Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Feb 23.
Published in final edited form as: J Fam Psychol. 2009 Jun;23(3):355–363. doi: 10.1037/a0015399

Child and Mother Play in Three U.S. Cultural Groups: Comparisons and Associations

Linda R Cote 1,2, Marc H Bornstein 1
PMCID: PMC2827484  NIHMSID: NIHMS170743  PMID: 19586198

Abstract

Child and mother play (n = 113 20-month-olds) among Latino immigrants from South America, Japanese immigrants, and European Americans in the United States was investigated. Culturally universal patterns of play dominated the findings. For example, no cultural differences in the prevalence of exploratory or symbolic play were found for either children or their mothers. Regardless of their culture, boys engaged in significantly more exploratory and less symbolic play than girls when they played by themselves. Few relations between child play in the two play sessions were found. Across cultural groups, children’s exploratory play was significantly positively related to both maternal demonstrations and solicitations of exploratory play. The results identify which realms of child growth, parenting, and family function call for special attention and cultural sensitivity, as well as which do not, in the dynamics of immigrant families.

Child and Mother Play in Three U.S. Cultural Groups: Comparisons and Associations

Observation of mothers and children engaged in universal, developmentally appropriate activities can help to reveal cultural influences on children’s development (Rogoff, 2003). Play constitutes such an activity during the second year of life. Cross-cultural research on children's play highlights the importance of culture-specific practices in structuring and organizing the environment in which children's social interactions and play activities take place (see Bornstein, Haynes, Pascual, Painter, & Galperín, 1999; Farver, 1999; Göncü, Tuermer, Jain, & Johnson, 1999; Haight, Wang, Fung, Williams, & Mintz, 1999).

During the second year of life children’s play normatively moves from being exploratory and object-oriented to symbolic and pretense-oriented. During the sensorimotor period, children’s initial interactions with objects are exploratory in nature, and their schemas characterize objects (for example, a ball) as something simply to be physically manipulated (Piaget, 1962). However, as children begin to engage in symbolic play, they use familiar objects in new ways, for example, by pretending that a ball is an orange that they are eating. Although there are individual differences in play development and related cognitive abilities, across cultures achieving representational competence is one of the principal developmental tasks of the second year of life (Gönçü & Gaskins, 2007; Haight et al., 1999; Piaget, 1962). However, cultural variation in play is also evident (Bornstein, 2007). Immigrant families may find culture-specific aspects of play in their culture of destination particularly challenging or perplexing during the process of acculturation, and culture-specific aspects of play from the immigrants’ culture of origin may be interpreted by clinicians, teachers, or others as problematic simply because they differ from those of the culture of destination.

The Importance of Studying Immigrant Children’s Play

The study of immigrant children’s play is important for a variety of reasons. First, immigration is a central social and psychological concern today. For example, nearly 25% of children under the age of 18 in the United States are either immigrants themselves or the children of immigrants (Hernandez, Denton, & Macartney, 2008), and the International Organization of Migration (http://www.iom.int/jahia/jsp/index.jsp) estimates that approximately 200 million people currently live outside the country of their birth or citizenship. Immigration is also a transformational force in parents and children, affecting all walks of their lives. For its part, play is a basic developmental milestone in child development and a key venue of early parent-child relationships. Finally, the achievement and sophistication of children’s pretend play has been linked to a variety of cognitive and social outcomes. Because pretend play is one manifestation of the development of symbolic thought (Piaget, 1962; Vygotsky, 1967), it is related to children’s representational competence, including language development (e.g., vocabulary, linguistic complexity, semantic diversity, and verbal fluency; e.g., Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1990). Language development of course is one area of particular concern for immigrant children because many are being reared in bilingual homes and thus have the added challenge of mastering two languages prior to schooling (Hernandez et al., 2008). In the social domain, pretend play fosters children’s perspective-taking ability, including their ability to understand the thoughts, feelings, and intentions of others (Rubin & Howe, 1985; Youngblade & Dunn, 1995). Such interpersonal understanding is critical to success in interpersonal relationships including those with peers, which become increasingly important for all children as they enter social groups outside the home and also begin schooling.

Rationale for Selection of Cultural Groups

Mother-child dyads from three cultural backgrounds that shared commonalities as well as dissimilarities with respect to general childrearing goals and views about children’s play were chosen for this study. Specifically, European American mothers socialize their children to be independent and autonomous (Harkness, Super, & Keefer, 1992; Harwood, Leyendecker, Carlson, Asencio, & Miller, 2002; Richman et al., 1988), and one way they do so is by engaging in object-directed activities, such as exploratory play, that allow their children to interact with and gain control over the physical environment (Bornstein, Azuma, Tamis-LeMonda, & Ogino, 1990). In contrast, a major socialization goal of Latino families is for children to learn to act appropriately and respectfully toward other people to develop properly (become bien educado or “well educated”; Harwood et al., 2002) rather than asserting their own wants and needs. Similarly, in Japan, infants and young children are encouraged to interact with other people to develop properly (Caudill & Weinstein, 1969), relate to others empathically (omoiyari; Lebra 1994), and interact with others appropriately (DeVos, 1993; Morelli & Rothbaum, 2007; Shand, 1985). In both Latin America and Japan socialization to develop interpersonal skills is embodied in person- rather than object-directed directed interactions (Bornstein et al., 1999; Caudill & Weinstein, 1969; Valenzuela, 1997), including engaging in pretend play and role play with others.

Consistent with these cultural differences in childrearing goals, previous researchers have reported cultural differences in play between these cultural groups. For example, European American children engage in more exploratory or object-directed play (both relative to symbolic play and in comparison with children in Argentina and Japan), whereas demographically similar children in both Argentina and Japan engage in more symbolic, person-directed play (see, Bornstein et al., 1999; Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, Cyphers, Toda, & Ogino, 1992). Although comparisons of play among immigrant families are scarce, research suggests that the play behaviors of Latino immigrants from South America and those of Japanese immigrants tend to differ from those of mothers and children in their respective cultures of origin and appear to be similar to European American mothers and children (Cote & Bornstein, 2005). No research has yet compared the play of Latin American immigrants from South America with Japanese American immigrants from Japan, as we do here.

Relations between Child and Mother Play

In addition to exploring intracultural differences in the mean levels of exploratory and symbolic play, we also investigated relations between child and mother play in each group. Because play is an activity through which parents socialize their children in the norms and values of their culture (Tamis-LeMonda, Katz, & Bornstein, 2002), we expected similarities in relations between mother and child play. Relations between children(s and mothers( exploratory and children’s and mothers’ symbolic play have been found among mother-child dyads in Japan, Argentina, and European Americans in the United States (Bornstein et al., 1999; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 1992). Based on these findings, and because play is an activity through which cultural values are transmitted from mother to child (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2002), we hypothesized that similar child-mother play would be positively related in all three U.S. cultural samples.

Cultural Differences in the Initiation of Child Play and Modality of Mothers’ Encouragement of Play

When European American mothers encourage children(s play by demonstrating or soliciting play, children(s play is more sophisticated than when children play alone or when children initiate play with their mothers (O’Connell & Bretherton, 1984; O’Reilly & Bornstein, 1993; Slade, 1987). However, cross-cultural differences characterize the extent to which children versus mothers initiate play (Haight et al., 1999). Specifically, European American mothers have been observed to follow their children’s lead and interests, which is consistent with European American mothers’ childrearing goal of fostering their children’s autonomy (Harkness et al., 1992; Harwood et al., 2002; Richman et al., 1988) rather than dictating the educational agenda (Farver, 1999). Thus, we predicted that European American children would be more likely to initiate play interactions during the joint play session than their mothers (i.e., European American children would engage in more child-directed than mother-directed play during joint play). In contrast, Latino parents in South America believe that caregivers need to direct children’s behavior if they are to learn obedience and become bien educado (Bornstein et al., 1999; Valenzuela, 1997) and believe that they are assisting children’s development when they direct their children’s actions. Consistent with these childrearing goals, Latino parents in South America have been observed to direct interactions with their children (Halgunseth, Ispa, & Rudy, 2006; Harwood et al., 2002). Previous research comparing European American and Japanese (sojourner) mothers’ interactions with their children in a shape-fitting task showed that Japanese mothers were more likely than European American mothers to change their children’s focus of attention and keep them on-task than European American mothers, which is consistent with the Japanese childrearing goal of fostering interdependence in the mother-child dyad (DeVos, 1993; Morelli & Rothbaum, 2007; Shand, 1985). Thus, we predicted that both Latino immigrant mothers and Japanese immigrant mothers would be more likely to initiate play interactions during the joint play session than their children (i.e., children would engage in more mother-directed than child-directed play during joint play).

In addition to these within-culture differences in terms of whether mothers or children take the lead in play interactions, we hypothesized cultural differences with respect to play initiation. Specifically, because mothers in Japan view the mother-child relationship as reciprocal and prize empathy over directiveness (Bornstein et al., 1992) we hypothesized that Japanese immigrant and European American children might engage in more child-directed and less mother-directed play than Latino immigrant children.

Because cultural differences in the emphasis on adult verbal and nonverbal behaviors have been reported, we coded mothers’ play as (physical, nonverbal) demonstrations or (verbal) solicitations. Specifically, U.S. American and Latino adults value verbal assertiveness and Japanese adults value verbal restraint (Caudill & Frost, 1972). Indeed, previous research with Americans of Latino and Japanese ethnicity has reported that Japanese American mothers speak less to their children than Latinos from South America (Bornstein & Cote, 2001). Thus, we hypothesized that Latino immigrant mothers would engage in more play solicitations and fewer play demonstrations than Japanese immigrant mothers. These differences in mothering styles would also have clinical implications for understanding differential dynamics of interactions in immigrant families of different cultures of origin.

This is a follow-up study to Cote and Bornstein (2005), in which play among child-mother dyads in Argentina, Latino immigrants to the United States, and European Americans was compared, and play among child-mother dyads in Japan, Japanese American immigrants to the United States, and European Americans was compared. In that paper, no differences between European Americans and either Latino immigrants or Japanese immigrants were found; however, the two immigrant samples were not compared to each other. Thus, this study extends previous work by (a) comparing Latino and Japanese immigrants’ play, (b) analyzing relations between child play in two play situations (solitary and joint play), and (c) analyzing relations between child and mother play when engaged in joint play.

Method

Participants

One hundred and thirteen 20-month-olds (ranging from 18.89 to 22.51 months) and their mothers from three U.S. American cultural groups participated: Latino immigrants from South America (n = 37), Japanese immigrants (n = 36), and European Americans (n = 40). All children were firstborn, healthy, and term, and approximately equal numbers of girls and boys participated. All mothers were married to their baby’s father, and the families were middle-class. As can be seen in Table 1, participating families were demographically similar to each other, regardless of cultural background.

Table 1.

Sociodemographic Characteristics of the Participants

Latino
Immigrant
n = 37
Japanese
Immigrant
n = 36
European
American
n = 40
Differences
Gender
(girls:boys)
16:21 17:19 19:21 χ2 (2, N = 113) = 0.17, ns
Agea
(days)
626.11
(22.57)
614.94
(18.93)
612.78
(6.10)
F (2, 110) = 6.56, p < .01a
Mother
Age 33.31
(4.67)
33.42
(4.05)
31.05
(5.39)
F (2, 110) = 3.06, ns
Educationb 5.94
(0.79)
5.69
(0.79)
5.92
(1.07)
F (2, 109) = 0.87, ns
Hours of
work per
week
18.25
(19.46)
12.18
(19.07)
16.17
(18.05)
F (2, 107) = 0.95, ns
Family
Nuclear
family
78.4% 91.7% 97.5% χ2 (2, N = 113) = 7.70, p < .05

Notes. M (SD) unless otherwise specified. Sample sizes for some analyses are smaller due to missing data.

a

Latino immigrant children were older than Japanese immigrant or European American children.

b

Because differences exist between countries in the duration, quality, and content of schooling, bicultural researchers adjusted mothers’ years of schooling so that the scales were equivalent to the 7-point Hollingshead (1975) index.

Participants were from a large East coast U.S. metropolitan area and are representative of middle-class mothers in their particular cultural group in that area (U.S. Census Bureau, 2001). Families were recruited using a variety of methods common to research with young children who are not yet school-age (i.e., they were recruited from hospital birth notifications, patient lists of medical groups, newspaper birth announcements, advertisements in newspapers, but primarily from mass mailings). Prior to the conduct of these studies, parents of all participants were informed about the study and provided written consent. Treatment of human participants complied with the ethical standards defined by the American Psychological Association and was subject to oversight by the NICHD IRB.

Immigrant mothers participating in this study self-identified as South American or Japanese American; this is an important methodological issue for research with ethnic minorities (Marín & Marín, 1991). Both sets of mothers were immigrants (using Hernandez et al.’s, 2008, definition of immigrants). South American immigrant mothers’ first language was Spanish, and they were primarily from Argentina, Colombia, and Peru. (In areas of the United States that lack a large concentration of a single Latino group, immigrants tend to identify themselves by their regional affiliation rather than by their country of origin; see Winn, 1992. Furthermore, empirically there were no differences within the South American immigrant group on any dependent variables.) Japanese immigrant mothers’ first language was Japanese. Latino immigrant and Japanese immigrant mothers were bicultural (as indicated on the 5-point South American Acculturation Scale or Japanese American Acculturation Scale; Bornstein & Cote, 2001): Latino immigrant, M = 2.36 (SD = .43), Japanese immigrant, M = 2.20 (SD = .66), t (69) = 1.22, ns; they were either first- or second-generation Americans, 33:4 and 33:3, χ2 (1, N = 73) = 0.13, ns; and they had lived in the United States for M = 10.37 years (SD = 6.75) and M = 7.54 years (SD = 3.35), t (35.69) = 1.93, ns, respectively. European American participants were either fourth- or fifth-generation (i.e., most or all grandparents were born in the United States).

Procedures

Children and mothers were visited in their homes when the children were 20 months of age. Visits were scheduled at a time when the children were awake and alert, and no one was present except the child, mother, and observer. A standard set of toys that allowed for a variety of different play behaviors was placed on the floor in front of the child. After a 20- to 30-min acclimation period (McCune-Nicolich & Fenson, 1984), the child was videorecorded playing alone for 10 min and then the child and mother were videorecorded playing together for 10 min. Pilot testing undertaken prior to data collection ensured that the observational context was ethnographically valid (van de Vijver & Leung, 1997). The toy set (i.e., doll, blanket, tea set, telephone, train, 2 picture books, foam rubber ball, and nesting barrels) was chosen because it is both ethnographically valid and allows for a variety of different play behaviors (including exploratory and symbolic play; McCune-Nicolich & Fenson, 1984). Mothers also completed a demographic questionnaire.

Play Coding

Child and mother exploratory and symbolic play were coded using a mutually exclusive and exhaustive coding system derived from research on the early development of play (Bornstein & O'Reilly, 1993). The frequency and duration of the following levels of play were recorded: (0) default (no play); (1) unitary functional activity (e.g., throw the ball); (2) inappropriate combinatorial activity (e.g., put the ball into a teacup); (3) appropriate combinatorial activity (e.g., put the cup on the saucer); (4) transitional play (e.g., put the telephone to the ear without speaking); (5) self-directed pretense (e.g., drink from a cup); (6) other-directed pretense (e.g., pretend to feed the doll); (7) sequential pretense (e.g., dial and speak into the telephone); and (8) substitution pretense (e.g., pretend the block is a telephone receiver and speak into it). Children’s play in the mother-child joint play session was also coded as either child-initiated or mother-initiated. Mothers’ play demonstrations (i.e., modeling an action for the child; e.g., pouring from the tea kettle into a teacup) and solicitations (i.e., verbally encouraging the child to participate in a specific play activity; e.g., suggesting that the child pour tea from the kettle into a teacup) were coded separately. Play was coded continuously by noting the start time and play level, and play at a given level continued until there was a 10-s or longer break or the player touched another toy when the end time was noted (to 1 s). Minimum play time was set to 1 s.

Coders were unaware of the hypotheses and were trained to reliability (kappa κs ≥ .60; intraclass correlation ICCs ≥ .80) on a standard set of videorecords before they were allowed to code the videorecords for this study. Additionally, interrater reliability was checked every 10 videorecords to guard against coding drift. Interrater agreement was computed separately for each cultural group using κ for behaviors and ICC for frequency of maternal verbal solicitations based on approximately 15% of the videorecords: Child exploratory play, κs ≥ .77; child symbolic play, κs ≥.76; initiation of child play, κ s ≥.67; mother exploratory play demonstrations, κs ≥.75; mother symbolic play demonstrations, κs ≥ .71; mother exploratory play solicitations, ICCs ≥ .89; and mother symbolic play solicitations, ICCs ≥ .92. Play reliability was not systematically related to cultural group.

Computing the Dependent Variables

Frequency and duration scores for the eight play levels were aggregated into exploratory (sum of levels 1–4) and symbolic (sum of levels 5–8) play scores. Four indices of play were computed, separately for play session (solitary, joint), separately for play partner (child, mother), separately for who initiated child play in the joint play session (child, mother), and separately for exploratory and symbolic: raw frequency of play bouts, the proportion of play bouts, raw duration of play, and the proportion duration of play. These four indices (raw frequency, proportional frequency, raw duration, proportional duration) have consistently been found to be positively and significantly correlated, including with the current samples (e.g., Bornstein, Haynes, Legler, O’Reilly, & Painter, 1997; Cote & Bornstein, 2005). Therefore, the four indices were aggregated into mean standard scores, which resulted in eight dependent variables: mean z-scores of child exploratory and symbolic play in the solitary play session, mean z-scores of child exploratory and symbolic play in the joint play session that was child-initiated or mother-initiated, and mothers’ demonstrations of exploratory and symbolic play. In addition, for maternal solicitations of child play, two indices of exploratory and symbolic play were developed: the raw frequency of play bouts that were exploratory or symbolic, and the proportion of play bouts that were exploratory or symbolic. The two (raw and proportional) scores of frequency of play were then aggregated into two measures of maternal solicitations by taking the mean z-scores separately for exploratory and symbolic play. This resulted in two additional dependent variables: maternal solicitations of exploratory and symbolic play.

Results

Examination of univariate and multivariate distributions of the dependent variables revealed that all were normally distributed. Because differences in girls’ and boys’ play have sometimes been reported (Bornstein et al., 1999; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 1992), child gender was used as a factor in the MANOVAs. Furthermore, because child age differed among groups (Table 1), and we wished to rule out child age as a possible confound (because older children are more likely to engage in symbolic play than younger children), child age was used as a covariate in all analyses. Wilks’ lambda is reported for all multivariate tests, and pairwise comparisons are t-tests with Bonferroni’s correction. Because interaction effects supersede main effects, only interaction effects are reported. Due to space limitations, only significant results are reported, however the Tables contain all data -- i.e., means (Ms) and standard deviations (SDs) for each DV controlled for child age presented separately by cultural background and child gender.

Child Solitary Play

A repeated-measures ANOVA with two between-subjects factors, Cultural background (Latino immigrant, Japanese immigrant, European American) and Child gender, one covariate (child age), and two dependent variables (child exploratory and symbolic play) was performed to investigate cultural and gender differences in children’s engagement in exploratory and symbolic play when they played by themselves. There was a significant multivariate interaction of Play Type × Gender, F (1, 107) = 4.57, p < .05, η2p = .04. Univariate ANOVAs revealed that boys engaged in significantly more exploratory play (M = .14, SD = .83) and significantly less symbolic play (M = −.14, SD = .91) than girls (exploratory, M = −.19, SD = .83; symbolic, M = .20, SD = .92): exploratory, F (1, 107) = 4.51, p < .05, η2p = .04, and symbolic, F (1, 107) = 3.84, p = .05, η2p = .04. Neither the multivariate main effect of Cultural background, nor any of the interactions involving Cultural background, was significant. (See Table 2 for full data.)

Table 2.

Cultural and Gender Differences in Child Solitary Play

DV Latino Immigrant Japanese Immigrant European American
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
Exploratory
play
−.28
(.67)
.06
(.72)
−.09
(.66)
.24
(.68)
−.21
(.68)
.12
(.69)
Symbolic
play
.24
(.74)
−.10
(.79)
.18
(.73)
−.16
(.75)
.20
(.75)
−.15
(.76)

Note. Data are M (SD), controlling for child age.

A post-hoc power analysis was computed to determine whether the sample size of 113 provided sufficient power to detect a medium-sized effect in an ANOVA design with two between-subjects factors with two and three levels and two dependent variables. With an effect size of .25 for between-subjects effects (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007), α = .05, and N = 113, the power estimate was .99 for this design, indicating sufficient power to detect a medium or large within × between effect.

Child Joint Play

A MANOVA with two between-subjects factors, Cultural background (Latino immigrant, Japanese immigrant, European American) and Child gender, one covariate (child age), and four dependent variables (child-initiated exploratory and symbolic play, mother-initiated exploratory and symbolic play) was performed to investigate cultural and gender differences in the kinds of play that children engaged in when they played with their mothers and whether play differed depending on who initiated play. A significant multivariate interaction of Cultural Background × Play Type × Initiator emerged, F (2, 106) = 3.89, p < .05, η2p = .04. Japanese immigrant children engaged in more exploratory play (M = .41, SD = .73) and less symbolic play (M = −.39, SD = .83) when they initiated the play interaction than when their mothers initiated play (maternal exploratory, M = −.05, SD = .94; symbolic, M = .12, SD = .83):exploratory, F (1, 106) = 7.04, p < .01, η2p = .06; symbolic, F (1, 106) = 8.49, p < .01, η2p = .07. Both Latino immigrant and Japanese immigrant children engaged in more exploratory than symbolic play when they initiated play: Latino immigrants, F (1, 106) = 9.44, p < .01, η2p = .08, exploratory, M = .46, SD = .75; symbolic, M = −.24, SD = .85; Japanese immigrants, F (1, 106) = 12.32, p = .001, η2p = .10 (means for Japanese immigrants are reported above). No (cultural) differences emerged with respect to whether mothers or children initiated play overall (Table 3).

Table 3.

Cultural and Gender Differences in Child Joint Play

DV Latino Immigrant Japanese Immigrant European American
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
Child-initiated
Exploratory
play
.33
(.57)
.60
(.62)
.28
(.58)
.55
(.60)
−.30
(.63)
−.04
(.64)
Symbolic
play
−.23
(.65)
−.26
(.71)
−.37
(.66)
−.41
(.68)
−.16
(.71)
−.19
(.73)
Mother-initiated
Exploratory
play
.21
(.73)
.24
(.79)
−.06
(.74)
−.04
(.77)
.03
(.81)
.06
(.82)
Symbolic
play
−.13
(.65)
−.18
(.70)
.14
(.65)
.10
(.68)
−.18
(.71)
−.23
(.73)

Note. Data are M (SD), controlling for child age.

Mothers’ Play

A MANOVA with two between-subjects factors, Cultural background (Latino immigrant, Japanese immigrant, European American) and Child gender, one covariate (child age), and four dependent variables (maternal demonstrations and solicitations of exploratory and symbolic play) was performed to investigate cultural and gender differences in the modality that mothers used to encourage different types of play. There was a significant interaction of Cultural background× Play modality, F (2, 108) = 5.54, p < .01, η2p = .09. Although univariate tests revealed a significant difference across cultural groups for demonstrations (wherein Japanese and South American immigrant mothers demonstrated more than European American mothers), F (2, 108) = 3.60, p < .05, η2p = .06, pairwise comparisons did not reach statistical significance (p < .05). Univariate tests also showed that European American mothers solicited play significantly more (M = .04, SD = .39) than they demonstrated (M = −.13, SD = .28), F (1, 108) = 7.55, p < .01, η2p = .07 (Table 4).

Table 4.

Cultural and Gender Differences in Mothers’ Play

DV Latino Immigrant Japanese Immigrant European American
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
Demonstrations
Exploratory
play
.13
(.60)
.29
(.65)
−.06
(.60)
.10
(.62)
−.08
(.62)
.09
(.63)
Symbolic
play
−.12
(.63)
−.17
(.68)
.04
(.63)
−.01
(.65)
−.23
(.65)
−.28
(.66)
Solicitations
Exploratory
play
.20
(.75)
.52
(.81)
−.07
(.75)
.25
(.77)
.02
(.76)
.34
(.78)
Symbolic
play
−.11
(.66)
−.20
(.71)
−.26
(.66)
−.35
(.67)
−.05
(.67)
−.14
(.69)

Note. Data are M (SD), controlling for child age.

A post-hoc power analysis was computed to determine whether the sample size of 113 provided sufficient power to detect a medium-sized effect in a MANOVA design with two between-subjects factors (one factor having two levels and the other three levels) and two within-subjects factors each with two levels (i.e., 4 dependent variables). With an effect size of .25 for between-subjects effects (Faul et al., 2007), α = .05, and N = 113, the power estimate was .84 for this design, indicating sufficient power to detect a medium or large within× between effect.

Relations between Child Solitary and Joint Play

Pearson correlations were performed to explore relations between children’s play in the two play settings. As Table 5 shows, few relations between children’s play in the two play settings emerged.

Table 5.

Relations between Children’s Play in the Two Play Situations

Solitary Play

Joint play Exploratory Symbolic

Child-initiated exploratory .33*
.01
.04

Mother-initiated
exploratory
.12
.07
.00

Child-initiated and
mother-initiated collapsed,
exploratory
.29+
.02
.04

Child-initiated symbolic .15
.11
.30+

Mother-initiated symbolic .04
.17
.11

Child-initiated and
mother-initiated collapsed,
symbolic
.12
.02
.31*

Note: Data are Pearson correlations. Correlations appear in the following order within cells: Latino immigrant, Japanese immigrant, European American.

+

p < .10,

*

p≤ .05,

**

p≤ .01,

***

p≤ .001

Relations between Child and Mother Play

Pearson correlations were performed to investigate relations between similar kinds of child and mother play. As Table 6 shows, with one exception, all of these relations were significantly positively correlated in every cultural group (all rs ≥ .34, ps ≤ .05) .

Table 6.

Relations between Child and Mother Play

Mother

Child Exploratory
Demonstrations
Exploratory
Solicitations
Symbolic
Demonstrations
Symbolic
Solicitations

Child-initiated
exploratory
.54*** .46**
.54*** .49**
.60*** .44**

Mother-initiated
exploratory
.46** .42**
.54*** .58***
50*** .35*

Child-initiated
symbolic
.58*** .36*
.37* .34*
.22 .44**

Mother-initiated
symbolic
.61*** .51***
.37* .65***
.37* .64***

Note: Data are Pearson correlations. Correlations appear in the following order within cells: Latino immigrant, Japanese immigrant, European American.

*

p≤ .05,

**

p≤ .01,

***

p≤ .001

Discussion

Children’s Play

Our findings support species-general developmental views of mother-child object and representational play (Piaget, 1962) and also point to the existence of species-general relations in child-mother play interactions (Bornstein, 1995). Taken together our findings of few cultural differences in children’s play, but differences in play type (exploratory or symbolic) depending on whether mothers or children initiate play, confirm previous reports that children’s play tends to be more sophisticated when their mothers encourage them than when children initiate play with their mothers or play alone (O’Connell & Bretherton, 1984; O’Reilly & Bornstein, 1993; Slade, 1987). Moreover, our results extend these findings to two immigrant samples. Developmentally, children first engage in exploratory play, and over the course of the second year of life, as their representational capacities advance, they begin to engage in symbolic play (Piaget, 1962). From an applied or clinical vantage, these results suggest that the play of Japanese and South American immigrant children is similar to that of European American children in the sense that it is more sophisticated when play is mother- rather than child-initiated, and that by playing with their mothers, children’s play develops.

Also concerning the universality of play, child gender emerged as a significant factor. Regardless of their culture, boys engaged in significantly more exploratory and less symbolic play than girls when they played by themselves (Lytton & Romney, 1991). These results confirm previous research on gender differences in children’s play that was conducted with European American, Latino families in Argentina, and dyads in Japan (Bornstein et al., 1999; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 1992). It is noteworthy that these gender differences were found only for children’s solitary play and not in child-mother joint play. It could be that girls, on average, experience more early caregiving socialization regarding interpersonal skills than do boys, or that girls are more susceptible to early caregiving socialization regarding interpersonal skills than are boys (Maccoby, 1988).

Few relations between child exploratory play in the solitary and joint play sessions were found, and few relations between child symbolic play in the two play sessions were found. This suggests that, across cultures, mothers influence their children’s play and that children play differently when their mothers are involved than when they play alone.

Mothers’ Play

European American mothers engaged in significantly more play solicitations than demonstrations (regardless of whether their play was exploratory or symbolic). This finding is consistent with previous research that has shown European American mothers stress verbal interactions (Bornstein, Tamis-LeMonda, & Haynes, 1999; Caudill & Schooler, 1973), although not necessarily in the play setting. Again, the parenting styles of European American mothers appear to differ from those of immigrant mothers, as do styles of family interaction and expectation.

Relations between Child and Mother Play

As expected, each of the cultural groups evidenced a consistent pattern of interrelations between child and mother play. For example, children’s exploratory play (whether it was child-initiated or mother-initiated) was significantly positively related to both maternal demonstrations and solicitations of exploratory play in all three cultural groups. These concordances indicate that children and mothers in each of these cultural groups coordinate in their play. Mothers who played more in pretense at 13 and 20 months had children who played more in pretense (Vibbert & Bornstein, 1989). Relations between children’s and mothers’ exploratory and symbolic play have also been found in Argentine, Japanese, and European American families (Bornstein et al., 1999; Tamis-LeMonda et al., 1992). The present findings broaden those results to exploratory play and also to acculturating groups.

Although the direction of effect in these correlational results is not clear, research has shown that, when mothers encourage children’s play by demonstrating or soliciting, children’s play is more sophisticated than when children initiate play with their mothers or play alone (Bornstein & O’Reilly, 1993; O’Connell & Bretherton, 1984; Slade, 1987). Children are likely taught to play in different ways by their mothers, and in ways valued by the culture in which their mother was reared. The finding that, when boys played alone, they engaged in more exploratory than symbolic play, but that this gender difference was absent when they played with their mothers, lends further support to this conclusion. From a clinical or applied point of view, these findings suggest that, regardless of ethnicity, parents might foster the character and quality of their children’s cognitive growth by structuring their play interactions according to those characteristics they hope to foster in their children. Parental goals should always be taken into account when assessing immigrants’ parenting behavior.

Conclusions

With respect to child development, parenting, and acculturation, our study suggests that immigrant samples of children and their mothers in the United States tend to resemble each other and European American children and mothers in their culture of destination. This suggests that there may be cultural universals in the expression of (at least some) developmentally important processes, such as the development of child and mother play. Future research should further explore the extent to which play and other important developmental processes are culturally universal or culture specific. For clinicians and others who work with immigrant parents and children, our results point to realms of child growth, parenting, and family function that require special attention and cultural sensitivity.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. We thank O. M. Haynes, J. Lampard, M. Ogino, N. Okazaki, K. Painter, L. Pascual, D. Putnik, K. Schulthess, W. Smith, and S. Toda for assistance.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at www.apa.org/pubs/journals/fam.

References

  1. Bornstein MH. Form and function: Implications for studies of culture and human development. Culture & Psychology. 1995;1:123–137. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bornstein MH. On the significance of social relationships in the development of children’s earliest symbolic play: An ecological perspective. In: Gönçü A, Gaskins S, editors. Play and development: Evolutionary, sociocultural, and functional perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2007. pp. 101–129. [Google Scholar]
  3. Bornstein MH, Azuma H, Tamis-LeMonda C, Ogino M. Mother and infant activity and interaction in Japan and in the United States: I. A comparative macroanalysis of naturalistic exchanges. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1990;13:267–287. [Google Scholar]
  4. Bornstein MH, Cote LR. Mother-infant interaction and acculturation I: Behavioral comparisons in Japanese American and South American families. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2001;25:549–563. [Google Scholar]
  5. Bornstein MH, Haynes OM, Legler JM, O’Reilly AW, Painter KM. Symbolic play in childhood: Interpersonal and environmental context and stability. Infant Behavior and Development. 1997;20:197–207. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bornstein MH, Haynes OM, Pascual L, Painter KM, Galperín C. Play in two societies: Pervasiveness of process, specificity of structure. Child Development. 1999;70:317–331. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bornstein MH, O’Reilly AW, editors. The role of play in the development of thought. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  8. Bornstein MH, Tal J, Rahn C, Galperin C, Lamour M, Ogino M, Pecheux M, Azuma H, Tamis-LeMonda C. Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology. 1992;28:593–603. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bornstein MH, Tamis-LeMonda CS, Haynes OM. First words in the second year: Continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Behavior and Development. 1999;22:65–85. [Google Scholar]
  10. Caudill W, Frost L. A comparison of maternal care and infant behavior in Japanese-American, American, and Japanese Families. In: Bronfenbrenner U, editor. Influences on human development. Hinsdale, IL: Dryden; 1972. pp. 329–342. [Google Scholar]
  11. Caudill WA, Schooler C. Child behavior and childrearing in Japan and the United States. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1973;157:323–338. doi: 10.1097/00005053-197311000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Caudill W, Weinstein H. Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry. 1969;32:12–43. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1969.11023572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cote LR, Bornstein MH. Child and mother play in cultures of origin, acculturating cultures, and cultures of destination. Special issue, International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2005;29(6):479–488. [Google Scholar]
  14. DeVos GA. A cross-cultural perspective: The Japanese family as a unit in moral socialization. In: Cowan PA, Field D, Hansen DA, Skolnick A, Swanson GE, editors. Family, self, and society: Toward a new agenda for family research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1993. pp. 115–142. [Google Scholar]
  15. Farver JM. Activity setting analysis: A model for examining the role of culture in development. In: Göncü A, editor. Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 99–127. [Google Scholar]
  16. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavioral Research Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Göncü A, Tuermer U, Jain J, Johnson D. Children’s play as cultural activity. In: Göncü A, editor. Children’s engagement in the world: Sociocultural perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1999. pp. 148–170. [Google Scholar]
  18. Haight WL, Wang X, Fung HH, Williams K, Mintz J. Universal, developmental, and variable aspects of young children’s play: A cross-cultural comparison of pretending at home. Child Development. 1999;70:1477–1488. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Halgunseth LC, Ispa JM, Rudy D. Parental control in Latino families: An integrated review of the literature. Child Development. 2006;77:1282–1297. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00934.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Harkness S, Super CM, Keefer CH. Learning to be an American parent: How cultural models gain directive force. In: Paul RA, Shweder RA, editors; Andrade RGD, Strauss C, editors. Publications of the Society for Psychological Anthropology: Human motives and cultural models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1992. pp. 163–178. (Series Eds.) (Vol. Eds.) [Google Scholar]
  21. Harwood R, Leyendecker B, Carlson V, Asencio M, Miller A. Parenting among Latino families in the U.S. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of parenting: Vol. 4. Applied parenting. 2nd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2002. pp. 21–46. [Google Scholar]
  22. Hernandez DJ, Denton NA, Macartney SE. Children in immigrant families: Looking to America’s future. Society for Research in Child Development Social Policy Report. 2008;22(3) [Google Scholar]
  23. Hollingshead AB. The four-factor index of social status. Yale University: 1975. Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  24. Lebra TS. Mother and child in Japanese socialization: A Japan-U.S. comparison. In: Greenfield P, Cocking R, editors. Cross-cultural roots of minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1994. pp. 259–274. [Google Scholar]
  25. Lytton H, Romney DM. Parents' differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. 1991;109:267–296. [Google Scholar]
  26. Maccoby EE. Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology. 1988;24:755–765. [Google Scholar]
  27. Marín G, Marín BV. Applied social research methods series: Vol. 23. Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  28. McCune-Nicolich L, Fenson L. Methodological issues in studying early pretend play. In: Yawkey TD, Pelligrini AD, editors. Child’s play: Developmental and applied. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984. pp. 81–124. [Google Scholar]
  29. Morelli G, Rothbaum F. Situating the child in context: Attachment relationships and self-regulation in different cultures. In: Kitayama S, Cohen D, editors. Handbook of cultural psychology. New York: Guilford press; 2007. pp. 500–527. [Google Scholar]
  30. O’Connell B, Bretherton I. Toddlers’ play, alone and with mother: The role of maternal guidance. In: Bretherton I, editor. Symbolic play: The development of social understanding. San Diego: Academic Press; 1984. pp. 337–368. [Google Scholar]
  31. O'Reilly AW, Bornstein MH. Caregiver-child interaction in play. In: Bornstein MH, O’Reilly AW, editors. The role of play in the development of thought. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1993. pp. 55–66. [Google Scholar]
  32. Piaget J. In: Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. Gattegno C, Hodgson FM, translators. New York: W. W. Norton; 1962. (Original work published 1951) [Google Scholar]
  33. Richman AL, Miller PM, Solomon MJ. The socialization of infants in suburban Boston. In: Damon W, editor; LeVine RA, Miler PM, West MM, editors. New Directions for Child Development Series: No. 40. Parental behavior in diverse societies. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 1988. pp. 65–74. (Series Ed.) (Vol. Eds.) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Rogoff B. The cultural nature of human development. London: Oxford University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  35. Rubin KH, Howe N. Toys and play behaviors: An overview. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. 1985;5(3):1–10. [Google Scholar]
  36. Shand N. Cultural influence in Japanese and American maternal role perception and confidence. Psychiatry. 1985;48:52–67. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1985.11024267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Slade A. A longitudinal study of maternal involvement and symbolic play during the toddler period. Child Development. 1987;58:367–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1987.tb01384.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH. Language, play, and attention at one year. Infant Behavior & Development. 1990;13:85–98. [Google Scholar]
  39. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Bornstein MH, Cyphers L, Toda S, Ogino M. Language and play at one year: A comparison of toddlers and mothers in the United States and Japan. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 1992;15:19–42. [Google Scholar]
  40. Tamis-LeMonda CS, Katz JC, Bornstein MH. Infant play: Functions and partners. In: Slater A, Lewis M, editors. Introduction to infant development. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. pp. 229–243. [Google Scholar]
  41. U. S. Census Bureau. Washington, D.C: Government Printing Office; Profile of the foreign-born population in the United States: 2000. 2001 (Current Population Reports, Series P23–206) [Electronic version]
  42. Valenzuela M. Maternal sensitivity in a developing society; The context of urban poverty and infant chronic undernutrition. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33:845–855. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.33.5.845. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. van de Vijver FJR, Leung K. Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  44. Vibbert M, Bornstein MH. Specific associations between domains of mother–child interaction and toddler referential language and pretense play. Infant Behavior and Development. 1989;12:163–184. [Google Scholar]
  45. Vygostky LS. Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet. 1967 [Google Scholar]
  46. Winn P. Americas: The changing face of Latin America and the Caribbean. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  47. Youngblade LM, Dunn J. Individual differences in young children’s pretend play with mother and sibling: Links to relationships and understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs. Child Development. 1995;66:1472–1492. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES