Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Mar 2.
Published in final edited form as: J Abnorm Psychol. 2008 Aug;117(3):534–551. doi: 10.1037/a0012907

Searching for an environmental effect of parental alcoholism on offspring alcohol use disorder: A genetically-informed study of children of alcoholics

Wendy S Slutske 1, Brian M D’Onofrio 2, Eric Turkheimer 3, Robert E Emery 4, K Paige Harden 5, Andrew C Heath 6, Nicholas G Martin 7
PMCID: PMC2830724  NIHMSID: NIHMS59312  PMID: 18729607

Abstract

The children-of-twins design was used to isolate a potentially causal environmental impact of having an alcoholic parent on offspring alcohol use disorder by examining whether the children of alcoholics were at a higher risk for alcohol use disorders than the children of non-alcoholic parents even after correlated familial factors were controlled. Participants were 1,224 male and female twins from 836 twin pairs selected from the Australian Twin Registry, 2,334 of their 18–39 year-old offspring, and 983 spouses of the twins. Lifetime histories of DSM-IV alcohol use disorders were obtained by structured psychiatric telephone interviews conducted individually with each of the family members. Comparisons of the offspring of twins discordant for alcoholism indicated that there was no longer a statistically significant difference between the children of alcoholics and the children of non-alcoholics after genetic and family environmental factors correlated with having an alcoholic parent were controlled. The results of this study suggest that the direct causal effect of being exposed to an alcoholic parent on offspring alcohol use disorder is modest at best.

Keywords: children of alcoholics, alcoholism, children of twins, multilevel modeling


It has been estimated that approximately 1 in 4 children under the age of 18 in the United States is exposed to alcohol abuse or dependence in the family (Grant, 2000), an estimate that has lead to the suggestion that "the extraordinary number of children in this country who are exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence defines one of today's major public health problems and demands a comprehensive public policy directed toward prevention and intervention" (Grant, 2000, p. 114). The assumption is that exposure to alcoholism in a parent is causally related to the increased risk of adverse outcomes, including the development of alcoholism1, among the children of alcoholics. This causal interpretation is compelling because the home life of children with alcoholic parents is often characterized by inadequate parenting, conflict, disorganization, or hardship (e.g. Ellis et al., 1997; Grant, 2000; Jacob & Leonard, 1994; Windle & Tubman, 1999). However, because exposure to an alcoholic parent is inextricably intertwined with a network of correlated genetic and environmental risk factors (Ellis et al., 1997), it has been a challenge to demonstrate empirically that this is an active ingredient leading to alcohol use disorders in the children of alcoholics. For example, in conventional children-of-alcoholics studies of parents and their biological offspring, exposure to an alcoholic parent is confounded with correlated genetic and family environmental risk factors.

There have been only a few genetically-informed studies that have examined alcohol use disorders in offspring exposed to an alcoholic parent while controlling for the genetic risk for alcoholism that is usually correlated with exposure. In adoption studies this is typically achieved by comparing the rates of alcohol use disorders in offspring who are adopted into the homes of alcoholic versus nonalcoholic parents. Such studies have failed to detect an influence of having an alcoholic adoptive parent on alcohol use disorders in the offspring. For example, in the Stockholm adoption study, the rates of alcohol abuse among 862 male adoptees reared by alcoholic versus nonalcoholic parents were 13% and 18%, respectively, and were 3.7% and 3.4%, respectively, among 913 female adoptees (Cloninger et al., 1985). Two of the Iowa adoption studies (Cadoret et al., 1985; Cadoret et al., 1987) have provided the only evidence of significant associations between alcohol problems in the adoptive family (including the parents, siblings, and more distant relatives) and adoptee alcohol problems, but once the analyses were restricted to parental alcohol problems in the adoptive family, the associations were no longer statistically significant. The Danish adoption study (Goodwin et al., 1974), using a slightly different research design, also failed to detect an influence of having an alcoholic rearing parent on alcohol use disorders in the offspring. The rate of alcohol use disorders among 20 adopted-away sons of biological parents with alcoholism (90% adopted into the homes of non-alcoholic foster parents) was compared to the rate among 30 offspring who remained with the alcoholic biological parent, that is, who were not adopted away. Sons who were raised by the alcoholic birth parent did not have higher rates of alcohol use disorders (20%) than the sons who were adopted away and raised by (mostly) non-alcoholic foster parents (25%).

An alternative genetically-informed research design that circumvents some of the shortcomings of the adoption study is the children-of-twins study (Gottesman & Bertelson, 1989; Heath et al., 1985; Nance & Corey, 1976). The concerns about representativeness that plague adoption studies (Cadoret, 1986; Rutter et al., 2001; Stoolmiller, 1999) are not as relevant to community-based studies of twins and their families. The children-of-twins design can provide a powerful test of the environmental impact of being exposed to an alcoholic parent while controlling for correlated genetic (and also environmental) factors. The children-of-twins design is based on similar logic as the Danish adoption study -- that is, to hold genetic risk constant while varying the environmental risk of being raised by an alcoholic parent. In the children-of-twins design, one can compare the offspring of monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs who are discordant for alcoholism. The non-alcoholic cotwin serves as a control for the alcoholic twin, and the offspring of these non-alcoholic cotwins are analogous to the offspring who are adopted-away from their alcoholic biological parents in the Danish adoption study. Higher rates of alcoholism in the offspring who are reared by the alcoholic twins compared to the rates in their cousins who are reared by the nonalcoholic cotwins provide evidence for a possible causal environmental influence of being exposed to an alcoholic parent. The children-of-twins design actually provides a more stringent test of this effect than the adoption study because, in addition to controlling for correlated genetic risk factors (completely in the case of MZ twin pairs, or partially in the case of dizygotic [DZ] twin pairs), it also controls for other unmeasured environmental factors correlated with parental alcoholism that twins can have in common -- things such as ethnicity, socioeconomic status, religious affiliation, urban versus rural residence (Whitfield et al., 2005), educational attainment, and shared child-rearing practices (i.e., potential environmental confounds). The children-of-twins design, however, does not provide a perfect environmental control; it cannot control for environmental factors that twins do not necessarily have in common. Although the children-of-twins design can get us much closer to isolating a specific environmental causal effect of parental AUD than most other research designs available, there still remain plausible alternate explanations. (Thus, in this paper, we often refer to the "potential" or "possible" causal influence of parental AUD, and when these qualifiers are left off for the sake of brevity, they are implied.)

Jacob et al. (2003) represents the only children-of-twins study that has examined the intergenerational transmission of alcohol use disorders. The participants were 1,213 male twins from the Vietnam Era Twin Registry, 1,183 of their 12–26 year-old offspring, and 862 mothers of the offspring. Offspring were divided into six different groups based on whether their father had a history of alcohol dependence (AD) or abuse (AA), whether their uncle had a history of AD or AA, and the twin zygosity of the father. Comparisons of the outcomes in the offspring in these six groups provided evidence for a potentially causal environmental influence of having an alcoholic father. For example, lifetime rates of AD and AA among 18–26 year-old offspring were 19% and 24% when the father had a diagnosis of AD, and were lower, 12% and 15%, when the father was unaffected but the genetically-identical twin of the father (the uncle) had a diagnosis of AD. Prior to this study, the popular belief that alcoholism in a rearing parent had a potentially causal influence on alcohol use disorder in offspring had consistently evaded empirical support. However, conclusions drawn from this study may need to be tempered in light of the findings of a recent paper presenting follow-up analyses of these data. Duncan et al. (2006) found that among the offspring of fathers with a history of AD, exposure to paternal alcohol-related symptomatology before age 13 was not related to AD or AA in the offspring, leading the authors to conclude that "genetic and high-risk environmental factors that are correlated with lifetime paternal alcoholism may be stronger predictors of offspring alcohol use disorder than fathers' problem drinking. "

The purpose of the present study was to attempt to replicate the findings of Jacob et al.'s (2003) landmark study by using the children-of-twins design to isolate a potentially causal environmental consequence of being exposed to an alcoholic parent on the development of alcohol use disorder in the offspring. The participants were 1,224 male and female individual twins from 836 twin pairs selected from the Australian Twin Registry, 2,334 of their 18–39 year-old offspring, and 983 spouses of the twins. Unlike many other "genetically-informative" investigations, our purpose was not to decompose variation in alcohol use disorder into genetic and environmental components -- this has been well-studied using other research designs and analytic approaches that are much better suited for this (e.g. Heath et al., 1997; Prescott et al., 1999; True et al., 1996). Although the CoT design is very useful for controlling for genetic and environmental factors that are correlated with having an alcoholic parent, it is not very useful for decomposing the extent to which these confounding factors are genetic or environmental. This requires extremely large sample sizes, particularly when the parent-offspring association is not large. The goal of the present study was to examine whether there is an important and possibly causal environmental influence of parental alcoholism on offspring alcohol use disorder while controlling for unmeasured familial factors, both genetic and environmental, that are correlated with parental alcoholism.

Methods

Participants

Data collection for this study proceeded in three stages. The twin parents were members of the volunteer Australian National Health and Medical Research Council Twin Registry whose initial involvement in the registry dated back to 1981. Information from a questionnaire completed by the twins in 1988–1989 (N=6,327; 83% response rate) and a telephone interview conducted in 1992–1993 (N=5,889; 86% response rate) was used to select twin pairs for inclusion in this children-of-twins (CoT) study. Spouses of the twins participated in a telephone interview conducted between 1994 and 1997 (N=3,844), and the offspring of the twins were interviewed between 1999 and 2002 (N=2,554).

Twins in this study were selected from the larger sample of 5,889 participants who took part in the structured psychiatric telephone interview survey (see Heath et al., 1997, or Slutske et al., 1997, for further details about zygosity determination, attrition, and the representativeness of this twin sample). All twin pairs with the following characteristics were selected for the CoT study: (a) either twin reported in the 1988 questionnaire that they had biological children born between the years 1964 and 1983, and (b) either twin had a history of AD, conduct disorder, major depression (all based on the 1992 interview) or a history of divorce (based on the 1988 questionnaire or the 1992 interview). In addition, a random sample of twin pairs were selected from among the remaining twin pairs with offspring born between 1964 and 1983 and no history of AD, conduct disorder, major depression, or divorce. Eighty-five percent of the twins who were contacted agreed to allow us to invite their eligible offspring to participate in the study. After excluding those without parental permission, 82% of the offspring selected agreed to participate and completed the telephone interview. The complete CoT sample included 2,554 offspring from 889 twin pairs, and data from the 1994–1997 interview of the co-parent (the spouses of the twins) were available for 2,048 (80%) of the offspring.

Previously we reported a median age-of-onset of DSM-III-R AD of 18 years in the original sample of male and female twins (men: mean = 18.2, SD = 3.0, range = 10–35; women: mean = 20.9, SD = 6.8, range = 10–56; Slutske et al., 1998). More recently, using a different method for ascertaining age-of-onset in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication study, Kessler et al. (2005) estimated that only one-quarter of individuals who eventually develop DSM-IV AD or AA in their lifetime will be affected by age 18 (AA) or 19 (AD), one-half will be affected by age 21 (AA) or 23 (AD), and three-quarters will be affected by age 29 (AA) or 31 (AD). Although included in previous studies using this sample (e.g., D'Onofrio et al., 2005; Harden et al., 2007; Lynch et al., 2005), 220 offspring 14–17 years of age were not included in this study because they had not yet progressed through enough of the age period of risk for the development of alcohol use disorders. The final CoT sample for this study included 2,334 offspring (mean age at interview in 1999–2002 = 25.9, SD = 5.2, range = 18–39 years, range = 18–30 years for 80% of the offspring), 1,224 twins from 836 pairs (mean age at interview in 1992–93 = 45.9, SD = 7.1), and 983 spouses of the twins who were the biological parents of the offspring (mean age at interview in 1994– 97 = 48.9, SD = 7.8). The 836 twin pairs included 412 MZ (132 male, 280 female) and 424 DZ (73 male-male, 177 female-female, 174 male-female) pairs.

The offspring in this study represented a relatively broad cross-section of the Australian general population of young adults. Fifty percent (n=1,176) of the offspring were female. Twenty-eight percent of the offspring were currently married and 68% had never married (22% of the never married were currently in "de facto" marriages). Sixty percent of the offspring were employed full-time, 28% were employed part-time, 26% were students, 16% were homemakers, and 4.5% were unemployed (the sum of these percentages exceed 100 because more than one of these occupational categories could be selected). Their yearly gross income ranged from less than AU$4,999 (10%) to AU$60,000 or more (9%), and the median yearly income at time of interview was AU$25,000–$29,999 (11%). Twenty-eight percent of the offspring described themselves as working class, 68% as middle class, and 3% as upper class. Offspring were most likely to report being raised in the Church of England (26%), Roman Catholic church (23%), or with no religion (18%). Five percent of the offspring acknowledged that their religion had formal rules against all alcohol use.

Data weighting

Because the samples for this study were selected based on a history of psychiatric disorders and divorce in the twin pairs, a set of sample-propensity data weights were constructed using the procedure outlined in Heath, Madden, and Martin (1998) to obtain unbiased estimates of parameters and their standard errors. The goal of the data weighting was to re-capture the characteristics of the original unselected twin sample and also to remove any other biases due to non-random attrition. Sample-propensity weights were constructed by identifying predictors of whether or not one or both twins from a pair from the larger sample were included in the CoT sample. Predictors of pair-wise rather than individual-wise inclusion were examined because selection occurred at the twin-pair level. Propensity weights were then constructed using the inverse probability of the inclusion of a twin pair in the CoT sample. The ability of these propensity weights to remove bias introduced by the sample selection and non-random attrition was tested by comparing the weighted and unweighted distributions for demographic, substance use, and psychiatric disorders in the smaller selected CoT sample and the larger original unselected twin sample (for more details, see Harden et al., 2007). Below we report the unweighted and weighted estimates of the means and prevalences of the alcohol use disorder phenotypes used in this study.

Measures

All of the main measures for this study were obtained via telephone interviews with the twins, their spouses, and their offspring. The SSAGA interview (Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism; Bucholz, et al., 1994), originally developed for the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism, was modified for use as a telephone interview in Australia (SSAGA-OZ). Interviews were administered by trained lay-interviewers who were blind to the psychiatric status of the other members of the twin families.

Alcohol use disorder in parents and offspring

Slightly different versions of the SSAGA-OZ interview were used for the twins, the spouses of the twins, and the offspring. Of particular relevance for this study is that the interviews of the twins preceded, whereas the interviews of the spouses and offspring were conducted subsequent to, the introduction of the DSM-IV. The AD and AA symptoms from the twins, spouses, and offspring were scored to be consistent across the family members (although this is not required in the CoT design), while also conforming as closely as possible to the DSM-IV criteria. Only two symptoms deviated slightly from the DSM-IV criteria. The AD withdrawal symptom did not include using a "closely related substance" to relieve or avoid withdrawal. Similarly, the AA symptom of "continued use despite persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by alcohol" did not include physical fights as a qualifying problem. In both cases, this was because these were not a part of the DSM-III-R criteria and were not assessed in the twins. Therefore, they were not included in the DSM-IV AD and AA symptoms and diagnoses for the spouses and offspring.

Two different alcohol use disorder phenotypes were used to characterize the parental generation: a continuous count, ranging from 0–11, of the lifetime occurrence of the 7 DSM-IV AD and 4 DSM-IV AA symptoms (alcohol use disorder [AUD] symptom count), and a lifetime diagnosis of DSM-IV AD2. Alcohol use disorder among the offspring was characterized using the 11-item AUD symptom count. The choice of alcohol use disorder phenotypes in the parental and offspring generations was based on several considerations including reliability, statistical power, a consideration of how best to characterize alcohol use disorder in a parent as an environmental agent, and prior research. Based on consistent empirical support, experts now agree that alcohol use disorders "are best described on a continuum of severity" (Helzer et al., 2006). For example, results of latent trait modeling of alcohol use disorder symptoms suggest that they all are indicators of a single underlying dimension (Kahler & Strong, 2006; Krueger et al., 2004). Because of the many advantages of using a dimensional measure of alcohol use disorder, the AUD symptom count was used as the outcome in the offspring. However, because it was not clear that the environmental risk associated with having an "alcoholic" parent was a continuously-graded dose-response phenomenon, and to allow for comparisons with previous studies, we also used categorical AD diagnoses as predictors in the parents. The one-year test-retest reliability (Pearson's r) of the AUD symptom count among 176 re-interviewed offspring was 0.67, and the internal consistency reliability (coefficient alpha) in the full sample of offspring was 0.79. The reliability of lifetime diagnoses of AD is high and exceeds that of most other psychiatric diagnoses, with test-retest reliabilities (kappa) over intervals of two weeks to fifteen months ranging from 0.61 to 0.76 (for a review of reliability studies, see Slutske et al., 1998). Means and lifetime prevalences of the two alcohol use disorder phenotypes are presented in Table 13. As with any general community sample, identified cases of AD were relatively mild on average and only a small fraction of those diagnosed with lifetime DSM-IV alcohol dependence in this Australian general community sample had ever sought treatment for their alcohol problems (weighted lifetime prevalences of 3.4%, 3.3%, and 3.0% among twins, spouses, and offspring, respectively).

Table 1.

Mean number of lifetime DSM-IV alcohol use disorder symptoms and lifetime prevalence of DSM-IV alcohol dependence among twins, spouses, and offspring in the original samples and in the weighted CoT samples.

unweighted
unselected
original sample
weighted
selected
CoT ≥ 18 sample
estimate SE N estimate SE N
Mean alcohol use disorder symptom count
twins
men 1.76 0.05 2,042 1.84 0.20 409
women 0.61 0.02 3,847 0.58 0.10 815
spouses
men 1.96 0.04 2,398 2.06 0.10 629
women 0.95 0.04 1,446 0.87 0.08 354
offspring
men --- --- 2.23 0.09 1,158
women --- --- 1.56 0.10 1,176
Lifetime prevalence of alcohol dependence (%)
twins
men 17.1 0.9 2,042 16.2 3.3 409
women 5.0 0.4 3,847 6.7 3.3 815
spouses
men 16.5 0.8 2,398 15.2 1.8 629
women 6.4 0.7 1,446 5.0 1.9 354
offspring
men --- --- 32.2 2.4 1,158
women --- --- 22.5 3.5 1,176

Note: CoT = children of twins. Only offspring ≥ age 18 and their parents are included in the CoT samples. The alcohol use disorder symptom count was an 11-item scale including the 7 symptoms of alcohol dependence and the 4 symptoms of alcohol abuse.

Other parental characteristics

Demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses were included as covariates in some of the analyses. Educational attainment was measured on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from less than 7 years of schooling to university post-graduate training. Each parent’s age at the birth of their first child was calculated based on the twins’ and their spouses’ reports of all their children’s birth dates. Church attendance was measured on a 5-point Likert Scale ranging from never to more than once a week. A history of divorce was based on reports from the parents and their offspring. Two substance use variables were included -- ever smoking cigarettes, and ever using any illegal drug. Parental histories of externalizing and internalizing psychopathology were indexed by the number of lifetime DSM-III-R symptoms of conduct disorder and major depression, respectively. Lifetime history of suicidality was based on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from no thoughts or plans of suicide to a serious suicide attempt (Statham et al., 1998).

Data analysis

For all of the analyses, the AUD symptom counts in the twins, spouses, and offspring were rank normalized using a Blom transformation to reduce the skewness of their distributions and then standardized to means of zero and standard deviations of one (data transformations were conducted separately for the twin, spouse, and offspring samples). Data weights were used for all of analyses, and all of the available data were analyzed by using multiple imputation (Little & Rubin, 1987) to estimate values for missing parental characteristics. Five imputed data sets were generated based on psychiatric histories in the twins, spouses, and all of the offspring in the family, as well as demographic characteristics of the twins and spouses. The uncertainty due to the missing values was reflected in the standard errors obtained from the combined analysis of the five imputed data sets.

Two different data analytic approaches were employed to examine the influence of parental AUD on offspring AUD: a descriptive between-family comparison of offspring mean AUD symptom counts grouped according to parental and twin pair history of AUD, and multilevel modeling of offspring AUD symptom counts using parental and twin pair history of AUD as level 2 and level 3 predictors. Both types of analyses were conducted using SAS PROC MIXED software (Littel et al., 2006) in order to properly take into account the clustering of the observations within nuclear and twin families. Following the recommendations made by the APA Task Force on Statistical Inference (Wilkinson and TFSI, 1999) and based on the important contributions of Cohen (1994), Kline (2004), Loftus (1996), Lykken (1991), and Schmidt (1996), the primary focus of this paper is the estimation of effect sizes and confidence intervals, rather than on null hypothesis significance testing.

Between-family means comparisons

In the descriptive between-family comparisons, all of the means were adjusted for the influence of offspring sex, age, age2, and spouse AUD (i.e. "assortative mating")4. The twin-spouse correlations for the 11-item AUD (r=.10, SE=.03, p<.001) and 7-item AD symptom counts (r=.13, SE=.03, p<.001) were statistically significant, but modest. These low correlations are within the range of estimates (rs = −0.21 to 0.40) obtained in other published community-based studies that have examined assortative mating for AUDs (Agrawal et al., 2006; Maes et al., 1998; McLeod, 1993; Kendler et al., 1994).

The descriptive between-family analyses started with a comparison of the mean AUD symptom counts for offspring divided into two groups based on whether the twin parent was affected versus unaffected with an AUD. This is the type of comparison available in a traditional children-of-alcoholics study, and includes the combined influence of the specific causal environmental effect of parental AUD as well as genetic and family environmental factors that are correlated with having an alcoholic parent.

The offspring were then further divided, based on whether the parent's co-twin was affected versus unaffected with an AUD, into the following four groups: concordant unaffected, discordant - parent unaffected, discordant - parent affected, and concordant affected (see Figure 1). These four groups of offspring were then subdivided based on the zygosity of the twin parent. The comparison of primary interest here was between the offspring of discordant - parent affected MZ twins versus the offspring of discordant - parent unaffected MZ twins. This comparison provides the most stringent test of a potential causal effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD. This comparison unconfounds the specific effect of parental AUD from correlated familial factors. If the AUD symptom counts among the offspring in the discordant - parent affected group is higher than observed among the offspring in the discordant - parent unaffected group, then a specific effect of parental AUD is implicated. Conversely, if there are equally elevated AUD symptom counts among the offspring in the discordant - parent affected group and the discordant - parent unaffected group, then correlated familial factors are implicated in the association between offspring and parental AUD. Pairwise comparisons of offspring groups were made based on differences between the standardized least squares means estimated in SAS PROC MIXED, along with the confidence interval associated with this difference in standardized means.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Illustration of the three types of twin families and four different types of nuclear families included in the study. Males are represented by boxes, females are represented by circles, and shading represents being affected with an alcohol use disorder. For illustration, male-male monozygotic twin pairs with three offspring are depicted, but female-female, female-male, dizyotic twin pairs, and twin families with anywhere from one to 10 children were also included in the study.

Analyses were conducted using each of the two parental phenotypes -- AUD symptom counts and AD diagnoses. For the purposes of these mean comparisons, the AUD symptom counts for the twin parents were split at less than 2 symptoms ("unaffected") and 2 or more symptoms ("affected"). Because AUD and AD symptom counts are continuously distributed, it is possible that there were discordant twin pairs who were "discordant" but differed by only a single symptom because the "affected" twin met the diagnostic threshold or cut-off, and the "unaffected" twin fell short by a single symptom (e.g. the affected twin had three AD symptoms and the unaffected co-twin had two). If this level of discordance represented a large proportion of the discordant twin pairs, an influence of parental AUD would be difficult to detect. There were 245 discordant twin pairs when discordance was defined as one twin reporting 2 or more AUD symptoms and the co-twin reporting less than two AUD symptoms, and 152 discordant twin pairs when discordance was defined as one twin being diagnosed with AD and the co-twin not being diagnosed with AD. The mean differences between the twins in the number of AUD or AD symptoms reported among these discordant pairs was 3.1 and 3.0, respectively; 51% and 66% differed by 3 or more symptoms, and only 15% and 13% of the discordant pairs differed by only a single symptom. Thus, in the majority of discordant pairs, the affected twin experienced substantially higher levels of AUD or AD symptomatology than did the unaffected co-twin.

Within-family multilevel modeling

An alternative within-family regression-based data analytic approach was also used. Unlike the simpler between-family means comparisons, the regression-based approach provided rigorous hypothesis testing of effects, made direct comparisons between cousins from discordant twin pairs, statistically controlled for a number of measured parental characteristics, and allowed for tests of sex differences. Three-level multilevel models (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Singer, 1998) were employed to account for the complex data structure of 2,334 individual offspring (level 1) nested within 1,224 nuclear families (level 2; sibships) nested within 836 twin families (level 3; "cousinships"). The mean numbers of offspring in each sibship and cousinship were 1.9 (SD = 0.9, range = 1–6) and 2.8 (SD = 1.6, range = 1–10), respectively. A series of six different multilevel regression models predicting offspring AUD symptom counts from parental and twin pair history of AUD were fit to the data (see Table 2 for a description of the six models; see D'Onofrio et al. [2005] for detailed algebraic information about the models). Two sets of analyses were conducted using either parental AUD symptom counts entered as a continuous predictor or parental AD diagnoses entered as a dichotomous predictor (coded 0=unaffected, 1=affected). The potential influence of assortative mating was taken into account in the multilevel models by including either spouse AUD symptom counts or AD diagnoses as level 2 predictors in Models 2–6 listed in Table 2. In both sets of analyses, AUD of the spouse provided an incremental contribution over and above twin pair AUD to the prediction of offspring AUD.

Table 2.

Summary of the multilevel regression models tested.

Model Description Parameters of interest Explanation and interpretation

1 unconditional model random effects The proportion of variation in offspring AUD explained by each of the three levels.

2 phenotypic model twin parent AUD and spouse parent AUD A traditional analysis of the effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD, correctly accounting for the levels of clustering in the data.

3 phenotypic model with covariates reductions in twin parent AUD and spouse parent AUD The extent to which the relation between parental and offspring AUD is reduced when controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses.

4 cotwin-control model between-twin-family effect of twins' AUD The effect of the mean AUD score for the twin pair on offspring AUD. (For a diagnosis, this would equal 0 for concordant unaffected twin pairs, 0.5 for discordant pairs, and 1 for concordant affected pairs.) This affects siblings and cousins equally.
within-twin-family effect of twin's AUD The difference between an individual twin parent AUD score and the mean AUD score for the twin pair. This affects siblings equally, but can differ for cousins. Twins who are concordant (or have the same number of symptoms) will both have scores of zero; in discordant pairs, the twin with an AUD (or with more AUD symptoms) will have a positive score, and the twin without an AUD (or with fewer symptoms) will have a negative score. This parameter estimates whether, among the offspring of discordant twin pairs, the offspring of the twin with an AUD (or with more AUD symptoms) have more AUD symptoms than their cousins. This provides a key test of a potential causal effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD.

5 within-zygosity cotwin-control model MZ within-twin-family effect of twin's AUD The within-twin-family effect from model 4, in the presence of a new interaction term, estimates whether, among the offspring of discordant MZ twin pairs, the offspring of the twin with an AUD (or more AUD symptoms) have more AUD symptoms than their cousins. This provides the most stringent test of a potential causal effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD.
DZ-MZ within-twin-family effect of twin's AUD The interaction of the within-twin-family effect from model 4 and twin zygosity (coded MZ=0 and DZ=1), in the presence of the within-twin-family effect from model 4, estimates whether the within-twin-family effect differs by zygosity. This provides a test of whether the familial confounds explaining the association between parental and offspring AUD are in part genetic.

6 within-zygosity cotwin-control model with covariates reduction in the between-twin-family effect of twins' AUD The extent to which the between-family association between parental and offspring AUD is reduced when controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses.
reduction in the MZ within-twin-family effect of twin's AUD The extent to which the potential causal effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD is reduced when controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses.

Note: AUD = alcohol use disorder, MZ = monozygotic, DZ = dizygotic

Four tests were conducted by including additional predictors in Model 2, but because these yielded non-significant effects (in both sets of analyses), they were not retained in subsequent models: (1) the interactive effect of twin and spouse AUD in predicting offspring AUD was tested by including their interaction term as a level 2 predictor, (2) the differential effects of paternal versus maternal AUD in predicting offspring AUD was examined (after recoding the twin and spouse AUD indicators into paternal and maternal AUD indicators) by including them as separate level 2 predictors, (3) the differential prediction of AUD in male versus female offspring was tested by including the interaction of parental AUD with offspring sex in the model as a level 1 predictor, and (4) differences in the prediction of AUD as a function of offspring age was tested by including the interaction of parental AUD with offspring age and/or age2 in the model as a level 1 predictor. Other psychopathology and substance use in the twins and spouses were included (as level 2 predictors) in Models 3 and 6 to examine the specificity of the effect of parental AUD and to isolate the effect of parental AUD from other measured risk factors.

Results

Prior to conducting the more complicated genetically-informed analyses based on offspring AUD symptom counts, we conducted standard children-of-alcoholics analyses of the relation between parental and offspring AD diagnoses in order to facilitate comparisons with previous children-of-alcoholics studies. Logistic regression analyses were conducted predicting offspring AD diagnosis from parental AD diagnosis accounting for the clustering in the data and using weights to account for the selected nature of the sample. Offspring of parents with a history of AD were significantly more likely to have a history of AD than offspring of parents without a history of AD (odds ratio = 2.7; 95% CI: 1.2 – 6.2; χ2 = 5.3, df = 1, p = 0.02). The strength of this association did not differ when the father versus the mother had a history of AD (χ2 = 0.8, df = 1, p = 0.38), or when the offspring was male or female (χ2 = 0.5, df = 1, p = 0.50).

Offspring mean AUD symptom counts by parental and twin pair history of alcohol use disorder

Using alcohol use disorder symptom counts in parents

The first two rows in Table 3 ("All Twins") show that offspring of parents with two or more lifetime symptoms of AUD had higher scores on the AUD symptom count than offspring of parents with fewer than two lifetime AUD symptoms (0.17 versus −0.06), although the difference in standardized means was small (effect size [d] = 0.24, 95% CI: 0.12 – 0.35). This association was probably not due to a causal effect of parental AUD, because the mean symptom count was the same among the offspring of the affected compared to the offspring of the unaffected twins from MZ discordant pairs (0.00 versus 0.02, d = −0.02, 95% CI: −0.31 – 0.28; Rows 4 and 5 in Table 3).57

Table 3.

Mean covariate-adjusteda offspring alcohol use disorder symptom counts by twin parent or twin pair alcohol use disorder symptom counts of less than 2 (unaffected) or 2 or more (affected) and family type.

Row # Alcohol Use Disorder in
Twin Parent or Twin Pair
Offspring Alcohol Use Disorder Symptom Count

M 95% CI N Offspring N Twins/Twin Pairs
All Twins
1 Twin Parent Unaffected −0.06 −0.12 – −0.01 1,778 922 twins
2 Twin Parent Affected 0.17 0.07 – 0.27 556 302 twins
Monozygotic Twin Pairs
3 Concordant - Unaffected −0.10 −0.18 – −0.02 809 271 pairs

4 Discordant - Parent Unaffected 0.02 −0.17 – 0.22 126 79 pairs
5 Discordant - Parent Affected 0.00 −0.23 – 0.23 86

6 Concordant - Affected 0.32 0.14 – 0.51 171 62 pairs
Dizygotic Twin Pairs
7 Concordant - Unaffected −0.07 −0.16 – 0.02 620 209 pairs

8 Discordant - Parent Unaffected 0.02 −0.13 – 0.17 223 166 pairs
9 Discordant - Parent Affected 0.03 −0.14 – 0.19 187

10 Concordant - Affected 0.41 0.19 – 0.62 113 49 pairs

Note. CI = confidence interval,

a

covariates included were offspring age, age2, and sex, and co-parent (spouse) alcohol use disorder. Offspring symptom counts were transformed and standardized to a mean of zero and standard deviation of one. Means were weighted to produce estimates from the entire Australian Twin Registry cohort, and the confidence intervals reflect the clustered nature of the data.

Using alcohol dependence diagnoses in parents

Offspring of parents with a lifetime diagnosis of AD had higher scores on the AUD symptom count than offspring of parents without a lifetime diagnosis of AD (0.25 versus −0.03, d = 0.29, 95% CI: 0.12 – 0.46; Rows 1 and 2 in Table 4). It was not possible to establish a causal effect of parental AD because the mean symptom count was not substantially higher among the offspring of the affected compared to the offspring of the unaffected twins from the MZ discordant pairs (0.21 versus 0.03, d = 0.17, 95% CI: −0.24 – 0.58; Rows 4 and 5 in Table 4)6.

Table 4.

Mean covariate-adjusteda offspring alcohol use disorder symptom counts by twin parent or twin pair history of alcohol dependence and family type.

Row # Alcohol Dependence in
Twin Parent or Twin Pair
Offspring Alcohol Use Disorder Symptom Count

M 95% CI N Offspring N Twins/Twin Pairs
All Twins
1 Twin Parent Unaffected −0.03 −0.08 – 0.01 2,116 1,102 twins
2 Twin Parent Affected 0.25 0.09 – 0.42 218 122 twins
Monozygotic Twin Pairs
3 Concordant - Unaffected −0.05 −0.12 – 0.02 1023 348 pairs

4 Discordant - Parent Unaffected 0.03 −0.25 – 0.31 78 48 pairs
5 Discordant - Parent Affected 0.21 −0.11 – 0.53 55

6 Concordant - Affected 0.41 −0.02 – 0.84 36 16 pairs
Dizygotic Twin Pairs
7 Concordant - Unaffected −0.05 −0.12 – 0.03 878 315 pairs

8 Discordant - Parent Unaffected 0.17 −0.03 – 0.37 138 104 pairs
9 Discordant - Parent Affected 0.23 0.02 – 0.45 110

10 Concordant - Affected 0.44 −0.22 – 1.10 17 5 pairs

Note. CI = confidence interval,

a

covariates included were offspring age, age2, and sex, and co-parent (spouse) alcohol use disorder. Offspring symptom counts were transformed and standardized to a mean of zero and standard deviation of one. Means were weighted to produce estimates from the entire Australian Twin Registry cohort, and the confidence intervals reflect the clustered nature of the data.

In both sets of analyses, offspring from twin pairs in which both twins were affected with an AUD were at higher risk for the development of AUD symptoms than offspring from discordant pairs in which only their parent was affected. This makes sense under the widely-held assumption that the etiology of AUD is multifactorial, and suggests that families with two affected twins will have a greater loading of the familial risk factors for AUD than families with only one affected twin. This increased familial loading may be due to either genetic or environmental factors that are shared by twins. In the previous CoT study of Jacob et al. (2003), offspring of parents corresponding to those from rows 5, 6, 9, and 10 in Table 4 were combined together into a single "MZ and DZ AD" group. The results presented in Table 4 suggest that there will be stronger evidence for a potential causal effect of parental AD when this combined group is used in analyses because the difference between this combined parent affected group (i.e., corresponding to rows 5, 6, 9, and 10 combined) and the parent unaffected group will be larger than the difference that is obtained when analyses are limited to the offspring of discordant twin pairs.

In the CoT design, evidence for genetic risk factors contributing to parent-offspring transmission can be demonstrated by observing more pathology in the offspring of the unaffected twins from MZ discordant pairs compared to the offspring of the unaffected twins from DZ discordant pairs. In neither set of between-family analyses was there such evidence of genetic risk contributing to parent-offspring transmission of AUD (Rows 4 and 8 in Table 3: 0.02 versus 0.02, d = 0.01, 95% CI: −0.24 – 0.25; Rows 4 and 8 in Table 4: 0.03 versus 0.17, d = −0.15, 95% CI: −0.49 – 0.20). However, the very broad confidence intervals around these effect size estimates indicate that effect sizes consistent with heritabilities as high as 100% and 80% could not be ruled out based on these results. This highlights the low power of this CoT design for detecting genetic effects.

Multilevel regression models of the association between parental and offspring AUD

Using alcohol use disorder symptom counts in parents

Table 5 presents the results of fitting the six multilevel regression models predicting offspring AUD symptom counts from AUD symptom counts in the parents. The unconditional model (Model 1) indicated that there was significant variation in offspring AUD attributable to all three levels -- the individual offspring, nuclear family, and twin family levels. The proportion of variation accounted for by the nuclear family level (0.21/0.21 + 0.07 + 0.60) can be interpreted as the sibling intraclass correlation for AUD symptoms (r = 0.24). Similarly, the proportion of variation accounted for by the twin family level (0.07/0.07 + 0.21 + 0.60) can be interpreted as the cousin intraclass correlation for AUD symptoms (r = 0.08). The remaining 68% of the variation in offspring AUD was accounted for by influences not shared with siblings or cousins (and also any measurement error).

Table 5.

Parameter estimates of multilevel models predicting offspring alcohol use disorder (AUD) symptom counts from AUD symptom counts in the twin parent or twin pair.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Parameter b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE
Random effects (variance components)
   Twin family level (3) .07* .03 .06* .03 .06* .03 .06* .03 .06* .03 .06* .03
   Nuclear family level (2) .21* .04 .20* .03 .19* .03 .20* .03 .20* .03 .19* .03
   Offspring level (1) .60* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02
Fixed effects
Twin pair AUD
   Parent AUD (level 2) .14* .02 .09* .03
   Between twin families (level 3) .18* .03 .18* .03 .13* .04
   Within twin families (level 2) .06 .04
   MZ within twin families (level 2) .07 .07 .05 .07
   DZ-MZ within twin families (level 2) −.01 .08 −.02 .08
Spouse AUD (level 2) .14* .03 .14* .03 .14* .03 .14* .03 .14* .03
Offspring covariates (level 1)
   Sex .45* .04 .46* .04 .45* .04 .45* .04 .46* .04
   Age .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04
   Age2 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001
Twin Covariatesa (level 2)
   Cigarette use .13* .05 .13* .05
   History of conduct disorder symptoms .08* .02 .08* .02
Spouse Covariatesa (level 2)
   Age at first child −.01* .006 −.01* .006

Note: Parameters are unstandardized. Parental and offspring symptom counts are distributed as z-scores. The parameters were weighted to produce estimates from the entire Australian Twin Registry cohort, and the standard errors reflect the clustered nature of the data.

a

Only covariates with significant unique effects are shown; the covariates included for both twins and spouses were: educational attainment, age at the birth of their first child, frequency of church attendance, divorce, use of cigarettes, use of any illegal substances, conduct disorder symptoms, symptoms of major depression, suicidality. See Table 2 for an explanation of the models.

The phenotypic model (Model 2) is similar to a traditional children-of-alcoholics study. This model confirmed a small but statistically significant effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD. Each standard deviation in AUD symptoms in the twin or in the spouse was associated with an additional 0.14 standard deviations in AUD symptoms among the offspring. The influence of maternal alcohol problems was slightly larger (b=0.11, 95% CI: 0.07 – 0.15) but not statistically different from paternal alcohol problems (b=0.08, 95% CI: 0.06 – 0.11) in predicting offspring AUD. The phenotypic model with covariates (Model 3) indicated that the effect of twin and spouse AUD on offspring AUD was still statistically significant after controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses.

The results of fitting the cotwin-control model (Model 4) indicated that there was a statistically significant between-twin-family effect of AUD in a twin pair (b = 0.18, 95% CI: 0.12 – 0.24), in other words, offspring of parents from twin pairs with a higher mean number of symptoms between the two twins had more AUD symptoms than unrelated offspring of parents from twin pairs with a lower mean number of symptoms between the two twins. There was not a statistically significant within-twin-family effect (b = 0.06, 95% CI: −0.02 – 0.14), meaning that offspring whose twin parent had more symptoms did not experience more AUD symptoms than their cousins whose twin parent had fewer symptoms, and the results of Model 5 indicated that there also was not a statistically significant within-twin-family effect among the offspring of MZ twin pairs (b = 0.07, 95% CI: −0.07 – 0.21), nor was there a zygosity difference in the within-twin-family effect of twin AUD (b = −0.01, 95% CI: −0.17 – 0.15), suggesting that the factors responsible for the parent-offspring transmission were unrelated to genetic risk. The results of Model 6 indicated that there was a reduced but still statistically significant between-twin-family effect of AUD in a twin pair even after controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses (b = 0.13, 95% CI: 0.05 – 0.21).

Using alcohol dependence diagnoses in parents

The pattern of results of the multilevel regression models that used AD diagnoses in the parents as predictors was similar to the models that used AUD symptom counts (see Table 6). An AD diagnosis in the twin or in the spouse was associated with increases of 0.29 (95% CI: 0.11 – 0.47) and 0.24 (95% CI: 0.08 – 0.40) standard deviations, respectively, in AUD symptoms among the offspring (Model 2). The influence of maternal AD was slightly larger (b=0.31, 95% CI: 0.03 – 0.53) but not statistically different from paternal AD (b=0.25, 95% CI: 0.11 – 0.38) in predicting offspring AUD. After controlling for demographic characteristics, other substance use, and other psychopathology in the twins and spouses, the influence of twin and spouse AD remained statistically significant (Model 3). Models 4–6 indicated that there were statistically significant between-twin-family effects (b = 0.45, 95% CI: 0.23 – 0.67) that remained significant even with the inclusion of covariates (b = 0.33, 95% CI: 0.09 – 0.57), but there was no evidence of statistically significant within-twin-family effects among all of the offspring (b = 0.10, 95% CI: −0.14 – 0.34), among the offspring of MZ twin pairs (b = 0.19, 95% CI: −0.22 – 0.60), or a zygosity difference in the within-twin-family effect of twin AUD (b = −0.13, 95% CI: −0.64 – 0.38).

Table 6.

Parameter estimates of multilevel models predicting offspring alcohol use disorder (AUD) symptom counts from AD diagnosis in the twin parent or twin pair.

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Parameter b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE
Random effects (variance components)
   Twin family level (3) .07* .03 .06* .03 .06* .03 .07* .03 .07* .03 .06* .03
   Nuclear family level (2) .21* .04 .22* .04 .20* .03 .21* .04 .21* .04 .20* .03
   Offspring level (1) .60* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02 .54* .02
Fixed effects
Twin pair Alcohol Dependence
   Parent AUD (level 2) .29* .09 .17* .09
   Between twin families (level 3) .45* .11 .45* .11 .33* .12
   Within twin families (level 2) .10 .12
   MZ within twin families (level 2) .19 .21 .10 .21
   DZ-MZ within twin families (level 2) −.13 .26 −.15 .25
Spouse Alcohol Dependence (level 2) .24* .08 .20* .08 .24* .08 .23* .08 .20* .08
Offspring covariates (level 1)
   Sex .45* .04 .46* .04 .45* .04 .45* .04 .46* .04
   Age .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04 .20* .04
   Age2 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001 −.004* <.001
Twin Covariatesa(level 2)
   Cigarette use .16* .05 .15* .05
   History of conduct disorder symptoms .08* .02 .08* .02
Spouse Covariatesa(level 2)
   Age at first child −.01* .006 −.01* .006

Note: Parameters are unstandardized. Offspring symptom counts are distributed as z-scores. The parameters were weighted to produce estimates from the entire Australian Twin Registry cohort, and the standard errors reflect the clustered nature of the data.

a

Only covariates with significant unique effects are shown; the covariates included for both twins and spouses were: educational attainment, age at the birth of their first child, frequency of church attendance, divorce, use of cigarettes, use of any illegal substances, conduct disorder symptoms, symptoms of major depression, and suicidality. See Table 2 for an explanation of the models.

A comparison of the results using the two analytic approaches

The results of the between-family means comparisons and within-family multilevel regression modeling were consistent in failing to detect a statistically significant effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD symptoms after controlling for correlated familial factors, and in suggesting that this effect was modest at best. In the between-family analyses the differences in standardized means ranged from −0.02 to 0.17 between the offspring of affected versus unaffected twins from discordant MZ twin pairs. In the within-family multilevel regression modeling, the differences between the offspring of twins from discordant MZ pairs were small and positive, with regression coefficients indicating that being the offspring of the twin with more AUD symptoms (when AUD symptom counts among the parents was used as the predictor) or the offspring of the affected twin in discordant pairs (when AD diagnoses among the parents was used as the predictor) were associated with increases in AUD symptoms in the offspring ranging from 0.07 to 0.19 standard deviations. In neither set of analyses was it possible to discern whether the correlated familial factors responsible for the association between offspring and parental AUD were correlated genetic factors or correlated environments.

Post hoc power calculations

By focusing on the size of parameter estimates and their associated confidence intervals, the issue of statistical power becomes less relevant. This is because: (a) even in the absence of statistical significance, an effect size and its confidence interval provides an important contribution to cumulative knowledge in psychology (Schmidt, 1996), and (b) once a study is conducted and the data are analyzed, "confidence intervals replace calculated power in describing results" (Wilkinson and TFSI, 1999, p. 596). Nonetheless, many readers expect to see a discussion of statistical power when statistically non-significant results are obtained. Post hoc power calculations may provide useful information for interpreting the results of this study, and provide a justification for why there wasn't more of a focus on parsing the correlated familial factors responsible for the association between offspring and parental AUD into genetic and environmental components. Thus we present approximate power for the between-family means comparisons based on published power tables (Cohen, 1988), and the power for the within-family multilevel regression modeling based on a series of simulations.

In the between-family means comparisons, there was inadequate power to detect the very small mean differences observed between the offspring of discordant - affected and discordant - unaffected twins in the combined sample of MZ and DZ discordant twin pairs6 (power ≤ 0.20), or among the offspring of affected versus unaffected twins discordant MZ twin pairs (power ≤ 0.20), respectable power to detect small effect sizes of 0.30 in the combined sample of the offspring of affected versus unaffected twins from discordant MZ and DZ twin pairs (power ≥ 0.74), and inadequate power to detect effect sizes less than 0.30 among the offspring of affected versus unaffected twins from discordant MZ twin pairs (power < 0.47). The power to detect a very small effect size of 0.13 between the offspring of unaffected MZ versus DZ twins from discordant pairs (an effect size consistent with a heritability of 52%) was also inadequate (power < 0.35)8.

In the within-family multilevel regression modeling that used AUD symptom counts of the parents as predictors, the power was inadequate (power = 0.16) to detect the small effect observed in the present study, but was respectable (power = 0.72) to detect a within-twin-family-effect of b = 0.15 (still a modest effect). Given the within-twin-family effect observed in the present study (b = 0.06), or an even larger within-twin-family effect of b = 0.15, the power was only 0.07 or 0.21 to detect a statistically significant interaction between the within-twin-family effect and twin zygosity. This interaction term provides a test of whether the familial confounds explaining the association between parental and offspring AUD are in part genetic (see Table 2). The power to parse the within-family confounds into genetic and environmental components was extremely low.

Effect of exposure to parental alcoholism

In general community samples, many individuals with a history of AUD will represent transient short-lived cases that remitted in early adulthood. The results of this study might be explained by the fact that some of the offspring of affected parents may not have been directly exposed to or had limited exposure to their parents' alcohol problems (Windle & Tubman, 1999). The recencies of AUD symptoms were not assessed in the twins, but were assessed in the spouses. Based on the self-reports of symptom recencies among the spouses with any AUD symptoms, we determined that 14% of the offspring were born after the last symptom occurrence, and thus were never directly exposed to the alcohol problems in this particular parent. However, any possible exposure or years of possible exposure to the alcohol problems of this parent from birth to age 18 (based on spouse self-reports of symptom onsets and offsets) did not significantly predict offspring symptom counts over and above spouse AUD symptom counts or diagnoses of AD.

Discussion

Among the 2,334 offspring of twins in this study, 556 (23.8%) had a twin parent with a history of at least two AUD symptoms, and 218 (9.3%) had a twin parent with a history of AD. The offspring who were the children of alcoholics had higher AUD symptom counts than the offspring who were the children of nonalcoholics (effect sizes of 0.24 and 0.29 based on the broader and narrower definitions of alcoholism in the parents, respectively). These small differences in AUD symptom counts in the children of alcoholics versus the children of nonalcoholics can potentially be explained by the combined influence of two types of risk factors: (a) exposure to alcoholism in a parent, including characteristics of the childhood rearing environment provided by, and direct modeling of the drinking behavior of, the alcoholic parent, and (b) familial risk factors that are associated with having an alcoholic birth parent, including susceptibility genes and environmental correlates such as religious affiliation and socioeconomic status.

Studying the offspring of twins that are discordant for alcoholism allows one to unconfound these two sets of potential explanatory variables. The first set of risk factors would only pertain to the offspring of the affected twins, whereas the second set of risk factors would pertain to both the offspring of the affected and the unaffected twins. The most stringent control for correlated genetic factors comes from a comparison of the offspring of MZ twins discordant for alcoholism. Among the 212 offspring of MZ twins discordant for the broader definition of alcoholism and the 133 offspring of MZ twins discordant for the narrower definition of alcoholism, there was no longer a statistically significant difference between the children of alcoholics and the children of nonalcoholics (effect sizes of −0.02 and 0.17). The results of this study suggest that if there is a potentially causal effect of being exposed to an alcoholic parent, it is modest at best.

Failure to detect a significant direct environmental effect of exposure to parental alcoholism is consistent with the results of adoption studies (Cadoret et al., 1985; Cadoret et al., 1987; Cloninger et al., 1985; Goodwin, et al., 1974; McGue et al., 1996), and a study of adult female twins and their parents (Kendler et al., 1994) but is inconsistent with the results of the only other CoT study of the intergenerational transmission of alcoholism (Jacob et al., 2003). The most likely explanation for this difference is in the disparate analytic approaches used in the two studies, in particular, the way that the data from the offspring of twin pairs who were concordant for alcoholism were handled. In the previous study, such offspring were combined together with offspring of twin pairs in which only the parent was affected, and the additional information gained by taking into account the density of alcoholism in the twin pair was ignored. Thus, the outcomes in these offspring contributed to the direct effect of parental alcoholism, that is, to the potential causal effect. In the present study, the density of alcoholism in the twin pair was taken into account in the between-family means comparisons by treating the offspring of concordant affected twin pairs as a distinct group, and in the multilevel analyses by modeling the effect of having 0, 1, or 2 members of a twin pair affected with alcoholism (or modeling the mean AUD symptom count for the twin pair when using continuous symptom counts among the parents as a predictor) as a between-twin-family effect, that is, a correlated familial effect. This correlated familial effect was a significant predictor of offspring alcohol use disorder (regression coefficients associated with increases in AUD symptoms in the offspring ranging from 0.15 and 0.45 standard deviations), and was also a much stronger predictor than the within-family effect of exposure to an alcoholic parent. This correlated familial effect was not specific to the offspring of alcoholic parents, but extended to the cousins of these offspring as well, even when the cousin's parent was unaffected. In other words, the nephews and nieces of alcoholics were also at increased risk for alcoholism.

Results from two of the Iowa adoption studies (Cadoret et al., 1985, Cadoret et al., 1987) suggest that this correlated familial effect may be due, at least in part, to non-genetic differences between families. In one study, the magnitude of the associations of adoptee alcohol abuse with alcohol problems in the adoptive parents, siblings, and more distant relatives (uncles, aunts, and grandparents) were equal (odds ratios = 3.1; Cadoret et al., 1985), and in the other study the magnitude of the associations between adoptee alcohol abuse and alcohol problems in the adoptive parents (odds ratio = 2.7) and in more distant relatives (odds ratio = 2.8) were nearly equal (Cadoret et al., 1987). (Because of small sample sizes, none of these associations in the Iowa adoption studies were statistically significant.) Cadoret et al (1985; 1987) speculated that these similar associations might be due to a common environmental factor such as commonly-held family attitudes towards alcohol consumption or cultural factors associated with family ethnicity. By incorporating parents, offspring, uncles/aunts, and nieces/nephews, the CoT design offers a unique perspective on the effective family environment. The results of this study are consistent with the hypothesis of a "common environmental factor" that Cadoret described over two decades ago, based on the results obtained from studies of adoptive families. Although the cause of the associations are unambiguous in data obtained from adoptive families, it was not possible with these CoT data to disentangle the extent to which the correlated familial factors responsible for the association between offspring and parental AUD were correlated environmental or correlated genetic factors.

Characterizing the familial risk for alcoholism

Alcoholism researchers are increasingly recognizing the value of using a more fine-grained approach to characterizing the familial risk for alcoholism, rather than a simple dichotomous classification based solely on a parent (Stoltenberg et al., 1998; Windle, 1996). For example, based on alcoholism in the parents and grandparents, studies have categorized offspring as coming from unigenerational or multigenerational alcoholism pedigrees (e.g. Finn & Pihl, 1988), low-, medium-, or high-risk families (e.g. Barnow et al., 2002; Windle, 1996), have used continuous counts of the number of affected relatives (e.g. Schuckit et al., 2006), or have used weighted counts based on the degree of relatedness of the relative to the offspring (e.g. Zhou et al., 2006). Other studies have also included the uncles and aunts of the offspring in determining the familial density of alcoholism (Hill & Yuan, 1999; Hill et al., 2000). The benefits of this approach include the increased statistical power gained from using a graded or continuous indicator of family history risk, and the identification of offspring at very high familial risk for alcoholism. Two results from the present study support this perspective. First, in all of the multilevel models, there was a significant additive (but not interactive) effect of twin and spouse alcoholism on offspring AUD symptoms. Offspring with both an alcoholic father and alcoholic mother had higher levels of AUD symptoms than offspring with only one alcoholic parent. Second, the density of alcoholism in the twin pair significantly predicted AUD symptoms in the offspring. Typically, the goal of creating an index of the familial density of alcoholism is to identify offspring who are at especially high genetic risk for alcoholism -- it is also likely that they are at especially high environmental risk for alcoholism as well.

Effect of level of exposure to an alcoholic parent

Unlike density of alcoholism in the nuclear and twin family, the level of exposure to an alcoholic parent was not related to offspring risk of developing AUD symptoms. Because of concerns that some of the offspring of alcoholic parents may never have been directly exposed to their parent's alcoholism, we identified instances in which the parent reported that their last AUD symptom offset occurred before the birth of the child (such information was only available for the spouses of the twins). The 14% of offspring who were born after their parent's "recovery" (similar to 24% obtained in another community-based study of children of alcoholics; DeLucia et al., 2001) were not at significantly lower risk for developing AUD symptoms than the offspring who were exposed to an alcoholic parent, and the number of years that the offspring could have been exposed to alcohol-related problems in the parent was also not related to offspring AUD over and above the number of AUD symptoms or an AD diagnosis in the parent. In the Danish Adoption study, there was also no systematic relation between the length of exposure to an alcoholic parent and AUD outcomes in the 30 sons who remained with their alcoholic birth parents (Goodwin et al., 1974), and in a community-based longitudinal study of children of alcoholics, there was no association between the recency of paternal AD symptoms (e.g. whether symptoms occurred in the past 3–5 years) and AUD diagnoses in offspring assessed in adolescence or in young adulthood (Chassin et al., 1999). In a recent paper presenting follow-up analyses of the data from the CoT study of Jacob et al. (2003), Duncan et al. (2006) reported that exposure to paternal alcoholism before age 13 was not related to AUD outcomes in the offspring of fathers with a history of AD. These findings cast further doubt on the hypothesis that there is a direct environmental effect of having an alcoholic parent on offspring AUD outcomes.

Sex-of-parent effects

The inclusion of large numbers of both male and female twins and spouses allowed for an examination of sex-of-parent effects based on direct interviews with both parents and their offspring. There is continuing debate over whether the offspring of alcoholic mothers are at increased risk for alcoholism compared to the offspring of non-alcoholic mothers, and whether the risk conferred by an alcoholic mother equals the risk conferred by an alcoholic father. The results of a meta-analysis of 32 studies of the families of alcoholics published between 1930 and 1982 suggested that there was a stronger influence of paternal than maternal alcoholism on the risk of alcoholism in offspring (Pollock et al., 1987). However, when only studies that included both fathers and mothers with alcoholism were examined, the influence of paternal and maternal alcoholism was nearly equal (McGue & Slutske, 1996). Additional within-study evidence from a large population-based twin study in which both parents were directly interviewed about their history of alcoholism (Kendler et al., 1994) and analyses of the family history reports from 5,877 participants in the National Comorbidity Survey (Kendler et al., 1997) also suggested that the offspring of alcoholic mothers were as likely to develop alcoholism as the offspring of alcoholic fathers. More recently, a large community-based longitudinal study (using either a combination of direct interview and family history reports or only family history reports) reported odds ratios of the associations between maternal versus paternal AUD and offspring AD of 1.7 (nonsignificant) and 2.3 (significant), respectively (Lieb et al., 2002). Although the researchers concluded from this that paternal, but not maternal, AUD was related to AD in the offspring, it is not clear without conducting a formal test of the difference between these associations that an important sex-of-parent effect exists (the confidence intervals substantially overlapped). In the present study, there was no evidence for sex-of-parent effects; paternal versus maternal AUD (either AUD symptoms or AD diagnoses) conferred comparable risk for AUD symptoms in the offspring. Although there is disagreement in the literature, there appears to be stronger evidence to support the hypothesis that there are no sex-of-parent effects in the intergenerational transmission of alcoholism.

Sex-of-offspring effects

There is less controversy in the literature about whether there are sex-of-offspring effects in the intergenerational transmission of alcoholism. Along with a number of other studies (e.g. Chassin et al., 1999; Kendler et al., 1997; Lieb et al., 2002; Lynskey et al., 1994), we found that parental AUD (either AUD symptoms or AD diagnoses) conferred the same risk for the development of AUD symptoms in male as in female offspring (although Sher et al. [1991] obtained a stronger effect of paternal alcoholism for young women than young men in a children-of-alcoholics study of college freshman). All of the studies are in agreement that both male and female children of alcoholic parents are at increased risk compared to the children of non-alcoholic parents for developing AUDs.

Specificity of effects

The associations between parent and twin pair AUD and offspring AUD, although somewhat reduced, persisted even after taking into account indicators of externalizing and internalizing psychopathology and other substance use in the parents. At least five more thorough investigations of the specificity of the effect of parental alcoholism on offspring AUD (and other outcomes) have been undertaken (Chassin et al., 1999; Hicks et al., 2004; Kendler et al., 1997; Lynskey et al., 1994; Ohannessian et al, 2004). The results of three of these studies (Chassin et al., 1999; Kendler et al., 1997; Lynskey et al., 1994) are consistent with the present study in finding evidence of effects that are unique to parental alcoholism, whereas the other two studies (Hicks et al., 2004; Ohannessian et al, 2004) found that all of the risk associated with parental alcoholism could be explained by more general effects shared with other psychiatric and substance use disorders. Rather than conclude that the risk associated with parental AUD is either completely shared or completely specific, we concur with Kendler et al. (1997) that there are most likely to be risks that are common to other parental disorders as well as risks that are specifically associated with parental AUD.

Lack of evidence for genetic effects

Although not a goal of this study, it is worth reflecting on reasons why there was no evidence for genetic influences in the intergenerational transmission of alcoholism in this CoT study. Similar results were also obtained in the previous CoT study of Jacob et al. (2003), who found that 11.7% and 11.1% of the 18–26 year old offspring of MZ versus DZ unaffected twins from pairs discordant for AD had a lifetime history of AD. Adoption studies, on the other hand, have found evidence for genetic effects, at least among men (e.g. Cadoret et al., 1985; Cadoret et al., 1987; Cloninger et al., 1981, Goodwin et al., 1973). One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the biological parents in the adoption studies, who were identified from alcoholism treatment programs or official records, represent more severe cases of alcoholism than those identified among the twins and spouses in the community-based CoT studies. Thus, the offspring of alcoholics in the adoption studies would be at higher genetic risk for alcoholism, on average, than the offspring of alcoholics in the CoT studies.

The results of this study also appear to conflict with the convincing evidence from community-based twin studies of the heritability of alcoholism (e.g. Heath et al., 1997; Prescott et al., 1999). The fact that genetic influences were not a significant contributor to the parent-offspring association in this CoT study, but substantially contribute to the variation in risk for AUD in twin studies, may suggest that the risk factors for the development of AUD may not completely overlap across parental and offspring generations (cf. Kendler et al., 1994). Another contributor to differences in the results of the CoT studies versus the twin studies is that the estimates of genetic influences obtained from the classic twin study also include genotype by shared environment interactions and genotype by environment correlations. To the extent that these are important contributors to variation in the risk for AUD, estimates of genetic influences obtained from twin studies may be larger than those obtained from other study designs.

The divergent findings in the CoT compared to the adoption and twin studies are probably due to differences in the statistical power for detecting genetic influences of the various study designs (Heath et al., 1985). The adoption and twin study are relatively powerful compared to the CoT study for detecting the effect of genetic influences. In parent-offspring adoption studies, genetic effects are inferred from comparing the similarity in biologically-related versus non-biologically-related parent-offspring dyads who share 50% and 0% of their genes, respectively. In twin studies, genetic effects are inferred from comparing the twin similarity in MZ versus DZ dyads who share 100% and 50% of their genes, respectively. In the CoT study, one way that genetic effects are inferred is by comparing the offspring of unaffected twins from MZ discordant pairs to the offspring of unaffected twins from DZ discordant pairs, who share 50% and 25% of their genes with their affected uncle or aunt, respectively. Comparisons from adoption and twin designs provide estimates of 1/2 of the overall genetic effect, whereas comparisons from the CoT design provide an estimate of 1/4 of the overall genetic effect. Thus, much larger sample sizes are required in the CoT study than in adoption and twin studies to detect a genetic effect.

Limitations

Several limitations of this study are worth noting. First, the statistical power was not adequate to detect as statistically significant the potential causal effect of parental AUD on offspring AUD symptoms, nor was power adequate to allow for a parsing of the familial confounds into genetic and environmental components. Given a larger sample size it is likely that the modest causal effect of parental AUD that was observed would have reached statistical significance. Second, a relatively broad definition of alcoholism was used to characterize the parents in this study, and the parents were ascertained from a volunteer twin registry. The alcoholic parents in this CoT study may have underrepresented the extremes of alcohol-related pathology and environmental adversity or disadvantage where an important effect of exposure to parental alcoholism might have been detected. Third, AUDs in the parents and offspring were based on retrospective lifetime diagnoses. More reliable diagnoses obtained prospectively over the course of a longitudinal study would have provided greater resolution in identifying a potential causal effect of parental AUD.

Implications

The modest effect sizes obtained in this study suggest that only a minority of children of alcoholics develop alcohol-related problems as a consequence of their exposure to an alcoholic parent, an observation that has been noted by other researchers (e.g. Orford & Velleman, 1990; Windle & Tubman, 1999). One direction for research on children of alcoholics has been to identify those characteristics of parents and children that can explain this heterogeneity of outcomes (Orford & Velleman, 1990; Windle & Tubman, 1999). For example, although some children of alcoholics may model the drinking behavior of their alcoholic parent, there is also some evidence to suggest that others may respond to alcoholism in their family by limiting their drinking (e.g. Chassin & Barrera, 1993; Hughes et al., 1985; Orford & Velleman, 1990), a phenomenon that Harburg et al. (1982) termed "aversive transmission. " Consistent with the aversive transmission hypothesis, Kendler et al. (1994) obtained a negative (albeit nonsignificant) estimate of vertical cultural transmission in a study of adult female twins and their parents.

Although the evidence from genetically-informed studies is relatively consistent in failing to find a substantial association between exposure to an alcoholic parent and offspring AUD, this is based on only a handful of studies with relatively small sample sizes. For example, the genetically-informed studies reviewed in the Introduction of this paper were based on the families of 58 alcoholic adoptive parents (Cloninger et al., 1985), 52 (Cadoret et al., 1985) and 21 (Cadoret et al., 1987) adoptive parents with any psychiatric problem (including alcohol-related problems), the families of 20 alcoholic parents with both adopted-away and non-adopted-away sons (Goodwin et al., 1974), and the families of 94 MZ twin pairs discordant for alcoholism (Jacob et al., 2003). Altogether, including the number of families of MZ twin pairs discordant for alcoholism studied in this report, only 293 or 324 families have been the focus of genetically-informed research on the environmental effect of parental alcoholism on offspring AUD.

Given the extraordinary number of children in this country who are exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence in the family (Grant, 2000), research that can disentangle the correlated genetic and environmental risk factors associated with such exposure is needed in the search to identify the active ingredients leading to the increased risk for the development of AUDs among the children of alcoholics. For example, because it has not yet been convincingly empirically demonstrated that exposure to an alcoholic parent is an active ingredient leading to AUDs in the children of alcoholics, it is unclear whether prevention and intervention programs focused on modifying parents' behaviors will have the anticipated consequence of reducing AUDs among the offspring. It is hoped that insights gained from genetically-informed research will eventually guide future prevention and intervention efforts because, as Moffitt (2005) has stated: "research that does not attack the co-occurrence of genetic and environmental risks will have only limited relevance for prevention" (p. 88).

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants AA00264, AA011998, AA07535, and AA10249, the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression, and the William T. Grant Foundation. Thanks to Dixie Statham for coordinating the data collection for the twins and spouses, to Alison Mackenzie for coordinating the data collection for the offspring of the twins, and to Edison Choe for assisting with the power analyses. We deeply appreciate the continued participation of the twins and their family members.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: The following manuscript is the final accepted manuscript. It has not been subjected to the final copyediting, fact-checking, and proofreading required for formal publication. It is not the definitive, publisher-authenticated version. The American Psychological Association and its Council of Editors disclaim any responsibility or liabilities for errors or omissions of this manuscript version, any version derived from this manuscript by NIH, or other third parties. The published version is available at http://www.apa.org/journals/abn/

1

There is no formal diagnostic category that corresponds to "alcoholism. " To be consistent with the literature on children of alcoholics, and also for the sake of brevity, we use the term "alcoholic" to refer to individuals with alcohol-related problems.

2

Analyses were also conducted with a categorical variable corresponding to any DSM-IV alcohol use disorder (i.e. meeting the diagnostic criteria for AD or AA). The results of these analyses were very similar to those for the two phenotypes used in this paper and the conclusions drawn were the same. The combination of these two diagnoses was judged to be too problematic because it required either experiencing at least 3 of 7 symptoms (for AD) or 1 of 4 symptoms (for AA). Thus, some individuals diagnosed had as few as a single symptom, whereas others with one or two symptoms of AD did not receive a positive diagnosis.

3

The mean AUD symptom count and the prevalence of lifetime AD diagnoses were significantly higher in the offspring generation than in the parental generation (p < .0001). In order to investigate the possible reasons underlying this difference, we compared the 231 twins who were between the ages of 30–39 when they were interviewed in 1992–93 with the 634 offspring who were also 30–39 years of age when interviewed in 1999–2002. The weighted mean AUD symptom counts (p = 0.58) and the lifetime prevalences of AD (p = 0.88) among the male and female twins versus male and female offspring who were 30–39 years of age when interviewed did not significantly differ from each other (for example, the weighted lifetime prevalences of AD among the male twins = 24% versus male offspring = 24%, p = 0.97; female twins = 13% versus female offspring = 11%, p = 0.85). This suggests that the differences in the means and prevalences of AUD in the parental and offspring generations do not reflect methodological differences in the interviews with the parents and the offspring, but instead must be due to age or birth cohort effects. Comparing individuals born in different years but assessed at the same ages is one method that is used in longitudinal studies to unconfound age and birth cohort effects from each other (another method is to compare individuals born in the same year but assessed at different ages). Our results suggest that the differences in the means and prevalences of AUD in the parental and offspring generations are due to age, rather than to birth cohort effects. Higher rates of problems among offspring than among parents would characterize all previous intergenerational studies of AUDs. In fact, similar age differences in the lifetime prevalence of AD are ubiquitous in the epidemiologic literature (see Rice et al., 2003 for a review of this issue). These age differences were especially evident in this study because of the very wide age range of the unweighted unselected original parent samples (e.g. twins ages ranged from 27–90 years; Slutske et al., 1997).

4

Analyses were also conducted without including spouse AUD as a covariate, and the results were nearly identical to the means analyses in which spouse AUD was included as a covariate. The average difference in the estimated means was only 0.01 in the two sets of analyses.

5

The choice to use a cut-off of 2 or more AUD symptoms in the parents was primarily based on statistical power -- 25% of the twin parents (n = 302) were at or above this cut-off. However, this threshold may have been too low to detect an influence of parental AUD. Therefore, the analyses were repeated using higher cut-offs of 3 or more AUD symptoms and 4 or more AUD symptoms; there were two few affected twin parents to examine the effect of using higher thresholds of between 5 to 11 AUD symptoms. The results using the higher cut-offs did not differ substantially from the results using a cutoff of 2 AUD symptoms. The effect sizes of the differences in the mean AUD symptom counts of offspring in Rows 1 versus 2 and Rows 4 versus 5 were d = 0.26 (95% CI: 0.13 – 0.40) and d = 0.16 (95% CI: −.20 – 0.53), respectively, with a cut-off of 3 AUD symptoms, and the effect sizes were d = 0.32 (95% CI: 0.14–0.50) and d = 0.08 (95% CI: −0.38–0.54), respectively, with a cut-off of 4 AUD symptoms. Using thresholds more extreme than 2 or more AUD symptoms yielded slightly larger differences in the AUD symptom counts of the offspring of affected versus unaffected twins from discordant pairs, but the differences were still small and not statistically significant.

6

Some previous studies have focused on a combined group of MZ and DZ discordant twin pairs either exclusively (e.g. Lynskey et al., 2003; Harden et al., 2007), or in addition to the more conservative comparison of discordant MZ twin pairs presented here (e.g. D'Onofrio et al., 2007, Slutske et al., 2004) in testing potentially causal environmental effects. The rationale is that DZ twins can also serve as a control, especially when there are no differences in the magnitude of the effect observed in discordant MZ versus DZ twin pairs (e.g. Lynskey et al., 2003). When the data from the offspring of discordant MZ and DZ twin pairs were combined, the mean AUD symptom counts among the offspring of affected versus the offspring of unaffected twins from discordant pairs were 0.02 versus 0.02, respectively (d = 0.00, 95% CI: −0.17 – 0.18) when using the broader definition of AUD in the twins (corresponding to Table 3), and 0.23 versus 0.12, respectively (d = 0.10, 95% CI: −0.13 – 0.34) when using the narrower definition of AUD in the twins (corresponding to Table 4). The results of the analyses collapsed across zygosity were very similar to the results obtained when only the offspring of discordant MZ twin pairs were included in the analyses, but the confidence intervals around the estimates were substantially narrower due to the increased sample size.

7

In this CoT sample (and in the larger unselected twin sample), twins with an AUD diagnosis had fewer offspring (they were less likely to have any offspring, although there was no difference in the number of offspring among those who had at least one offspring). Inspection of Table 3 and Table 4 reveals that there were fewer offspring among the affected than among the unaffected twins from discordant pairs. Among the 245 discordant twin pairs (79 MZ + 166 DZ) presented in Table 3, the affected and unaffected twins had 273 and 349 offspring, respectively, and among the 152 discordant twin pairs (48 MZ + 104 DZ) in Table 4, the affected and unaffected twins had 110 and 138 offspring, respectively. The analyses for Table 3 and Table 4 were re-run only including those offspring of twins in which both twins from the pair had at least one child in this CoT study (67% of the offspring) -- this restriction resulted in very similar numbers of offspring among the affected and unaffected twins from discordant pairs. Restricting the analyses to twins with offspring did not change any of the findings in this study. The reduced child-bearing among twins with a history of an AUD compared to twins without a history of an AUD probably did not affect the results of this study.

8

The effect size used in the power calculation for detecting a genetic influence was based on an extension of a regression technique used to analyze twin data (DeFries & Fulker, 1985). In standardized twin data, twice the difference between the means of MZ versus DZ cotwins of affected probands provides an estimate of the heritability of a continuous trait. Similarly, in standardized offspring of twin data, four times the difference between the means of nieces/nephews of MZ versus DZ affected aunts/uncles from discordant twin pairs provides an estimate of the heritability of a continuous trait. Power calculations were based on the heritability of the AUD symptom count among the twins in this sample (52%; 95% CI: 40–56%), which would translate into a standardized mean difference in the MZ and DZ offspring of discordant unaffected twins of 0.13 (i.e. 0.52 / 4). The very small mean difference expected with a moderately heritable trait highlights the low power of the CoT design for detecting genetic effects.

Contributor Information

Wendy S. Slutske, Department of Psychological Sciences and Midwest Alcoholism Research Center, University of Missouri-Columbia

Brian M. D’Onofrio, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University

Eric Turkheimer, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia.

Robert E. Emery, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia

K. Paige Harden, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia.

Andrew C. Heath, Department of Psychiatry and Midwest Alcoholism Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine

Nicholas G. Martin, Genetic Epidemiology Laboratory, Queensland Institute of Medical Research

References

  1. Agrawal A, Heath AC, Grant JD, Pergadia ML, Statham DJ, Bucholz KK, Martin NG, Madden PAF. Assortative mating for cigarette smoking and for alcohol consumption in female Australian twins and their spouses. Behavior Genetics. 2006;36:553–566. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9081-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnow S, Schuckit M, Smith TL, Preuss U, Danko G. The relationship between the family density of alcoholism and externalizing symptoms among 146 children. Alcohol & Alcoholism. 2002;37:383–387. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/37.4.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI, Jr, Reich T, Schmidt I, Schuckit MA. A new semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1994;55:149–158. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Cadoret RJ. Adoption studies: Historical and methodological critique. Psychiatric Developments. 1986;1:45–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Cadoret RJ, O'Gorman TW, Troughton E, Heywood E. Alcoholism and antisocial personality: interrelationships, genetic and environmental factors. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1985;42:161–167. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790250055007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Cadoret RJ, Troughton E, O'Gorman TW. Genetic and environmental factors in alcohol abuse and antisocial personality. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1987;48:1–8. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1987.48.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chassin L, Barrera M. Substance use escalation and substance use restraint among adolescent children of alcoholics. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 1993;7:3–20. [Google Scholar]
  8. Chassin L, Pitts SC, DeLucia C, Todd M. A longitudinal study of children of alcoholics: Predicting young adult substance use disorders, anxiety, and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1999;108:106–119. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.1.106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Second edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen J. The earth is round (p < .05) American Psychologist. 1994;49:997–1003. [Google Scholar]
  11. Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S. Inheritance of alcohol abuse: Cross-fostering analysis of adopted men. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1981;38:861–868. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780330019001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cloninger CR, Bohman M, Sigvardsson S, von Knorring A. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. Volume 3. NY: Plenum Press; 1985. Psychopathology of adopted-out children of alcoholics: The Stockholm Adoption Study. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. DeFries JC, Fulker DW. Multiple regression analysis of twin data. Behavior Genetics. 1985;15:467–473. doi: 10.1007/BF01066239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. DeLucia C, Belz A, Chassin L. Do adolescent symptomatology and family environment vary over time with fluctuations in paternal alcohol impairment? Developmental Psychology. 2001;37:207–216. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Harden KP, Heath AC, Madden PAF, Martin NG. The intergenerational transmission of childhood conduct problems: A children of twins study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2007;64:820–829. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Eaves LJ, Corey LA, Berg K, Solaas MH, Emery RE. The role of the Children of Twins design in elucidating causal relations between parent characteristics and child outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:1130–1144. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Heath AC, Madden PA, Martin NG. A genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2005;114:570–586. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Duncan AE, Scherrer JS, Fu Q, Bucholz KK, Heath AC, True WR, Haber JR, Howell D, Jacob T. Exposure to paternal alcoholism does not predict development of alcohol-use disorders in offspring: Evidence from an offspring-of-twins study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 2006;67:649–656. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2006.67.649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Ellis DA, Zucker RA, Fitzgerald HE. The role of family influences in development and risk. Alcohol Health & Research World. 1997;21:218–226. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Finn PR, Pihl RO. Risk for alcoholism: A comparison between two different groups of sons of alcoholics on cardiovascular reactivity and sensitivity to alcohol. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1988;12:742–747. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1988.tb01338.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Hermansen L, Guze SB, Winokur G. Alcohol problems in adoptees raised apart from alcoholic biological parents. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1973;28:238–243. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1973.01750320068011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Moller N, Hermansen L, Winokur G, Guze SB. Drinking problems in adopted and nonadopted sons of alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1974;31:164–169. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1974.01760140022003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gottesman II, Bertelsen A. Confirming unexpressed genotypes for schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1989;46:867–872. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810100009002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Grant BF. Estimates of US children exposed to alcohol abuse and dependence in the family. American Journal of Public Health. 2000;90:112–115. doi: 10.2105/ajph.90.1.112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Harburg E, Davis DR, Caplan R. Parent and offspring alcohol use: Imitative and aversive transmission. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1982;43:497–516. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1982.43.497. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Harden KP, Lynch SK, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D'Onofrio BM, Slutske WS, Waldron M, Heath AC, Statham DJ, Martin NG. A behavior genetic investigation of adolescent motherhood and offspring mental health problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:667–683. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.4.667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D'Onofrio BM, Slutske W, Heath A, Martin NG. Marital conflict and conduct disorder in children of twins. Child Development. 2007;78:1–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00982.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Dinwiddie SH, Slutske WS, Bierut LJ, Statham DJ, Dunne MP, Whitfield J, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental contributions to alcohol dependence risk in a national twin sample: Consistency of findings in women and men. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27:1381–1396. doi: 10.1017/s0033291797005643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heath AC, Kendler KS, Eaves LJ, Markell D. The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance: Informativeness of different relationships. Behavior Genetics. 1985;15:439–465. doi: 10.1007/BF01066238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Heath AC, Madden PA, Martin NG. Assessing the effects of cooperation bias and attrition in behavioral genetic research using data weighting. Behavior Genetics. 1998;28:415–427. doi: 10.1023/a:1021633127604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Helzer JE, Bucholz KK, Bierut LJ, Regier DA, Schuckit MA, Guth SE. Should DSM-IV include dimensional diagnostic criteria for alcohol use disorders? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;30:303–310. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00028.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hicks BM, Krueger RF, Iacono WG, McGue M, Patrick CJ. Family transmission and heritability of externalizing disorders: A twin-family study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2004;61:922–928. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.9.922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hill SY, Shen S, Lowers L, Locke J. Factors predicting the onset of adolescent drinking in families at high risk for developing alcoholism. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:265–275. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00841-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hill SY, Yuan H. Familial density of alcoholism and onset of adolescent drinking. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 1999;60:7–17. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hughes J, Stewart M, Barraclough B. Why teetotallers abstain. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1985;146:204–208. doi: 10.1192/bjp.146.2.204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jacob T, Leonard K. Family and peer influences in the development of adolescent alcohol abuse. In: Zucker R, Boyd G, Howard J, editors. The Development of Alcohol Problems: Exploring the Biopsychosocial Matrix of Risk. Rockville, MD: National Institutes of Health; 1994. [Google Scholar]
  37. Jacob T, Waterman B, Heath A, True W, Bucholz KK, Haber R, Scherrer J, Fu Q. Genetic and environmental effects on offspring alcoholism: New insights using an offspring-of-twins design. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2003;60:1265–1272. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kahler CW, Strong DR. A Rasch model analysis of DSM-IV alcohol abuse and dependence items in the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;30:1165–1175. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00140.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kendler KS, Davis CG, Kessler RC. The familial aggregation of common psychiatric and substance use disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey: A family history study. British Journal of Psychiatry. 1997;170:541–548. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.6.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Kendler KS, Neale MC, Heath AC, Kessler RC, Eaves LJ. A twin-family study of alcoholism in women. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1994;151:707–715. doi: 10.1176/ajp.151.5.707. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2005;62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Kline RB. Beyond Significance Testing: Reforming Data Analysis Methods in Behavioral Research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  43. Krueger RF, Nichol PE, Hicks BM, Markon KE, Patrick CJ, Iacono WG, McGue M. Using latent trait modeling to conceptualize an alcohol problems continuum. Psychological Assessment. 2004;16:107–119. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.2.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lieb R, Merikangas KR, Hofler M, Pfister H, Isensee B, Wittchen H-U. Parental alcohol use disorders and alcohol use and disorders in offspring: A community study. Psychological Medicine. 2002;32:63–78. doi: 10.1017/s0033291701004883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Littel RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD, Schabenberger O. SAS for Mixed Models. Second Edition. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  46. Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. New York: Wiley; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  47. Loftus GR. Psychology will be a much better science when we change the way that we analyze data. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1996;5:161–171. [Google Scholar]
  48. Lykken . What's wrong with psychology anyway? In: Cicchetti D, Grove WM, editors. Thinking Clearly About Psychology, Volume 1: Matters of Public Interest. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  49. Lynch SK, Turkheimer E, D'Onofrio BM, Mendle J, Emery RE, Slutske WS, Martin NG. A genetically informed study of the association between harsh punishment and offspring behavior problems. Journal of Family Psychology. 2006;20:190–198. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lynskey MT, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. The effect of parental alcohol problems on rates of adolescent psychiatric disorders. Addiction. 1994;89:1277–1286. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1994.tb03306.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lynskey MT, Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Slutske WS, Madden PAF, Nelson EC, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Escalation of illicit drug use in early-onset cannabis users versus co-twin controls. Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;289:427–433. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Maes HHM, Neale MC, Kendler KS, Hewitt JK, Silberg JL, Foley DL, Meyer JM, Rutter M, Simonoff E, Pickles A, Eaves LJ. Assortative mating for major psychiatric diagnoses in two population-based samples. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:1389–1401. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. McLeod JD. Spouse concordance for alcohol dependence and heavy drinking: Evidence from a community sample. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1993;17:1146–1155. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1993.tb05220.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. McGue M, Sharma A, Benson P. Parent and sibling influences on adolescent alcohol use and misuse: Evidence from a U.S. adoption cohort. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1996;57:8–18. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1996.57.8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. McGue M, Slutske W. The inheritance of alcoholism in women. In: Howard JM, Martin SE, Mail PD, Hilton ME, Taylor ED, editors. Women and Alcohol: Issues for Prevention Research. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Research Monograph 32. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  56. Moffitt TE. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behaviors: Evidence from behavioral-genetic research. Advances in Genetics. 2005;55:41–104. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2660(05)55003-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nance WE, Corey LA. Genetic models for the analysis of data from the families of identical twins. Genetics. 1976;83:811–826. doi: 10.1093/genetics/83.4.811. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ohannessian CM, Hesselbrock VM, Kramer J, Kuperman S, Bucholz KK, Schuckit MA, Nurnberger JI. The relationship between parental alcoholism and adolescent psychopathology: A systematic examination of parental comorbid psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2004;32:519–533. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000037781.49155.a6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Orford J, Velleman R. Offspring of parents with drinking problems: Drinking and drug-taking as young adults. British Journal of Addiction. 1990;85:779–794. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1990.tb01691.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Pollock VE, Schneider LS, Gabrielli WF, Goodwin DW. Sex of parent and offspring in the transmission of alcoholism: A meta-analysis. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 1987;175:668–673. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198711000-00004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Prescott CA, Aggen SH, Kendler KS. Sex differences in the sources of genetic liability to alcohol abuse and dependence in a population-based sample of U.S. twins. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1999;23:1136–1144. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1999.tb04270.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods. 2nd Edition. Thousand Oaks, NJ: Sage; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  63. Rice JP, Neuman RJ, Saccone NL, Corbett J, Rochberg N, Hesselbrock V, Bucholz KK, McGuffin P, Reich T. Age and birth cohort effects on rates of alcohol dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2003;27:93–99. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000047303.89421.AA. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, Eaves L. Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:291–324. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.3.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Schmidt FL. Statistical significance testing and cumulative knowledge in psychology: Implications for training of researchers. Psychological Methods. 1996;1:115–129. [Google Scholar]
  66. Schuckit MA, Smith TL, Pierson J, Danko GP, Beltran IA. Relationships among level of response to alcohol and the number of alcoholic relatives in predicting alcohol-related outcomes. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2006;30:1308–1314. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00158.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sher KJ. Children of Alcoholics: A Critical Appraisal of Theory and Research. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  68. Sher KJ, Walitzer KS, Wood PK, Brent EE. Characteristics of children of alcoholics: putative risk factors, substance use and abuse, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1991;100:427–448. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.100.4.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Singer JD. Using SAS PROC MIXED to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 1998;24:323–355. [Google Scholar]
  70. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Modeling genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: a study of 2682 adult twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1997;106:266–279. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.2.266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin NG. Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1998;107:363–374. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.107.3.363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Slutske WS, Hunt-Carter EE, Nabors-Oberg RE, Sher KJ, Bucholz KK, Madden PAF, Anokhin A, Heath AC. Do college students drink more than their non-college-attending peers? Evidence from a population-based longitudinal female twin study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2004;113:530–540. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Slutske WS, True WR, Scherrer JF, Goldberg J, Bucholz KK, Heath AC, Henderson WG, Eisen SA, Lyons MJ, Tsuang MT. Long-term reliability and validity of alcoholism diagnoses and symptoms in a large national telephone interview survey. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1998;22:583–588. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb04292.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Statham DJ, Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Bierut L, Madden PAF, Dinwiddie SH, Slutske WS, Dunne MP, Martin NG. Suicidal behavior: an epidemiologic and genetic study. Psychological Medicine. 1998;28:839–855. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798006916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Stoolmiller M. Implications of the restricted range of family environments for estimates of heritability and non-shared environment in behavior-genetic adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin. 1999;125:392–409. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.4.392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Stoltenberg SF, Mudd SA, Blow FC, Hill EM. Evaluating measures of family history of alcoholism: Density versus dichotomy. Addiction. 1998;93:1511–1520. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.1998.931015117.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. True WR, Heath AC, Romeis JC, Bucholz K, Slutske W, Scherrer JF, Lin N, Eisen SA, Goldberg J, Lyons MJ. Models of treatment seeking for alcoholism: the role of genes and environment. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 1996;20:1577–1581. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01702.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Whitfield JB, Zhu G, Heath AC, Martin NG. Choice of residential location: Chance, family influences, or genes? Twin Research and Human Genetics. 2005;8:22–26. doi: 10.1375/1832427053435391. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wilkinson L the Task Force on Statistical Inference. Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist. 1999;54:594–604. [Google Scholar]
  80. Windle M. On the discriminative validity of a family history of problem drinking index with a national sample of young adults. Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1996;57:378–386. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1996.57.378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Windle M, Tubman JG. Children of alcoholics. In: Silverman WK, Ollendick T, editors. Developmental issues in the clinical treatment of children and adolescents. Allyn & Bacon: Needham Heights, MA; 1999. pp. 393–414. [Google Scholar]
  82. Zhou Q, King KM, Chassin L. The roles of familial alcoholism and adolescent family harmony in young adults' substance dependence disorders: Mediated and moderated relations. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115:320–331. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES