Skip to main content
Health Promotion International logoLink to Health Promotion International
. 2009 Oct 29;25(1):14–23. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dap048

Testing the hierarchy of effects model: ParticipACTION's serial mass communication campaigns on physical activity in Canada

C L Craig 1,2,*, A Bauman 2,1, B Reger-Nash 3
PMCID: PMC2824601  PMID: 19875461

SUMMARY

The hierarchy of effects (HOE) model is often used in planning mass-reach communication campaigns to promote health, but has rarely been empirically tested. This paper examines Canada's 30 year ParticipACTION campaign to promote physical activity (PA). A cohort from the nationally representative 1981 Canada Fitness Survey was followed up in 1988 and 2002–2004. Modelling of these data tested whether the mechanisms of campaign effects followed the theoretical framework proposed in the HOE. Campaign awareness was measured in 1981. Outcome expectancy, attitudes, decision balance and future intention were asked in 1988. PA was assessed at all time points. Logistic regression was used to sequentially test mediating and moderating variables adjusting for age, sex and education. No selection bias was observed; however, relatively fewer respondents than non-respondents smoked or were underweight at baseline. Among those inactive at baseline, campaign awareness predicted outcome expectancy which in turn predicted positive attitude to PA. Positive attitudes predicted high decision balance, which predicted future intention. Future intention mediated the relationship between decision balance and sufficient activity. Among those sufficiently active at baseline, awareness was unrelated to outcome expectancy and inversely related to positive attitude. These results lend support to the HOE model, in that the effects of ParticipACTION's serial mass media campaigns were consistent with the sequential rollout of its messages, which in turn was associated with achieving an active lifestyle among those initially insufficiently active. This provides support to an often-used theoretical framework for designing health promotion media campaigns.

Keywords: mass media, community intervention, population based, social marketing

INTRODUCTION

The widespread recognition of the health risks and associated public health burden of low levels of physical activity (PA) has stimulated the development of population-wide health promotion efforts. Informational strategies are an initial component of a comprehensive program for PA, by motivating and encouraging people to become active and providing tips on overcoming barriers to increasing PA (Task Force on Community Preventive Services, 2002). Other community-based approaches that have been found to be effective range from ‘point of decision prompts’ through complex multi-faceted community-wide interventions with multiple strategies and targeting changes at the individual, interpersonal, community/setting and societal/organizational levels (Task Force on Community Preventive Services, 2002). The effects associated with community-wide health promotion programs with a mass communication component (Reger-Nash et al., 2008) and national mass media campaigns (Booth et al., 1992; Wimbush et al., 1998; Hillsdon et al., 2001; Bauman et al., 2003; Cavill and Bauman, 2004; Craig et al., 2007) illustrate the utility of periodic mass health communication efforts as one in a mix of strategies to increase PA. Adopting and maintaining a physically active lifestyle is a complex behaviour change process and mass communication campaigns have been employed in an attempt to initially encourage individuals in this process. Understanding the impact of mass campaigns requires further exploration of some of the theory underpinning the putative outcomes of mass-reach communications campaigns. An opportunity to do this exists in Canada, where ParticipACTION was launched in 1971 ‘to motivate all Canadians be more active and to improve general levels of fitness over the long term’ (Edwards, 2004). ParticipACTION is one of the longest running mass health promotion campaigns in the world, with messages appearing regularly on 350 television and cable stations, and an estimated 580 radio stations, and various print media including 110 daily and 950 community newspapers (Edwards, 2004).

In 1971, social marketing for health was not well developed as a discipline (Kotler and Zaltman, 1971), although it is now well established in health promotion (Smith et al., 2006). Moreover, mass campaign evaluation strategies are now well developed (Cavill and Bauman, 2004; Bauman et al., 2006). One proposed model of media campaign effects is that they result, at least in part, from a ‘cascade’, from initial short-term campaign awareness through intermediate effects on perceptions, attitudes and beliefs, that are dependent on the messaging strategy (McGuire, 1984; Cavill and Bauman, 2004). Finally, long-term effects include trialing PA, and the long-term adoption and maintenance of an active lifestyle. This model is widely used in mass media campaign planning, but has seldom been empirically tested in population-level research.

A post hoc analysis described the sequencing of ParticipACTION's mass media messages during its first two decades of operation (ParticipACTION Archive). In the initial phase, 1973–1975, the main focus of the campaign was to influence outcome expectancy through ParticipACTION's tagline, ‘the Canadian movement for personal fitness’, and its messages (Table 1). Messages were also designed to influence knowledge and social norms. In the late 1970s, campaigns reinforced outcome expectancy and social norms. However, despite a few messages such as ‘low cost’ and depictions of adults playing like kids, it was primarily the ‘Walk a block a day’, and ‘Fitness now’ campaigns in the mid-late 1970s that attempted to influence attitudes and decision balance for PA. With the inclusion of a few messages to shape knowledge, the mid-phase from 1981–1985, represented a call to action through its 3-year long ‘Do it! Campaign’. It paired the overall ‘do it’ message with messages reinforcing social outcomes and attitudes. In summary, the sequencing of messages through the rollout of ParticipACTION's campaigns could be theoretically characterized as ‘campaign awareness’ leading to outcome expectancy, in turn leading to positive attitudes, then decision balance and finally intention to be active.

Table 1:

Constructs associated with ParticipACTION's television and radio messages

Campaigna Messages Probable constructb
60 year Swede 1973 Poor fitness level of Canadians; Get Canada moving Outcome expectancy
Canada's kids 1973 Fitness decreases as early as 6; Obesity & postural defects increasing Heath beliefs
Canadian movement for personal fitness; Get moving Outcome expectancy
Food for thought 1974 Get into shape & your body works better; Movement for personal fitness, you will be a better person for it Outcome expectancy
Word association 1974 Overcome fear of exercise; Low cost; Ways you can like it Attitude
Many ways to get active and be healthy Decision balance
Movement for personal fitness you will be a better person for it Outcome expectancy
Road Hockey (1975) Adult playing and having fun like a child Attitude
Sleep better and feel better Outcome expectancy
Motivated 1975 Squeezing in PA is a question of motivation–Funeral director ad Intention
Walk a block 1975 Walk a block a day it's a beginning; It's an easy way to get a little fitter Attitude
Taxi ad—convenient; Standard block ad—easy Decision balance
Walking the beat ad—exercises legs and that exercises the heart Outcome expectancy
Tennis racket 1976 Exercise 3–4 times a week Knowledge
Getting in shape is not as hard as you think Attitude
Scream 1976 Get active 3 times a week;Get fit feel better; Knowledge
Scream to feel better Outcome expectancy
Fat to fit 1979 Go to fit from fat; Be active and get into shape Outcome expectancy
Canadians are into fitness now Social norm
Fitness now 1979 (rollerskating) Hate exercise—take up something you like Decision balance
Canadians are into fitness now Social norm
Harvey lasy bones 1981 Get back into action; Get back into shape Outcome expectancy
Don't just think about it do it 1983–1985 Don't just think about it do it; Let's see you do it; Get with the action Intention (Call to action)
Do it for your muscles; You look strong and healthy Outcome expectancy
Not fatiguing Attitude
Challenge, 1985 Our community, fitness in the nation Social norm
Have fun Attitude
We can win-let's see us, do it Intention
Skating 1984 Canada's outdoors the greatest fitness facility in the world Social norm
Let's see you do it. Intention

aParticipACTION Archive Project.

bThe construct was determined post hoc by examining the content of the message.

ParticipACTION's name and logo are recognized by most Canadian adults (Bauman et al., 2004). Yet it is not known if the campaign impact was limited to those receptive to its promotional messages, such as those already more active. Therefore, the purpose of this paper is to examine the communication effects proposed by the hierarchy of effects (HOE) model associated with ParticipACTION's sequential introduction of campaign messages, and whether any effects on PA behaviour and its antecedents were observed among those insufficiently and sufficiently physically active at baseline.

METHODS

Sample

Participants were respondents of the 1981 Canada Fitness Survey (CFS) and its two prospective follow-ups, the 1988 Campbell Survey on Well-Being in Canada (CSWB) and the September 2002 to April 2004 Physical Activity Longitudinal Study (PALS). Details of the sampling method, protocol, attrition and potential biases have been published elsewhere (Craig et al., 2005). Briefly, the CFS (n = 23 500 aged 7 and older) was a randomly selected multi-stage, cluster sample of the non-institutionalized population. The PALS cohort was established as a 20% subsample of CFS participants. As such, the CSWB follow-up (n = 4200) was also a randomly selected multi-stage, cluster sample of the non-institutionalized population. Respondents to both the CFS and CSWB completed self-administered questionnaires and fitness measures conducted within their homes. Subsequent to the CSWB, new family members were added to the cohort. The current study was limited to those 13 years or older in 1981 who answered all pertinent 1981 (n = 3313) and 1988 questions (n = 2462). Of these participants, 1247 responded in 2002–2004 to a mailed survey. Although no selection bias was evident, examination of the follow-up data revealed that fewer respondents smoked or were underweight at baseline compared to non-respondents (Craig et al., 2005). PALS was approved by the Ethics Review Board of the University of Montreal. In 1981, 1988 and 2002–2004, informed written consent was obtained either from a parent or guardian or directly from participants 18 years and older.

Measures

Awareness of ParticipACTION was asked as prompted recall of its logo (Have you ever seen this logo? Yes/no/don't know) reproduced in the 1981 self-administered questionnaire. In 1988, additional questions operationalizing outcome expectancy, attitudes, decision balance and future intention were asked by self-administered questionnaire. Outcome expectancy was assessed by ‘How much does (or would) participation in vigorous physical activity help you to’, followed by 17 itemized outcomes on a Likert scale from 1 ‘a great deal’ to 5 not at all’ (Stephens and Craig, 1990). The six items that probed aspects contained in ParticipACTION's messages (feel better physically; feel better mentally; improve/maintain overall fitness; improve/maintain cardiovascular fitness; improve/maintain muscular strength and endurance; improve/maintain flexibility) were summed to create an overall score (Cronbach α = 0.92) and categorized as high (≤12, equivalent to providing the highest or second highest rating on the 5-point scale for each of the six items), medium (13–23) or low (≥24). Attitudes were characterized using a 5-point scale with bipolar anchors (fun/boring; harmful/beneficial; pleasant/unpleasant; inconvenient/convenient; painful/not painful; difficult/easy). Responses were dichotomized as positive (1 or 5 depending on the item) or not and a sum of at least four positive items were considered a high positive attitude (Cronbach α = 0.77). High decision balance was defined as strong agreement with the statement ‘if you wanted to, you could easily participate in vigorous PA 3 or more times a week for at least 20 minutes at a time’. Finally, future intention to participate in vigorous PA in the following year was asked (never; less than once a week; one to two times per week; three times per week, four to five times per week; six or more times a week).

PA was asked using adaptations of the Minnesota Leisure-Time Physical Activity Questionnaire (Taylor et al., 1978) with reasonable criterion validity and good one month test–retest reliability (Craig et al., 2002). The follow-up version reduced the number of activities probed, eliminated the use of a prompt card to aid in the recall of physical activities and changed the recall of occasions from number per month for each of the 12 previous months to number in the year, but these change did not impact comparability of the instruments (Craig et al., 2002). A weekly MET-minute score was calculated by summing the products of occasions by duration per occasion in minutes by the standard metabolic cost of each activity reported and then dividing by 52 (Craig et al., 2004). ‘Sufficient physical activity’ was defined as accumulating at least 1260 MET-minutes, an amount equivalent to 1 h of moderate-intensity walking daily (Craig et al., 2004).This cut point is consistent with the hour a day of PA recommended in Canada's Physical Activity Guideline, but higher than the widely used minimal recommendation of at least ‘30 minute of moderate intensity activity on 5 days a week’ (Haskell et al., 2007).

Participant's age at baseline in 1981 was categorized as: 13–24 years; 25–44 years and 45 years and older. Educational attainment was classified as the highest level of education reported by follow-up in 2002–2004 (less than secondary, secondary, some post secondary, university).

Analysis

Logistic regression was used to test the cascade of communication effects according to the rollout of ParticipACTION messages, with age, sex and education as covariates. In order to examine the individual effects posited by the model, a series of sequential models was tested for those ‘insufficiently active for health’ and for those ‘sufficiently active’ at baseline. Subsequently, moderation and mediation of effects among all participants were tested using the method of Baron and Kenny (Baron and Kenny, 1986). A variable was tested as a moderator by determining if there was a significant interaction effect between it and the preceding predictor in the model. A variable was determined to be a mediator if: (i) the proposed mediator was a significant predictor of the subsequent variable in the model; (ii) the preceding variable in the model predicted both the proposed mediator and the subsequent variable in the model and (iii) when including both the preceding variable and the proposed mediator in the model, the preceding variable was no longer a predictor. All analysis was performed using SPSS version 15 with Holm adjustments applied during inference to limit the occurrence of Type 1 errors due to the number of tests required (Aickin and Gensler, 1996; Bender and Lange, 2001). The reported results were based on all data points available; results did not differ when restricted to participants with data at all time periods.

RESULTS

In total, 1106 males and 1356 females participated in the 1988 follow-up. These represented 692 individuals aged 13–24 years; 1138 aged 25–44 years and 632 aged 45 years and older. The majority had some post-secondary (n = 854) or university education (n = 614) with 446 having secondary and 470 having less than secondary education. Similar patterns in the distribution of characteristics were seen in 2002–2004 with respect to sex (n = 495 males, 631 females), but the distribution of baseline age group (n = 338 aged 13–24 years; 610 aged 25–44 years; 178 aged 45 years and older) and education (131, less than secondary; 190, secondary; 439, some post-secondary, 361, university) reflected the higher relative loss to follow-up of those in the 45 years and older age group.

Table 2 shows the likelihood of being aware of ParticipACTION at baseline, intending to be active regularly in the 12 months following 1988 and of being sufficiently active in 2002–2004, adjusting for demographic factors. At baseline, the likelihood of recognizing ParticipACTION's logo was lower among those older than 25 years compared to the youngest age group and was higher among those who completed at least secondary school. A similar pattern was found in 1988 with respect to intention to participate in vigorous activities at least three times weekly. In the 2002–2004 follow-up survey, females were 27% less likely than males to accumulate sufficient activity, but no relationship with age at baseline or educational attainment was observed.

Table 2:

Socio-demographic correlates of 1981 awareness, 1988 intention and 2003 physical activity level

Aware of ParticipACTION 1981 (n = 2462)
Intend to be active 3 or more times weekly 1988 (n = 2462)
Sufficiently active
2003 (n = 1126)
% AORa % AORa % AORa
Males 87 Referent 40 38
Females 88 1.13 (0.85, 1.50) 39 1.02 (0.87, 1.20) 30 0.73 (0.57, 0.93)
Age group in 1981
 13–24 years 97 Referent 47 34
 25–44 years 93 0.46 (0.27, 0.76) 40 0.74 (0.62, 0.92) 34 0.98 (0.74, 1.30)
 45 years and older 73 0.14 (0.08, 0.23) 31 0.62 (0.48, 0.79) 33 0.96 (0.64 1.43)
Education
 <secondary 75 Referent 28 29
 Secondary 90 1.85 (1.21, 2.83) 37 1.33 (0.99 1.76) 30 0.88 (0.54, 1.44)
Some post secondary 92 2.08 (1.44, 3.01) 41 1.54 (1.20, 1.99) 32 0.95 (0.62, 1.46)
 University 92 1.79 (1.19, 2.69) 48 2.06 ( 1.58, 2.69) 40 1.36 (0.88, 2.10)

aAdjusted for age, sex and educational attainment.

Most participants recalled the ParticipACTION logo at baseline and had high outcome expectancy scores at the first follow-up (Table 3). A higher percentage of initial, intermediate and longer term outcomes were observed among those sufficiently active compared to those insufficiently active at baseline in 1981. This was true for all outcomes: 1981 logo recall; 1988 high outcome expectancy; high positive attitude; high decision balance score, intention to be active at least three times a week in the subsequent year and sufficient activity at follow-up (2002–2004).

Table 3:

Potential communication effects among those insufficiently and sufficiently active at baseline

Insufficiently active at baseline (1981) % (95% CI) Sufficiently active at baseline (1981) % (95% CI)
Recalled ParticipACTION logo (1981) 89.8 (88.4, 91.1) 92.6 (90.2, 94.5)
High outcome expectancy score (1988) 71.8 (69.7, 73.8) 78.8 (75.4, 81.9)
High positive attitude (1988) 17.0 (15.4, 18.8) 23.9 (20.6, 27.5)
High decision balance score (1988) 41.8 (39.6, 44.1) 52.8 (48.7, 56.8)
Intend to be active in vigorous activity ≥3 weekly (1988) 35.7 (33.5, 37.9) 53.6 (49.6, 57.6)
Sufficiently activity at follow-up (2002–2004) 29.4 (26.5, 32.6) 48.4 (42.5, 54.3)

Table 4 shows the cascade of communication effects analysed separately for those ‘insufficiently’ and ‘sufficiently’ active at baseline. Each of the following relationships among those ‘insufficiently active’ at baseline were significant: 1981 awareness led to outcome expectancy that predicted positive attitude, then decision balance, then intention and finally led from intention to ‘sufficient activity’ (2002–2004). In contrast, among those sufficiently active at baseline, awareness did not predict outcome expectancy, nor did outcome expectancy predict positive attitude. However, all other relationships further down the hypothesized cascade of effects were significant. As social norm was not measured, the pathways from awareness to social norms and subsequent predictors could not be tested.

Table 4:

Cascading communication effects among those insufficiently and sufficiently active at 1981 baseline

Communication effects among those insufficiently active (1981) Communication effects among those sufficiently active (1981)
AORa (95% CI) AORa (95% CI)
Aware (1981)→Outcome expectancy (1988) (n = 1866) (n = 595)
 Not aware Referent
 Moderate outcome expectancy 1.7 (0.87, 3.36) 1.7 (0.35, 8.67)
 High outcome expectancy 1.9 (1.01. 3.56)*,** 3.3 (0.76, 14.20)
Outcome expectancy (1988)→positive attitude (1988) (n = 1866) (n = 595)
 Low outcome score Referent
 Moderate outcome score 3.8 (0.90, 16.33) 0.5 (0.12, 2.31)
 High outcome score 11.2 (2.71, 46.59)**,*** 1.6 (0.39, 6.20)
Positive attitude (1988)→decision balance (1988) (n = 1866) (n = 595)
 Lower attitude score Referent
 High positive attitude score 2.9 (2.24–3.74)**,**** 3.3 (2.12, 5.00)**,****
Decision balance (1988)→intention (1988) (n = 1866) (n = 595)
 Lower decision balance score Referent
 High decision balance score 5.1 (4.07, 6.16)**,**** 3.5 (2.44, 4.89)**,****
Intention (1988)→sufficiently active (2002–4) (n = 1404) (n = 480)
 Less than 3 times weekly Referent
 3 or more times weekly 2.1 (1.59, 2.89)**,**** 4.0 (2.32, 6.74)**,****

aAdjusted for age, sex and educational attainment.

*p<0.05 prior to Holm's adjustment for multiple comparisons.

**p<0.05 after Holm's adjustment for multiple comparison.

***p<0.001 prior to Holm's adjustment for multiple comparisons.

****p=0.000 prior to Holm's adjustment for multiple comparisons.

Figure 1 summarizes these communication effects, and associates them with the timing of ParticipACTION's campaigns, after testing each variable in the cascade for moderation and mediation. All variables were independent predictors and none were moderators. (That is, outcome expectancy and positive attitudes were independent predictors of high decision balance with no interaction effect between the two; outcome expectancy and positive attitudes were significant predictors of future intention with no interaction; and so on.) As a result of testing mediation, it was found that positive attitudes did not mediate the relationship between high outcome expectancy and high decision balance. Similarly, neither positive attitudes nor high decision balance mediated the relationship between high outcome expectancy and future intention. Furthermore, having a high decision balance did not mediate the association between having a high positive attitude and future intention to be active. However, future intention (1988) mediated the relationship between a high decision balance score in 1988 and being sufficiently activity in 2002–2004 (decision balance→sufficient activity, AOR 1.47, 95% CI 1.14–1.88, p < 0.003); (decision balance→sufficient activity controlling for future intention, AOR 1.03, 95% CI 0.78–1.35, p = 0.85).

Fig. 1:

Fig. 1:

Post hoc analysis of the hierarchy-of-effects model associated with ParticipACTION's messaging.

DISCUSSION

This study examined an often-used but little tested theoretical HOE model, which proposes a cascade of cognitive and behavioural effects following mass-reach social marketing campaigns (McGuire, 1984; Cavill and Bauman, 2004; Bauman et al., 2008). This is an important contribution to understanding the theory underpinning effects observed following health promotion campaigns. We found that the observed relationships between variables associated with ParticipACTION's campaign timing lend some support to the model of cascading communication effects. ParticipACTION sequentially introduced new messages while reinforcing earlier messages. It is not surprising, therefore, to observe relationships between earlier variables in the cascade and ones further down the hierarchy. It is interested to note that a model of cascading communication effects was a better fit among the target audience, those ‘insufficiently active’ at baseline, than among those already sufficiently active.

One concern in mass communications to increase PA may be whether the communication effects are observed among the primary target audience, those who are insufficiently active, or only among those already active. At baseline, recall of ParticipACTION was quite high among both the insufficiently and sufficiently active. This suggests that ParticipACTION campaign was effective in reaching all Canadians so that those recalling the messages were not simply those ‘predisposed’ to paying attention to a message about PA. The subsequent pattern of communication effects was similar between these two population groups; however, only the effects in the hierarchy from positive attitude to high decision balance through future intention to sufficient activity at follow-up were significant among those sufficiently active at baseline. The lack of relationship between recall and outcome expectancy and outcome expectancy and positive attitude may be due in part to the smaller sample size. Other possible explanations may include potentially higher levels of outcome expectancy and positive attitude among those active so that messaging to overcome barriers and alter elements of decision balance may have been more relevant to this audience and therefore represented a more appropriate starting point for assessing the cascade of effects. The appropriate targeting of initial messages to various population segments lies outside the realm of this study and warrants future investigation.

Similar to these results, earlier evaluations of campaigns to increase PA among adults found changes in the proximal communication effects posited by the HOE model (Bauman et al., 2001; Hillsdon et al., 2001; Reger-Nash et al., 2002; Craig et al., 2006). In Australia (Bauman et al., 2001) and the USA (Reger-Nash et al., 2002), campaigns have demonstrated changes in awareness, knowledge of PA and sometimes, in short-term PA. In the UK, increases in knowledge were associated with campaign timing, but were not directly attributable to recall of the campaign (Hillsdon et al., 2001), whereas in Scotland (Wimbush et al., 1998) changes were observed in beliefs as well as knowledge and these were associated with recall. In Canada, awareness and ownership of pedometers was associated with recall of a campaign to increase walking through pedometer use (Craig et al., 2006). In contrast, findings related to the more distal impact of campaigns on PA are equivocal: increases in walking were found in an earlier Australian campaign (Booth et al., 1992) and those in Canada (Craig et al., 2007) and the USA (Reger-Nash et al., 2002), but not in the later Australian campaign (Bauman et al., 2001), and not the ones in the UK (Wimbush et al., 1998; Hillsdon et al., 2001). However, these studies were not designed as a test of HOE. One of the two previous studies testing the HOE found some support for the model in the evaluation of the VERB campaign targeting young adolescents (Bauman et al., 2008). Among this group of young teenagers, awareness led to an increase in understanding, which in turn led directly to behaviour change bypassing other hypothesized mediators in a cascade of effects model. An evaluation of the adoption of family planning methods in Tanzania found that a cascade of effects model was useful in determining the effects of the campaign in moving groups of people through a stages of change process toward adoption (Vaughn and Rogers, 2000). The present study further adds to this literature by examining the applicability of the HOE in evaluating communication effects associated with individual adult behaviour change resulting from a long-term mass media campaign in Canada.

One limitation of the study was the length of time between exposure to the messaging and follow-up. For example, among those who were sufficiently active at baseline, no relationship was observed between awareness of the early campaigns and its early messaging. It may be that those sufficiently active at baseline already had a higher degree of outcome expectancy associated with PA than those insufficiently active and that the early messages served to reinforce their existing outcome expectancies rather than shape them. Alternatively, it is possible that those active at baseline were positively influenced by the early campaigns prior to measurement of the variables. In addition, the vigorous activity message that permeated society in the 1980s and was reflected in the 1988 items was later superseded by national recommendations of more moderate-intensity PA (Health Canada & Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology, 1998; Haskell, 2007). Therefore, the results provide support for potential mediation of intervening variables, but more frequent follow-up assessments would have been needed for definitive assessment of mediation.

Although its initial focus was to improve fitness, as the ParticipACTION's goal suggests, campaign messages were primarily promoting participation in PA. Messages in the first two decades were to portray PA as ‘fun’ with only a few disease-oriented messages. Therefore, most Canadians would have understood the brand as a single behaviourally focused and recognizable campaign (Bauman et al., 2004).

Increasing individual motivation through sustained mass communication campaigns is not the only pathway to becoming more active. Ecological models provide a framework for understanding the interrelationships of individual, social and physical environmental factors resulting in a myriad of other pathways influencing PA levels. As a result of this complex interplay of messages, strategies and programs in Canada, reported leisure time PA has increased in Canada over the 20-year period covered by this study (Craig et al., 2004). Given this shift and the cascade of potential communication effects observed particularly among the insufficiently active, it appears that sequential messages may have contributed, at least in part, to the overall changes in population-level PA. This supports the contention that media messages may positively contribute to sustained efforts at achieving population-level change (Cavill and Bauman, 2004). These findings further suggest that the HOE model may indeed be useful in planning and evaluating serial population-level health promotion campaigns, and help us better understand the ways in which sustained social marketing campaigns may influence population awareness of an issue, create a predisposition to change and contribute to increased levels of PA, especially for those insufficiently active.

Conflict of interest. C.L.C. been involved on various committees and advisory groups for ParticipACTION. The Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research Institute (CFLRI) was previously contracted by the ParticipACTION to perform research and surveillance—C.L.C. is President of CFLRI but received no personal compensation for the contracted work.

FUNDING

This study was funded in part by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Strategic Joint Initiative Grant on Society, Culture and the Health of Canadians II Grant No. 839-2000-1032) and by a grant from the National Cancer Institute (United States, R03 CA117549-01). The 1981 Canada Fitness Survey was supported by Fitness and Amateur Sport (now the Physical Activity Unit of the Public Health Agency of Canada) and the 1988 Campbell's Survey on Well Being was funded by Fitness and Amateur Sport, the National Health Research and Development Program of Health Canada (Grant No. 6606-3217-46), and the Campbell Soup Company Ltd.

REFERENCES

  1. Aickin M., Gensler J. Adjusting for multiple testing when reporting research ressults: the Bonferroni vs Holm methods. American Journal of Public Health. 1996;86:726–728. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.5.726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baron R. M., Kenny D. A. The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1986;51:1173–1182. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.51.6.1173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bauman A. E., Bellew B., Owen N., Vita P. Impact of an Australian Mass Media Campaign Targeting Physical Activity in 1998. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2001;21:41–47. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(01)00313-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauman A., Armstrong T., Davies J., Owen N., Brown W., Bellew B., et al. Trends in physical activity participation and the impact of integrated campaigns among Australian adults, 1997–99. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health. 2003;27:76–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2003.tb00384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bauman A., Madill J., Craig C. L., Salmon A. ParticipACTION: this mouse roared, but did it get the Cheese? Canadian Journal of Public Health. 2004;95(Suppl. 2):S14–S19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bauman A., Smith B. J., Maibach E. W., Reger-Nash B. Evaluation of mass media campaigns for physical activity. Evaluation and Program Planning. 2006;29:312–322. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bauman A., Bowles H. R., Huhman M. Testing a hierarchy-of-effects model: pathways from awareness to outcomes in the VERB(TM) Campaign: 2002–2003. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2008;34:S249–S256. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bender R., Lange S. Adjusting for multiple testing—when and how? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology. 2001;54:33–349. doi: 10.1016/s0895-4356(00)00314-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Booth M., Bauman A., Oldenburg B., Owen N., Magnus P. Effects of a national mass-media campaign on physical activity participation. Health Promotion International. 1992;7:241–247. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cavill N., Bauman A. Changing the way people think about health-enhancing physical activity: do mass media campaigns have a role? Journal of Sports Sciences. 2004;22:771–790. doi: 10.1080/02640410410001712467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Craig C. L., Russell S. J., Cameron C. Reliability and validity of Canada's physical activity monitor for assessing trends. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise. 2002;34:1462–1467. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200209000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Craig C. L., Russell S. J., Cameron C., Bauman A. Twenty year trends of physical activity among Canadian adults. Canadian Journal of Public Health. 2004;95:59–63. doi: 10.1007/BF03403636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Craig C. L., Gauvin L., Cragg S. E., Katzmarzyk P. T., Stephens T. M., Russell S. J., et al. Towards a social epidemiological perspective on physical activity and health: the aims, design, and methods of the Physical Activity Longitudinal Study (PALS) Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2005;3:272–284. [Google Scholar]
  14. Craig C. L., Cragg S. E., Tudor-Locke C., Bauman A. Proximal impact of Canada on the Move: The relationship of campaign awareness to pedometer ownership and use. Canadian Journal Public Health. 2006;97(Suppl. 1):S22–S27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Craig C. L., Tudor-Locke C., Bauman A. Twelve-month effects of Canada on the move: a population-wide campaign to promote pedometer use and walking. Health Education Research. 2007;22:406–413. doi: 10.1093/her/cyl093. Epub 13 September 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Edwards P. No country mouse: thirty years of effective marketing and health communications. Canadian Journal of Public Health 2004. 2004;95(Suppl. 2):S6–S13. doi: 10.1007/BF03403997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Haskell W. L., Lee I. M., Pate R. R., Powell K. E., Blair S. N., Franklin B. A., et al. Physical activity and public health: updated recommendation for adults from the American College of Sports Medicine and the American Heart Association. Circulation. 2007;116:1081–1093. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.107.185649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Health Canada & Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology. Canada's Physical Activity Guide to Healthy Active Living. 1998 Government of Canada. Cat No. H39-429/1998-1. ISBN 0-662-86627-7. http://www.phac-aspc.gc.ca/pau-uap/paguide/index.html . [Google Scholar]
  19. Hillsdon M., Cavill N., Nanchahal K., Diamond A., White I. R. National level promotion of physical activity: results from England's ACTIVE for LIFE Campaign. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health. 2001;55:755–761. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.10.755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Kotler P., Zaltman G. Social marketing: an approach to planned social change. Journal of Marketing. 1971;35:3–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. McGuire W. J. Public communication as a strategy for inducing health-promoting behavioral change. Preventive Medicine. 1984;13:299–319. doi: 10.1016/0091-7435(84)90086-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. ParticipACTION Archive. www.usask.ca/archives/ParticipACTION/english/home.html . [Google Scholar]
  23. Reger B., Cooper L., Booth-Butterfield S., Smith H., Bauman A., Wootan M., et al. Wheeling walks: a community campaign using paid media to encourage walking among sedentary older adults. Preventive Medicine. 2002;35:285–292. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2002.1074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Reger-Nash B., Bauman A., Cooper L., Chey T., Simon K. J., Brann M., et al. WV walks: replication with expanded reach. Journal of Physical Activity and Health. 2008;5:19–27. doi: 10.1123/jpah.5.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Smith B. J., Tang K. C., Nutbeam D. WHO Health Promotion Glossary: new terms. Health Promotion International. 2006;21:340–345. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dal033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Stephens T. M., Craig C. L. The Well-Being of Canadians: Highlights of the 1988 Campbell's Survey. Ottawa: Canadian Fitness and Lifestyle Research Institute; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  27. Task Force on Community Preventive Services. Recommendations to Increase Physical Activity in Communities. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2002;22:67–72. [Google Scholar]
  28. Taylor H. L., Jacobs D. R., Jr., Schucker B., Knudsen J., Leon A. S., Debacker G., et al. A questionnaire for the assessment of leisure-time physical activities. Journal of Chronic Disease. 1978;31:741–775. doi: 10.1016/0021-9681(78)90058-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Vaughan P. W., Rogers E. M. A staged model of communication effects: Evidence from an Entertainment–Education Radio Soap Opera in Tanzania. Journal of Health Communication. 2000;5:203–227. doi: 10.1080/10810730050131398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Wimbush E., MacGregor A., Fraser E. Impacts of a national mass media campaign on walking in Scotland. Health Promotion International. 1998;13:45–53. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Health Promotion International are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES