. 1995;21:349–386. doi: 10.1146/annurev.so.21.080195.002025. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  214. Williams DR, Collins CA. Racial residential segregation: A fundamental cause of racial disparities in health. Public Health Reports. 2001;116:404–415. doi: 10.1093/phr/116.5.404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  215. Williams D, Gonzalez H, Williams S, Mohammed S, Moomal H, Stein D. Perceived discrimination, race, and health in South Africa. Social Science & Medicine. 2008;67:441–452. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  216. Williams DR, Jackson PB. Social sources of racial disparities in health. Health Affairs. 2005;24(2):325–334. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.24.2.325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  217. Williams DR, Neighbors HW. Racism, discrimination and hypertension: Evidence and needed research. Ethnicity & Disease. 2001;11:800–816. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  218. Williams DR, Neighbors HW, Jackson JS. Racial/ethnic discrimination and health: Findings from community studies. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(2):200–208. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.2.200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  219. Williams DR, Spencer M, Jackson JS. Race, stress, and physical health: The role of group identity. In: Contrada RJ, Ashmore RD, editors. Self, Social Identity, and Physical Health: Interdisciplinary Explorations. New York: Oxford University Press; 1999. pp. 71–100. [Google Scholar]
  220. Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson J, Anderson N. Racial differences in physical and mental health: Socioeconomic status, stress, and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology. 1997;2(3):335–351. doi: 10.1177/135910539700200305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  221. Wise LA, Palmer JR, Cozier YC, Hunt MO, Stewart EA, Rosenberg L. Perceived racial discrimination and risk of uterine leiomyomata. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass) 2007;18(6):747–757. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181567e92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  222. Wyatt SB, Williams DR, Calvin R, Henderson F, Walker E, Winters K. Racism and cardiovascular disease in African Americans: Evidence and implications for the Jackson Heart Study. The American Journal of the Medical Sciences. 2003;325(6):315–331. doi: 10.1097/00000441-200306000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  223. Yehuda R, Bryant R, Marmar C, Zohar J. Pathological Responses to Terrorism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:1793–1805. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  224. Yoder KA, Whitbeck LB, Hoyt DR, LaFromboise T. Suicidal ideation among American Indian youths. Archives of Suicide Research. 2006;10(2):177–190. doi: 10.1080/13811110600558240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  225. Young M, Stuber J, Ahern J, Galea S. Interpersonal discrimination and the health of illicit drug users. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse. 2005;31(3):371–391. doi: 10.1081/ADA-200056772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  226. Zamboni BD, Crawford I. Minority stress and sexual problems among African-American gay and bisexual men. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2007;36(4):569–578. doi: 10.1007/s10508-006-9081-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES