Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2011 Jan 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010 Jan–Mar;24(1):49–55. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3181bd66a3

Relationship Satisfaction and Emotional Language in Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer’s Disease Patients and Spousal Caregivers

Elizabeth A Ascher 1, Virginia E Sturm 2, Benjamin H Seider 1, Sarah R Holley 1, Bruce L Miller 2, Robert W Levenson 1
PMCID: PMC2838197  NIHMSID: NIHMS153948  PMID: 20220322

Abstract

We studied the impact of two types of dementia on marital satisfaction and on the emotional language spouses use during conflictive marital interactions. Fifteen frontotemporal dementia (FTD) and 16 Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patient-caregiver couples, as well as 21 control couples, discussed a relationship problem in a laboratory setting. Marital satisfaction was assessed via questionnaire, and emotion language was quantified using text analysis. FTD couples reported lower marital satisfaction than AD and control couples. During the interactions, FTD and AD caregivers used significantly more negative emotion words than their patient spouses (no spousal differences were found in control couples). FTD caregivers also used more negative words than AD caregivers and controls. We interpret these findings as reflecting challenges that the behavioral changes in FTD create for maintaining a healthy marital bond.

Keywords: frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, marital satisfaction, emotional language

INTRODUCTION

Caregiving for dementia patients is typically provided by their spouses;1,2 thus, understanding the effects of dementia on marriage is particularly important. Existing research on these marriages has focused on Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and has relied largely on caregiver reports of relationship qualities. This research indicates that dementia creates a number of difficulties for caregiver spouses, including deterioration in relationship quality, lower relationship satisfaction, and decreased emotional and physically intimacy.36 The AD literature consistently identifies the behavioral symptoms of dementia (e.g., agitation, apathy, disinhibition, loss of insight, wandering) as key contributors to greater perceived caregiver burden2,711 and lower relationship satisfaction.3,12

Given the impact of behavioral symptoms, we would expect differences in relationship quality between patient-caregiver couples to be associated with varying degrees of behavioral disturbance. While the primary impairments in AD are cognitive, in frontotemporal dementia (FTD), the primary deficits are behavioral, manifested in striking social, emotional, and personality changes.1324 Behavioral changes do occur in AD, but they often appear much later in the course of the illness.2527 In contrast, behavioral changes in FTD occur early,28,29 distinguishing these patients from those with AD and vascular dementia.30 Studies comparing caregiving in AD and FTD find that FTD caregivers experience a greater degree of general burden, stress, and depression, and feel less satisfied in their caregiving roles than AD caregivers, despite comparable levels of functional impairment between FTD and AD patients.3135 To our knowledge, no previous studies have compared the quality of the marriage and the nature of marital interaction between these two diseases.

In the present study we examined marital satisfaction and marital interaction in AD and FTD couples. Marital satisfaction was assessed using a standard questionnaire.36 Observational studies of marital interaction have been rare in the dementia literature.but see 37 To obtain an ecologically valid, dynamic snapshot of the interactions of patient-caregiver couples, we utilized a marital interaction paradigm that we have used extensively with non-patient couples in which spouses discuss and try to resolve a relationship conflict.38 We applied a text analysis methodology39 to transcripts of these discussions to assess positive and negative emotional language used by patients and caregivers during the interactions. This approach to assessing emotional language is based on the assumption that the words that appear in natural, spontaneous language reflect underlying psychological states and are less prone to the biases of self-report than questionnaire measures.40 Text analysis of emotional language has been used with a number of different kinds of interacting dyads41,42 including interactions of married couples.43,44

Given the generally stressful nature of caring for a dementia patient, we hypothesized that caregivers of both AD and FTD patients would report lower marital satisfaction and use more negative emotional language and less positive emotional language than controls. Because behavioral symptoms are strongly associated with decreased relationship satisfaction for caregivers3 and because FTD patients are more likely to show such symptoms that AD patients,30 we hypothesized that FTD caregivers would have lower marital satisfaction, would use more negative emotional language and less positive emotional language than AD caregivers.

Because FTD patients have a dramatic lack of awareness of their disease4547 and a proclivity to inflate positive traits and underreport negative ones,48 we hypothesized that FTD patients would show a positive bias on our measures (i.e., higher marital satisfaction, less negative and more positive emotional language than both their spouses and AD patients). Given AD patients’ tendencies to minimize marital problems compared to their spouses,49,50 we hypothesized that AD patients would also show a positive bias on our measures compared to their spouses.

METHODS

Participants

Overview

Participants were 52 male-female couples recruited by the Memory and Aging Center (MAC) at the University of California, San Francisco as part of a larger study of neurodegenerative diseases. In each couple, one member was either a dementia patient (diagnosed with FTD or AD) or a neurologically healthy control. The other member was the patient or control’s spouse or domestic partner (there was one domestic partnership in each of the three groups; the remainder were marriages). Group sizes, mean ages, and sex distribution are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Age, Sex, and Dementia Severity

Group N Age
Sex
Patient CDR box score
mean (SD)
adj mean (SE)
F/M
adj meana (SE)
Caregiver Patient Couple Caregiver Patient
FTD couples 15 56.90 (7.60) 60.37 (6.09) 58.64 (1.53) 13/2 2/13 6.93 (0.51)
AD couples 16 54.02 (7.03) 59.28 (5.28) 56.65 (1.48) 10/6 6/10 4.56g** (0.50)
Control couplesb 21 64.03 (6.60) 66.58 (7.88) 65.30c***,d** (1.30) 15/6 6/15 −0.09f*** (0.45)

adj mean (SE)

Total 52 58.31 (0.99) 62.08e*** (0.93) 60.19 (0.83) 38/14 14/38 3.80 (0.27)

FTD = frontotemporal dementia; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; SD = standard deviation; adj = adjusted; SE = standard error; F = female; M = male; CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale.

a

Corrected for patient age.

b

For controls, the “patient” was the member of the couple who participated in the larger study being conducted through the UCSF MAC and UC Berkeley, as a patient analog; the “caregiver” was the member of the couple who participated in a subset of study tasks, as a caregiver analog. Unless otherwise noted, analyses reporting on “patients” include control patient analogs and analyses reporting on “caregivers” include control caregiver analogs.

c

Control couples > AD couples

d

Control couples > FTD couples

e

Patients > caregivers

f

Controls < FTD & AD

g

AD < FTD

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Recruitment and diagnostic process

Dementia patients were recruited from the pool of referrals to the MAC and had to meet research criteria for either FTD18 or probable AD.51 Patients were diagnosed by trained MAC staff through a review of data from neurological assessments, clinical interviews, case histories, neuropsychological tests, and brain imaging. AD patients tend to be older than FTD patients, thus, to equalize age, we recruited early-onset AD patients. Control couples were recruited from the local community via advertisements and word-of-mouth and were neurologically and psychiatrically healthy as determined by a complete evaluation at the MAC.

All patients and controls were required to have a spouse or partner willing to participate. In the remainder of this paper, members of control couples are identified as either the “control patient,” to refer to patient analogs or the “control caregiver” to refer to caregiver analogs.

Measures

Dementia severity

The Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR)52 was administered by trained MAC staff. The CDR includes a structured interview administered to caregivers that assesses six domains of daily functioning. The CDR is widely used as a measure of dementia severity, with higher scores indicating greater impairment. The CDR box score was computed by summing the subscores for each of the six domains53 (scores range from 0 to 18).

Marital satisfaction

The Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale36 was administered to both partners of each couple. (One couple in the FTD group and another in the control group did not complete the Locke-Wallace questionnaire for unknown reasons.) The Locke-Wallace is a well-established self-report measure of marital satisfaction containing 15 items, e.g., “When disagreements arise, they usually result in (a) husband giving in, (b) wife giving in, (c) agreement by mutual give and take.” Scores range from 2 to 158, with higher scores indicating greater marital satisfaction. Scores of 100 or above are generally considered to indicate satisfied couples, with scores below 100 indicating dissatisfied couples.

Procedure

General procedure

Participants in the present study were evaluated at the University of California, Berkeley using a comprehensive battery of tasks assessing emotional functioning.54 One of these tasks, a social interaction task38 (described below) was used to assess emotional language.

Social interaction task

Couples participated in a 15-minute conversation about an area of conflict in their relationship. The pair chose the conversation topic in advance with the guidance of an experimenter. The conversation was preceded by a five-minute silent baseline period. Couples were seated in chairs facing each other. Each participant wore a small microphone attached to the shirt collar to record speech. Audio and video recordings of the interaction were made; however, for the present study, only the audio recording was used.

Data Reduction

Text analysis

From the audio recording of each interaction, a verbatim transcript was prepared according to common transcription standards.55 The transcripts were analyzed using a text analysis program (Oedipus Text) written by one of the authors (R.W.L.). The program compared each word in the transcript against a dictionary of emotion words and assigned it to a discrete emotion category (described below). It then presented the word in its context (the preceding sentence, the sentence it occurred in, and the following sentence) to a trained coder, blind to diagnostic grouping, who determined whether the word was in fact used in an emotional way. This context-coding was done to avoid counting non-emotional homonyms (e.g., “what do you mean,”) and phrases (e.g., “I’m afraid I don’t understand”).

Emotion word dictionary

Inclusion of words in the emotion dictionary was based on a number of studies of the emotional lexicon.5658 This composite list consisted exclusively of words that met the criteria for an emotion state as defined by Ortony et al.57 (e.g., happy, angry, sad, elated, passionate, relieved), and did not include emotion-related words that do not refer to emotional states per se (e.g., baffled, abandoned, tingly). There were approximately 1500 emotion words in the dictionary (approximately 300 word roots, plus all possible variations) in two superordinate valence categories: positive and negative. These categories were derived from 27 subordinate discrete categories of emotion44 as follows: 12 categories (amusement, excitement, general positive emotion, interest, joy, love-general, love-parental, love-romantic, pride, relaxation, relief, satisfaction) were collapsed into the positive emotion category; and 15 categories (anger, anxiety, apathy, contempt, disgust, embarrassment, envy, fear, general negative emotion, grief, guilt, jealousy, pain, sadness, shame) were collapsed into the negative emotion category.

Because of low base rates in many of the 27 subordinate categories, we relied on the superordinate valence categories for all analyses. Three variables were computed for each speaker: number of positive words, number of negative words, and total number of words (emotional plus nonemotional) used during the 15-minute conversation. Speakers’ total words were used as covariates in all emotional language analyses.

Data Analysis

Analyses were conducted using 3 × 2 mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) for age and analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) for marital satisfaction and emotional language. In these analyses, diagnosis (FTD, AD, or control) was treated as a between-subjects factor and partner (caregiver or patient) was treated as a within-subjects factor (reflecting the interdependence of spousal data).59 In the ANCOVAs, age and dementia severity were used as covariates (see below). Significant main effects and interactions were followed up with Bonferroni-adjusted pairwise comparisons of the estimated marginal means.

RESULTS

Sex, Age, and Dementia Severity

Before testing our hypotheses, we assessed differences in sex, age, and dementia severity (Table 1). A Fisher’s exact test of sex revealed no significant differences in the proportions of caregiver and patient sex across the three groups (p = .33).

An ANOVA with age as the dependent variable revealed significant main effects for diagnosis, F(2,49) = 10.95, p < .001, and partner, F(1,49) = 15.39, p < .001, but no significant Diagnosis × Partner interaction, F(2,49) = 0.72, p = .49. Follow-up tests of the main effects revealed that control couples were significantly older than both FTD (p < .01) and AD couples (p < .001), but that FTD and AD couples did not differ from each other (p = 1.00). Across the sample, caregivers were younger than patients (p < .001).

An ANCOVA of dementia severity using the CDR box score as the dependent variable and patient age as a covariate revealed a significant main effect for diagnosis, F(2,48) = 51.67, p < .001. Post-hoc comparisons showed all three groups were significantly different from each other. As expected, control patients were less impaired than both FTD and AD patients (p < .001). Between the two dementia groups, AD patients showed less impairment than FTD patients (p < .01).

Because differences were found both in age and dementia severity, these were used as covariates in all subsequent analyses.

Marital Satisfaction

The ANCOVA for marital satisfaction (controlling for caregiver age, patient age, and CDR1) revealed a significant main effect for diagnosis, F(2,44) = 8.57, p < .001. The main effect for partner, F(1,44) = 0.48, p = .49, and the Diagnosis × Partner interaction, F(2,44) = 1.20, p = .31, were not significant. Follow-up comparisons showed that FTD couples were less satisfied than both AD (p < .001) and control (p < .01) couples. AD and control couples were not significantly different from each other (p = 1.00). Adjusted means and standard errors for marital satisfaction are reported in Table 2.

Table 2.

Marital Satisfaction and Emotional Language

Group Marital satisfaction
Emotional language
N Locke-Wallace
N Negative
Positive
adj meana (SE)
adj meanb (SE)
adj meanb (SE)
Caregiver Patient Couple Caregiver Patient Caregiver Patient
FTD 14 78.53 (8.75) 97.90 (9.07) 88.22c***,d** (6.72) 15 13.74e***,f** (1.79) 4.20 (1.55) 16.60 (3.62) 12.94 (2.14)
AD 16 117.71 (6.17) 118.13 (6.39) 117.92 (4.74) 16 5.59g* (1.26) 3.13 (1.09) 15.23 (2.54) 10.77 (1.50)
CTL 20 126.91 (7.95) 121.34 (8.24) 124.12 (6.11) 21 1.06 (1.65) 3.33 (1.42) 16.02 (3.32) 9.55 (1.96)

Grp = group; FTD = frontotemporal dementia; AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CTL = control; adj = adjusted; SE = standard error.

a

Corrected for age and CDR box score.

b

Corrected for age, total words and CDR box score.

c

FTD < AD

d

FTD < control

e

FTD caregivers > FTD patients

f

FTD caregivers > AD & control caregivers

g

AD caregivers > AD patients

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001

Emotional Language

ANCOVAs were conducted on negative emotion words and positive emotion words, each controlling for CDR and both partners’ ages, as well as total words uttered by each speaker. The ANCOVA for negative emotion words revealed a significant main effect for diagnosis, F(2,44) = 5.02, p < .05, and a significant Diagnosis × Partner interaction, F(2,44) = 9.64, p < .001, but no significant main effect for partner, F(1,44) = 0.86, p = .36. Given the significant interaction, the diagnosis main effect was not interpreted.

The Diagnosis × Partner interaction was followed up with simple effects tests of the adjusted means. Examining each partner within each diagnostic group, FTD caregivers used more negative emotion words than FTD patients, F(1,44) = 31.56, p < .001. AD caregivers also used more negative emotion words than AD patients, F(1,44) = 4.28, p < .05, though this difference was no longer significant when the analysis was repeated within the two dementia groups only.see note 1 above There were no significant differences between control caregivers and patients, F(1,44) = 2.12, p = .15. Looking at the three diagnoses within each partner, there were significant differences within caregivers, F(2,44) = 10.70, p < .001, but not within patients, F(2,44) = 0.22, p = .81. FTD caregivers used more negative emotion words than both AD (p < .001) and control (p < .001) caregivers. AD and control caregivers were not significantly different from each other (p = .19).

The ANCOVA for positive emotion words revealed no significant main effects for diagnosis, F(2,44) = 0.26, p = .77, or partner, F(1,44) = 0.07, p = .79, and no Diagnosis × Partner interaction, F(2,44) = 0.10, p = .90. Adjusted means and standard errors for emotional language are reported in Table 2.

DISCUSSION

Prior research has found that caregiving for a spouse with dementia takes a toll on marital satisfaction. In the present study, we extended this work by examining marital satisfaction and the use of emotional language and by including both AD and FTD patient-caregiver couples.

As hypothesized, we found that FTD couples had lower levels of marital satisfaction than AD and control couples. We believe this finding reflects the fact that the behavioral and emotional symptoms of FTD (e.g., apathy, disinhibition, lack of empathy) are particularly destructive for the marital bond. We had also hypothesized that dementia patients would report higher levels of marital satisfaction than caregiving spouses. We found no evidence of this for either patient group. If we assume that the caregiving spouse’s rating of marital satisfaction is accurate, then FTD patients may be more aware of relationship quality than they are in other interpersonal domains such as personality change48 and awareness of others’ emotions.24 We did not find evidence supporting the observation that AD patients minimize marital problems compared to their spouses.49,50 However, in the population we studied, marital satisfaction was relatively high in AD couples. Thus, there may not have been a great deal of dissatisfaction for AD patients to minimize.

As hypothesized, FTD caregivers used more negative emotional language than AD and control caregivers during a social interaction. Within dementia couples, caregivers – particularly FTD caregivers – used more negative words than their patient spouses, but no such differences were found for control couples. Importantly, these differences were found after controlling for dementia severity. Negative emotion has been strongly associated with poor marital outcomes in studies of non-patient couples at all ages.60,61 Thus, these findings involving negative emotional language provide a new window onto the difficulties that FTD causes for the marriage.

Our hypothesis that we would find a lack of positive emotional language in FTD couples was not supported. This may reflect the marital interaction task we used, which focused on an area of marital conflict and, thus, does not elicit a great deal of positive emotion.60 The question of whether there are deficits in positive emotional language in dementia couples could be profitably revisited in a future study using a discussion topic that is more conducive to the expression of positive emotion.

Taking the marital satisfaction and emotional language findings together, the lower marital satisfaction reported by both spouses and the greater use of negative emotional language by caregiving spouses in FTD couples underscores the profound impact that FTD has on the marriage. The discrepancy between marital satisfaction and emotional language in the FTD patients (i.e., they report lower marital satisfaction but do not use greater negative emotional language) points to one of the subtle challenges of dealing with FTD patients. In these patients, declines in cognitive, social, and emotional functioning may proceed at different rates,17,19,20,62,63 resulting in inconsistencies that are disturbing for their partners (e.g., accurate assessment of marital distress that is not reflected in the use of negative emotional language).

The burden associated with caring for a family member with AD has been extensively documented2,64,65 and clearly is extremely taxing for caregivers. However, our finding that AD couples reported similar levels of marital satisfaction and used similar amounts of negative emotional language to control couples suggests that AD may burden, but not erode, the marital relationship. This conclusion is further supported by the mean marital satisfaction scores in AD couples (adjusted mean = 117.92), which are in the range indicative of satisfied marriages, compared to those of FTD couples (adjusted mean = 88.22), which are in the range indicative of dissatisfied marriages.

Why might this be? We believe that when a loved one loses the ability to be empathic and to connect emotionally, as in FTD,18,24 caregivers are likely to become frustrated, hurt, and angry, and that relationship satisfaction will decline. In contrast, when a loved one starts to forget things and forget people and becomes increasingly confused, but still maintains the ability to be empathic and to connect emotionally, as in AD,66 caregivers are likely to experience sadness and grief, combined with more positive emotions such as affection, sympathy, and love. Consistent with this, despite caregiving stress and burden, AD caregivers have reported feeling as close, if not closer, to their patient spouses than before the illness.3,6

Limitations and Future Directions

As noted earlier, the present study expanded on the existing literature on dementia and marriage by measuring both marital satisfaction and the use of emotional language during marital interaction and by including two kinds of dementia. Because we examined relationship satisfaction and the use of emotional language at a particular moment in time, however, we were limited in our ability to characterize the longitudinal relationship and interactions between increasing dementia severity and changes in the marriage.67 Further, our dementia couples were recruited from a tertiary care specialty clinic; thus, we do not know if our results would generalize to dementia patients and caregivers in the population at large.68,69

Our findings of differences in marital satisfaction and negative emotional language in FTD and AD couples point to the importance of considering interventions that target the difficulties specific to each form of dementia. Dementia caregiver intervention research has made great strides in developing theoretical models, testing a range of therapeutic approaches, and demonstrating successful burden reduction in a number of domains.65 However, the bulk of this research has been focused exclusively on AD caregivers. Future work is needed that includes FTD patients and their caregivers and that takes into account the particular difficulties that FTD symptoms create for relationships.

Conclusion

This study underscores the profound challenges that FTD creates for couples, highlighting the lowered relationship satisfaction and increased negative emotional language associated with the disease. Caring for a loved one suffering from a neurodegenerative illness is difficult and painful regardless of the particular disease. Providing this care in FTD, when the patient begins to lose the capacity for emotional responsivity, empathy, self-awareness, and social appropriateness, may be particularly damaging to the marital bond.

Acknowledgments

Supported by grants from the National Institute on Aging AG017766, AG019724; National Institute of Mental Health MH020006; and the State of California Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center of California 03-75271

We gratefully acknowledge our funding sources: National Institute on Aging, National Institute of Mental Health, and the State of California Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center of California. We thank Jennifer Arter and Emily Caesar for transcription management.

Footnotes

1

Because of the floor effect for control patients’ CDR scores, the analyses for marital satisfaction and emotional language were repeated comparing FTD and AD only, controlling for dementia severity (along with all other covariates included in the original analyses). The secondary analyses revealed the same pattern of differences between the two dementia groups as found when all three groups were included.

References

  • 1.Ory MG, Hoffman RR, 3rd, Yee JL, et al. Prevalence and impact of caregiving: A detailed comparison between dementia and nondementia caregivers. Gerontologist. 1999;39:177–185. doi: 10.1093/geront/39.2.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Torti FMJ, Gwyther LP, Reed SD, et al. A multinational review of recent trends and reports in dementia caregiver burden. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2004;18:99–109. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000126902.37908.b2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.de Vugt ME, Stevens F, Aalten P, et al. Behavioural disturbances in dementia patients and quality of the marital relationship. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003;18:149–154. doi: 10.1002/gps.807. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fearon M, Donaldson C, Burns A, et al. Intimacy as a determinant of expressed emotion in carers of people with Alzheimer’s disease. Psychol Med. 1998;28:1085–1090. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798007156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Morris LW, Morris RG, Britton PG. The relationship between marital intimacy, perceived strain and depression in spouse caregivers of dementia sufferers. Br J Med Psychol. 1988;61:231–236. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1988.tb02784.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Simonelli C, Tripodi F, Rossi R, et al. The influence of caregiver burden on sexual intimacy and marital satisfaction in couples with an Alzheimer spouse. Int J Clin Pract. 2008;62:47–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-1241.2007.01506.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Black W, Almeida OP. A systematic review of the association between the Behavioral and Psychological Symptoms of Dementia and burden of care. Int Psychogeriatr. 2004;16:295–315. doi: 10.1017/s1041610204000468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Coen RF, Swanwick GR, O’Boyle CA, et al. Behaviour disturbance and other predictors of carer burden in Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 1997;12:331–336. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rymer S, Salloway S, Norton L, et al. Impaired awareness, behavior disturbance, and caregiver burden in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2002;16:248–253. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200210000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schulz R, O’Brien AT, Bookwala J, et al. Psychiatric and physical morbidity effects of dementia caregiving: Prevalence, correlates, and causes. Gerontologist. 1995;35:771–791. doi: 10.1093/geront/35.6.771. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Takahashi M, Tanaka K, Miyaoka H. Depression and associated factors of informal caregivers versus professional caregivers of demented patients. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005;59:473–480. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01401.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lawrence RH, Tennstedt SL, Assmann SF. Quality of the caregiver--care recipient relationship: Does it offset negative consequences of caregiving for family caregivers? Psychol Aging. 1998;13:150–158. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.13.1.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Boxer AL, Miller BL. Clinical features of frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;19:S3–S6. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000183086.99691.91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Fernandez-Duque D, Black SE. Impaired recognition of negative facial emotions in patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2005;43:1673–1687. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Goodkind M, McCarthy M, Levenson RW. Emotion regulation in frontotemporal lobar degeneration and Alzheimer’s disease. Poster presented at: Emotion Preconference for the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Meeting; January 25–26, 2006; Palm Springs, CA. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Keane J, Calder AJ, Hodges JR, et al. Face and emotion processing in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:655–665. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00156-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lindau M, Almkvist O, Kushi J, et al. First symptoms--frontotemporal dementia versus Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2000;11:286–293. doi: 10.1159/000017251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Neary D, Snowden JS, Gustafson L, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology. 1998;51:1546–1554. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.6.1546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pachana NA, Boone KB, Miller BL, et al. Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2:505–510. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700001673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Perry RJ, Hodges JR. Differentiating frontal and temporal variant frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 2000;54:2277–2284. doi: 10.1212/wnl.54.12.2277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rosen HJ, Pace-Savitsky K, Perry RJ, et al. Recognition of emotion in the frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17:277–281. doi: 10.1159/000077154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sturm VE, Ascher EA, Miller BL, et al. Diminished self-conscious emotional responding in frontotemporal lobar degeneration patients. Emotion. 2008;8:861–869. doi: 10.1037/a0013765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sturm VE, Rosen HJ, Allison S, et al. Self-conscious emotion deficits in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain. 2006;129:2508–2516. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Werner KH, Roberts NA, Rosen HJ, et al. Emotional reactivity and emotion recognition in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neurology. 2007;69:148–155. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000265589.32060.d3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chen JC, Borson S, Scanlan JM. Stage-specific prevalence of behavioral symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease in a multi-ethnic community sample. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2000;8:123–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McKeith I, Cummings J. Behavioural changes and psychological symptoms in dementia disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2005;4:735–742. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(05)70219-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, et al. The stage specific temporal course of Alzheimer’s disease: Functional and behavioral concomitants based upon cross-sectional and longitudinal observation. Prog Clin Biol Res. 1989;317:23–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gregory CA, Serra-Mestres J, Hodges JR. Early diagnosis of the frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia: How sensitive are standard neuroimaging and neuropsychologic tests? Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1999;12:128–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Shinagawa S, Ikeda M, Fukuhara R, et al. Initial symptoms in frontotemporal dementia and semantic dementia compared with Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;21:74–80. doi: 10.1159/000090139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Bathgate D, Snowden JS, Varma A, et al. Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia. Acta Neurol Scand. 2001;103:367–378. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2001.2000236.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Boutoleau-Bretonniere C, Vercelletto M, Volteau C, et al. Zarit burden inventory and activities of daily living in the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25:272–277. doi: 10.1159/000117394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.de Vugt ME, Riedijk SR, Aalten P, et al. Impact of behavioural problems on spousal caregivers: a comparison between Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:35–41. doi: 10.1159/000093102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kaiser S, Panegyres PK. The psychosocial impact of young onset dementia on spouses. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2006;21:398–402. doi: 10.1177/1533317506293259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mioshi E, Bristow M, Cook R, et al. Factors underlying caregiver stress in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2009;27:76–81. doi: 10.1159/000193626. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Riedijk SR, De Vugt ME, Duivenvoorden HJ, et al. Caregiver burden, health-related quality of life and coping in dementia caregivers: A comparison of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:405–412. doi: 10.1159/000095750. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Locke HJ, Wallace KM. Short marital-adjustment and prediction tests: Their reliability and validity. Marriage Fam Living. 1959;21:251–255. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Gallagher-Thompson D, Dal Canto PG, Jacob T, et al. A comparison of marital interaction patterns between couples in which the husband does or does not have Alzheimer’s disease. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2001;56:S140–S150. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.3.s140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Levenson RW, Gottman JM. Marital interaction: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;45:587–597. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.45.3.587. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Roberts CW. Text Analysis for the Social Sciences: Methods for Drawing Statistical Inferences from Texts and Transcripts. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Pennebaker JW, Mehl MR, Niederhoffer KG. Psychological aspects of natural language use: Our words, our selves. Annu Rev Psychol. 2003;54:547–577. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Cervantes CA, Callanan MA. Labels and explanations in mother-child emotion talk: Age and gender differentiation. Dev Psychol. 1998;34:88–98. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hölzer M, Pokorny D, Kächele H, et al. The verbalization of emotions in the therapeutic dialogue: A correlate of therapeutic outcome? Psychother Res. 1997;7:261–273. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Marchitelli L, Levenson RW. When couples converse: The language and physiology of emotion. Poster presented at: Society for Psychophysiological Research Annual Meeting; October 14–18, 1992; San Diego, CA. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Rosenthal LH. Talking about feelings: The language of emotion and its relationship to physiology. Diss Abstr Int B. 1999;60:1314. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Eslinger PJ, Dennis K, Moore P, et al. Metacognitive deficits in frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:1630–1635. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.053157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Salmon E, Perani D, Collette F, et al. A comparison of unawareness in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79:176–179. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.122853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Williamson C, Alcantar O, Rothlind J, et al. Standardized measurement of self-awareness deficits in FTD and AD. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009 doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.166041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rankin KP, Baldwin E, Pace-Savitsky C, et al. Self awareness and personality change in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2005;76:632–639. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.042879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wright LK. The impact of Alzheimer’s disease on the marital relationship. Gerontologist. 1991;31:224–237. doi: 10.1093/geront/31.2.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wright LK. Affection and sexuality in the presence of Alzheimer’s disease: A longitudinal study. Sex Disabil. 1998;16:167–179. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. doi: 10.1212/wnl.34.7.939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43:2412–2414. doi: 10.1212/wnl.43.11.2412-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Daly E, Zaitchik D, Copeland M, et al. Predicting conversion to Alzheimer disease using standardized clinical information. Arch Neurol. 2000;57:675–680. doi: 10.1001/archneur.57.5.675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Levenson RW, Ascher E, Goodkind M, et al. Laboratory testing of emotion and frontal cortex. In: Goldenberg G, Miller BL, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology: Vol. 88 (3rd series). Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology. Edinburgh: Elsevier; 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mergenthaler E, Stinson CH. Psychotherapy transcription standards. Psychother Res. 1992;2:125–142. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Averill JR. A semantic atlas of emotional concepts. Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology. 1975;53:30. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Ortony A, Clore GL, Foss MA. The referential structure of the affective lexicon. Cogn Sci. 1987;11:341–364. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Shaver P, Schwartz J, Kirson D, et al. Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987;52:1061–1086. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.52.6.1061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Cook WL. Dyadic Data Analysis. New York: The Guilford Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Carstensen LL, Gottman JM, Levenson RW. Emotional behavior in long-term marriage. Psychol Aging. 1995;10:140–149. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.10.1.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Gottman JM, Levenson RW. Marital processes predictive of later dissolution: Behavior, physiology, and health. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1992;63:221–233. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.63.2.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Lough S, Gregory C, Hodges JR. Dissociation of social cognition and executive function in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurocase. 2001;7:123–130. doi: 10.1093/neucas/7.2.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lough S, Kipps CM, Treise C, et al. Social reasoning, emotion and empathy in frontotemporal dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44:950–958. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Dunkin JJ, Anderson-Hanley C. Dementia caregiver burden: A review of the literature and guidelines for assessment and intervention. Neurology. 1998;51:S53–60. doi: 10.1212/wnl.51.1_suppl_1.s53. discussion S65–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Schulz R, Martire LM. Family caregiving of persons with dementia: Prevalence, health effects, and support strategies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2004;12:240–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Rankin KP, Kramer JH, Miller BL. Patterns of cognitive and emotional empathy in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005;18:28–36. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152225.05377.ab. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Vitaliano PP, Young HM, Russo J, et al. Does expressed emotion in spouses predict subsequent problems among care recipients with Alzheimer’s disease? J Gerontol. 1993;48:P202–9. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.4.p202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Kokmen E, Ozsarfati Y, Beard CM, et al. Impact of referral bias on clinical and epidemiological studies of Alzheimer’s disease. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:79–83. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(95)00031-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Layde PM, Broste SK, Desbiens N, et al. Generalizability of clinical studies conducted at tertiary care medical centers: a population-based analysis. J Clin Epidemiol. 1996;49:835–841. doi: 10.1016/0895-4356(96)00006-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES