Skip to main content
Neuroepidemiology logoLink to Neuroepidemiology
. 2008 Sep 11;31(3):167–173. doi: 10.1159/000154929

Functional Decline in Cognitive Impairment – The Relationship between Physical and Cognitive Function

Tung Wai Auyeung a,c,*, Timothy Kwok b, Jenny Lee b, Ping Chung Leung a, Jason Leung a, Jean Woo b
PMCID: PMC2824577  PMID: 18784415

Abstract

Background

Physical function decline is associated with dementia, which might either be mediated by the coexisting sarcopenia or directly related to the impaired cognition. Our objectives are to examine the relationship between cognitive function and performance-based physical function and to test the hypothesis that cognitive function is related to poor physical function independent of muscle mass.

Methods

We measured muscle strength, performance-based physical function and muscle mass using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry and cognitive function using the cognitive part of the Community Screening Instrument of Dementia (CSI-D) in 4,000 community-dwelling Chinese elderly aged >65 years. A CSI-D cognitive score of >28.40 was considered as cognitively impaired. The effect of cognitive impairment on muscle strength and physical function was analyzed by multivariate analysis with adjustment for age, appendicular skeletal mass (ASM), the Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE) and other comorbidities.

Results

In both genders, the cognitively impaired (CSI-D cognitive score >28.40) group had a weaker grip strength (–5.10 kg, p < 0.001 in men; −1.08 kg in women, p < 0.001) and performed worse in the two physical function tests (in men, 6-meter walk speed, −0.13 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 1.42 s, p < 0.001; in women, 6-meter walk speed, −0.08 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 1.48 s, p < 0.001). After adjustment for age, ASM, PASE and other comorbidities, significant differences in grip strength (–2.60 kg, p < 0.001 in men; −0.49 kg, p = 0.011 in women) and the two physical function tests persisted between the cognitively impaired and nonimpaired group (in men, 6-meter walk speed, −0.072 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 0.80 s, p = 0.045; in women, 6-meter walk speed, −0.049 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 0.98 s, p < 0.001).

Conclusions

Poor physical function and muscle strength coexisted with cognitive impairment. This relationship was independent of muscle mass. It is likely therefore that the functional decline in dementia might be related directly to factors resulting in cognitive impairment independently of the coexisting sarcopenia.

Key Words: Sarcopenia, Dementia, Cognition, Chinese elderly, Physical function, Muscle

Introduction

The relationship between cognitive impairment and functional disability has been investigated in various cross-sectional surveys, which demonstrated that cognitive decline was associated with functional disability [1,2,3,4,5]. Lower cognitive function was also associated with poor physical performance in gait speed, standing balance and chair stand tests [6, 7]. Even those with only mild cognitive impairment exhibited more gait and balance impairment than cognitively normal subjects [8], and impaired equilibrium and limb coordination were clinically demonstrable in a series of patients suffering from mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease [9].

In a 3-year follow-up study, dementia was found to be the major cause of functional dependence in the elderly [10], and in a cohort of older Mexican Americans, Raji et al. [11] demonstrated that poor cognition was associated with a greater risk of incident disability in a 7-year period. More recently, Atkinson et al. [12] observed that global and executive functions predicted faster gait speed decline over 3 years.

However, the causal relationship between dementia and the development of functional decline could be bidirectional. Black and Rush [13] demonstrated that cognitive and functional decline appeared to influence the development of each other. Camicioli et al. [14] had revealed that motor slowing was evident prior to the development of cognitive impairment in a group of healthy older persons. Wang et al. [15] demonstrated that lower levels of performance-based physical function were associated with an increased risk of dementia and therefore suggested that poor physical function might in fact precede the onset of dementia. Similarly, weak handgrip strength at baseline was found to be associated with more rapid cognitive decline over a 7-year period [16]. Most recently in a prospective study, Inzitari et al. [17] observed that gait speed predicted decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults after 5 years.

On the other hand, Nourhashemi et al. [18] found that low cognitive function was associated with muscle loss in a group of over 7,000 community-dwelling older women. The occurrence of sarcopenia with aging is multifactorial, which may involve decreased exercise and activity, malnutrition, hormonal changes, oxidative stress and possibly an inflammatory process [19], of which some are also common in cognitive decline. It is possible that dementia and sarcopenia share certain mechanisms of pathogenesis. However, sarcopenia by itself has been demonstrated to be related to functional disability [20,21,22,23]. It seemed therefore that the apparent association between cognitive impairment and functional decline could have been mediated by coexisting sarcopenia in cognitively impaired persons. In fact Raji et al. [11] have demonstrated that older American Mexicans with poor cognition had a steeper decline in muscle strength over 7 years than those with good cognition. They postulated that sarcopenia might be the missing link between poor cognition and subsequent disability.

In the present study, we measured muscle mass by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DEXA) and physical function by performance-based tests. The DEXA scan offered an objective measurement of muscle mass while the performance-based tests were less subjected to reporting bias than the self-reported disability questionnaires. To our knowledge, apart from the American Mexican cohort [11, 16], very few studies have examined the intertwining relationship between muscle, cognition and disability at the same time. We hypothesized that cognition affects physical function directly and not just indirectly through the coexisting muscle degeneration. Our objective was to examine the relationship between cognition and physical function by performance-based tests and to investigate whether this relationship persists after controlling for the coexisting muscle degeneration effect in cognitively impaired older persons.

Methods

Four thousand community-dwelling men and women aged 65 years or over were invited to attend a health check carried out in the School of Public Health of the Chinese University of Hong Kong between August 2001 and December 2003 by placing recruitment notices in community centers for the elderly and housing estates. This project was primarily examining the bone mineral density of older Chinese adults. Talks were also given at these centers explaining the purpose, procedures and investigations to be carried out. Written informed consents were obtained. Only ethnical Chinese subjects were recruited. We excluded those who (1) were unable to walk without assistance of another person, (2) had had a bilateral hip replacement because that would have affected the bone mineral density measurement, (3) were not competent to give informed consent, and (4) had medical conditions, in the judgment of the study physicians, which made it unlikely that they would survive the duration of the study (3 years). The sample was stratified so that approximately 33% were in each of the age groups: 65–69, 70–74 and 75 years and over. The study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Chinese University of Hong Kong.

A questionnaire containing information regarding demographics, smoking habit, physical activity level (Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly, PASE, score) [24] and medical history including heart disease, diabetes mellitus, stroke and arthritis was administered by trained interviewers. Heart disease includes coronary heart disease, heart failure and history of myocardial infarction. The presence or absence of disease was based on the subjects’ report of their physician's diagnoses, supplemented by the identification of drugs brought to the interviewers.

Cognitive function was assessed by trained interviewers using the cognitive score of the Chinese version of the Community Screening Instrument of Dementia (CSI-D), validated in different cultural and educational settings [25]. The cutoff point for probable dementia is 28.4 [25, 26]. The average grip strength was calculated using that of both hands (Jamar Hand dynamometer 5030 J1, Sammons Preston Inc., Bolingbrook, Ill., USA). Subjects were asked to stand up with folded arms from a chair 5 times, and the time required was recorded. The time to walk 6 m at normal pace was measured.

We measured muscle mass by DEXA using a Hologic QDR 2000 densitometer (Hologic, Waltham, Wash., USA). We measured both whole-body and appendicular muscle mass. In our subjects, appendicular skeletal mass (ASM) was calculated by the summation of muscle mass measured in the 4 limbs, with the operator adjusting the cut lines of the limbs according to specific anatomical landmarks as described by Heymsfield et al. [27]. All data used in the analysis were on participants who clearly fitted within the DEXA field of view. During the course of the study, the DEXA systems were regularly matched to quality assurance scans to ensure there was no drift.

Statistical Methods

Men and women were analyzed separately in all statistical tests. The cognitively impaired and nonimpaired subjects as defined by the CSI-D cognitive score were compared by Student's t test for the continuous variables and the χ2 test for the categorical variables. The relationship between performance-based physical function and cognitive impairment was further examined by 3 separate multiple linear regression models. In each model, each of the three physical function results was taken as dependent variable, and cognitive impairment (CSI-D cognitive score >28.4), age, ASM, PASE score, arthritis, diabetes mellitus, stroke and any heart disease were entered as independent variables. To adjust for the effect of body build on muscle mass, the whole procedure was repeated with the covariate ASM replaced by ASM/body weight. All tests were 2-sided, and a p value of less than 0.05 was taken as statistically significant. The statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS version 13.0.

Results

We studied 2,000 men and 2,000 women aged 65 or older, with a mean age of 72.5 ± 5.2 years. All of them were independent in ambulation and community dwellers. The male subjects had a higher education level (p < 0.001), Mini Mental State Examination score (p < 0.001), PASE score (p < 0.001) and muscle mass (p < 0.001; table 1). Therefore all subsequent analyses were done separately for men and women. In both genders, the cognitively impaired group had a weaker grip strength (–5.10 kg in men, p < 0.001; −1.08 kg in women, p < 0.001) and performed worse in the two physical function tests (in men, 6-meter walk speed, −0.13 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 1.42 s, p < 0.001; in women, 6-meter walk speed, −0.08 m/s, p < 0.001, chair stand test, 1.48 s, p < 0.001; table 2). The relationship between physical function and cognitive impairment was further examined by multiple linear regression with adjustment for other covariates. The results were tabulated for men and women in table 3. After adjustment for age, ASM, PASE score and 4 comorbid conditions (heart disease, arthritis, diabetes mellitus and past stroke) that could adversely affect the performance-based tests, cognitively impaired subjects of both genders performed consistently worse in all the 3 tests. Repeating the regression by replacing ASM with ASM/body weight only slightly altered the results. The adjusted and unadjusted differences in the physical function test in both genders were tabulated in table 4.

Table 1.

Comparison of baseline characteristics between men and women

Men (n = 2,000) Women (n = 2,000) p value
Age, years 72.3 ± 5.0 72.5 ± 5.3 0.26
Body weight, kg 62.4 ± 9.3 54.5 ± 8.4 <0.001
Body mass index 23.4 ± 3.1 23.9 ± 3.4 <0.001
Ex- or current smokers, % 63.8 9.5 <0.001
Years of education, % <0.001
 0years 5.2 37.7
 1–6 years 55.2 45.2
 7–12years 26.1 11.3
 >12 years 13.6 6.0
PASEscore 97.2 ± 50.2 85.3 ± 33.1 <0.001
CSI-D cognitive score =28.40, % 4.9 25.3 <0.001
MMSEscore 26.9 ± 2.7 24.2 ± 3.9 <0.001
Whole-body muscle mass, kg 44.2 ± 5.4 33.83 ± 4.1 <0.001
ASM, kg 19.1 ± 2.6 13.81 ± 1.9 <0.001
Arthritis,% 24.3 21.8 0.067
Diabetes mellitus, % 14.7 14.3 0.753
Stroke,% 5.5 3.3 0.001
Heart disease, % 18.3 16.5 0.102

Values are expressed as means ± standard deviation or percentages. MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination. Statistical comparison was made by unpaired Student t test for continuous variables and χ2 test for categorical variables.

Table 2.

Comparison between the cognitively impaired and non-impaired groups

Impaired group Nonimpaired group p value
Age, years
 Men 76.43 ± 0.59 72.18 ± 0.11 <0.001
 Women 74.64 ± 0.26 71.87 ± 0.13 <0.001
Whole-body muscle mass, kg
 Men 42.14 ± 0.56 44.34 ± 0.12 <0.001
 Women 33.69 ± 0.19 33.88 ± 0.12 0.350
ASM, kg
 Men 18.12 ± 0.27 19.23 ± 0.06 <0.001
 Women 13.73 ± 0.08 13.84 ± 0.05 0.264
Grip strength, kg
 Men 26.36 ± 0.71 31.46 ± 0.14 <0.001
 Women 19.44 ± 0.19 20.52 ± 0.11 <0.001
Six-meter walk speed, m/s
 Men 0.89 ± 0.024 1.02 ± 0.004 <0.001
 Women 0.85 ± 0.009 0.93 ± 0.005 <0.001
Chair stand test, s
 Men 13.99 ± 0.45 12.57 ± 0.088 <0.001
 Women 14.45 ± 0.27 13.07 ± 0.12 <0.001
PASEscore
 Men 84.2 ± 5.60 97.9 ± 1.14 0.008
 Women 79.4 ± 1.39 87.3 ± 0.86 <0.001

Values are expressed as means ± standard error. Statistical comparison was made by unpaired Student t test.

Table 3.

Multivariate analysis of performance-based physical function tests

Covariates Mean ± SD/% Unit Mean difference per unit change
Mean difference per unit change
Grip strength, kg Walking speed, m/s Chair stand test, s

adjusted for ASM adjusted for ASM/weight adjusted for ASM adjusted for ASM/weight adjusted for ASM adjusted for ASM/weight
Males
 Cognitive impairment 4.9 yes/no −2.64 (−3.75, −1.53) −3.22 (−4.43, −2.01) −0.07 (−0.11, −0.03) −0.08 (−0.12, −0.03) 0.80 (0.01, 1.58) 0.81 (0.03, 1.6)
 Age, years 72.3 ± 5.0 5.0 −1.52 (−1.78, −1.27) −2.05 (−2.32, −1.79) −0.06 (−0.07, −0.05) −0.06 (−0.07, −0.05) 0.63 (0.45, 0.8) 0.62 (0.45, 0.79)
 PASEscore 97.2 ± 50.2 50.2 −0.46 (0.22, 0.7) −0.53 (0.27, 0.8) 0.03 (0.02, 0.04) 0.03 (0.02, 0.04) −0.36 (−0.53, −0.19) −0.35 (−0.52, −0.17)
 Diabetes 14.7 yes/no −1.15 (−1.83, −0.48) −0.32 (−1.06, 0.42) −0.02 (−0.04, 0.01) −0.01 (−0.04, 0.02) 0.23 (−0.25, 0.71) 0.19 (−0.29, 0.66)
 Stroke 5.5 yes/no −1.91 (−2.96, −0.87) −1.69 (−2.83, −0.55) −0.09 (−0.13, −0.05) −0.09 (−0.13, −0.05) 1.03 (0.29, 1.78) 1.00 (0.26, 1.75)
 Heartdisease 18.3 yes/no −0.36 (−0.98,0.26) −0.17 (−0.51, 0.85) −0.03 (−0.05, −0.003) −0.02 (−0.04, 0.002) 0.53 (0.09, 0.97) 0.48 (0.04, 0.92)
 Arthritis 24.3 yes/no −0.84 (−1.4, −0.29) −0.22 (−0.83, 0.38) −0.02 (−0.04, −0.003) −0.02 (−0.04, 0.002) 0.41 (0.02, 0.81) 0.38 (−0.01, 0.77)
 ASM, kg 19.1 ± 2.6 2.6 −2.53 (2.28, 2.77) 0.02 (0.01, 0.03) −0.03 (−0.2, 0.14)
 ASM/weight 0.31 ± 0.02 0.02 −0.61 (0.39, 0.83) 0.01 (0.002, 0.02) −0.13 (−0.27, 0.01)

Females
 Cognitive impairment 25.3 yes/no −0.47 (−0.85, −0.09) −–0.4 (−0.81, 0.01) −0.05 (−0.07, −0.03) −0.05 (−0.07, −0.03) 0.99 (0.47, 1.5) 1.00 (0.49, 1.52)
 Age, years 72.5 ± 5.3 5.3 −0.81 (−0.98, −0.64) −1.15 (−1.33, −0.97) −0.05 (−0.06, −0.04) −0.05 (−0.06, −0.04) 0.68 (0.44, 0.91) 0.63 (0.4, 0.86)
 PASEscore 85.3 ± 33.1 33.1 −0.30 (0.13, 0.47) −0.37 (0.19, 0.56) 0.03 (0.02, 0.04) 0.03 (0.02, 0.04) −0.38 (−0.61, −0.15) −0.35 (−0.58, −0.12)
 Diabetes 14.3 yes/no −0.77 (−1.23, −0.31) −0.54 (−1.03, −0.04) −0.02 (−0.04, 0.004) −0.02 (−0.05, 0) 0.59 (−0.03, 1.21) 0.64 (0.01, 1.26)
 Stroke 3.3 yes/no −0.78 (−1.68,0.13) −1.07 (−2.04, −0.1) −0.04 (−0.09, 0.01) −0.04 (−0.09, 0.01) 0.92 (−0.31, 2.14) 0.87 (−0.35, 2.1)
 Heart disease 16.5 yes/no −0.36 (−0.79, 0.08) −0.01 (−0.49, 0.46) −0.02 (−0.04, 0.01) −0.01 (−0.04, 0.01) 0.22 (−0.37, 0.81) 0.23 (−0.37, 0.82)
 Arthritis 21.8 yes/no −0.70 (−1.1, −0.31) −0.27 (−0.69, 0.16) −0.05 (−0.07, −0.03) −0.05 (−0.07, −0.03) 1.36 (0.83, 1.89) 1.39 (0.86, 1.92)
 ASM, kg 13.81 ± 1.9 1.9 −1.59 (1.42, 1.75) 0.00 (−0.01, 0.01) 0.24 (0.02, 0.47)
 ASM/weight 0.26 ± 0.02 0.02 −0.41 (0.27, 0.55) 0.01 (−0.0001, 0.01) −0.07 (−0.24, 0.11)

95% confidence limits are given in parentheses.

p < 0.05.

Table 4.

Adjusted and unadjusted physical function test results of the cognitively impaired as compared to the nonimpaired

Unadjusted difference (p value) Adjusted difference1 (p value) Adjusted difference2 (p value)
Grip strength, kg
 Men −5.10 (<0.001) −2.64 (<0.001) −3.21 (<0.001)
 Women −1.08 (<0.001) −0.49 (0.011) −0.42 (0.044)
Six-meter walk speed, m/s
 Men −0.13 (<0.001) −0.072 (0.001) −0.076 (<0.001)
 Women −0.08 (<0.001) −0.049 (<0.001) −0.050 (<0.001)
Chair stand test, s
 Men −1.42 (<0.001) −0.80 (0.045) −0.82 (0.041)
 Women −1.48 (<0.001) −0.98 (<0.001) −1.00 (<0.001)

Statistical adjustment was made by multiple linear regression.

Figures in parentheses are p values.

1

Adjusted for age, ASM, PASE score, arthritis, diabetes mellitus, stroke and any heart disease.

2

Adjusted for age, ASM/body weight, PASE score, arthritis, diabetes mellitus, stroke and any heart disease.

Discussion

We found that poorer physical function coexisted with cognitive impairment, which is compatible with previous observations [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9]. After the adjustment for muscle mass and other possible confounding factors, the difference in physical function performance between the cognitively impaired and nonimpaired groups persisted though the difference had narrowed considerably, illustrating the residual confounding effect of the covariates entered into the regression model, namely muscle mass, age, comorbidities and physical activity level. These adjusted differences in physical function performance, though statistically significant, were relatively modest and may not be translated into meaningful clinical outcomes. Past studies have demonstrated that physical function disturbances might precede the onset of dementia [14,15,16,17]. Based on our results, we propose that impaired physical function such as walking and getting up from a chair in the cognitively impaired might contribute directly to later disabilities, independently of any muscle degeneration associated with dementia. However, it is also possible that the cognitively impaired had difficulty in understanding the instruction of the 3 tests and therefore their performance was adversely affected.

In the present study, over 25% of female subjects were classified as cognitively impaired by the CSI-D cognitive score, which was much higher than the 4.95% in the male counterpart. These results are comparable with two previous surveys conducted in elderly Hong Kong Chinese [28, 29]. The lower education in elderly Hong Kong Chinese women partly explained the discrepancy in prevalence between the genders. Hence, in this study, separate analysis for males and females was performed in order to avoid any confounding effect of the considerable difference in education level.

Although the CSI-D has been used as an instrument to screen for ‘probable dementia’, over 25% prevalence in women might suggest overclassification in the absence of a clinician-based diagnostic process. This might have resulted in a heterogeneous group of women subjects who might not have dementia [29]. As such, we have chosen the term ‘cognitive impairment’ rather than ‘probable dementia’ to describe our subjects. The CSI-D has been developed and tested to be a culturally and educationally unbiased screening tool for dementia with 87% sensitivity and 83% specificity [30] but has not been used as a measure of the severity of cognitive impairment [25, 26]. Therefore, the validated cutoff point was employed in the present study to define cognitive impairment [26]. Nevertheless, the use of a screening tool instead of a clinical diagnosis was a major limitation in this survey.

As to whether reduced physical activity in the cognitively impaired could account for poorer physical function due to deconditioning, we have attempted to eliminate the confounding effect of daily physical activities by adjusting for the PASE score in the multivariate analysis. Our results suggested that impaired muscle strength and physical function were directly related to poor cognition independent of comorbidities, inactivity and muscle mass.

Good physical function preserves muscle mass and prevents cognitive deterioration, as has been revealed by several previous studies [31,32,33]. Being cross-sectional, our results were unable to establish the directionality of the interrelationship among cognition, muscle loss and function. Although we have hypothesized that cognition affects physical function directly and indirectly via muscle degeneration, the results could be interpreted otherwise. It is possible that the interrelationship is even more complex, such as a bidirectional cause-effect relationship or shared etiological pathogenesis in sarcopenia and dementia via sex hormone decline [34,35,36] or cytokine activation [37, 38].

Our sample is the largest reported in older Chinese adults. However, our study was limited by the subjects being recruited by walk-in voluntary participation via recruitment in community centers. As the subjects were more likely to be physically capable and health conscious than the general elderly population, this would affect the generalizability of the findings to the general elderly population.

Conclusion

Poor physical function and muscle strength coexisted with cognitive impairment, independently of muscle mass. It is likely, therefore, that the functional decline in dementia might be related directly to factors resulting in cognitive impairment independently of the coexisting sarcopenia.

Acknowledgement

This study is jointly supported by the Hong Kong Research Grant Council and grant 5R01 AR049439-02 from the National Institute of Health.

References

  • 1.Warren EJ, Grek A, Conn D. A correlation between cognitive performance and daily functioning in elderly people. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1989;2:96–100. doi: 10.1177/089198878900200209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bassett SS, Folstein MF. Cognitive impairment and functional disability in the absence of psychiatric diagnosis. Psychol Med. 1991;21:77–84. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700014677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Grigsby J, Kaye K, Baxter J, Shetterly SM, Hamman RF. Executive cognitive abilities and functional status among community-dwelling older persons in the San Luis Valley Health and Aging Study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1998;46:590–596. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1998.tb01075.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Li CY, Wu SC. Effects of cognitive impairment and loss of physical capacities on survival of the elderly. Neuroepidemiology. 1999;18:322–326. doi: 10.1159/000026227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kuo HK, Leveille SG, Yu YH, Milberg WP. Cognitive function, habitual speed, and late-life disability in the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) 1999–2002. Gerontology. 2007;53:102–110. doi: 10.1159/000096792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rosano C, Simonsick EM, Harris TB, et al. Association between physical and cognitive function in healthy elderly: the Health, Aging and Body Composition Study. Neuroepidemiology. 2005;24:8–14. doi: 10.1159/000081043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fitzpatrick AL, Buchanan CK, Nahin RL, et al. Ginkgo Evaluation of Memory Study Investigators Associations of gait speed and other measures of physical function with cognition in a healthy cohort of elderly persons. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:1244–1251. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.11.1244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Buchman AS, et al. Lower extremity motor function and disability in mild cognitive impairment. Exp Aging Res. 2007;33:355–371. doi: 10.1080/03610730701319210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Franssen EH, Souren LE, Torossian CL, Reisberg B. Equilibrium and limb coordination in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1999;47:463–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb07240.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Aguero-Torres H, Fratiglioni L, Gou Z, Viitanen M, Strauss E, Winblad B. Dementia is the major cause of functional dependence in the elderly: 3-year follow-up data from a population-based study. Am J Public Health. 1998;88:1452–1456. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.10.1452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Raji MA, Kuo Y, Snih SA, Markides KS, Peek KM, Ottenbacher KJ. Cognitive status, muscle strength, and subsequent disability in older Mexican Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53:1462–1468. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53457.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Atkinson HH, Rosano C, Simonsick EM, et al. Cognitive function, gait speed decline, and comorbidities: the health, aging, and body composition study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2007;62:844–850. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.8.844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Black SA, Rush R. Cognitive and functional decline in adults aged 75 and older. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:1978–1986. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50609.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Camicioli R, Howieson D, Oken B, Sexton G, Kaye J. Motor slowing precedes cognitive impairment in the oldest old. Neurology. 1998;50:1496–1498. doi: 10.1212/wnl.50.5.1496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wang L, Larson EB, Bowen JD, van Belle G. Performance-based physical function and future dementia in older people. Arch Intern Med. 2006;166:1115–1120. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Alfaro-Acha A, Snih SA, Raji MA, Kuo YF, Markides KS, Ottenbacher KJ. Handgrip strength and cognitive decline in older Mexican Americans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61A:859–865. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.8.859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Inzitari M, Newman AB, Yaffe K, et al. Gait speed predicts decline in attention and psychomotor speed in older adults: the health aging and body composition study. Neuroepidemiology. 2007;29:156–162. doi: 10.1159/000111577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Nourhashemi F, Andrieu S, Gillette-Guyonnet S, et al. Is there a relationship between fat-free soft tissue mass and low cognitive function? Results from a study of 7,105 women. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:1796–1801. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50507.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Roubenoff R, Hughes VA. Sarcopenia: current concepts. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2000;55A:M716–M724. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.12.m716. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Melton LJ, III, Khosla S, Crowson CS, O'Connor MK, O'Fallon WM, Riggs BL. Epidemiology of sarcopenia. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48:625–630. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Janssen I, Heymsfield SB, Ross R. Low relative skeletal muscle mass (sarcopenia) in older persons is associated with functional impairment and physical disability. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:889–890. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50216.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Rolland Y, Lauwers-Cances V, Cournot M, et al. Sarcopenia, calf circumference, and physical function of elderly women: a cross-sectional study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2003;51:1120–1124. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2003.51362.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Lee JS, Auyeung TW, Kwok T, Lau EM, Leung PC, Woo J. Associated factors and health impact of sarcopenia in older Chinese men and women: a cross-sectional study. Gerontology. 2007;53:166–172. doi: 10.1159/000107355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Washburn RA, Smith KW, Jette AM, Janny CA. The Physical Activity Scale for the Elderly (PASE): development and evaluation. J Clin Epidemiol. 1993;46:153–162. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(93)90053-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Prince M, Acosta D, Chiu H, et al. 10/66 Dementia Research Group Dementia diagnosis in developing countries: a cross cultural validation study. Lancet. 2003;361:909–917. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12772-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Chan TS, Lam LC, Chiu HF, Prince M. Validity and applicability of the Chinese version of Community Screening Instrument for Dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2003;15:10–18. doi: 10.1159/000066672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Heymsfield SB, Smith R, Aulet M, et al. Appendicular skeletal muscle mass: measurement by dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry. Am J Clin Nutr. 1990;52:214–218. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/52.2.214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Woo J, Ho SC, Lau S, Lau J, Yuen YK. Prevalence of cognitive impairment and associated factors among elderly Hong Kong Chinese aged 70 years and over. Neuroepidemiology. 1994;13:50–58. doi: 10.1159/000110358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lam LC, Tam CW, Lui VW, et al. Prevalence of very mild and mild dementia in community-dwelling older Chinese people in Hong Kong. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20:135–148. doi: 10.1017/S1041610207006199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hall KS, Hendrie HH, Brittain HM, et al. The development of a dementia screening interview in two distinct languages. Int J Methods Psychiatric Res. 1993;3:1–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Yaffe K, Barnes D, Nevitt M, Lui LY, Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women. Arch Intern Med. 2001;161:1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lytle ME, Vander BJ, Pandav R, Dodge HH, Ganguli M. Exercise level and cognitive decline: the MoVIES project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004;18:57–64. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000126614.87955.79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292:1454–1461. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Baumgartner RN, Waters DL, Gallagher D, Morley JE, Garry PJ. Predictors of skeletal muscle mass in elderly men and women. Mech Ageing Dev. 1999;107:123–136. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(98)00130-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yaffe K, Lui LY, Zmuda J, et al. Sex hormones and cognitive function in older men. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002;50:707–712. doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50166.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Iannuzzi-Sucich M, Prestwood KM, Kenny AM. Prevalence of sarcopenia and predictors of skeletal muscle mass in healthy, older men and women. J Gerontol Med Sci. 2002;57A:M772–M777. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.12.m772. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Visser M, Pahor M, Taaffe DR, et al. Relationship of interleukin-6 and tumor necrosis factor with muscle mass and muscle strength in elderly men and women: the Health ABC Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2002;57A:M326–M332. doi: 10.1093/gerona/57.5.m326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Weaver JD, Huang MH, Albert M, et al. Interleukin-6 and risk of cognitive decline. MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Neurology. 2002;13:371–378. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.3.371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Neuroepidemiology are provided here courtesy of Karger Publishers

RESOURCES